Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation 1/2019

Open Access 01.12.2019 | Research

Coordination of reach-to-grasp in physical and haptic-free virtual environments

verfasst von: Mariusz P. Furmanek, Luis F. Schettino, Mathew Yarossi, Sofia Kirkman, Sergei V. Adamovich, Eugene Tunik

Erschienen in: Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Background

Virtual reality (VR) offers unprecedented opportunity as a scientific tool to study visuomotor interactions, training, and rehabilitation applications. However, it remains unclear if haptic-free hand-object interactions in a virtual environment (VE) may differ from those performed in the physical environment (PE). We therefore sought to establish if the coordination structure between the transport and grasp components remain similar whether a reach-to-grasp movement is performed in PE and VE.

Method

Reach-to-grasp kinematics were examined in 13 healthy right-handed young adults. Subjects were instructed to reach-to-grasp-to-lift three differently sized rectangular objects located at three different distances from the starting position. Object size and location were matched between the two environments. Contact with the virtual objects was based on a custom collision detection algorithm. Differences between the environments were evaluated by comparing movement kinematics of the transport and grasp components.

Results

Correlation coefficients, and the slope of the regression lines, between the reach and grasp components were similar for the two environments. Likewise, the kinematic profiles of the transport velocity and grasp aperture were strongly correlated across the two environments. A rmANOVA further identified some similarities and differences in the movement kinematics between the two environments - most prominently that the closure phase of reach-to-grasp movement was prolonged when movements were performed in VE.

Conclusions

Reach-to-grasp movement patterns performed in a VE showed both similarities and specific differences compared to those performed in PE. Additionally, we demonstrate a novel approach for parsing the reach-to-grasp movement into three phases- initiation, shaping, closure- based on established kinematic variables, and demonstrate that the differences in performance between the environments are attributed to the closure phase. We discuss this in the context of how collision detection parameters may modify hand-object interactions in VE. Our study shows that haptic-free VE may be a useful platform to study reach-to-grasp movements, with potential implications for haptic-free VR in neurorehabilitation.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12984-019-0525-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Abkürzungen
C
closure phase
hdPA
hand distance to the object at peak aperture (cm)
hf-VE
haptic free virtual environment
HMD
head-mounted display
I
initiation phase
MT
movement time (ms)
PA
peak aperture (cm)
PE
physical environment
PV
peak transport velocity (cm/s)
rTPA
relative time to peak aperture (%)
rTPV
relative time to peak transport velocity (%)
S
shaping phase
snPA
size normalized peak aperture (a.u.)
VE
virtual environment
VR
virtual reality

Background

Widespread accessibility to highly specialized and advanced digital technology has made virtual reality (VR) more and more common in daily life. While VR was originally developed as an enhancement for video games and entertainment, more recent applications of virtual environments (VE) are used in a number of rapidly growing fields including virtual assembly [1], medical training [2] and rehabilitation [36]. Each of these applications requires the user to manually interact with virtual objects within the immersive computer-generated environment. Depending on the particular goal, the user may need to reach and grasp to a diverse set of objects in order to manipulate their position or state.
Such reach-to-grasp movements are a standard of VE based upper-extremity (UE) neurorehabilitation [7]. Though devices exist to provide haptic feedback of object properties, these often-expensive devices [8] have numerous limitations. Namely, wearable haptic devices are heavy, leading to a slowing down of movement, and non-wearable haptic devices restrict natural motion in the environment, for review see [1, 9]. These factors, along with cost and availability, greatly limit the widespread application of VE for rehabilitation. In contrast, motion tracking has a broad spectrum of commercially available products that support mobility at low cost [10], therefore making haptic-free VE (hf-VE) a more accessible option. Key to its use in rehabilitation, VE provides the opportunity for adaptive and progressive motor learning through perceptual modifications such as activity and workplace scaling, and error augmentation, resulting in greater dosage, intensity and engagement in training [11, 12]. While this capability makes VE a powerful technology for rehabilitation, it is still unclear to what extent movements produced in hf-VE resemble those produced in the physical environment (PE).
A comprehensive characterization of the kinematic differences between reach-to-grasp movements produced in the two environments will provide important information regarding the feasibility of hf-VE for UE rehabilitation. Therefore, in this investigation we sought to systematically describe the differences between environments by characterizing phase specific differences the reach and grasp components of reach-to-grasp.
To date, there have been few investigations of reach-to-grasp movements in VE. Magdalon and collaborators [13] reported that subjects instructed to reach and grasp three types of virtual and physical objects (a can, a screwdriver and a pen) reached significantly slower in VE, with a relative time to peak velocity (rTPV) around 10% earlier and a longer relative deceleration time. Though aperture scaling was preserved in VE, peak aperture (PA) was wider in VE when grasping the smaller objects (screwdriver and pen). In a follow-up experiment by the same group [14], several prehension measures were compared across VE and PE in individuals who have had stroke. They concluded that participants used similar movement strategies, and subjects were able to scale aperture to object size but with longer delays between time to peak transport velocity (TPV) and PA. It is important to note that all of the above studies comparing movement kinematics in PE versus VE provided some sort of haptic feedback. Given the dearth of evidence about how reach-to-grasp is coordinated in VE without haptics, we set out to compare performance in hf-VE and PE.
It is generally agreed that the transport and the grasp, though relatively independently controlled, also show evidence of temporal coordination [1420]. Some of this evidence includes a TPA at 60–70% of transport time as well as a PV during the first half of the movement [16, 21]. Levin et al. [14] tested the effects of VE with haptic feedback on the kinematics of reach-to-grasp movements. In order to quantify the coordination of the reach and grasp, the authors expressed temporal coordination as the delay between the times of arm peak velocity and hand maximal aperture. Their results showed that there were longer delays between the landmarks in VE with respect to movements in PE, suggesting a possible loss of coordination. Given the tight relationship between the transport and grasp in natural movements, our second goal was to determine if the kinematics of the individual components, as well as the coordination between the components, is preserved when performing under visual feedback alone (haptic-free) reach-to-grasp movements in VE.
We hypothesized that wider grip apertures and longer movement times would be observed in hf-VE (hypothesis 1). Moreover, we also hypothesized that coordination between the transport and grasp components would be preserved across environments (hypothesis 2). To test these hypotheses, we asked healthy subjects to produce reach-to grasp movements to size- position-matched physical and virtual objects of different sizes (manipulating the grasp component) and distances (manipulating the transport component) in both PE and VE.

