Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2021

Open Access 01.12.2021 | Research

Development and validity of the Value-based Stigma Inventory (VASI): a value-sensitive questionnaire for the assessment of mental health stigma

verfasst von: Sophia Rieckhof, Christian Sander, Sven Speerforck, Elke Prestin, Matthias C. Angermeyer, Georg Schomerus

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2021

Abstract

Background

It has been hypothesized that mental illness stigma differs according to what matters most to people, and that this results in value-based differences in stigma within societies. However, there is a lack of stigma measures that account for a broad range of values, including modern and liberal values.

Methods

For the development of the Value-based Stigma Inventory (VASI) a preliminary item-pool of 68 VASI-items was assembled by mental health and stigma experts. For psychometric evaluation, we tested the VASI in an online sample of the general population (n = 4983).

Results

Based on item-characteristics as well as explorative and confirmatory factor analyses, a final version of the VASI was developed, comprising 15 items and 5 subscales. The VASI shows good psychometric properties (item difficulty = 0.34 to 0.67; mean inter-item correlation r = 0.326; Cronbach’s α = 0.879). Medium to high correlations with established stigma scales (SDS, SSMI), medium associations with instruments assessing personal values (PVQ, KSA-3) and small to no associations with a social desirability scale (KSE-G) attest to good convergent and discriminatory validity of the new instrument. Normative values for the VASI subscales are presented.

Conclusions

The developed VASI can be used to assess public stigma of mental illness including personal stigma-relevant value orientations.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12888-021-03427-4.
Sophia Rieckhof and Christian Sander contributed equally to this work.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
CFA
Confirmatory Factor Analysis
CFI
Comparative Fit Index
EFA
Exploratory Factor Analysis
ESS
European Social Survey
EV
Eigenvalues
KMO
Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin
KSA-3
Authoritarianism Short Scale
KSE-G
Social Desirability–Gamma Short Scale
PCA
Principal Component Analysis
PPMI
Prejudice against People with Mental Illness Scale
PVQ
Portrait Values Questionnaire
RMSEA
Root Mean Square Error of Approximation
SDS
Social Distance Scale
SRMR
Standardized Root Mean Residual
SSMI
Self-Stigma in People with Mental Illness scale
TLI
Tucker-Lewis Index
VASI
Value-based Stigma Inventory
WMM
What Matters Most

Background

The stigma of mental illness has been shown to vary between different cultures [1]. To conceptualize cultural differences in mental illness stigma, Yang and co-workers [2] have introduced the theory of What Matters Most (WMM), predicting that stigma is most visible when threatening the loss or diminution of what matters most within a cultural context. They argue that culture specific measures of stigma are needed to measure stigma in those cultural interactions that are most relevant to the respondents of a specific cultural background.
Not only between, but also within cultures there are differences of WMM, visible for example in the growing polarization of Western societies. Inglehart and Norris [3] found that it is not so much socioeconomic status but primarily opposing cultural world views that play a central role in the formation of the Left-Right cleavage. They state that the shift to post-materialistic and cosmopolitan values is accompanied by a protesting departure from liberal values of tolerance and self-realization. Differentiations between the value dimension of the progressive, cosmopolitan milieu and the traditionalist, communitarian milieu are made by various authors [46].
For understanding such differences in mental illness stigma within cultures, Schomerus and Angermeyer [7] apply the WMM theory to the formation of stigma within Western societies. The authors hypothesize that, although conservative/authoritarian values have traditionally been found to be associated with mental illness stigma, liberal/modern values can similarly lead to stigma, but have not been thoroughly examined in this respect. Assuming a ‘blind spot in stigma research’, Schomerus and Angermeyer [7] hypothesize that liberal-minded people might also stigmatize people with mental illness, if these are perceived as a potential threat to their liberal values.
The theory that individuals of various outgroups are more likely prejudiced and discriminated if they are perceived as a threat to personal values is empirically supported [2, 813]. Brandt and coworkers [14] showed in three independent studies intolerance of both conservatives and liberals of groups whose values and worldviews contradict with their ideological worldview. To counter the assumed blind spot among mental illness stigma research, instruments are needed that measure mental illness stigma with regard to situations relevant both to people with conservative/authoritarian and liberal/modern values.

Aims & research hypotheses

The present study reports on the development and validation of a Value-based Stigma Inventory (VASI). To ensure the validity of the instrument, we examine aspects of discriminatory and convergent validity, setting up several assumptions. According to Cohen [15], correlation coefficients between the new instrument and established stigma scales should be moderate to high (≥0.5) and moderate (> 0.3) between the new instrument and established value scales. Both authoritarian and liberal values should be associated with increased stigmatization in respective subscales. Finally, correlation coefficients between the new instrument and an established social desirability scale should be small.

Methods

Creation of a preliminary item pool (VASI-68)

First, based on the heuristic model of party competition in Western societies by Inglehart and Norris [3] and the theory of basic values of Schwartz [16] we distinguished six possible value orientations: liberal, authoritarian, community welfare, individual well-being, performance and hedonism. Second, items were generated by a team of mental health stigma experts and a person with lived experience (MA, EP, SR, GS) relating stigma and exclusion to potential conflicts with these value orientations. For example, the item If you are living with a person with mental illness, it is difficult to lead a life according to your own ideas was created to illustrate a potential conflict with individualism and hedonism. Third, by means of a rating by an extended team of experts (including MA, SR, CS, SS and GS), items were reformulated and reduced from 156 to 68, taking into account criteria such as comprehensibility, unambiguity, everyday relevance, face validity and possible response tendencies.

