Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Public Health 1/2017

Open Access 01.12.2017 | Research article

The effect of a micronutrient powder home fortification program on anemia and cognitive outcomes among young children in rural China: a cluster randomized trial

verfasst von: Renfu Luo, Ai Yue, Huan Zhou, Yaojiang Shi, Linxiu Zhang, Reynaldo Martorell, Alexis Medina, Scott Rozelle, Sean Sylvia

Erschienen in: BMC Public Health | Ausgabe 1/2017

Abstract

Background

Anemia early in life has been associated with delayed cognitive and motor development. The WHO recommends home fortification using multiple micronutrient powders (MNPs) containing iron as a strategy to address anemia in children under two. We evaluated the effects of a program freely distributing MNP sachets to caregivers of infants in rural China.

Methods

We conducted a cluster-randomized controlled trial in Shaanxi province, enrolling all children aged 6–11 months in target villages. Following a baseline survey, investigators randomly assigned each village/cluster to a control or treatment group. In the treatment group, caregivers were instructed to give MNPs daily. Follow-up was after 6, 12, and 18 months of intervention. Primary outcomes were hemoglobin concentrations and scores on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development.

Results

One thousand, eight hundred and-two eligible children and their caregivers were enrolled. At baseline 48% (870) of children were anemic and 29% (529) were developmentally delayed. Six hundred and-ten children (117 villages) were assigned to the control group and 1192 children (234 villages) were assigned to the treatment group. Assignment to the treatment group was associated with an improvement in hemoglobin levels (marginal effect 1.77 g/L, 95% CI 0.017–3.520, p-value = 0.048) and cognitive development (marginal effect 2.23 points, 95% CI 0.061–4.399, p-value = 0.044) after 6 months but not thereafter. There were no significant effects on motor development. Zero effects after the first 6 months were not due to low compliance, low statistical power, or changes in feeding behavior. Hemoglobin concentrations improved in both the treatment and control groups over the course of the study; however, 22% (325) of children remained anemic at endline, and 48% (721) were cognitively delayed.

Conclusions

Providing caregivers with MNP sachets modestly hastened improvement in hemoglobin levels that was occurring absent intervention; however, this improvement did not translate into improved developmental outcomes at endline.

Trial registration

ISRCTN44149146; prospectively registered on 15th April 2013.
Abkürzungen
BSID
Bayley scales of infant development
ITT
Intent-to-treat
MNPs
Multiple micronutrient powders

Background

Despite a spectacular rise in well-being in China over the past three decades, a large number of individuals remain in poverty. As a result of this poverty and associated factors, under-nutrition remains stubbornly high, particularly among children in China’s poor rural areas. Although rates of stunting and wasting are low, a large fraction of children in poor regions suffer from micronutrient deficiencies [13]. By far, the most common of these is iron deficiency [46]. According to data from China’s Food and Nutrition Surveillance System, anemia rates among young children under 2 years of age in 2005 were 30–40% in poor rural areas nationally [3, 79]. More geographically-focused studies have shown anemia rates higher than 50% in some areas [912]. Given that there are more than 8 million children born each year in rural areas, this implies that roughly 3 million children under 2 years old in rural China are anemic [13]. In China, it is postulated that 90% of anemia in infants and young children results from iron deficiency [14].
Anemia in early childhood is known to be particularly pernicious as it can serve to perpetuate poverty by keeping children from reaching their developmental potential. In the short run, anemia and iron deficiency in the first years of life is a major risk factor for cognitive, physical and emotional delays [1523]. In the longer run, it can negatively affect educational attainment and even reduce earnings in adulthood [2427]. These consequences may be irreversible, even if anemia is corrected later in childhood [2428].
In settings with high prevalence of anemia (>20%) among children under 2 years of age, the WHO recommends home fortification of foods with multiple micronutrient powders (MNPs) [29]. This recommendation is based on a Cochrane review of eight randomized controlled trials that concluded MNPs to be effective in improving iron status and reducing anemia [30]. A more recent meta-analysis including several more recent studies also concluded that MNPs were effective in increasing hemoglobin concentration, but also noted substantial heterogeneity in effects across geographical settings [31]. In a recent cluster-randomized trial in Colombia, investigators found that MNPs were ineffective at reducing anemia or improving cognitive outcomes [31, 32]. They attribute the lack of efficacy of MNPs to the particular evolution of anemia with age among the Colombian population (early peak at under 1 year and rapid decline thereafter) and to a declining relative contribution of iron deficiency to remaining anemia as children age in Colombia. These characteristics of anemia set Colombia apart from settings where MNPs have been effective. In addition to geography, existing trials have also varied in aspects of study design including the age of targeted children, the duration of supplementation, and the timing of endline measurements [31].
Following the recommendations of the WHO, China’s government has announced intentions to implement a program to distribute MNPs through local clinics in nationally-designated poverty counties [33]. The program has already been initiated in select areas; however, the effectiveness of MNPs has yet to be rigorously evaluated in China.
We present the results of a large-scale effectiveness trial of home fortification using MNPs in villages in rural western China. In line with the national program, we designed the trial to begin providing supplements when children were 6–11 months old and track outcomes every 6 months over 18 months. In addition to hemoglobin and anemia, we evaluate effects on cognitive and psychomotor development measured using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID), given the well-established link between early childhood anemia and cognitive and physical delays [1523]. Such developmental delays in early childhood can have significant consequences long term. Studies have linked early childhood interventions to increased college attendance, employment, earnings as well as reductions in teen pregnancy and criminal activity [3436].

Methods

Study design, sampling frame, and participants

We carried out a cluster-randomized controlled trial in southern Shaanxi Province. Shaanxi is a relatively poor province in Western China and ranks 19 out of 31 provinces nationally in terms of wealth. To identify the sample, we first selected 174 townships from 11 nationally-designated poverty counties in southern Shaanxi. All townships in each county were included except (1) the one township per county that housed the county seat and (2) those townships that did not have any villages with at least 800 people. These exclusion criteria were chosen to ensure a rural sample and increase the likelihood that sampled villages had a sufficient number of children 6–11 months of age.
Within each township, sample villages were selected in April, 2013. We used official government data to compile a list of villages in each township. We then randomly selected 2 villages from the list in each township. An additional village was selected in randomly chosen townships to meet power requirements. Our final sample consisted of 351 villages. A list of all registered births over the past 12 months was obtained from the local family planning official in each village. All children in our desired age range (6–11 months) were enrolled in the study. A second cohort of children in our desired age range (6–11 months) was enrolled from the same sample villages in October 2013. Overall, the baseline sample included 1802 children.
Following the baseline survey for the first cohort, villages were allocated to an MNP arm or a control arm. For the first 6 months of the study, a random half of the MNP arm also received text message reminders. (Results are qualitatively similar excluding this group – this analysis is reported in Table 6 in Appendix.) The intervention period lasted for 18 months for both cohorts (from April 2013 to October 2014 for cohort 1 and from October 2013 to April 2015 for cohort 2).

Randomization and masking

Following the baseline survey, we randomly assigned 117 villages to the control group and 234 villages to the micronutrient group using computer code (in Stata version 12). Randomization was stratified within counties.
We did not use a placebo in the control group for ethical and practical reasons. Caregivers were not aware that they were in an RCT.