Methods

Participants

Thirteen healthy subjects, 2 females (age: 23.9 ± 6.8 years old, body mass: 74.7 ± 9.9 kg, height: 1.7 ± 0.13 m) free from neurological, muscular, or orthopedic conditions, took part in the study. Prior to data collection, all participants provided informed consent approved by the Northeastern University Institutional Review Board (NU IRB# 15–10-22) with the ethical standards of the Helsinki Declaration. All participants were right-handed based on their preferred hand for writing, eating and throwing [21].

Apparatus

Kinematic data were recorded from small IRED markers attached to the tips of thumb and index finger, and wrist at the center of the line running between the ulnar and radial styloid process. Marker data were captured using a six-camera active infrared motion tracking system (PPT Studio N, WorldViz, CA, USA). A custom 3D immersive VE (UNITY ver. 5.6.1f1, 64 bits, Unity Technologies SF) was displayed to the subject via the Oculus HMD - head-mounted display (Rift DK2, VR, LLC.). A pair of IRED markers on the HMD were used to co-register head movements with the VE. The virtual environment was calibrated so that the objects were located at the same distance as in the physical environment. Kinematic data was tied to the framerate of the HMD which was set to 75 Hz.
The objects used in the study (physical and virtual) were three different sized rectangular prisms with equal width (W) and height (H) of 2.5 cm and 8 cm, respectively. Sizes (S) of the graspable dimension were: Large - 7.2 cm, Medium - 5.4 cm, Small - 3.6 cm (see Fig. 1). During the experiment, the objects were placed in three different positions along the proximal to distal axis measured from the start switch to the center of an object (D): Near - 24 cm, Middle - 30 cm, Far - 36 cm). All objects were rotated along their vertical axis (Z) to 65 degrees measured from horizontal (X) axis to make them easier to grasp without excessive wrist extension (see Fig. 1). The physical objects were 3D printed using PLA thermoplastic filaments, and weighed 30, 44, and 59 g, for Small, Medium and Large, respectively.

Procedure

Subjects sat comfortably in front of a table. They placed their dominant (right) hand on the table in a comfortable pinch position lightly holding a wooden peg which maintained a starting distance between thumb and index of 1.5 cm to prevent interference between the infrared markers. The position of the peg was 24 cm to the right, and 12 cm in front, of the body midline. The thumb was placed on the start switch next to the peg. Each trial began with an auditory signal, cuing the subject to reach, grasp, and lift the virtual or physical object (see Fig. 1). Subjects were instructed to reach naturally toward the object with the dominant upper limb, without leaning their trunk forward, at their preferred speed keeping their hand relatively parallel to the table to reduce vertical movements. The subjects’ view of their fingertip position in the VE condition was projected as two green spheres (0.8 cm, diameter) rendered at the 3D position of the fingertip IRED markers. To grasp the object, subjects placed the fingertip spheres of the thumb and index finger (pincer grip) approximately on the middle of each object’s lateral surface (see Fig. 1, panel b). Contact with the virtual object was controlled by a collision detection algorithm which colored the virtual object red when the fingertip spheres reached the location of the object (to make virtual objects easy to grasp the total collision error margin was set to 1.2 cm). As the object was lifted from the table, the subject kept it in the vertical position before putting the object back down (in VR, the object was held above the table until it disappeared) and returning their hand to the starting position. In PE, subjects completed the reach-to-grasp task in a dark room, seeing only the glow-in-the-dark object and the illuminated IRED markers on their fingertips (see Additional file 1: Video 1). Overhead lights were turned on between trials to prevent dark adaptation. Therefore, visual information remained similar across the environments.
Additional file 1: Video 1. (AVI 2110 kb)
Before data collection, subjects were familiarized with the setup and procedure, particularly to the VE environment. Familiarization consisted of 135 trials in VE (15 trials × 3 object sizes and × 3 distances). Five second ‘rest’ periods were provided after every 15 trials.
The experiment consisted of 216 trials (108 trials in the PE block, 108 trials in the VE block). Three seconds were allowed for each trial. After every 12 trials, a 5-s ‘rest’ period was provided. A 5-min ‘rest’ was provided between blocks.

Analysis

Data were analyzed offline using custom Matlab routines (The Mathworks, Natick, MA). Trials were cropped from movement onset (start switch) to moment offset (contact of both thumb and index finger with the object). In the physical environment, offset was defined as the moment when aperture stopped decreasing. In VR, offset was defined as the timestamp when the virtual object was successfully grasped (thumb and index finger markers met the collision detection criteria). Kinematic data were lowpass filtered at 6 Hz with a 4th order Butterworth filter.
Based on the markers attached to fingertips (thumb, index), the following kinematics were calculated. Grasping components: movement time (MT) as the time between movement onset and offset, grip aperture as the 2D (horizontal plane) distance in time between thumb and index markers at each sample, peak aperture (PA) (maximum value of grip aperture), and time to peak aperture (TPA). Size normalized aperture at each sample was calculated by first subtracting the aperture at movement onset, −then normalizing this value to the aperture at movement offset (object contact). The rationale for this normalization was two-fold. First, markers were attached to the dorsum of the digits (on the nail) in order to not create a barrier between the finger pads and the object that would interfere with grasping. In the VE, the collision detection algorithm was dependent on the position of these markers. This created a discrepancy between PE and VE in the final grip aperture (at the time of grasp) roughly equal to the width of the finger from pad to nail. The described normalization accounts for this discrepancy. Second, normalization in this way permitted the direct comparison of aperture profiles and features, such as the size normalized peak aperture (snPA), between different size objects. To better understand motor planning, we also analyzed digit positions along the vertical object at movement offset (object grasp). Specifically, we calculated the vertical distance between the thumb and index finger. This measure is analogous to the COP difference that has been commonly calculated in studies of digit force planning [22, 23], though obviously lacks information about forces on the object.
Based on the wrist marker, the following transport components were calculated: the 2D position of the transport component in the horizontal plane. Transport velocity was calculated as the first derivative of wrist position. Peak of transport velocity (PV) and time to peak transport velocity (TPV) were subsequently obtained. In order to account for variability in movement times between subjects and trial condition for, time to peak measures were time normalized as percentage of MT (relative time to peak aperture - rTPA, and relative time to peak transport velocity - rTPV). Finally, hand distance to the object at peak aperture - hdPA (the 2D distance from wrist marker to the object at peak aperture) was used to determine whether the lack of haptic feedback affects the organization of aperture closure.