Data collection

A sample of the German adult population who were registered in a professional online-access panel (KANTAR) responded to an online survey containing 68 VASI items and other questionnaires related to mental health stigma and personal values. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Leipzig (239/20-ek) and conducted in accordance to the declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided informed consent. Data collection was carried out in cooperation with a social and market research institute (USUMA).
Sampling was stratified for age, gender and place of residence of the respondents, resulting in a sample matching the general population for these characteristics. A total of 8136 people responded to the invitation to the survey. Of these, 199 (2.4%) did not agree to participate, 681 (8.4%) did not complete the questionnaire and 754 (9.3%) could not participate since the predefined quota for age, gender or place of residence were already accomplished.
After pretesting the questionnaire, a minimum time of 11 min to respond to all items had been determined to reduce the number of rapid guessers [17]. A total of 1.480 (18.2%) responders fell below this time limit and were therefore excluded. A further 10 data sets had to be excluded due to a high proportion of item non-response. In total, 5012 unique datasets were obtained.

Sample

From the dataset of 5012 cases, 29 datasets (0.6%) were excluded due to missing values in at least one of the 68 VASI items. Therefore, the final sample comprises of 4983 data sets. Of these, 2373 (47.6%) were from male and 2604 (52.3%) from female participants, while 6 participants (0.1%) identified as divers. The average age of the sample was 47.4 years (SD = 14.387, range 18-74 years). Further socio-demographic data can be found in Table 1.
Table 1.
Characteristics of the study population
Characteristics
German Population a
Total Sample (n = 983)
Gender, n (%)
 male
48.9
47.6
 female
51.1
52.3
 divers
0.1
Age-Groups, n (%)
 18–24 years
9.0
8.9
 25–34 years
15.2
14.1
 35–44 years
14.7
17.1
 45–54 years
17.2
22.7
 55–64 years
17.8
24.1
  ≥ 65 years
26.0
13.0
Educational Level, n (%)
 low
33.6
12.5
 middle
29.9
37.5
 high
32.5
50.0
aAnnotation: population data according to the Federal Statistical Office (www-genesis.​destatis.​de): data on gender and age groups as of 31 December 2019 based on extrapolation of the population trend after the 2011 microcensus; data on educational level based on the 2018 survey of the population > 15 years (excluding the population in shared accommodation)

Survey

The survey was subdivided in order to allow the use of different instruments, while at the same time ensuring a tolerable survey length for the individual participant. All survey participants first answered the preliminary VASI items. The items were presented in randomized order to avoid sequence effects. Participants were asked to respond to each item on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
After answering the VASI-items, participants were randomly assigned to one of two groups differing with respect to the applied value measures (see Fig. 1). Subgroup A (N = 2489) processed several items on socio-political opinions that are not used for this publication. Subgroup B (N = 2494) answered the Portrait Values Questionnaire (PVQ, [18]). The 21-item version (PVQ-21) was used as applied in the European Social Survey (ESS). For a better representation of the subscale universalism, an additional item from the long version representing the subscale universalism was added (He wants everyone to be treated justly, even people he doesn’t know. It is important to him to protect the weak in society, [19]). Both subgroups answered the Authoritarianism Short Scale (KSA-3 [20]).
Afterwards, both groups were again divided randomly (ratio 4:1) into two subgroups, which answered different stigma scales. The first subgroup (N = 3986) processed the subscales Aware and Agree of the Self-Stigma in People with Mental Illness scale (SSMI-short form [21], German version: [22]) and the Social Distance Scale (SDS [23], German version: [24]). The latter was used in a modified version with two additional items for a more contemporary recording of social distance (To what extent would you invite such a person to your home?, To what extent would you give such a person your phone number?). The second subgroup (N = 997) answered the Prejudice against people with mental Illness scale (PPMI [25], results not used for this publication). Finally, all participants completed the Social Desirability–Gamma Short Scale (KSE-G [26]).