Interventions

We used a Heinz-produced MNP called “NurtureMate.” The powder is tasteless and each daily-dose sachet contains 6 mg of iron as well as zinc, vitamins A, C, D, B1, B2,B6, B12, and folic acid (Table 1). NurtureMate is recommended for infants and toddlers aged 6–36 months. The full composition of the NurtureMate sachets was based on the general standard for complementary food supplements in China [37] and was approved for use in the national program to distribute MNPs through local clinics in poverty counties (piloting for the national program began after the start of this study, but not in our study regions).
Table 1
Composition of NurtureMate home fortification powdersa
Nutrients
Unit
Average content per packet (1 g)
RNI or AI in China (6–11 months) per dayb
RNI or AI in China (12–36 months) per day
RNI or AI (%)
6–11 month
12–36 month
Iron (ferrous lactate)
mg
6.0
10
12
60%
50%
Zinc (zinc sulfate)
mg
4.80
8
9
60%
53%
Vitamin A
μgRE
200
400
500
50%
40%
Vitamin C
mg
50.0
50
60
100%
83%
Vitamin D
μg
5.0
10
10
50%
50%
Vitamin E
mg
1.55
3
4
52%
39%
Vitamin B1
mg
0.30
0.3
0.6
100%
50%
Vitamin B2
mg
0.50
0.5
0.6
100%
83%
Vitamin B6
mg
0.30
0.3
0.5
100%
60%
Vitamin B12
μg
0.5
0.5
0.9
100%
56%
Folic acid
μg
66
80
150
83%
44%
Niacin
mg
3.0
3
6
100%
50%
Energy
kJ
15
  
Protein
g
0
  
Fat
g
0
  
Carbohydrate
g
0.9
  
aNurtureMate can be mixed in with either water or complementary foods, such as porridge, and is recommended for children aged 6–36 months. Recommended consumption is 5–7 sachets per week (one per day)
bRNI: Recommended Nutrient Intake. AI: Adequate Intakes
Caregivers in the MNP arm were given a 6-month supply of sachets every 6 months. At the first distribution they were also given information about the causes and consequences of anemia along with oral and written instructions on how to use the powder, specifically to give one packet per day mixed with the child’s food. During the first distribution, households were also given a plastic storage envelope in which to store the empty NurtureMate sachets. Thus, the treatment was designed to include components of a policy distributing MNP sachets (including information on sachets and use and interaction every 6 months through sachet distribution). This allows us to identify the effects of such a policy, beyond the impact of MNPs themselves.

Data collection

The research team conducted four rounds of data collection for each cohort. The initial baseline survey was conducted in April 2013 for cohort 1 and in October 2013 for cohort 2. During each survey round, nurses from Xi’an Jiaotong Medical School collected data on hemoglobin concentrations from all children. Hemoglobin concentrations were measured using HemoCue Hb 201+ finger prick systems (HemoCue Ltd., Ängelholm, Sweden).
Teams of enumerators collected socioeconomic data from study households. Each child’s primary caregiver was identified and administered a survey on child, parent and household characteristics including each child’s gender, birth order, maternal age and education. Each child’s age was obtained from his or her birth certificate. The primary caregiver was identified by each family as the individual most responsible for the child’s care (typically the child’s mother or grandmother).
As part of the household survey in each round of data collection, the primary caregiver was asked about the diets of children in the household. Caregivers were asked whether the child’s diet during the previous day included items from each of seven food categories: meat or fish; beans, nuts or seeds; milk products; eggs; fruits and vegetables rich in Vitamin A; other fruits and vegetables; and grains. We designed questions about food consumption as a series of yes/no questions to minimize measurement error. Detailed questions were also asked about breastfeeding and formula consumption.
Enumerators tallied unused and empty packets to assess compliance in follow-up surveys. Although there is some potential for measurement error, this compliance measure is unlikely to be biased because households and enumerators had little incentive to misreport use of MNP sachets. Further, measures based on counting empty sachets were consistent with the count of unopened sachets and with responses given by caregivers about sachet use.
All children were also administered the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID) Version I, an internationally-scaled test of children’s cognitive and motor development [38]. The BSID is widely used in research on early childhood development, and is often considered a “gold standard” for assessing early childhood development [39]. The test was formally adapted to the Chinese language and environment in 1992 and scaled according to an urban Chinese sample [40, 41]. Following other published studies that use the BSID to assess child development in China [4244], it was this officially adapted version of the test that was used in this study. All BSID enumerators attended a week-long training course on how to administer the BSID, including a 2.5 day experiential learning program in the field. The test was administered in the household using a standardized set of toys and detailed scoring sheet. The BSID takes into consideration each child’s age in days, as well as whether he or she was premature at birth. These two factors, combined with the child’s performance on a series of tasks using the standardized toy kit, contribute to the establishment of the two sub-indices: MDI, which evaluates memory, habitation, problem solving, early number concepts, generalization, classification, vocalization and language to produce a measure of cognitive development; and PDI, which evaluates gross motor skills (rolling, crawling and creeping, sitting and standing, walking, running and jumping) and fine motor skills to produce a measure of psychomotor development [45]. For the analysis, raw scores for each of these sub-indices are normalized using the distribution of scores in a reference group of healthy children in China of the same age supplied by the testing company. These normalized scores have an expected mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 16, with impairment for each index defined as a score below 80.

Statistical analysis

We analytically calculated the sample size for the study to detect a 0.2 standardized effect on MDI scores. Based on previous studies [41, 4649], we assumed an intracluster correlation of 0.1 and that baseline scores would account for 50% of the variation in scores at endline. We further assumed that there would be four sample children per village after attrition. Based on these parameters, we calculated that we required 112 villages/group to detect a standardized effect of 0.2 at 80% power given a significance level of 0.05. We added five villages to each group to overpower the study and to account for potential attrition as funding allowed.
The primary outcomes of the study were hemoglobin (measured in g/L) and the two subindices of the BSID: the MDI and PDI. We also report effects on anemia (using the WHO definition of altitude-adjusted Hb concentration below 110 g/L) [50] for comparability with existing lieterature, but note that there is uncertainty over the appropriateness of hemoglobin thresholds to define anemia status [51].
We estimated the treatment effects on our primary outcomes using intent-to-treat (ITT) analysis. We used multiple regression analysis to estimate ITT impacts. When estimating effects on hemoglobin concentration, we adjust for child gender and age and also control for Hb test machine fixed effects. When estimating effects on BSID scores, we control for child gender, age, caregiver engagement (playing/singing yesterday) and BSID tester (enumerator) fixed effects. All regressions control for cohort fixed effects and county (strata) fixed effects to account for the randomization procedure. In all analyses, we account for clustering within villages (the level of randomization) using Huber-White cluster-adjusted standard errors.
In order to accommodate partial compliance and to estimate the effect of MNP sachet use on hemoglobin concentrations and development scores, we use an instrumental variables approach to estimate the average treatment on the treated effect [52]. Specifically, we instrument the total number of sachets consumed (as of each survey round) with a variable indicating whether the child was assigned to the MNP group (the instrumental variable). This allows us to estimate the causal effect of MNP sachet consumption, assuming a linear relationship.
In order to aid in the interpretation of effects on the primary outcomes of the study (hemoglobin and BSID scores), we also assess impacts of the MNP intervention on feeding behavior. The MNP intervention may cause caregivers to feed children more nutritious foods if, for example, the intervention increased awareness of or attention to nutrition generally. On the other hand, it could also “crowd-out” more nutritious foods if MNPs are viewed by caregivers as a substitute for food sources of micronutrients. For each of the seven food categories, we estimate the ITT effects in the same manner as with the primary outcomes, but controlling only for cohort and county fixed effects.
Statistical analyses were conducted using STATA 13.0 (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA). P-values below 0.05 are considered statistically significant.