Statistics

Linear Pearson’s correlations were used to compare aperture and transport velocity trajectories. A 2 × 3 × 3 repeated measures analysis of variance (rmANOVA) with factors Environment (PE, VR), Object Size (Large, Medium, Small), and Object Distance (Near, Middle, Far) was used to evaluate differences between the environments, separately for all calculated kinematics.
A 2 × 3 rmANOVA with factors Environment (PE, VE) and Phase (Initiation, Shaping, Closure, see definitions in the ‘three phases of the reach-to-grasp movement’ paragraph) was utilized to track differences between environments and phases of reach-to-grasp movements, see also Fig. 7. The Shapiro-Wilk test was used to verify the normality of data distribution; all variables met this assumption. The Mauchly’s sphericity test was used to validate assumptions for repeated measures ANOVAs; Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were applied to the factors that violated the assumption of sphericity. The Levene’s test was used to verify homogeneity of the samples data variance; all data satisfied the assumption of variance homogeneity’.
Significant effects were further explored using pairwise contrasts with Bonferroni corrections.
To quantify reach-to-grasp coupling, linear Pearson correlations between TPA and TPV (both real and normalized) were calculated. To compare slopes between linear regressions and to assess the vertical distance between the thumb and index finger positions both for PE and VE t-test was used. The threshold for statistical significance was set as at p < .05. All statistical analyses were performed using Statistica (ver. 13, Dell Inc.). All variables are presented as means with standard deviations.

Results

Comparison of trajectories between physical and virtual environments

Correlational analyses examining differences in the relationship between aperture and transport velocity profiles between the physical (PE) and virtual (hf-VE) environment indicated largely invariant movement patterns (see Fig. 2). On average, across all subjects and conditions, correlation coefficients were r = 0.97 ± 0.03, p < .001, and r = 0.95 ± 0.05, p < .001 for velocity and aperture, respectively. Pearson’s correlation coefficients between all conditions are presented in Table 1. Invariant movement patterns are also evident when the positions of individual markers are compared in the horizontal plane. Figure 3 shows the mean positions of markers corresponding to the transport (wrist marker - W) and the grasp (thumb - T and index finger - I), overlaid for the two environments and for all conditions.
Table 1
Pearson correlation between physical and virtual transport velocity and aperture profiles
PE vs VE
profiles
Transport velocity
Aperture
Large
Near
0.96 ± 0.03
0.95 ± 0.05
Middle
0.96 ± 0.02
0.96 ± 0.05
Far
0.97 ± 0.03
0.96 ± 0.04
Medium
Near
0.98 ± 0.02
0.96 ± 0.04
Middle
0.96 ± 0.01
0.95 ± 0.05
Far
0.98 ± 0.02
0.94 ± 0.04
Small
Near
0.96 ± 0.03
0.96 ± 0.07
Middle
0.97 ± 0.03
0.96 ± 0.04
Far
0.96 ± 0.04
0.97 ± 0.03
Average:
0.97 ± 0.03
0.95 ± 0.05
All values are significant, p < 0.001, PE - physical environment, VE - virtual environment, data represents mean with standard deviation

Comparison of kinematics between physical and virtual environments

All calculated spatial and temporal kinematics (mean, sd), separately for all conditions are presented in Table 2.
Table 2
Kinematics calculated during reach-to-grasp movement for all conditions
Variables/Conditions
 
Large
Medium
Small
 
Near
Middle
Far
Near
Middle
Far
Near
Middle
Far
Movement Time
MT (ms)
PE
904.3
977.0
1027.3
934.5
1002.8
1057.4
939.3
1033.3
1121.7
sd
±95.0
±119.7
±117.2
±115.5
±112.5
±120.0
±119.6
±138.1
±167.5
VE
1053.1
1113.8
1144.6
1001.6
1037.2
1134.8
978.1
1025.5
1080.9
sd
±157.4
±155.6
±143.5
±170.9
±143.2
±156.5
±135.5
±125.1
±150.5
Peak Aperture
PA (cm)
PE
11.1
11.1
11.0
9.5
9.4
9.6
7.8
7.9
7.9
sd
±0.6
±0.5
±0.5
±0.5
±0.6
±0.5
±0.6
±0.5
±0.5
VE
10.0
10.1
10.1
8.7
9.0
9.0
7.5
7.7
7.7
sd
±1.3
±1.3
±1.1
±1.2
±1.3
±1.2
±1.2
±1.1
±1.2
Relative Time to Peak Aperture
rTPA (%)
PE
66.4
67.1
69.0
66.1
67.4
66.7
64.4
65.5
65.1
sd
±6.2
±5.4
±5.2
±5.9
±6.2
±6.6
±5.8
±6.9
±7.0
VE
60.2
61.7
61.8
58.8
59.3
60.7
57.1
59.7
61.5
sd
±9.1
±8.4
±9.0
±7.1
±9.1
±9.6
±9.8
±8.9
±9.0
Size Normalized Peak Aperture
snPA (a.u.)
PE
1.4
1.4
1.3
1.6
1.5
1.6
2.1
2.1
2.1
sd
±0.1
±0.1
±0.1
±0.1
±0.2
±0.2
±0.3
±0.3
±0.3
VE
1.7
1.7
1.7
2.0
2.1
2.2
2.7
2.6
2.5
sd
±0.3
±0.2
±0.2
±0.3
±0.3
±0.4
±0.7
±0.6
±0.7
Peak Transport Velocity
PV (cm/s)
PE
61.6
73.2
83.5
60.2
69.9
82.5
57.8
68.2
77.7
sd
±7.0
±9.1
±10.7
±8.1
±9.7
±11.1
±7.6
±9.0
±11.9
VE
51.5
64.0
74.3
52.9
67.4
75.8
53.6
66.2
74.4
sd
±6.3
±9.6
±13.3
±8.9
±9.6
±11.9
±6.7
±7.9
±9.5
Relative Time to Peak Transport Velocity
rTPV (%)
PE
32.0
31.5
31.7
31.6
32.6
32.0
34.0
32.3
31.9
sd
±5.4
±4.5
±4.8
±6.1
±3.6
±5.0
±4.5
±5.3
±4.4
VE
33.2
32.7
32.0
34.3
32.0
32.5
34.8
34.6
35.0
sd
±6.8
±6.7
±5.5
±6.1
±5.0
±5.6
±6.2
±5.4
±5.1
Hand Distance to the Object at Peak Aperture
hdPA (cm)
PE
2.5
2.6
2.7
2.5
2.6
3.0
3.1
3.1
3.5
sd
±0.7
±0.9
±0.8
±0.9
±0.9
±1.1
±1.2
±1.0
±1.4
VE
3.3
3.6
4.2
4.0
4.3
4.8
4.6
5.0
5.7
sd
±1.5
±1.7
±2.0
±1.6
±2.1
±2.6
±1.8
±2.2
±2.4
PE - Physical environment, VE - Virtual environment, data represents mean with standard deviation (sd)