Statistical analyses

For psychometric analysis, item distributions were tested for normality using the Shapiro-Wilks test. Item difficulty was calculated as pm = sum of item scores / (N * maximum item score) and item-total correlations (rit) were calculated as measure of item discriminatory power. According to Bortz and Döring [27] discriminatory power should be > 0.5, while items with discriminatory power < 0.3 therefore require a scale revision, e.g. by deleting the affected items.
Exploratory and confirmatory analyses of the factorial structure were carried out in two independent subgroups to which the survey participants were randomly assigned during the study. First, in subsample A exploratory factor analyses were carried out using a principal component analysis (PCA) approach, including all items of the preliminary VASI item pool with satisfactory item discriminatory power (rit > 0.3). The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure and the Bartlett test were used to check if requirements for factor analysis were met. Eigenvalues (> 1) and the scree plot are used as criteria for determining the optimal number of factors. To obtain factors that are as uncorrelated as possible, an orthogonal rotation technique was used. According to Gorsuch [28], a varimax rotation is inappropriate because a general factor (mental health stigma) can be expected. Therefore, an Equamax rotation was chosen, which would distribute the variance of the factor loadings as evenly as possible over the factors. Factor loadings < 0.3 were not considered, as such low factor loadings indicate that a variable has less than 10% common variance with the factor [29, 30].
Subsequently, confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) using maximum likelihood estimations were performed in subsample B to confirm the factorial structure. Normality of the data was examined using the Mardia test. The adequacy of the model fit was assessed using goodness-of-fit indices recommended by Beauducel and Wittmann [31] and DiStefano and Hess [32], with cutoffs specified in various publications [3335]: Chi2-test (with Bollen-Stine-Bootstrap in case of multivariate non-normality); Comparative Fit Index (CFI, cutoff ≥0.95), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI, cutoff > 0.95), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA, cutoff < 0.06) and Standardized Root Mean Residual (SRMR, cutoff < 0.08).
Scores of the resulting VASI subscales were analysed using descriptive statistics: mean, standard deviations as well as quartiles and selected percentiles. Cronbach’s α was computed as a measure of internal consistency. To investigate discriminative and convergent validity of the VASI, Spearman’s rank correlations coefficients were calculated.
The significance level was set at α = 0.05 (two-tailed). All statistical analyses were conducted using the statistical software SPSS 24 (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 24). For the confirmatory factor analyses, IBM SPSS AMOS 26.0 was used.

Results

Table 1 depicts the sociodemographic characteristics of the total sample (compared to the German population). The overall sample corresponds to the German population in terms of gender distribution, but comprises fewer participants in the age segment > 65 years, as these population strata can generally hardly be reached by online surveys. With regard to the educational level, our sample shows a higher proportion of respondents with high educational levels, which is also typical of online studies. The two subgroups A and B were created by random assignment and accordingly do not differ in terms of the socio-demographic characteristics.

VASI-68

Item analysis

The 68 preliminary VASI-items were first subjected to an item analysis (results not shown). The Shapiro-Wilks test confirmed that the distribution of all items differed significantly from a normal distribution (all p < .01). Out of the 68 items, 53 showed a significantly positive skewness (maximum 1.47), while 10 items were negatively skewed (minimum − 0.42). Kurtosis was significantly negative in 53 items (minimum − 0.93) and positive in 11 items (maximum 1.65). Difficulty indices were of medium size (range 0.34 to 0.84). The corrected item-total-correlations (rit) ranged from 0.11 to 0.74 with rit < 0.3 in 6 items, which were therefore removed from the VASI item pool for the next evaluations.

Exploratory factor analysis and item reduction

In order to determine the factorial structure of the VASI and to reduce the number of items, an explorative factor analysis was carried out on the remaining 62 VASI items in subsample A (N = 2989). The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of 0.980 and the Bartlett test (p < .001) showed that the requirements for factor analysis were met. A Principal Component Analysis (PCA) resulted in seven components with eigenvalues (EV) > 1, explaining a cumulative 53.3% of the variance. Based on the screeplot a strong general factor (EV = 21.018, explained variance 33.9%) could be identified on which all items had factor loadings > 0.3 with polarities corresponding to the direction of the respective item wording.
Since item compilation had been based on six topics and the EV of the 7th component (1.001) was only slightly above the usual cutoff, in the next step a PCA with Equamax-rotation was performed with a fixed amount of six factors, which still explained a large amount of variance (51.6%). All items had factor loadings >.300 on at least one component, yet several items showed cross loadings > 0.3 on at least one additional component.
Based on the item loadings, components could be described meaningfully in terms of content, although some deviations from the original six topics of the item compilation were noted. In the sense of the intended item reduction, for each component, the three items with the highest factor loadings were selected from all items that could be clearly assigned to a respective component (i.e. without cross-loading > 0.3 on other components). In a final revision, in order to avoid redundancies in the item wording, some items were exchanged with items of lower loadings but more relevant content. However, only two items could be allocated to the 6th component, as any possible 3rd item charged on at least one of the other five components. Therefore, the final item selection comprised of 15 items allocated to five components. These 15 items were again subjected to a PCA with five fixed components and Equamax-rotation to check whether the item reduction had an impact on the allocation of items to components (KMO = 0.899; Bartlett: p < .001; explained variance: 68.7%). Factor loadings on the respective components were high (range .656 to .853) and no item had loadings > 0.3 on more than one component (see Table 2).
Table 2.
Final VASI-15 Items and factor loadings
Items
ePCA
CFA
F1
(SR)
F2
(PE)
F3
(RE)
F4
(MV)
F5
(SE)
If you live together with a mentally ill person, it is difficult to lead a life according to your own ideas.
.853
−.134
.090
.085
.164
.801
Living together with a mentally ill person restricts one’s own quality of life.
.841
−.178
.156
.086
.156
.841
In interacting with a person with mental illness, you invest a lot of energy and get only little in return.
.656
−.208
.176
.269
.172
.673
People with mental illness are a valuable addition to society.
−.155
.805
.031
−.040
−.117
.672
In general, I feel comfortable spending time with a person who is mentally ill.
−.195
.774
−.108
−.038
.005
.642
Interacting with mentally ill people can be very enriching for oneself.
−.090
.743
−.099
−.214
−.208
.753
It is damaging my reputation if a mental illness becomes known in my family.
.145
−.057
.812
.137
.062
.613
Having mentally ill people in the neighbourhood impairs the attractiveness of my residential area.
.125
−.087
.765
.197
.274
.785
Just like beggars, mentally ill people taint the appearance of the city.
.143
−.043
.689
.229
.287
.732
Going easy on people with mental illness in the workplace is unfair to those who do not have a mental illness.
.150
−.126
.102
.788
.076
.618
Mental illness is often only an excuse for laziness.
.091
−.030
.249
.717
.223
.637
Nowadays people who are mentally ill are shown too much consideration.
.149
−.142
.220
.704
.289
.770
Mentally ill people commit particularly cruel crimes.
.199
−.060
.100
.149
.803
.679
Mentally ill people represent a great danger for children.
.199
−.160
.278
.251
.707
.795
The neighbourhood should be warned about people with severe mental illness.
.135
−.165
.267
.193
.705
.697
Eigenvalue
5.574
0.908
1.716
0.949
1.158
 