Results

A total of 1834 children were enrolled at baseline across the two cohorts (Fig. 1). We excluded 32 children due to serious illnesses. Table 2 shows characteristics of children included in the analysis sample at baseline. Of the 1802 children, 1592 (88%) remained in the sample at 12–17 months of age, 1585 (88%) remained in the sample at 18–23 months of age, and 1490 (82%) remained in the sample at 24–29 months of age. No villages (clusters) were lost. Attrition rates did not vary significantly between the experimental arms (Table 7 in Appendix).
Table 2
Baseline characteristics by experimental arm (N = 1802)
Characteristics
Control Group
(n = 610)
Free MNP Group1
(n = 1192)
P 2
Social economic status
 Age in months3
9.46 ± 1.90
9.50 ± 1.83
0.75
  Girls (%)
49.0 (299)
46.5 (544)
0.26
 Low birth weight (%)
4.4 (27)
4.7 (56)
0.80
  First birth (%)
62.3 (380)
62.7 (747)
0.88
 Families received social security support (%)
24.4 (149)
23.2 (276)
0.45
Caregiver and mother characteristics
 Mother is primary caregiver (%)
79.2 (483)
81.8 (975)
0.31
 Maternal education ≥9 years (%)
78.4 (478)
82.3 (981)
0.07
 Maternal age (year)
26.2 ± 4.3
26.5 ± 4.7
0.25
Child feeding practices
 Ever breastfed (%)
87.7 (535)
89.3 (1064)
0.36
 Exclusive or predominant breastfeeding <6 Months (%)
37.7 (230)
37.8 (451)
0.97
 Still breastfed ≥12 Months (%)
39.2 (239)
37.3 (444)
0.14
 Ever formula-fed (%)
63.4 (387)
66.4 (792)
0.07
 Supplementary feeding after six months (%)
65.9 (402)
65.3 (778)
0.87
Parenting practices
 Played with baby yesterday with toys (%)
49.8 (304)
46.7 (557)
0.12
 Sang song to baby yesterday (%)
29.0 (177)
28.7 (342)
0.78
Child nutrition status
 Hemoglobin concentration (g/L) 4
109.3 ± 13.0
109.1 ± 12.5
0.71
 Anemia prevalence (%)4
49.3 (297)
48.5 (573)
0.88
Child development status
 MDI test score5
97.2 ± 17.0
96.6 ± 16.9
0.87
 PDI test score6
90.1 ± 18.0
90.1 ± 16.7
0.85
Data are presented as mean ± SD or % (n) for categorical variables
1MNP = Multiple Micronutrient Powder
2 P-values from tests of the null hypothesis of no difference between MNP and control groups, adjusted for cohort and county fixed effects and clustering within villages
3 Data missing for 4 children in the free MNP group
4Data missing for 7 children in control group and 10 in the free MNP group
5Data missing for 1 child in control group and 4 in the free MNP group
6Data missing for 2 children in control group and 5 in the free MNP group
7Data missing for 4 children in control group and 2 in the free MNP group
At baseline, the average hemoglobin concentration of 6–11 month olds was 109 g/L. Nearly half of the sample children (49%) were anemic. Fourteen percent of children were delayed in their cognitive development (MDI score < 80) and 24% of children were delayed in their psychomotor development (PDI score < 80); 29% of children had either cognitive or psychomotor delay.
The average number of sachets consumed each month in the MNP group over the course of the study was 14 (SD 11.6—Table 8 in Appendix). The average number of sachets consumed declined slightly across the survey waves from 16 sachets per month in the first 6-month period to 13 sachets in the third.
Figure 2 shows the average hemoglobin concentration (Panel A) and anemia prevalence (Panel B) with 95% confidence intervals over the four survey rounds, separately for the MNP and control group. Overall, there is a steady increase in hemoglobin levels (and a steady decrease in anemia rates) over the course of the study in both groups. In the control group, average hemoglobin rose from 109 g/L on average when children were 6–11 months old to 118 g/L when children were 24–29 months old. Corresponding anemia rates more than halved, but remained high (23%).
Trends in the MDI (Panel C) and PDI (Panel D) indices also are presented in Fig. 2. Over the course of the study there was a marked decline in average MDI scores from 97 to 81. The number of children with cognitive delay increased from 14% at 6–11 months of age to 49% at 24–29 months of age. In contrast to MDI, we find that average PDI scores increased over time, rising from 90 to 102.
Table 3 shows the estimated treatment effects of the MNP intervention on hemoglobin concentration, anemia status and the two BSID sub-indices based on the ITT analysis. For each outcome, we report impact estimates at 6, 12, and 18 months after the start of the intervention.
Table 3
Effects of multiple micronutrient powder provision on haemoglobin (Hb) concentration, anaemia prevalence, and Bayley mental and psychomotor development scores
 
Hb concentration (g/L)1
Anemia prevalence (Hb <110 g/L)1
Bayley MDI (points)2
Bayley PDI (points)2
 
Marginal Effect (95% CI)
P value
Marginal Effect (95% CI)
P value
Marginal Effect (95% CI)
P value
Marginal Effect (95% CI)
P value
Effect of MNP distribution after 6 months (children 12–18 months of age)
1.769 (0.017–3.520)
0.048*
− 0.058 (− 0.127–0.010)
0.095
2.230 (0.061–4.399)
0.044*
− 0.266 (− 3.328–2.796)
0.864
Effect of MNP distribution after 12 months
− 0.115 (− 2.189–1.959)
0.913
0.034 (− 0.039–0.107)
0.357
0.487 (− 2.071–3.046)
0.708
− 0.461 (− 2.938–2.016)
0.715
Effect of MNP distribution after 18 months
0.127 (− 1.850–2.105)
0.899
− 0.001 (− 0.071–0.070)
0.982
0.828 (− 1.787–3.444)
0.534
0.084 (− 2.277–2.445)
0.944
Intracluster Correlation (ICC)3
0.09
0.08
0.02
R-squared
0.14
0.10
0.27
0.21
Observations
6092
6092
6204
6186
1Intention to treat analysis, and regression estimates from multiple linear models adjusted for gender, age, Hb test machine and county fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the village level. Hb data missing for 349 observations (17 in baseline, 115, 144 and 73 in 6 months, 12 months and 18 months follow-up)
2Intention to treat analysis, and regression estimates from multiple linear models adjusted for gender, age, parenting practice (whether caregiver used toys played with child the previous day, sang to child previous day), BSID tester and county fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the village level. MDI data missing for 214 observations (5 in baseline, 101, 88 and 20 in 6 months, 12 months and 18 months follow-up), and PDI data missing for 233 children (7 in baseline, 102, 98 and 26 in 6 months, 12 months and 18 months follow-up)
3Intracluster correlation (ICC) at endline (final wave). No ICC is reported for anemia as this is a binary outcome
*p-value < 0.05 significant
The effect of the MNP intervention on average hemoglobin concentration was 1.77 g/L (p = 0.048; 95% CI 0.02—3.52 g/L) after 6 months (when children were 12 to 17 months of age). This effect did not persist, however. At 12 and 18 months after the start of the intervention, estimated effects are not statistically different from zero. After 6 months of the intervention, we find a reduction in anemia of six percentage points (18%); however, this does not reach statistical significance at 5%. We find no effect on anemia status after 12 or 18 months of intervention.
The effect of the MNP intervention on cognition (MDI) shows a pattern similar to that of hemoglobin concentration. After 6 months of supplementation, the effect of MNP consumption on the MDI sub-index was 2.23 points (p = 0.044; 95% CI 0.061—4.399), equal to 0.13 SD of the baseline distribution in the control group. We did not detect a significant effect on MDI after 12 or 18 months (p > 0.5). We did not detect any significant effect on PDI scores in any of the time periods. No significant harms were reported.
We find no significant effects for any time period when estimating effects on the probability of delay (MDI or PDI < 80) rather than the continuous MDI and PDI measures. These results are reported in Tables 9 and 10 in Appendix.
Table 4 reports the effects of MNP consumption estimated using instrumental variables (IV) in order to account for imperfect compliance. We estimate that each additional sachet consumed during the first 6 months of the program increased hemoglobin concentration by 0.023 g/L (p = 0.04; 95% CI 0.001—0.045). This estimate suggests that with full compliance (the recommended one sachet per day), MNPs increased hemoglobin concentration by 4.14 g/L (180 days × 0.023 g/L) assuming a linear relationship. The implied increase is 2.07 g/L with 50% compliance. Similar to the ITT estimates, however, we do not find a significant effect of MNPs on hemoglobin concentrations after 12 or 18 months even after adjusting for compliance. The analysis does not find any significant effects on anemia prevalence in any of the time periods after the baseline.
Table 4
Dose–response effects of multiple micronutrient powder sachet consumption on heamoglobin (Hb) concentration, anaemia prevalence, and Bayley mental and psychomotor development scores
 