Movement time

A 2 × 3 × 3 repeated measures ANOVA (see Table 3) revealed a significant main effect of Environment for MT, (F(1, 12) = 5.55, p = 0.037, η2 = 0.32). On average, subjects needed more time to reach-to-grasp objects in VE (1063.3 ± 40.9 ms) vs. PE (999.7 ± 32 ms). Expectedly, the analysis also showed a main effect of factor Distance (F(1.3, 16) = 111.79, p < 0.0001, η2 = 0.9). Analysis of the interaction between Environment and Size factors (F(2, 24) = 13.51 p < 0.0001, η2 = 0.53), showed that subjects required more time to grasp the Large object in VE (1103 ± 159.1 ms) than in PE (969.5 ± 123.7 ms), post-hoc (p < 0.0001, d = 0.94). The differences in MT between Medium and Small object sizes across environments were not significant, see Fig. 5, panel a. The significant interaction in MT suggests that differences between environments are size-, but not distance-dependent. Since MT between environments exhibited significant differences, further analyses of temporal features were conducted using time-normalized data.
Table 3
The 2 × 3 × 3 repeated measure analysis of variance ANOVA
Variables
Environment
(E)
Size
(S)
Distance
(D)
Interaction
Figure 5
Movement Time
MT (ms)
F(1, 12) = 5.5
p = 0.037
η2 = 0.32
NS
F(1.3, 16) = 111.8
p < 0.0001
η2 = 0.9
E*S
F(2, 24) = 13.5
p < 0.0001
η2 = 0.53
panel a
Peak Aperture
PA (cm)
NS
F(2, 24) = 697.3
p = 0.0001
η2 = 0.98
F(2, 24) = 6.6
p = 0.0051
η2 = 0.35
E*S,
F(1.3, 13.5) = 12.8
p = 0.002
η2 = 0.52
E*D
F(2, 24) = 5.3
p = 0.012
η2 = 0.3
 
Relative Time to Peak Aperture
rTPA (%)
F(1, 12) = 20.2
p = 0.0007
η2 = 0.63
F(2, 24) = 3.9
p = 0.034
η2 = 0.24
F(2, 24) = 17.7
p = 0.0001
η2 = 0.6
NS
 
Size Normalized Peak Aperture
snPA (a.u.)
F(1, 12) = 24.2
p = 0.0003
η2 = 0.67
F(1, 24) = 84.9
p = 0.0001
η2 = 0.88
NS
NS
panel b
Peak Transport Velocity
PV (cm/s)
F(1, 12) = 26.2
p = 0.0002
η2 = 0.69
NS
F(1.25, 15) = 254.3
p = 0.0001
η2 = 0.95
E*S
F(2, 24) = 6.9
p = 0.0042
η2 = 0.37
panel c
Relative Time to Peak Transport Velocity
rTPV (%)
NS
F(2, 24) = 4.7
p = 0.018
η2 = 0.28
NS
NS
 
Hand Distance to the Object at Peak Aperture
hdPA (cm)
F(1, 12) = 15.6
p = 0.0019
η2 = 0.56
F(2, 24) = 20.2
p = 0.0001
η2 = 0.63
F(1.4, 15) = 13.6
p = 0.0008
η2 = 0.36
E*S
F(2, 24) = 6.7
p = 0.0046
η2 = 0.28
panel d
NS – Not significant

Peak aperture

As expected, we noted a significant main effect of PA for factor Size (F(2, 24) = 697.35, p = 0.0001, η2 = 0.98), indicating that aperture was scaled to target object size. There was a significant interaction between factors Environment and Size (F(1.3, 13.5) = 12.8, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.52). Post-hoc analyses showed significant differences between Large (p < 0.001, d = 1.01) and Medium (p < 0.001, d = 0.59) objects when comparing grasping in PE and VE. Interestingly, a significant main effect of Distance was also found for PA (F(2, 24) = 6.61, p = 0.005, η2 = 0.35). Subsequent post-hoc analyses revealed that this effect was due to differences between grasps executed to the Near and Far distances (d = 0.08). The Environment and Distance interaction was also significant (F(2, 24) = 5.26, p = 0.012, η2 = 0.3), with post-hoc tests showing significant differences between environments at all of the distances (p values< 0.01, d values > 0.35).

Relative time to peak aperture

Analysis of rTPA revealed a significant main effect of Environment (F(1, 12) = 20.18, p = 0.0007, η2 = 0.63), showing that peak aperture occurred later in PE compared to VE. Moreover, the Size (F(2, 24) = 3.88, p = 0.034, η2 = 0.24) and Distance (F(2, 24) = 17.74, p = 0.0001, η2 = 0.6) factors also showed significant differences. Post-hoc tests indicated significant differences between Large and Small objects for Size (p = 0.031, d = 0.25), as well as between Near and Middle (p = 0.002, d = 0.15), and Near and Far (p < 0.001) distances for the Distance effect. There were no significant interactions.

Size normalized peak aperture

In order to take a closer look at the effect on PA between environments regardless of object size, we performed a rmANOVA on the PA measures normalized to object size. Analysis revealed a significant main effect of Environment (F(1, 12) = 24.2, p = 0.0003, η2 = 0.67) indicating greater overshoot when grasping the virtual objects. Post-hoc analyses showed significant differences between all object sizes (p values < 0.001, d values > 1.09), indicating that participants opened their hand proportionally wider for smaller objects (the ‘margin of safety’ was not the same for all object sizes), see Fig. 5, panel b. Note that variability was also larger in VE, which might reflect uncertainty of grasping the virtual objects (see Fig. 4). There were no other significant main effects nor interactions.