Explained Variance
14.285
13.315
13.901
13.326
13.873
 
Annotations: factor loadings are shown for components of exploratory principal component analysis (ePCA) in subsample A (N = 2489) and of confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) in subsample B (N = 2494). Subscales: SR Self-realization, PE Personal Enrichment, RE Reputation, MV Meritocratic Values, SE Security, PCA initial Eigenvalues and explained variance after Equamax rotation are given

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA)

To verify the validity of the exploratory factor analysis (see Fig. 2), a CFA was performed on the 15 selected items using data from subsample B (N = 2494). A significant Mardia test (z = 58.244, p < .001) indicated the absence of a multivariate normal distribution, although skewness and excess of all items were below the limits proposed by West and colleges [36]. Despite the implementation of a Bollen-Stine-Bootstrap, the Chi2 test reached significance (X2(80) = 502.902; p = .001), which questioned the fit of the model. However, other goodness-of-fit indices consistently attested a good model fit (CFI = 0.969; TLI = 0.959; RMSEA = 0.046; SRMR = .0368). Factor loadings were again high for all items (range .614 to .842, see Table 2). A description of the VASI scales from a content perspective is given in the next section.

VASI-15

Description of VASI-subscales and item analysis

Table 3 shows the 15 final VASI items, with their assignment to the five VASI subscales, which are described as follows:
Table 3.
Item characteristics of the final 15 VASI-items
 
VASI-item
Scale
Mean
Median
SD
γm
ω
pm
rit
1
If you live together with a mentally ill person, it is difficult to lead a life according to your own ideas.
SR
3.29
3.0
1.108
−0.339
−0.505
0.659
0.533
2
Going easy on people with mental illness in the workplace is unfair to those who do not have a mental illness.
MV
2.34
2.0
1.146
0.489
−0.546
0.468
0.493
3a
People with mental illness are a valuable addition to society.
PE
2.72
3.0
1.040
0.073
−0.233
0.544
0.400
4
Mental illness is often only an excuse for laziness.
MV
1.76
1.0
1.019
1.234
0.777
0.353
0.510
5
Living together with a mentally ill person restricts one’s own quality of life.
SR
3.15
3.0
1.128
−0.187
−0.627
0.629
0.573
6
It is damaging to my reputation if a mental illness becomes known in my family.
RE
1.76
1.0
1.021
1.264
0.837
0.352
0.456
7a
Interacting with mentally ill people can be very enriching for oneself.
PE
3.36
3.0
1.079
−0.278
−0.345
0.671
0.522
8
Having mentally ill people in the neighborhood impairs the attractiveness of my residential area.
RE
1.71
1.0
0.977
1.370
1.316
0.342
0.572
9a
In general, I feel comfortable spending time with a person who is mentally ill.
PE
2.84
3.0
1.056
0.029
−0.311
0.567
0.414
10
In interacting with a person with mental illness, you invest a lot of energy and get only little in return.
SR
2.75
3.0
1.160
0.139
−0.738
0.550
0.591
11
Mentally ill people commit particularly cruel crimes.
SE
2.33
2.0
1.179
0.471
−0.641
0.467
0.539
12
Just like beggars, mentally ill people taint the appearance of a city.
RE
1.72
1.0
1.001
1.365
1.223
0.345
0.564
13
Nowadays people who are mentally ill are shown too much consideration.
MV
2.17
2.0
1.120
0.663
− 0340
0.434
0.613
14
The neighborhood should be warned about people with severe mental illness.
SE
2.29
2.0
1.198
0.525
−0.676
0.459
0.578
15
Mentally ill people represent a great danger for children.
SE
2.36
2.0
1.071
0.495
−0.404
0.452
0.645
Annotations: a inverse coded, SD standard deviation, γm skewness, ω kurtosis, pm item difficulty, rit corrected item-total correlations. Subscales: SR Self-Realization, PE Personal Enrichment, RE Reputation, MV Meritocratic Values, SE Security
Subscale 1 Self-realization (SR) focusses on the extent to which the presence of people with mental illness in the immediate environment is perceived as restricting one’s well-being and enjoyment of life as well as the ability to achieve one’s individual life goals.
Subscale 2 Personal Enrichment (PE) includes the extent to which dealing with mentally ill persons is perceived as an enrichment for one’s life and represents an openness to diversity. Underlying are humanistic values. In contrast to the other VASI scales, these values are not threatened but rather promoted by dealing with the mentally ill persons. Accordingly, the items of this scale are inversely coded and need to be inverted before they can be included in the calculation of an overall VASI score.
Subscale 3 Reputation (RE) deals with the loss of social status associated with mental illness. It addresses the extent to which being associated with mentally ill people would be considered detrimental to one’s reputation.
Subscale 4 Meritocratic Values (MV) deals with two aspects of individualistic values. First, an underlying belief that too much consideration is given to mentally ill people, which is perceived as unfair against those who are not mentally ill. Mentally ill people are perceived as receiving benefits for which the respondent feels more entitled. Second, the scale includes the attitude of personal responsibility in conjunction with a high level of self-improvement, according to the principle that everyone is responsible for themselves. Both aspects can be summarized under the label Meritocratic Values, where people achieve and earn what they deserve.
Subscale 5 Security (SE) deals with the perceived dangerousness of mentally ill people, to whom a propensity to violence and a willingness to commit particularly serious crimes are attributed. The attitudes recorded on this scale correspond most closely to the stigmatising attitudes associated with authoritarianism attitudes.
The VASI subscales Self-realization and Security relate to the similarly titled subscales of the basic values of Schwartz [16], whereas Personal Enrichment is comparable to Schwartz´ Self-Transcendence. Reputation and Meritocratic Values are factors which are not to be equated with the basic values of Schwartz in their importance.
Item characteristics of the 15 VASI items are shown in Table 3. The difficulty indices ranged from 0.34 to 0.67, thus attesting to a medium difficulty. The mean inter-item correlation was r = 0.326, thus attesting to an optimal item homogeneity. Corrected item-total correlations ranged from 0.40 to 0.65 and the internal consistency was good for the total VASI scale (Cronach’s α = 0.879) and acceptable for the five VASI subscales (SR: α = 0.805; PE: α = 0.729; RE: α = 0.756; MV: α = 0.721; SE: α = 0.769). Mean Scores of the VASI scales separated by gender and age-groups are shown in Table 4. The final VASI questionnaire and notes on its evaluation can be found in Additional file 1.
Table 4.
VASI-15 Mean Scores
 