Hb concentration (g/L)1
Anemia prevalence (Hb <110 g/L)1
Bayley MDI (points)2
Bayley PDI (points)2
 
Marginal Effect (95% CI)
P value
Marginal Effect (95% CI)
P value
Marginal Effect (95% CI)
P value
Marginal Effect (95% CI)
P value
Effect of MNP sachet consumption after 6 months (dose–response per sachet)
0.023 (0.001–0.045)
0.040*
− 0.001 (− 0.002–0.0001)
0.087
0.028 (0.002–0.054)
0.034*
− 0.001 (− 0.038–0.035)
0.943
Effect of MNP sachet consumption after 12 months
− 0.002 (− 0.026–0.022)
0.883
0.000 (− 0.001–0.001)
0.427
0.007 (− 0.022–0.035)
0.640
− 0.004 (− 0.031–0.023)
0.774
Effect of MNP sachet consumption after 18 months
0.002 (− 0.022–0.027)
0.861
− 0.000 (− 0.001–0.001)
0.977
0.015 (− 0.016–0.047)
0.346
0.003 (− 0.026–0.031)
0.843
R-squared
0.14
0.10
0.26
0.21
Observations
6003
6003
6113
6096
1 Average Treatment on the Treated analysis, using the treatment assignment as an instrumental variable for the total number of sachets used in the preceding six months. Regression estimates from multiple linear models adjusted for gender, age, Hb test machine and county fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the village level. Hb data missing for 349 children (17 in baseline, 115, 144 and 73 in 6 months, 12 months and 18 months follow-up) and feeding behavior data missing for 206 children (74, 91 and 41 in 6 months, 12 months and 18 months follow-up)
2 Average Treatment on the Treated analysis, using the treatment assignment as an instrumental variable for the total number of sachets used in the preceding six months. Regression estimates from multiple linear models adjusted for gender, age, parenting practice (whether caregiver used toys played with child the previous day, sang to child previous day), BSID tester and county fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the village level. MDI data missing for 214 children (5 in baseline, 101, 88 and 20 in 6 months, 12 months and 18 months follow-up), PDI data missing for 233 children (7 in baseline, 102, 98 and 26 in 6 months, 12 months and 18 months follow-up), and feeding behavior data missing for 206 children (74, 91 and 41 in 6 months, 12 months and 18 months follow-up)
*p-value < 0.05 significant
For MDI, the IV analysis suggests that each additional sachet consumed increased MDI by 0.028 points (p = 0.034; 95% CI 0.002—0.054) at 6 months. By 12 months after the start of the intervention, however, this effect also subsided. The analysis did not detect any effects on MDI at 12 or 18 months. We did not detect any significant effect on PDI scores in any of the time periods.
Results for feeding behavior are presented in Table 5. At baseline (when children were 6–11 months of age), we find that few children in either group were fed iron rich foods such as meat (around 18% were fed meat or fish) or legumes (around 18% report feeding beans, nuts and seeds). Most children were fed grains (approximately 86%), milk products (around 58%), and fruits and vegetables rich in Vitamin A (around 55%) the previous day. Eggs were fed to around 34% of children and other fruits and vegetables were fed to 48% of children.
Table 5
Feeding of sample child by age and experimental arm1
Food fed the previous day:
6–11 months
12–17 months
18–23 months
24–29 months
Control Group
(n = 285)
MNP Group2
(n = 549)
P3
Control Group
(n = 514)
MNP Group2
(n = 989)
P3
Control Group
(n = 510)
MNP Group2
(n = 988)
P3
Control Group
(n = 511)
MNP Group2
(n = 968)
P3
Meat or fish
18.3 (52)
18.4 (101)
0.94
34.8 (179)
39.1 (386)
0.09
51.0 (210)
51.1 (505)
0.89
56.2 (287)
57.2 (553)
0.62
Beans, nuts, or seed products
17.9 (51)
18.0 (99)
0.90
26.9 (138)
26.6 (263)
0.78
36.5 (186)
35.1 (347)
0.45
42.3 (216)
38.6 (374)
0.13
Milk products
57.9 (165)
60.1 (330)
0.46
78.4 (402)
81.0 (801)
0.19
82.1 (417)
81.7 (806)
0.79
67.1 (343)
72.9 (704)
0.03*
Eggs
33.7 (96)
30.6 (168)
0.39
45.3 (233)
47.1 (465)
0.43
44.1 (225)
47.7 (471)
0.20
45.1 (230)
46.7 (452)
0.47
Vegetables and fruits rich in Vitamin A
55.3 (157)
54.8 (300)
0.84
73.0 (375)
78.9 (779)
0.02*
80.6 (411)
81.8 (807)
0.60
80.8 (413)
84.6 (819)
0.07
Other vegetables and fruits
47.7 (136)
45.0 (247)
0.44
59.0 (303)
61.3 (606)
0.43
69.8 (306)
68.5 (677)
0.71
69.2 (353)
67.1 (649)
0.32
Grains
85.6 (244)
86.3 (474)
0.77
94.8 (487)
95.2 (941)
0.78
97.8 (498)
97.4 (962)
0.55
97.9 (500)
99.1 (960)
0.07
Data are presented as % (n) for categorical variables
1 We only collected detail information of diet for cohort 2 in the baseline (child aged 6–11 months), but for child aged 12–30 months, we collected detailed information of diet for cohort 1 and cohort 2
2MNP = Multiple Micronutrient Powder
3 P-values from tests of the null hypothesis of no difference between MNP and control groups, adjusted for cohort and county fixed effects and clustering within villages *p-value < 0.05 significant
How feeding practices transition as children age is also evident in Table 5. Focusing on the responses of caregivers in the control group across the four survey waves, three patterns are of particular note. First, a dramatic overall change in diet occurs between when children are 6–11 months to when they are 12–17 months of age. There is a significant increase in the consumption of all food categories. This is not unexpected as this is a time when solid foods are introduced.
Second, there is an initial increase followed by a leveling off of consumption of grains, eggs, and fruits and vegetables. Nearly all children are consuming grains by 12–17 months. Around 45% of children are consuming eggs by 12–17 months. The consumption of fruits and vegetables is increasing until children are 18–23 months. By this time around 80% of children were fed vegetables or fruits rich in Vitamin A the previous day and around 70% had been fed other fruits and vegetables.
Finally, there is a gradual increase across the four waves in the consumption of iron-rich foods (meat and fish; beans, nuts and seeds). However, by 24–29 months consumption of beans, nuts and seeds remains below 50% and the consumption of meat and fish remains below 60%.
These patterns are largely consistent across both the control and intervention groups; in other words, we do not find that the intervention had important effects on feeding behavior. Although children in the intervention group consume significantly higher quantities both of vegetables and fruits rich in Vitamin A at 12–17 months, and of milk products at 24–29 months, there are few significant differences overall. There is no evidence that the intervention crowded out nutritious foods.