Peak transport velocity

There was a significant main effect of Environment for PV, (F(1, 12) = 26.24, p = 0.0002, η2 = 0.69), which confirms the fact that movement speed (on averages 70.5 ± 2.35 cm/s for PE vs 64.5 2.58 cm/s in VR) was faster in the physical relative to the virtual environment. As expected, no main effect of Size was found (F(1, 12) = 3.1, p = 0.068, η2 = 0.2), however a significant main effect of Distance (F(1.25, 15) = 254.3, p = 0.0001, η2 = 0.95), indicated that the further the object was from the starting position, the faster the transport component. Similar to MT, PV showed a significant =interaction between Environment and Size (F(2, 24) = 6.93, p = 0.0042, η2 = 0.37). Post-hoc tests revealed significant differences between PE and VE for the Large (p < 0.001, d = 0.71) and Medium sized objects (p < 0.01, d = 0.4).

Relative time to peak transport velocity

Analysis of rTPV revealed a significant main effect of Size (F(2, 24) = 4.72, p = 0.018, η2 = 0.28). Post-hoc tests showed that rTPV occurred later for smaller compared to large objects (p = 0.023, d = 0.29). There were no other significant main effects nor interactions.

Hand distance to the object at peak aperture

The ANOVA on hdPA showed a significant main effect of factor Environment (F(1, 12) = 15.63, p = 0.0019, η2 = 0.56). In general, subjects began closing their grip relatively earlier in VE (4.4 ± 0.57 cm) compared to PE (2.8, ±0.26 cm). A significant main effect of factor Distance (F(1.4, 15) = 13.65, p = 0.0008, η2 = 0.36) showed that the further the object is, the later subjects began to close their grip aperture. A significant main effect of factor Size (F(2, 24) = 20.25, p = 0.0001, η2 = 0.63) revealed a similar trend, showing that the smaller the object is, the later closure of grip aperture is started. The significant interaction of factors Environment and Size (F(2, 24) = 6.7, p = 0.0046, η2 = 0.28) showed that the difference of hdPA between environments is mainly size dependent. Post-hoc analyses revealed that hdPA for all object sizes was significantly different between PE and VE (p values < 0.001, d values > 0.75), see Fig. 5, panel d.

Coupling between kinematics in physical and virtual environments

A graphic representation of reach-to-grasp coupling is presented on Fig. 6. First, temporal data were plotted of the reaching component against the grasping component both for PE and hf-VE. Then each of these data sets were fitted with linear regression models. All regressions were found to be significant (p values < 0.0001). Non-significant differences between PE and hf-VE in the slopes of linear fits for TPA and TPV (t = 0.16, p = 0.872, d = 0.25), and rTPA and rTPV (t = - 1.12, p = 0.285, d = 0.35), indicated that the coupling between reaching and grasping was preserved (e.g., fitted regression lines are close to parallel) and independent of the environment in which movement was performed.

Three phases of the reach-to-grasp movement

Based on the observed significant main effect and interaction between environment and object size on movement time (see Fig. 5, panel a), we decided it would be interesting to further investigate differences in movement time at a more granular level. To do so, we plotted transport velocity against size-normalized aperture. The resulting curves revealed three distinct phases of the reach-to-grasp movement which we labeled: Initiation (I) from movement onset to peak transport velocity, Shaping (S) from peak transport velocity to peak aperture, and Closure (C) from peak aperture to movement offset (see Fig. 7, panel b).
A 2 × 3 rmANOVA with factors Environment (PE, VE) and Phase (Initiation, Shaping, Closure) on the proportion of movement time accounted for by each phase, showed a significant interaction between Environment and Phase (F(2, 24) = 20.5, p = 0.001, η2 = 63). Post-hoc tests revealed that the proportion of movement dedicated to the initiation phase remained the same across environments. However, there was a significant difference in the proportion of the total movement time during the shaping phase (p = 0.001, d = 1.32), with the time allotted in VE reduced, possibly in order to enable more precise grasping during the closure phase (p = 0.006, d = 0.85), (see Fig. 7, panel c and d).

Digits position

The analysis of vertical distance between the thumb and index at movement offset (object grasp) showed no significant differences between physical (0.56 ± 0.58), and virtual (0.42 ± 0.53) environment (t = 1.81, p = 0.07, d = 0.25), see Fig. 8.