N
VASI-15 Total Score
Self-Realization (SR)
Personal Enrichment (PE)
Reputation (RE)
Meritocratic Values (MV)
Security (SE)
Males
  ≤ 24 years
288
7.26 (±1.935)
8.73 (±2.749)
8.49 (±2.598)
5.34 (±2.466)
6.48 (±2.690)
7.24 (±2.715)
 25–34 years
479
7.52 (±2.140)
8.72 (±2.969)
8.60 (±2.407)
5.99 (±3.109)
6.68 (±2.985)
7.62 (±3.053)
 35–44 years
445
7.70 (±2.109)
9.27 (±2.962)
9.18 (±2.546)
5.93 (±2.826)
6.66 (±2.935)
7.46 (±2.975)
 45–54 years
549
7.52 (±2.061)
9.37 (±2.894)
9.37 (±2.717)
5.44 (±2.435)
6.41 (±2.636)
7.02 (±2.897)
 55–64 years
521
7.23 (±1.950)
9.10 (±2.962)
9.16 (±2.470)
5.23 (±2.404)
6.03 (±2.594)
6.62 (±2.773)
  ≥ 65 years
235
7.30 (±1.978)
9.15 (±2.717)
9.28 (±2.448)
5.23 (±2.269)
6.40 (±2.424)
6.42 (±2.837)
 All age-groups
2517
7.44 (±2.046)
9.08 (±2.911)
9.04 (±2.559)
5.55 (±2.646)
6.44 (±2.747)
7.10 (±2.917)
Females
  ≤ 24 years
258
6.59 (±1.561)
8.30 (±2.518)
8.12 (±2.406)
4.48 (±1.897)
5.57 (±2.223)
6.46 (±2.355)
 25–34 years
447
7.29 (±1.931)
8.76 (±2.987)
9.14 (±2.626)
5.15 (±2.559)
6.24 (±2.794)
7.18 (±2.782)
 35–44 years
437
7.65 (±2.099)
9.48 (±3.096)
9.35 (±2.778)
5.32 (±2.545)
6.65 (±2.781)
7.48 (±2.975)
 45–54 years
528
7.36 (±2.018)
9.16 (±2.994)
9.12 (±2.532)
4.98 (±2.497)
6.58 (±2.736)
7.01 (±3.031)
 55–64 years
531
7.29 (±2.033)
9.29 (±2.997)
9.37 (±2.625)
4.78 (±2.195)
6.11 (±2.515)
6.89 (±2.915)
  ≥ 65 years
257
7.33 (±1.678)
9.44 (±2.840)
9.57 (±2.387)
4.63 (±1.781)
6.24 (±2.322)
6.78 (±2.601)
 All age-groups
2458
7.30 (±1.962)
9.11 (±2.953)
9.16 (±2.613)
4.94 (±2.344)
6.29 (±2.635)
7.02 (±2.855)
Total Sample
  ≤ 24 years
546
6.94 (±1.797)
8.52 (±2.648)
8.31 (±2.513)
4.94 (±2.255)
6.05 (±2.519)
6.88 (±2.578)
 25–34 years
928
7.41 (±2.043)
8.74 (±2.973)
8.86 (±2.526)
5.57 (±2.884)
6.46 (±2.902)
7.40 (±2.932)
 35–44 years
883
7.67 (±2.106)
9.36 (±2.986)
9.26 (±2.668)
5.62 (±2.705)
6.65 (±2.857)
7.46 (±2.974)
 45–54 years
1078
7.45 (±2.041)
9.27 (±2.944)
9.24 (±2.630)
5.21 (±2.476)
6.49 (±2.686)
7.01 (±2.962)
 55–64 years
1054
7.26 (±1.991)
9.19 (±2.978)
9.26 (±2.551)
5.00 (±2.310)
6.07 (±2.554)
6.75 (±2.846)
  ≥ 65 years
492
7.32 (±1.825)
9.30 (±2.783)
9.43 (±2.418)
4.92 (±2.048)
6.32 (±2.370)
6.61 (±2.719)
 All age-groups
4981
7.37 (±2.006)
9.09 (±2.931)
9.09 (±2.587)
5.25 (±2.519)
6.36 (±2.693)
7.06 (±2.886)