Discussion and conclusion

In a large-scale cluster randomized trial of multiple micronutrient powders (MNPs) in rural China, we found that home fortification using MNPs significantly increased hemoglobin levels after 6 months of supplementation (when children were 12 to 17 months of age) but that hemoglobin levels were not significantly higher than the control group after 12 months of supplementation (when children were 18 to 23 months of age) or after 18 months of supplementation (when children were 24 to 29 months of age). During this period from 6 to 30 months of age, a time in which children’s diets are in transition, hemoglobin concentrations in the control group increased rapidly (although anemia levels remained high). The MNP intervention hastened this improvement during the first 6 months, however, these positive effects subsided thereafter as hemoglobin levels accelerated absent intervention as children aged from 12 to 17 months to 18–23 months of age.
The effects of the intervention on cognition (as measured by MDI scores on the BSID) followed a similar pattern of effects over time to that observed on hemoglobin concentration. We found a significant improvement on the MDI score at 6 months. However–although it would be reasonable to hypothesize a sustained or delayed effect of supplementation on cognitive development even after MNPs no longer affected hemoglobin–cognitive scores were also not significantly higher than the control group after 12 or 18 months of supplementation. MNPs had no detectable effect on motor development at any point during the trial.
Using an IV analysis to adjust for partial compliance, we find a similar pattern of results, suggesting that the lack of effects in later rounds was not due to reduced consumption of the MNP sachets. Moreover, nil effects after the 6 month mark were unlikely due to low power. The study retained power to detect small effects throughout the trial as evidenced by relatively tight confidence intervals on impact estimates in later rounds.
Although it is difficult to say what led to this lack of effects, we note that our results are strikingly similar to those of a recent trial testing MNPs in Colombia [31, 32]. The authors of the Colombia study suggest that the zero/low effects in that study may have been due to the changing nature of the causes of low hemoglobin and rapidly decreasing anemia rates over time as children (in the control group) age. As with our study, they find that hemoglobin concentrations in the control group increased substantially as children aged from 12 to 30 months. This is in contrast to less rapid increases in hemoglobin concentration (absent intervention) in other regions where trials of MNPs have been successful [53, 54]. We find no evidence that the MNP intervention changed food consumption suggesting that the lack of observed effects is not due to a crowding-out of nutritious foods.
Another possibility is that the amount of iron in the sachets used in this study (6 mg) was simply too low. Most previous trials of MNPs used sachets that contained 12.5 mg of iron [30, 31]. The amount of iron in the sachets was set at 6 mg in order to comply with regulations in China [37]. Sachets with similarly low iron content have been suggested for use in a national program distributing sachets through local health centers.
One major difference between this study and the proposed national program is that the national program would provide MNP sachets through local health centers while we delivered sachets to the homes of caregivers. The average effect of the national program is therefore likely to be smaller than what we find in this study with home delivery, assuming that take-up rates would be lower. The provision of sachets through local health centers may also lead to selective take-up, potentially to the disadvantage of anemic children most in need of intervention.
This study makes important contributions to the evidence base on the use of MNPs to address micronutrient deficiency among children. The main strength of the study is that it is an effectiveness trial designed to mimic the effects of a government program. Most existing studies of home fortification programs have employed weekly or biweekly visits from researchers to a tightly controlled researcher-implemented environment [5558], an approach which would be costly to implement at scale. In our study, we delivered the NurtureMate every 6 months to the households, and left households alone to resume their regular routines.
This study also employed a more extensive data collection effort than previous trials. First, we evaluate effects on both anemia and measures of cognitive development. The three rounds of follow-up surveys at 6 month intervals also allow us to investigate how impacts of MNPs evolve over time. We are aware of only two other studies of micronutrient powders that collected as many rounds of follow-up information [53, 54].
Despite these strengths, our study does have several limitations. First, the observed attrition rate was relatively high: 28% of sample children were missing from at least one of the three survey rounds. Overall, however, 97% of the sample was followed-up at least once, which is high compared to previous studies. Attrition rates were similar between the treatment and control groups (as shown in Table 7 in Appendix), but results could have been affected if mechanisms driving attrition differed across groups. We find no evidence of this and do not believe that our findings are invalidated by attrition. A second limitation is that we were unable to conduct information on biomarkers beyond hemoglobin. Additional indicators of iron status, markers of inflammation and additional mineral and vitamin biomarkers would have been desirable. Third, the trial was conducted in one poor region of western China; results may not generalize to other settings. Finally, while we speculate above, we are unable to say definitively why MNPs were ineffective at reducing anemia rates as children aged past 1 year. A priority for future studies should be to better understand the drivers of anemia in young children and how these change as children age.
In sum, our results show that MNPs (using the formulation of China’s proposed national program) did not improve anemia or cognitive outcomes of children after 18 months of age. Although anemia rates appear to fall quickly as children age, they remained high at 2 years of age. Such results suggest that the government might consider raising the level of iron that is allowed in MNP supplements. More alarming is the high level of cognitive delay among 2 year olds. Given that the national program to distribute MNPs is unlikely to address high levels of cognitive delay among children, there is an urgent need for effective strategies to promote the development of young children in China’s rural areas. Without effective interventions, a large fraction of China’s children will be prevented from reaching their full potential.

Acknowledgements

We thank the Xi’an Jiaotong School of Medicine for assistance with the surveys. We would also like to thank the dedicated leaders and local cadres at the National Health and Family Planning Commission for their unparalleled assistance in implementing this study.

Funding

International Initiative for Impact Evaluation (3ie), National Natural Science Foundation of China (71473239, 71703083), UBS Optimus Foundation, China Medical Board, Bank of East Asia, Huaqiao Foundation, Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation-HBGDki Initiative, Noblesse, H.J. Heinz Company Foundation.
The funders of the study had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report. The corresponding author had full access to all the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
The protocol was approved by the institutional review boards of Stanford University (No. 25734) and Sichuan University (No. 2013005–01). All participating caregivers gave their oral consent for their own and their child’s involvement in the program.
My manuscript does not contain data from any individual person.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Anhänge

Appendix

Table 6
Effects of multiple micronutrient powder provision on heamoglobin (Hb) concentration, anaemia prevalence, and Bayley mental and psychomotor development scores by experimental assignment in first period
 
Hb concentration (g/L)1
Anemia prevalence (Hb <110 g/L)1
Bayley MDI (points)2
Bayley PDI (points)2
Trial arms compared
Difference (95% CI)
P value
Marginal effect (95% CI)
P- value
Difference (95% CI)
P- value
Marginal effect (95% CI)
P- value
Free vs control after 6 months follow-up3
1.840 (− 0.189–3.868)
0.075
− 0.048 (− 0.129–0.034)
0.252
3.025 (0.443–5. 606)
0.022*
− 1.624 (− 5.166–1.918)
0.368
Free vs control after 12 months3
− 0.502 (− 2.760–1.757)
0.662
0.026 (− 0.056–0.108)
0.535
1.978 (− 0.899–4.855)
0.177
− 0.846 (− 3.807–2.114)
0.574
Free vs control after 18 months3
− 0.718 (− 2.930–1.494)
0.524
0.026 (− 0.054–0.107)
0.523
2.808 (− 0.159–5.774)
0.064
− 0.162 (− 2.785–2.419)
0.902
Text vs control after 6 months4
1.715 (− 0.347–3.777)
0.103
− 0.069 (− 0.146–0.007)
0.075
1.423 (− 1.184–4.030)
0.284
1.061 (− 2.379–4.501)
0.544
Text vs control after 12 months 4
0.266 (− 2.220–2.752)
0.833
0.042 (− 0.046–0.129)
0.348
− 0.70 (− 3.788–2.389)
0.656
− 0.062 (− 2.886–2.763)
0.966
Text vs control after 18 months 4
0.936 (−1.419–3.290)
0.435
− 0.027 (− 0.110–0.056)
0.526
− 0.776 (− 3.90–2.348)
0.626
0.347 (− 2.600–3.294)
0.817
R-squared
0.14
0.10
0.27
0.21
Observations
6092
6092
6204
6186
1 Intention to treat analysis, and regression estimates from multiple linear models adjusted for gender, age, Hb test machine and county fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the village level
2 Intention to treat analysis, and regression estimates from multiple linear models adjusted for gender, age, parenting practice (whether caregiver used toys played with child the previous day, sang to child previous day), BSID tester and county fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the village level
3 “Free” group received free MNP sachets but not daily text message reminders
4 “Text” group received free MNP sachets and daily text message reminders for the first six months of the trial
*p-value < 0.05 significant
Table 7
Comparison of attrition across MNP and control groups (N = 1802)
Dependent Variable:
Lost to follow up at least once (yes = 1, no = 0) 1
Trial arms compared
Difference (95% CI)
P value
MNP vs control
0.018 (− 0.026–0.063)
0.412
R-squared
0.03
Observations
1802
1Regression estimates from multiple linear models adjusted for county fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the village level
Table 8
The average number sachets consumed monthly in MNP group
Intervention time
Free MNP Groupa
 