Discussion

The main goal of this study was to determine the potential of current VR systems as a platform for research and rehabilitation [4, 11, 24]. In order to assess the characteristics of movements produced in VR, we tested healthy participants in a comparable reach-to-grasp task in both the PE and the VE. Importantly, we decided not to include haptic feedback, as previous studies have, due to the relatively lower state of development of that technology which results in reduced accessibility and practicality [1, 8, 9].
While most of the previous work involving VE has provided haptic feedback to the participants [13, 14, 25], we were interested in determining whether visual feedback alone would support the production of properly coordinated reach-to-grasp movements. The characteristics of prehension movements observed in our study showed that even when haptic information is absent during reach-to-grasp actions in VE, the behavior across environments showed very similar kinematic patterns. High correlations between aperture (r = 0.95 ± 0.05) and transport velocity profiles (r = 0.97 ± 0.03, see Fig. 2), as well as similar trajectories of individual markers (index finger, thumb and wrist, see Fig. 3) support this notion. Similarly, as expected, grasp aperture was scaled to object size independently of the environment where the movement was carried out. These results coincide with previous studies comparing movement kinematics in PE and VE [13, 14, 26].
Many studies have postulated that in order to grasp any object successfully, the transport and grasp components must be coordinated [17, 18, 27, 28]. Moreover, the temporal relationship between the components appears to depend on task goal, object properties, and experience [18]. In our setup we observed that even if there were some differences in movement kinematics, coordination between the reach and grasp components was preserved across environments (see Fig. 6). Similarly, though the typical landmarks of the movement were slightly different across environments, in VE they still occurred within normal ranges: TPA occurred between 60 and 70%, and TPV between 30 and 40% of movement time [16, 17, 19]. We suggest that coordination across environments is probably a more relevant measure of whether VE is useful for research and rehabilitation than any specific landmark. The fact that people produce natural movements (they do not move ‘robotically’) suggests that they are selecting and deploying the appropriate movements at relatively consistent latencies.
Having said that, the differences we observed between environments as the participants deployed their grasping movements are important to understand for the possible improvement of this technology. For example, MT exhibited differences between PE and VE, as did hdPA and snPA. Some of these results were not unexpected, as previous studies have found that in VE individuals move slower, particularly showing longer deceleration times both during reaching and grasping as well as in pointing tasks [13, 14, 25, 26, 29]. Similarly, the increase in peak aperture we observed in VE for small-sized objects, was previously reported by [13]. Larger grip aperture in VE (especially during grasps to smaller objects) have been reported in previous studies comparing physical and virtual environments [13, 14]. The effects described suggest to us there was increased perceptual uncertainty during target acquisition in VE. Therefore, a strategy to increase the safety margin in terms of visibility of both the target and the hand during the movement is needed [30]. Given the geometry of our setup, where the marker (avatar) representing the tip of the index finger is eclipsed by the distant edge of the virtual object near the time of peak aperture, it is possible that participants required more time and larger distance between the digits compared to PE, when acquiring the target object. This effect was evident when the large virtual object was reached and grasped. The analysis of the timing of the reach-to-grasp phases confirmed that subjects needed noticeably more time during the closure phase in VE (see Fig. 7).
The demarcations of the reach-to-grasp phases (shown in Fig. 7) could be useful for researchers and clinicians as a means of evaluating the proportion of total movement time spent in each phase of the reach-to-grasp movement. This analysis differentiates between a mostly internally guided - initiation phase, an execution phase when sensory feedback of object state is obtained, and a closure phase when handshape is adjusted. Based on the proportion of reach-to-grasp phases, the programming and evaluation of functional recovery process could be better understood and tailored to individual patient needs.
A related question involves the participants’ behavioral response to the collision detection algorithm employed in our study. The collision detection algorithm determines whether the limits of a virtual object collide with those of the thumb and index finger avatars [3133]. Depending on the dimensions of the overlap allowed by the algorithm, an object may be too hard or too easy to ‘contact’. Therefore, this algorithm must be carefully calibrated in order to adjust collision detection to the participant’s ability. In this experiment, the virtual objects changed color once collision with the avatars was detected, allowing the subject to grasp and lift the object [34]. Therefore, as participants gauged the appropriate digit location to trigger the grasp they seemed to have taken longer to close their digits. This effect was observed as a relatively early beginning of aperture closing in VE (hand distance to object at peak aperture). We believe that it is possible to control a large portion of such uncertainty by modifying the collision detection algorithm and we are currently testing this possibility.
Finally, it is important to discuss the possibility that adding haptic feedback to virtual reality environments may result in a reduction of the perceptual uncertainty our participants encountered [35]. It is possible that, in the absence of haptic feedback, grasping an object became an aperture matching task under visual guidance, which might be a limitation of the study. While that may be the case, our participants were able to produce natural goal-directed movements within hf-VE.
Furthermore, lack of differences in the vertical distance between the digits at object grasp indicates that participants treated the virtual object as if it had physical properties. Placement of the digits at object contact in order to prevent roll of the object during grasp and lift has been previous shown to indicate anticipatory force control [36]. In the VE utilized here, nothing prevented the participants from grasping the virtual object with misalignment of the thumb and index, which would cause a rotation of a real object. However, this was not observed and, on average, the vertical distance between the thumb and index was in fact smaller in VE than PE. Cumulatively these results suggesting that visual feedback in current high-end virtual reality systems may be sufficient for research and rehabilitation purposes if tasks are carefully designed and calibrated.

Conclusions

We found that prehension movements are largely similarly coordinated in both physical and our 3D-immersed virtual environments. That is, the intrinsic plans for reach-to-grasp in hf-VE did not change movement structure nor the motor programs deployed. Nonetheless, we observed that intrinsic motor plans in VE may be tuned (scaled), particularly in the closure phase, perhaps due to perceptual uncertainty and/or due to lack of haptic feedback. Therefore, we suggest that if the collision detection algorithm is properly controlled, VR may be suitable for accurate and precise reach-to-grasp movements since the virtual environment presents appropriate visuo-motor scaling for reach-to-grasp coordinated action towards different object sizes and distances. This property, as well as the relatively easy manipulation of visual feedback, leverages VR as a flexible tool for research and rehabilitation.

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Alex Huntoon and Samuel Berin for their work on the virtual reality platform that enabled this work. We also thank Madhur Mangalam, Sambina Anthony and Tessa Griffin for their helpful comments in editing of the manuscript.