Discriminant & convergent validity

Correlations of the VASI-scores and other scales are depicted in Table 5. As expected, the highest correlations were found between the overall VASI score and the individual VASI subscales (R = .636 to .787). Intercorrelations between the five VASI-subscales were of medium size, with highest correlations between subscale Security and subscale Meritocratic Values (R = .522) as well as with subscale Reputation (R = .518) and lowest correlations between subscale Personal Enrichment and subscales Reputation (R = .307), Meritocratic Values (R = .355) and Security (R = .367).
Table 5.
Rank-Correlation coefficients (Spearman) between VASI scores and instruments assessing stigmatization and personal values
  
N
VASI-15 Total Score
Self-Realization (SR)
Personal Enrichment (PE)
Reputation
(RE)
Meritocratic Values
(MV)
Security
(SE)
Scale Inter-correlation
VASI-15 Total Score
4983
1
.738***
−.636***
.718***
.751***
.787***
Subscale Self-Realization
4983
.738***
1
−.419***
.400***
.405***
.453***
Subscale Personal Enrichment
4983
−.636***
−.419***
−1
−.307***
−.355***
−.367***
Subscale Reputation
4983
.718***
.400***
−.307***
1
.507***
.518***
Subscale Meritocratic Values
4983
.751***
.405***
−.355***
.507***
1
.522***
Subscale Security
4983
.787***
.453***
−.367***
.518***
.522***
1
Convergent validity
SDS: Mean Score
3984
.676***
.455***
−.582***
.450***
.463***
.575***
SSMI: Aware Score
3986
.120***
.105***
.022
.088***
.090***
.162***
SSMI: Agree Score
3986
.670***
.421***
−.343***
.506***
.547***
.625***
KSA-3: Authoritarianism Score
4983
.334***
.214***
−.190***
.231***
.297***
.296***
PVQ: Self-Enhancement Score
2494
.129***
.052**
−.063**
.152***
.121***
.111***
PVQ: Self-Transcendence Score
2491
−.412***
−.194***
.318***
−.379***
−.387***
−.283***
PVQ: Conservation Score
2494
.071***
.078***
−.074***
−.009
.021
.099***
PVQ: Openness Score
2493
−.102***
−.094***
.136***
−.082***
−.038
−.036
Discriminant validity
KSE-G: positive qualities Score
4981
−.120***
−.062***
.119***
−.147***
−.085***
−.054***
KSE-G: negative qualities Score
4982
.182***
.093***
−.022
.218***
.186***
.151***
KSE-G: Social Desirability Score
4980
−.192***
−.100***
.075***
−.228***
−.175***
−.136***
Annotations: * p < .050; ** p < .010; *** p < .001; coefficients > |.200| are depicted in bold font
Concerning associations with other stigma related scales, a strong positive correlation between the VASI total score and the SDS total score was shown (R = .676). Of the VASI subscales, the strongest correlations with social distance were found for Security (R = .575) and Personal Enrichment (R = −.582). The two sub-scales of the SSMI show very different association patterns with the VASI. Strong positive correlations were found for the SSMI-subscale Stereotype Agreement, which measures individual endorsement of the common public stereotypes, and the VASI total score (R = .670) as well as the VASI-subscale Security (R = .625) and as well as moderate associations for the other VASI-subscales. Neither the VASI total score, nor any VASI-subscale showed considerable correlations (R > .02) for the SSMI subscale Stigma Awareness, which assesses whether people know existing stereotypes about mental illnesses.
Associations between VASI scores and value-specific scales were in most cases significant but of smaller magnitude. Small positive correlations were found between the VASI total score (R = .334) and all VASI subscales (R = .190 to .297). We observed a recurring pattern across all VASI subscales mainly regarding two values. The Self-Transcendence scale (PVQ) showed a negative correlation with all VASI subscales, whereas the Authoritarianism scale (KSA-3) showed an overall positive correlation (the inversely coded Personal Enrichment subscale showing opposite relations). Authoritarianism was most strongly related to the VASI subscales Security and Meritocratic Values.
Associations with the KSE-G scores assessing social desirability were also significant, although arguably only due to the large sample size, as most correlations coefficients were well below 0.2. Only the VASI subscale Reputation showed small correlations with the KSE-G social desirability score (R = −.228). Looking at the two KSE-G subscales, one can assume that this correlation is mainly based on an association with the subscale Minimizing negative properties (R = .218).