Average number of sachets consumed monthly
First 6 months intervention
15.73 ± 12.1
Between month 7 and 12 of intervention
14.1 ± 11.0
Between month 13 and 18 of intervention
12.9 ± 11.6
Average
14.3 ± 11.6
Data are presented as mean ± SD
aMNP = Multiple micronutrient powders
Table 9
Effects of multiple micronutrient powder provision on Bayley mental and psychomotor development delay
 
Mental delay (MDI < 80)1
Psychomotor delay (PDI < 80)1
Marginal Effect (95% CI)
P value
Marginal Effect (95% CI)
P value
Effect of MNP distribution after 6 months (children 12–18 months of age)
− 0.037 (− 0.087–0.013)
0.147
0.000 (− 0.061–0.062)
0.995
Effect of MNP distribution after 12 months
− 0.049 (− 0.109–0.011)
0.111
0.025 (− 0.025–0.075)
0.715
Effect of MNP distribution after 18 months
− 0.019 (− 0.081–0.042)
0.533
0.015 (− 0.036–0.066)
0.944
R-squared
0.23
0.13
Observations
6204
6186
1 Intention to treat analysis, and regression estimates from multiple linear models adjusted for gender, age, parenting practice (whether caregiver used toys played with child the previous day, sang to child previous day), BSID tester and county fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the village level. MDI data missing for 214 observations (5 in baseline, 101, 88 and 20 in 6 months, 12 months and 18 months follow-up), and PDI data missing for 233 children (7 in baseline, 102, 98 and 26 in 6 months, 12 months and 18 months follow-up)
Table 10
Dose–response effects of multiple micronutrient powder provision on Bayley mental and psychomotor development delay
 