Availability of data and material

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health grant # R01NS085122 (ET) and grant # R01HD53801 (SA), National Institute on Disability, Independent Living, and Rehabilitation Research grant # 90RE5021 (SA) National Science Foundation grant # CBET-1804550 (ET), and the Global Resilience Institute at Northeastern University.
All participants provided informed consent that was approved by the Northeastern University Institutional Review Board (NU IRB# 15–10-22) with the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Seth A, Vance JM, Oliver JH. Virtual reality for assembly methods prototyping: a review. Virtual Reality. 2010;15(1):5–20.CrossRef Seth A, Vance JM, Oliver JH. Virtual reality for assembly methods prototyping: a review. Virtual Reality. 2010;15(1):5–20.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Li L, Yu F, Shi D, Shi J, Tian Z, Yang J, Wang X, Jiang Q. Application of virtual reality technology in clinical medicine. Am J Transl Res. 2017(9, 9):3867–80. Li L, Yu F, Shi D, Shi J, Tian Z, Yang J, Wang X, Jiang Q. Application of virtual reality technology in clinical medicine. Am J Transl Res. 2017(9, 9):3867–80.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Holden MK. Virtual environments for motor rehabilitation: review. CyberPsychol Behav. 2005;8(3):187–211.CrossRef Holden MK. Virtual environments for motor rehabilitation: review. CyberPsychol Behav. 2005;8(3):187–211.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Rose T, Nam CS, Chen KB. Immersion of virtual reality for rehabilitation - review. Appl Ergon. 2018;69:153–61.CrossRef Rose T, Nam CS, Chen KB. Immersion of virtual reality for rehabilitation - review. Appl Ergon. 2018;69:153–61.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Adamovich SV, Merians AS, Boian R, Lewis JA, Tremaine M, Burdea GS, Recce M, Poizner H. A virtual reality-based exercise system for hand rehabilitation post-stroke. Presence-Teleop Virt. 2005;14(2):161–74.CrossRef Adamovich SV, Merians AS, Boian R, Lewis JA, Tremaine M, Burdea GS, Recce M, Poizner H. A virtual reality-based exercise system for hand rehabilitation post-stroke. Presence-Teleop Virt. 2005;14(2):161–74.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Fluet GG, Merians AS, Qiu QY, Davidow A, Adamovich SV. Comparing integrated training of the hand and arm with isolated training of the same effectors in persons with stroke using haptically rendered virtual environments, a randomized clinical trial. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2014;11.CrossRef Fluet GG, Merians AS, Qiu QY, Davidow A, Adamovich SV. Comparing integrated training of the hand and arm with isolated training of the same effectors in persons with stroke using haptically rendered virtual environments, a randomized clinical trial. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2014;11.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Merians AS, Tunik E, Adamovich SV. Virtual reality to maximize function for hand and arm rehabilitation: exploration of neural mechanisms. Stud Health Technol Inform. 2009;145:109–25.PubMedPubMedCentral Merians AS, Tunik E, Adamovich SV. Virtual reality to maximize function for hand and arm rehabilitation: exploration of neural mechanisms. Stud Health Technol Inform. 2009;145:109–25.PubMedPubMedCentral
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Burdea GC. Haptics issues in virtual environments. Computer Graphics International. 2000, Proceedings;(2000):295–302. Burdea GC. Haptics issues in virtual environments. Computer Graphics International. 2000, Proceedings;(2000):295–302.
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Borst CW, Volz RA. Evaluation of a haptic mixed reality system for interactions with a virtual control panel. Presence-Teleop Virt. 2005;14(6):677–96.CrossRef Borst CW, Volz RA. Evaluation of a haptic mixed reality system for interactions with a virtual control panel. Presence-Teleop Virt. 2005;14(6):677–96.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Smeragliuolo AH, Hill NJ, Disla L, Putrino D. Validation of the leap motion controller using markered motion capture technology. J Biomech. 2016;49(9):1742–50.CrossRef Smeragliuolo AH, Hill NJ, Disla L, Putrino D. Validation of the leap motion controller using markered motion capture technology. J Biomech. 2016;49(9):1742–50.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Adamovich SV, Fluet GG, Tunik E, Merians AS. Sensorimotor training in virtual reality: a review. NeuroRehabilitation. 2009;25(1):29–44.PubMedPubMedCentral Adamovich SV, Fluet GG, Tunik E, Merians AS. Sensorimotor training in virtual reality: a review. NeuroRehabilitation. 2009;25(1):29–44.PubMedPubMedCentral
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Pallesen H, Andersen MB, Hansen GM, Lundquist CB, Brunner I. Patients' and health Professionals' experiences of using virtual reality Technology for Upper Limb Training after stroke: a qualitative substudy. Rehab Res Pract. 2018. Pallesen H, Andersen MB, Hansen GM, Lundquist CB, Brunner I. Patients' and health Professionals' experiences of using virtual reality Technology for Upper Limb Training after stroke: a qualitative substudy. Rehab Res Pract. 2018.
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Magdalon EC, Michaelsen SM, Quevedo AA, Levin MF. Comparison of grasping movements made by healthy subjects in a 3-dimensional immersive virtual versus physical environment. Acta Psychol. 2011;138(1):126–34.CrossRef Magdalon EC, Michaelsen SM, Quevedo AA, Levin MF. Comparison of grasping movements made by healthy subjects in a 3-dimensional immersive virtual versus physical environment. Acta Psychol. 2011;138(1):126–34.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Levin MF, Magdalon EC, Michaelsen SM, Quevedo AA. Quality of grasping and the role of haptics in a 3-D immersive virtual reality environment in individuals with stroke. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2015;23(6):1047–55.CrossRef Levin MF, Magdalon EC, Michaelsen SM, Quevedo AA. Quality of grasping and the role of haptics in a 3-D immersive virtual reality environment in individuals with stroke. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2015;23(6):1047–55.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Jeannerod M: Attention and performance, IX. 1981, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ:153–169. Jeannerod M: Attention and performance, IX. 1981, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ:153–169.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Jeannerod M. The timing of natural Prehension movements. J Motor Behav. 1984;16(3):235–54.CrossRef Jeannerod M. The timing of natural Prehension movements. J Motor Behav. 1984;16(3):235–54.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Gentilucci M, Chieffi S, Scarpa M, Castiello U. Temporal coupling between transport and grasp components during prehension movements: effects of visual perturbation. Behav Brain Res. 1992;47(1):71–82.CrossRef Gentilucci M, Chieffi S, Scarpa M, Castiello U. Temporal coupling between transport and grasp components during prehension movements: effects of visual perturbation. Behav Brain Res. 1992;47(1):71–82.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Marteniuk RG, Leavitt JL, Mackenzie CL, Athenes S. Functional-relationships between grasp and transport components in a Prehension task. Hum Movement Sci. 1990;9(2):149–76.CrossRef Marteniuk RG, Leavitt JL, Mackenzie CL, Athenes S. Functional-relationships between grasp and transport components in a Prehension task. Hum Movement Sci. 1990;9(2):149–76.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Castiello U. The neuroscience of grasping. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6(9):726–36.CrossRef Castiello U. The neuroscience of grasping. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6(9):726–36.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat van Vliet PM, Sheridan MR. Coordination between reaching and grasping in patients with hemiparesis and healthy subjects. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2007;88(10):1325–31.CrossRef van Vliet PM, Sheridan MR. Coordination between reaching and grasping in patients with hemiparesis and healthy subjects. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2007;88(10):1325–31.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Paulignan Y, MacKenzie C, Marteniuk R, Jeannerod M. Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements. 1. The effects of changing object position. Exp Brain Res. 1991;83(3):502–12.CrossRef Paulignan Y, MacKenzie C, Marteniuk R, Jeannerod M. Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements. 1. The effects of changing object position. Exp Brain Res. 1991;83(3):502–12.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Davare M, Parikh PJ, Santello M. Sensorimotor uncertainty modulates corticospinal excitability during skilled object manipulation. J Neurophysiol. 2019. Davare M, Parikh PJ, Santello M. Sensorimotor uncertainty modulates corticospinal excitability during skilled object manipulation. J Neurophysiol. 2019.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Shibata D, Santello M. Role of digit placement control in sensorimotor transformations for dexterous manipulation. J Neurophysiol. 2017;118(5):2935–43.CrossRef Shibata D, Santello M. Role of digit placement control in sensorimotor transformations for dexterous manipulation. J Neurophysiol. 2017;118(5):2935–43.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Rizzo AS, Kim GJ. A SWOT analysis of the field of virtual reality rehabilitation and therapy. Presence-Teleop Virt. 2005;14(2):119–46.CrossRef Rizzo AS, Kim GJ. A SWOT analysis of the field of virtual reality rehabilitation and therapy. Presence-Teleop Virt. 2005;14(2):119–46.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Viau A, Feldman AG, McFadyen BJ, Levin MF. Reaching in reality and virtual reality: a comparison of movement kinematics in healthy subjects and in adults with hemiparesis. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2004;1(1):11.CrossRef Viau A, Feldman AG, McFadyen BJ, Levin MF. Reaching in reality and virtual reality: a comparison of movement kinematics in healthy subjects and in adults with hemiparesis. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2004;1(1):11.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Knaut LA, Subramanian SK, McFadyen BJ, Bourbonnais D, Levin MF. Kinematics of pointing movements made in a virtual versus a physical 3-dimensional environment in healthy and stroke subjects. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2009;90(5):793–802.CrossRef Knaut LA, Subramanian SK, McFadyen BJ, Bourbonnais D, Levin MF. Kinematics of pointing movements made in a virtual versus a physical 3-dimensional environment in healthy and stroke subjects. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2009;90(5):793–802.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Saling M, Mescheriakov S, Molokanova E, Stelmach GE, Berger M. Grip reorganization during wrist transport: the influence of an altered aperture. Exp Brain Res. 1996;108(3):493–500.CrossRef Saling M, Mescheriakov S, Molokanova E, Stelmach GE, Berger M. Grip reorganization during wrist transport: the influence of an altered aperture. Exp Brain Res. 1996;108(3):493–500.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Haggard P, Wing A. Coordination of hand aperture with the spatial path of hand transport. Exp Brain Res. 1998;118(2):286–92.CrossRef Haggard P, Wing A. Coordination of hand aperture with the spatial path of hand transport. Exp Brain Res. 1998;118(2):286–92.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Robert MT, Levin MF. Validation of reaching in a virtual environment in typically developing children and children with mild unilateral cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2018;60(4):382–90.CrossRef Robert MT, Levin MF. Validation of reaching in a virtual environment in typically developing children and children with mild unilateral cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2018;60(4):382–90.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Rand MK, Lemay M, Squire LM, Shimansky YP, Stelmach GE. Role of vision in aperture closure control during reach-to-grasp movements. Exp Brain Res. 2007;181(3):447–60.CrossRef Rand MK, Lemay M, Squire LM, Shimansky YP, Stelmach GE. Role of vision in aperture closure control during reach-to-grasp movements. Exp Brain Res. 2007;181(3):447–60.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Fu Q, Ushani A, Jentoft L, Howe RD, Santella M. Human reach-to-grasp compensation with object pose uncertainty. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2013;2013:6893–6.PubMed Fu Q, Ushani A, Jentoft L, Howe RD, Santella M. Human reach-to-grasp compensation with object pose uncertainty. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2013;2013:6893–6.PubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Youn J, Wohn K: Realtime collision detection for virtual reality applications. In: Proceedings of IEEE virtual reality annual international symposium: 18–22 sept. 1993 1993; 1993: 415–421. Youn J, Wohn K: Realtime collision detection for virtual reality applications. In: Proceedings of IEEE virtual reality annual international symposium: 18–22 sept. 1993 1993; 1993: 415–421.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Fu QS, Santello M: Towards a complete description of grasping kinematics: a framework for quantifying human grasping and manipulation. 2011 Annual International Conference of the Ieee Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (Embc) 2011:8247–8250. Fu QS, Santello M: Towards a complete description of grasping kinematics: a framework for quantifying human grasping and manipulation. 2011 Annual International Conference of the Ieee Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (Embc) 2011:8247–8250.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Prachyabrued M, Borst CW. Visual feedback for virtual grasping. In: 2014 IEEE symposium on 3D user interfaces (3DUI): 29–30 march 2014 2014; 2014. p. 19–26.CrossRef Prachyabrued M, Borst CW. Visual feedback for virtual grasping. In: 2014 IEEE symposium on 3D user interfaces (3DUI): 29–30 march 2014 2014; 2014. p. 19–26.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Bozzacchi C, Volcic R, Domini F. Effect of visual and haptic feedback on grasping movements. J Neurophysiol. 2014;112(12):3189–96.CrossRef Bozzacchi C, Volcic R, Domini F. Effect of visual and haptic feedback on grasping movements. J Neurophysiol. 2014;112(12):3189–96.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Fu Q, Zhang W, Santello M. Anticipatory planning and control of grasp positions and forces for dexterous two-digit manipulation. J Neurosci. 2010;30(27):9117–26.CrossRef Fu Q, Zhang W, Santello M. Anticipatory planning and control of grasp positions and forces for dexterous two-digit manipulation. J Neurosci. 2010;30(27):9117–26.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Coordination of reach-to-grasp in physical and haptic-free virtual environments
verfasst von
Mariusz P. Furmanek
Luis F. Schettino
Mathew Yarossi
Sofia Kirkman
Sergei V. Adamovich
Eugene Tunik
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2019
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation / Ausgabe 1/2019
Elektronische ISSN: 1743-0003
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12984-019-0525-9