Discussion

This study reports the development and the validity of the Value-based Stigma Inventory (VASI). It is the first questionnaire measuring mental illness stigma referring to a value-based concept. In addition to conservative/authoritarian value attitudes, an attempt was made to measure stigmatization in connection with liberal/modern values.
During the VASI development a preliminary pool of 68 items was generated by a mental health stigma expert team and a person with lived experience. These were surveyed with established stigma and value scales via an online panel. One strength is the questionnaire development by means of a large sample (N = 4983), which allows to capture the various value orientations prevalent among the German population. For the subscale development, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was carried out within one half of the sample, in which five factors were identified. Taking factor loadings and item contents into account, three items per factor were selected. This model could be verified by a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) within the other half of the sample. The Bollen-Stine corrected Chi2 test did reach significance, thus questioning the good model fit, however, the chi-square value is known to be susceptible to large sample sizes [32, 37], therefore it cannot be excluded that the statistical significance is mainly due to the large N of our sample. Accordingly, other goodness-of-fit indices consistently met the required cutoffs [3335], thus attesting a good model fit.
A five-factor structure of value-based stigma is proposed, forming the subscales Self-Realization, Personal Enrichment, Reputation, Meritocratic Values and Security, which are medium to high interrelated and represent a spectrum from liberal/modern to conservative/authoritarian stigma-associated values. Alternatively to this recommended calculation of 5 subscale scores, a total score can also be calculated (see Additional file 1).
The validity of the new instrument was investigated using established scales for the assessment of mental health stigma (SDS; SSMI), personal values (PVQ-21; KSA-3), as well as social desirability (KSE-G). The findings suggest a good convergent validity, with medium to high correlation coefficients between the VASI subscales and the SDS- and SSMIAgree-scores corresponding to our assumptions. At the VASI subscale level the correlation coefficients in both stigma scales are highest in Security and lowest in Personal Enrichment. As could be expected, people who are more likely to feel threatened and endangered by people with mental illness tend to have a greater need for social distance, while people who experience dealing with the mentally ill as enriching are more open for social contacts. In accordance with the recommendations by Cohen [15], we found correlations between the VASI scales and value scales to be of medium range. Correlations of the VASI subscales with the value scales approve that Security is associated with an authoritarian value orientation and Personal Enrichment is related to values of self-transcendence. This indicates that people with authoritarian values are stigmatizing to a higher degree than others, whereas values of self-transcendence function as a protective factor against stigmatization. This trend appears constantly on all VASI subscales. Contrary to our assumptions, the subscales do not significantly differ in the correlations with different values. This indication for the existence of a single stigma relevant value dimension (authoritarian values vs. self-transcendence) should nevertheless be viewed critically. It can be assumed that people with liberal values are more in conflict with their values (tolerance vs. individualism) than people with authoritarian values. Therefore, they may quickly see through the liberal stigma items and tend to respond in terms of social desirability. This could be considered in more subtle item formulations or the use of implicit measurement methods [38] in the future. Furthermore, a response shift due to an assumed higher education level among participants with a liberal basic attitude is possible.
An expansion of the liberal/modern concept of values, which was viewed primarily in connection with openness and tolerance, with the aspect of individualism should be considered as a possibility based on the data and should be further investigated in the future. The factor Self-Realization reflect aspects of personal development and self-optimization, which are associated with stigmatization of people with mental illness when presenting as a potential threat to relevant individualistic values. These results point to a field of tension between an anti-stigmatizing attitude and liberal stigmatization. For example, Wetherell and coworkers [39] found that both conservatives and liberals discriminate against ideologically divergent groups if those violate their core values. Thereby abstract values can mitigate (universalism, egalitarianism) or reinforce (traditionalism) discrimination. This suggests that discrimination takes place among liberal-minded people, but in contrast to conservative persons, it might be mitigated by their liberal values.
Concerning discriminatory validity, low correlations to the subscale SSMIAware are shown. In distinction to SSMIAgree it is not measuring stigma per se [40], but rather the awareness of stereotype in the public. The VASI asks for agreement with stereotypical opinions of mental illness, therefore the differences in correlations between the two SSMI subscales are as to be expected. The correlation with the social desirability scale are within an acceptable small range except for the subscale Reputation and the KSE-G subscale Negative Qualities. It can be argued that people who hide own negative characteristics in order to appear socially desirable are likely to associate mental illness with loss of reputation and prestige.
With the VASI scale, we introduce a valid instrument that integrates public stigma of mental health with individual value preferences. The scale includes items pertaining to stigma relevant values beyond authoritarianism, as well as to stigma protective values. This extension of the content of the stigma assessment by a multifaceted value concept is an important strength of this scale and distinguishes it from other already established stigma scales.