Mental delay (MDI < 80)1
Psychomotor delay (PDI < 80)1
 
Marginal Effect (95% CI)
P value
Marginal Effect (95% CI)
P value
Effect of MNP distribution after 6 months (children 12–18 months of age)
− 0.000 (− 0.001–0.0002)
0.146
− 0.000 (− 0.000–0.000)
0.936
Effect of MNP distribution after 12 months
− 0.001 (− 0.001–0.000)
0.101
0.000 (− 0.00–0.00)
0.323
Effect of MNP distribution after 18 months
− 0.000 (− 0.001–0.000)
0.396
0.00 (− 0.001–0.001)
0.667
R-squared
0.23
0.12
Observations
6204
6186
1 Intention to treat analysis, and regression estimates from multiple linear models adjusted for gender, age, parenting practice (whether caregiver used toys played with child the previous day, sang to child previous day), BSID tester and county fixed effects. Standard errors are clustered at the village level. MDI data missing for 214 observations (5 in baseline, 101, 88 and 20 in 6 months, 12 months and 18 months follow-up), and PDI data missing for 233 children (7 in baseline, 102, 98 and 26 in 6 months, 12 months and 18 months follow-up)
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Luo R, Wang X, Zhang L, et al. High anemia prevalence in western China. Southeast Asian J Trop Med Public Health. 2011;42:1204–13.PubMed Luo R, Wang X, Zhang L, et al. High anemia prevalence in western China. Southeast Asian J Trop Med Public Health. 2011;42:1204–13.PubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen YC, Tang SC, Le H, Yu XM, Wang DG, Hao M. Compare health development of rural areas in different districts of China: about social economy and inhabitant’s health condition [in Chinese]. Chin Health Econ. 2006;25:37–8. Chen YC, Tang SC, Le H, Yu XM, Wang DG, Hao M. Compare health development of rural areas in different districts of China: about social economy and inhabitant’s health condition [in Chinese]. Chin Health Econ. 2006;25:37–8.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Chang S, Wu H, Jia FM, Chen CM. Analysis on the changes of nutritional status in China anemia status of children under 5 in China [in Chinese]. Wei Sheng Yan Jiu. 2007;36(2):210–2.PubMed Chang S, Wu H, Jia FM, Chen CM. Analysis on the changes of nutritional status in China anemia status of children under 5 in China [in Chinese]. Wei Sheng Yan Jiu. 2007;36(2):210–2.PubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Balarajan Y, Ramakrishnan U, Öaltin E, Shankar AH, Subramanian SV. Anaemia in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet. 2011;378(9809):2123–35.CrossRefPubMed Balarajan Y, Ramakrishnan U, Öaltin E, Shankar AH, Subramanian SV. Anaemia in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet. 2011;378(9809):2123–35.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Stoltzfus RJ, Mullany L, Black RE. Iron deficiency anaemia. In: Ezzati M, Lopez AD, Rodgers A, Murray CLJ, editors. Comparative quantification of health risks: global and regional burden of disease attributable to selected major risk factors. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2004. p. 163–209. Stoltzfus RJ, Mullany L, Black RE. Iron deficiency anaemia. In: Ezzati M, Lopez AD, Rodgers A, Murray CLJ, editors. Comparative quantification of health risks: global and regional burden of disease attributable to selected major risk factors. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2004. p. 163–209.
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Black RE, Victora CG, Walker SP, et al. Maternal and child undernutrition and overweight in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet. 2013;382(9890):427–51.CrossRefPubMed Black RE, Victora CG, Walker SP, et al. Maternal and child undernutrition and overweight in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet. 2013;382(9890):427–51.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat The Nutrition Development Report of Chinese Children Aged 0–6. Beijing, China. Ministry of Health. 2012;2012:1–18. The Nutrition Development Report of Chinese Children Aged 0–6. Beijing, China. Ministry of Health. 2012;2012:1–18.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen C, He W, Wang Y, Deng L, Jia F. Nutritional status of children during and post-global economic crisis in China. Biomed Environ Sci. 2011;24(4):321–8.PubMed Chen C, He W, Wang Y, Deng L, Jia F. Nutritional status of children during and post-global economic crisis in China. Biomed Environ Sci. 2011;24(4):321–8.PubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Cui Y, Yang L, Tan D, Du Q. Analysis on the status of anemia among children under 3 years old in 10 provinces in western China [in Chinese]. Chan J Epidemiol. 2007;28(12):1159–61. Cui Y, Yang L, Tan D, Du Q. Analysis on the status of anemia among children under 3 years old in 10 provinces in western China [in Chinese]. Chan J Epidemiol. 2007;28(12):1159–61.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Dong CX, Ge PF, Zhang CJ, et al. Effects of different feeding practices at 0–6 months and living economic conditions on anemia prevalence of infants and young children [in Chinese]. Wei Sheng Yan Jiu. 2013;42(4):596–9.PubMed Dong CX, Ge PF, Zhang CJ, et al. Effects of different feeding practices at 0–6 months and living economic conditions on anemia prevalence of infants and young children [in Chinese]. Wei Sheng Yan Jiu. 2013;42(4):596–9.PubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Cui Y, Qu Q, Yang L, Wu Q. Analysis on anemia status of children under 3 years old in western China [in Chinese]. Chin J Public Health. 2008;24(9):1052–3. Cui Y, Qu Q, Yang L, Wu Q. Analysis on anemia status of children under 3 years old in western China [in Chinese]. Chin J Public Health. 2008;24(9):1052–3.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Luo R, Shi Y, Zhou H, Yue A, Zhang L, Sylvia S, Medina A, Rozelle S. Anemia and feeding practices among infants in rural Shaanxi Province in China. Nutrients. 2014;6:5975–91.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Luo R, Shi Y, Zhou H, Yue A, Zhang L, Sylvia S, Medina A, Rozelle S. Anemia and feeding practices among infants in rural Shaanxi Province in China. Nutrients. 2014;6:5975–91.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Lin LM, Song XF, Liu YL, Ma GF, Tan CW, Jiang JX, Liu M, Liu CY, Wang L. Relationship between vitamin a deficiency and anemia for Chinese children. Chin J Child Health Care. 2003;11:242–4. (in Chinese) Lin LM, Song XF, Liu YL, Ma GF, Tan CW, Jiang JX, Liu M, Liu CY, Wang L. Relationship between vitamin a deficiency and anemia for Chinese children. Chin J Child Health Care. 2003;11:242–4. (in Chinese)
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Grantham-McGregor S, Ani C. A review of studies on the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in children. J Nutr. 2001;131:649S–66S.PubMed Grantham-McGregor S, Ani C. A review of studies on the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in children. J Nutr. 2001;131:649S–66S.PubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Akman M, Cebeci D, Okur V, et al. The effects of iron deficiency on infants’ developmental test performance. Acta Paediatr. 2004;93:1391–6.CrossRefPubMed Akman M, Cebeci D, Okur V, et al. The effects of iron deficiency on infants’ developmental test performance. Acta Paediatr. 2004;93:1391–6.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Hasanbegovic E, Sabanovic S. Effects of iron therapy on motor and mental development of infants and small children suffering from iron deficiency anaemia. Med Arh. 2004;58:227–9.PubMed Hasanbegovic E, Sabanovic S. Effects of iron therapy on motor and mental development of infants and small children suffering from iron deficiency anaemia. Med Arh. 2004;58:227–9.PubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Idjradinata P, Pollitt E. Reversal of developmental delays in iron deficient anaemic infants treated with iron. Lancet. 1993;341:1–4.CrossRefPubMed Idjradinata P, Pollitt E. Reversal of developmental delays in iron deficient anaemic infants treated with iron. Lancet. 1993;341:1–4.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Lozoff B, Brittenham GM, Wolf AW, et al. Iron deficiency anemia and iron therapy effects on infant developmental test performance. Pediatrics. 1987;79:981–95.PubMed Lozoff B, Brittenham GM, Wolf AW, et al. Iron deficiency anemia and iron therapy effects on infant developmental test performance. Pediatrics. 1987;79:981–95.PubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Lozoff B, Wolf AW, Jimenez E. Iron-deficiency anemia and infant development: effects of extended oral iron therapy. J Pediatr. 1996;129:382–9.CrossRefPubMed Lozoff B, Wolf AW, Jimenez E. Iron-deficiency anemia and infant development: effects of extended oral iron therapy. J Pediatr. 1996;129:382–9.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Walter T, de Andraca I, Chadud P, et al. Iron deficiency anemia: adverse effects on infant psychomotor development. Pediatrics. 1989;84:7–17.PubMed Walter T, de Andraca I, Chadud P, et al. Iron deficiency anemia: adverse effects on infant psychomotor development. Pediatrics. 1989;84:7–17.PubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Lozoff B, Klein NK, Nelson EC, et al. Behavior of infants with iron-deficiency anemia. Child Dev. 1998;69:24–36.CrossRefPubMed Lozoff B, Klein NK, Nelson EC, et al. Behavior of infants with iron-deficiency anemia. Child Dev. 1998;69:24–36.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Sachdev H, Gera T, Nestel P. Effect of iron supplementation on mental and motor development in children: systematic review of randomised controlled trials. Public Health Nutr. 2005;8:117–32.CrossRefPubMed Sachdev H, Gera T, Nestel P. Effect of iron supplementation on mental and motor development in children: systematic review of randomised controlled trials. Public Health Nutr. 2005;8:117–32.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Hagen J, et al. Poorer behavioral and developmental outcome more than 10 years after treatment for iron deficiency in infancy. Pediatrics. 2000;105:e51.CrossRefPubMed Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Hagen J, et al. Poorer behavioral and developmental outcome more than 10 years after treatment for iron deficiency in infancy. Pediatrics. 2000;105:e51.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Smith JB. Double burden of iron deficiency in infancy and low socioeconomic status: a longitudinal analysis of cognitive test scores to age 19 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2006;160:1108–13.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Smith JB. Double burden of iron deficiency in infancy and low socioeconomic status: a longitudinal analysis of cognitive test scores to age 19 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2006;160:1108–13.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Maluccio JA, Hoddinott J, Behrman JR, et al. The impact of improving nutrition during early childhood on education among Guatemalan adults. Econ J. 2009;119:734–63.CrossRef Maluccio JA, Hoddinott J, Behrman JR, et al. The impact of improving nutrition during early childhood on education among Guatemalan adults. Econ J. 2009;119:734–63.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Haas JD, Brownlie T IV. Iron deficiency and reduced work capacity: a critical review of the research to determine a causal relationship. J Nutr. 2001;131:676S–88S. discussion 688S–690SPubMed Haas JD, Brownlie T IV. Iron deficiency and reduced work capacity: a critical review of the research to determine a causal relationship. J Nutr. 2001;131:676S–88S. discussion 688S–690SPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Walker SP, Wachs TD, Meeks Gardner J, et al. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369(9556):145–57.CrossRefPubMed Walker SP, Wachs TD, Meeks Gardner J, et al. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369(9556):145–57.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat De-Regil LM, Suchdev PS, Vist GE, Walleser S, Peña-Rosas JP. Home fortification of foods with multiple micronutrient powders for health and nutrition in children under two years of age (review). Evid-Based Child Health Cochrane Rev J. 2013;8(1):112–201.CrossRef De-Regil LM, Suchdev PS, Vist GE, Walleser S, Peña-Rosas JP. Home fortification of foods with multiple micronutrient powders for health and nutrition in children under two years of age (review). Evid-Based Child Health Cochrane Rev J. 2013;8(1):112–201.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Andrew A, Attanasio O, Fitzsimons E, Rubio-Codina M. Why is multiple micronutrient powder ineffective at reducing anaemia among 12–24 month olds in Colombia? Evidence from a randomised controlled trial. SSM-Popul Health. 2016;2:95–104.CrossRef Andrew A, Attanasio O, Fitzsimons E, Rubio-Codina M. Why is multiple micronutrient powder ineffective at reducing anaemia among 12–24 month olds in Colombia? Evidence from a randomised controlled trial. SSM-Popul Health. 2016;2:95–104.CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Attanasio OP, Fernández C, Fitzsimons EOA, Grantham-McGregor SM, Meghir C, Rubio-Codina M. Using the infrastructure of a conditional cash transfer program to deliver a scalable integrated early child development program in Colombia: cluster randomized controlled trial. BMJ. 2014;349:g5785.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Attanasio OP, Fernández C, Fitzsimons EOA, Grantham-McGregor SM, Meghir C, Rubio-Codina M. Using the infrastructure of a conditional cash transfer program to deliver a scalable integrated early child development program in Colombia: cluster randomized controlled trial. BMJ. 2014;349:g5785.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Gertler P, Heckman J, Pinto R, et al. Labor market returns to an early childhood stimulation intervention in Jamaica. Science. 2014;344(6187):998–1001.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gertler P, Heckman J, Pinto R, et al. Labor market returns to an early childhood stimulation intervention in Jamaica. Science. 2014;344(6187):998–1001.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
35.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Walker SP, Chang SM, Vera-Hernandez M, Grantham-McGregor S. Early childhood stimulation benefits adult competence and reduces violent behavior. Pediatrics. 2011;127(5):849–57.CrossRefPubMed Walker SP, Chang SM, Vera-Hernandez M, Grantham-McGregor S. Early childhood stimulation benefits adult competence and reduces violent behavior. Pediatrics. 2011;127(5):849–57.CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Ministry of Health and Standardization Administration of the People's Republic of China. General standard for complementary food supplement (GB-T 22570–2008). 2008. Ministry of Health and Standardization Administration of the People's Republic of China. General standard for complementary food supplement (GB-T 22570–2008). 2008.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Bayley N. The Bayley scales of infant development: the mental scale. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1974. Bayley N. The Bayley scales of infant development: the mental scale. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1974.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Yi S, Luo X, Yang Z, et al. The revising of Bayley scales of infant development (BSID) in China [in Chinese]. Chin J Clin Psychol. 1993;1:71–5. Yi S, Luo X, Yang Z, et al. The revising of Bayley scales of infant development (BSID) in China [in Chinese]. Chin J Clin Psychol. 1993;1:71–5.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Huang H, Tao SD, Zhang YW, et al. Standardization of Bayley scales of infant development in shanghai. Chin J Child Health. 1993;1:158–60. Huang H, Tao SD, Zhang YW, et al. Standardization of Bayley scales of infant development in shanghai. Chin J Child Health. 1993;1:158–60.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Li Q, Yan H, Zeng L, et al. Effects of maternal micronutrient supplementation on the mental development of infants in rural western China: follow-up evaluation of a double-blind, randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2009;123:e685.CrossRefPubMed Li Q, Yan H, Zeng L, et al. Effects of maternal micronutrient supplementation on the mental development of infants in rural western China: follow-up evaluation of a double-blind, randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2009;123:e685.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Chang S, Zeng L, Brouwer ID, et al. Effect of iron deficiency anemia in pregnancy on child mental development in rural China. Pediatrics. 2013;131:e755–63.CrossRefPubMed Chang S, Zeng L, Brouwer ID, et al. Effect of iron deficiency anemia in pregnancy on child mental development in rural China. Pediatrics. 2013;131:e755–63.CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Wu W, Sheng D, Shao J, et al. Mental and motor development and psychosocial adjustment of Chinese children with phenylketonuria. J Paediatr Child Health. 2011;47:441–7.CrossRefPubMed Wu W, Sheng D, Shao J, et al. Mental and motor development and psychosocial adjustment of Chinese children with phenylketonuria. J Paediatr Child Health. 2011;47:441–7.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Bayley N. Bayley scales of infant development. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1969. Bayley N. Bayley scales of infant development. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1969.
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Ermis B, Demirel F, Demircan N, Gurel A. Effect of three different iron supplementations in term health infants after 5 months of life. J Trop Pediatr. 2002;48(5):280–4.CrossRefPubMed Ermis B, Demirel F, Demircan N, Gurel A. Effect of three different iron supplementations in term health infants after 5 months of life. J Trop Pediatr. 2002;48(5):280–4.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Berger J, Ninh NX, Khan NC, et al. Efficacy of combined iron and zinc supplementation on micronutrient status and growth in Vietnamese infants. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2006;60(4):443–54.CrossRefPubMed Berger J, Ninh NX, Khan NC, et al. Efficacy of combined iron and zinc supplementation on micronutrient status and growth in Vietnamese infants. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2006;60(4):443–54.CrossRefPubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Pasricha S, Hayes E, Kalumba K, Biggs B. Effect of daily iron supplementation on health in children aged 4–23 months: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Lancet Glob Health. 2013;1(2):e77–86.CrossRefPubMed Pasricha S, Hayes E, Kalumba K, Biggs B. Effect of daily iron supplementation on health in children aged 4–23 months: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Lancet Glob Health. 2013;1(2):e77–86.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Luo R, Shi Y, Zhang L, et al. Nutrition and educational performance in rural China’s elementary schools: results of a randomized control trial in Shaanxi Province. Econ Dev Cult Change. 2012;60(1):735–72.CrossRef Luo R, Shi Y, Zhang L, et al. Nutrition and educational performance in rural China’s elementary schools: results of a randomized control trial in Shaanxi Province. Econ Dev Cult Change. 2012;60(1):735–72.CrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Greenland S. An introduction to instrumental variables for epidemiologists. International of Journal Epidemiology. 2000;29:722–9.CrossRef Greenland S. An introduction to instrumental variables for epidemiologists. International of Journal Epidemiology. 2000;29:722–9.CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Soofi S, Cousens S, Iqbal SP, et al. Effect of provision of daily zinc and iron with several micronutrients on growth and morbidity among young children in Pakistan: a cluster-randomised trial. Lancet. 2013;382(9886):29–40.CrossRefPubMed Soofi S, Cousens S, Iqbal SP, et al. Effect of provision of daily zinc and iron with several micronutrients on growth and morbidity among young children in Pakistan: a cluster-randomised trial. Lancet. 2013;382(9886):29–40.CrossRefPubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Jack SJ, Ou K, Chea M, Chhin L, Devenish R, Dunbar M, et al. Effect of micronutrient sprinkles on reducing anemia: a cluster-randomized effectiveness trial. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166:842–50.CrossRefPubMed Jack SJ, Ou K, Chea M, Chhin L, Devenish R, Dunbar M, et al. Effect of micronutrient sprinkles on reducing anemia: a cluster-randomized effectiveness trial. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166:842–50.CrossRefPubMed
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Zlotkin S, Arthur P, Schauer C, Antwi KY, Yeung G, Piekarz A. Home-fortification with iron and zinc sprinkles or iron sprinkles alone successfully treats anemia in infants and young children. J Nutr. 2003;133(4):1075–80. Zlotkin S, Arthur P, Schauer C, Antwi KY, Yeung G, Piekarz A. Home-fortification with iron and zinc sprinkles or iron sprinkles alone successfully treats anemia in infants and young children. J Nutr. 2003;133(4):1075–80.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Adu-Afarwuah S, Lartey A, Brown KH. Home fortification of complementary foods with micronutrient supplements is well accepted and has positive effects on infant iron status in Ghana. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;87(4):929–38. Adu-Afarwuah S, Lartey A, Brown KH. Home fortification of complementary foods with micronutrient supplements is well accepted and has positive effects on infant iron status in Ghana. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;87(4):929–38.
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Kounnavong S, Sunahara T, Mascie-Taylor CGN, et al. Effect of daily versus weekly home fortification with multiple micronutrient powder on haemoglobin concentration of young children in a rural area, Lao People ’s Democratic Republic: a randomised trial. Nutr J. 2011;10:129. Kounnavong S, Sunahara T, Mascie-Taylor CGN, et al. Effect of daily versus weekly home fortification with multiple micronutrient powder on haemoglobin concentration of young children in a rural area, Lao People ’s Democratic Republic: a randomised trial. Nutr J. 2011;10:129.
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Menon P, Rue lMT, Loechl CU, et al. Micronutrient Sprinkles reduce anemia among 9-to 24-mo-old children when delivered through an integrated health and nutrition program in rural Haiti. J Nutr. 2007;137(4):1023–30. Menon P, Rue lMT, Loechl CU, et al. Micronutrient Sprinkles reduce anemia among 9-to 24-mo-old children when delivered through an integrated health and nutrition program in rural Haiti. J Nutr. 2007;137(4):1023–30.
Metadaten
Titel
The effect of a micronutrient powder home fortification program on anemia and cognitive outcomes among young children in rural China: a cluster randomized trial
verfasst von
Renfu Luo
Ai Yue
Huan Zhou
Yaojiang Shi
Linxiu Zhang
Reynaldo Martorell
Alexis Medina
Scott Rozelle
Sean Sylvia
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2017
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Public Health / Ausgabe 1/2017
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2458
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-017-4755-0

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2017

BMC Public Health 1/2017 Zur Ausgabe