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Neu in den Fachgebieten Neurologie und Psychiatrie

Akuter Schwindel: Wann lohnt sich eine MRT?

28.04.2024 Schwindel Nachrichten

Akuter Schwindel stellt oft eine diagnostische Herausforderung dar. Wie nützlich dabei eine MRT ist, hat eine Studie aus Finnland untersucht. Immerhin einer von sechs Patienten wurde mit akutem ischämischem Schlaganfall diagnostiziert.

Niedriger diastolischer Blutdruck erhöht Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Komplikationen

25.04.2024 Hypotonie Nachrichten

Wenn unter einer medikamentösen Hochdrucktherapie der diastolische Blutdruck in den Keller geht, steigt das Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Ereignisse: Darauf deutet eine Sekundäranalyse der SPRINT-Studie hin.

Frühe Alzheimertherapie lohnt sich

25.04.2024 AAN-Jahrestagung 2024 Nachrichten

Ist die Tau-Last noch gering, scheint der Vorteil von Lecanemab besonders groß zu sein. Und beginnen Erkrankte verzögert mit der Behandlung, erreichen sie nicht mehr die kognitive Leistung wie bei einem früheren Start. Darauf deuten neue Analysen der Phase-3-Studie Clarity AD.

Viel Bewegung in der Parkinsonforschung

25.04.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Neue arznei- und zellbasierte Ansätze, Frühdiagnose mit Bewegungssensoren, Rückenmarkstimulation gegen Gehblockaden – in der Parkinsonforschung tut sich einiges. Auf dem Deutschen Parkinsonkongress ging es auch viel um technische Innovationen.