Limitations and perspectives

Although the sample of this study is still comparatively large, there are two limitations inherent in data collection via online panel. First, the sample cannot be regarded as representative of the German-speaking resident population, as no statistical random selection was carried out. Participants with older age and a higher education were overrepresented. However, it can be assumed that the size of the sample ensures that people with the most diverse attitudes and values were included. Second, a relatively high percentage of survey participants (18.2%) were not included because they answered the survey below the pre-determined minimum response time of 11 min. When determining the time limit, we were guided by the processing times of independent consultants with questionnaire experience, the distribution of the pretest processing times and the recommendations of a visual inspection of the frequency distribution of response times by Wise and DeMars [41]. Although this time limit was intended to exclude participants with rapid guessing behavior, nevertheless an exclusion of participants with solution behavior but a high processing time cannot be excluded.
Reliability was determined by calculating the internal consistencies. However, since we conducted a one-time, anonymous online survey, no information on the test-retest reliability of the VASI can be derived from this study. The investigation of retest reliability needs to be considered in future use of the instrument.

Conclusions

In total the study offers sufficient data to substantiate the validity of the VASI. The questionnaire showed a clear factor structure and convergent and discriminant validity. The use of the VASI can be suggested in further studies to measure public stigma of mental illness including personal stigma relevant value orientations. The VASI forms an interface between psychiatric, psychological and sociological fields of research. One possible field of application could be the detection of stigma in different socio-economic milieus in order to identify different patterns of discrimination against persons with mental illness.

Acknowledgments

Not applicable.

Declarations

Ethical approval was given by the medical ethics committee the University of Leipzig with the following reference number: 239/20-ek. All participants gave written informed consent. The study was conducted in accordance to the declaration of Helsinki.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

All authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Inglehart RF, Norris P. Trump, Brexit, and the Rise of Populism: Economic Have-Nots and Cultural Backlash [Faculty Research Working Paper Series]. Havard: Havard Kennedy School; 2016. Inglehart RF, Norris P. Trump, Brexit, and the Rise of Populism: Economic Have-Nots and Cultural Backlash [Faculty Research Working Paper Series]. Havard: Havard Kennedy School; 2016.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodhart D. The road to somewhere: the populist revolt and the future of politics. New York: Oxford University Press; 2017. Goodhart D. The road to somewhere: the populist revolt and the future of politics. New York: Oxford University Press; 2017.
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Koppetsch C. Die Gesellschaft des Zorns: Rechtspopulismus im globalen Zeitalter. Bielefeld: transcript-Verlag; 2019.CrossRef Koppetsch C. Die Gesellschaft des Zorns: Rechtspopulismus im globalen Zeitalter. Bielefeld: transcript-Verlag; 2019.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Crandall CS, Schaller M, editors. Social Psychology of Prejudice: Historical and Contemporyry Issues. Lawrence, Kansas 66045: LEWINIAN PRESS; 2004. Crandall CS, Schaller M, editors. Social Psychology of Prejudice: Historical and Contemporyry Issues. Lawrence, Kansas 66045: LEWINIAN PRESS; 2004.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Stephan WG, Stephan CW. An integrated threat theory of prejudice. In: Oskamp S, editor. Reducing prejudice and discrimination. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2000. p. 23–46. Stephan WG, Stephan CW. An integrated threat theory of prejudice. In: Oskamp S, editor. Reducing prejudice and discrimination. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2000. p. 23–46.
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Biernat M, Vescio TK. Values and prejudice. In: Crandall CS, Schaller M, editors. Social psychology of prejudice: historical and contemporary issues. Kansas, Lawrence: Lewian Press; 2004. p. 191–216. Biernat M, Vescio TK. Values and prejudice. In: Crandall CS, Schaller M, editors. Social psychology of prejudice: historical and contemporary issues. Kansas, Lawrence: Lewian Press; 2004. p. 191–216.
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Wise SL, Ma L. Setting response time thresholds for a CAT Item Pool: The Normative Threshold Method; Annual meeting of the National Council on Measurement in Education, Vancouver, Canada. 2012: 163-183. Wise SL, Ma L. Setting response time thresholds for a CAT Item Pool: The Normative Threshold Method; Annual meeting of the National Council on Measurement in Education, Vancouver, Canada. 2012: 163-183.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Kemper CJ, Beierlein C, Bensch D, Kovaleva A, Rammstedt B. Eine Kurzskala zur Erfassung des Gamma-Faktors sozial erwünschten Antwortverhaltens: Die Kurzskala Soziale Erwünschtheit-Gamma (KSE-G). GESIS-Working Papers 2012|25. Köln: GESIS2012. Kemper CJ, Beierlein C, Bensch D, Kovaleva A, Rammstedt B. Eine Kurzskala zur Erfassung des Gamma-Faktors sozial erwünschten Antwortverhaltens: Die Kurzskala Soziale Erwünschtheit-Gamma (KSE-G). GESIS-Working Papers 2012|25. Köln: GESIS2012.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Gorsuch RL. Factor analysis. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1983. Gorsuch RL. Factor analysis. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1983.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Kline P. An easy guide to factor analysis. London: Routledge. London: Routledge; 1994. Kline P. An easy guide to factor analysis. London: Routledge. London: Routledge; 1994.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Bühner M. Einführung in die Test- und Fragebogenkonstruktion. 3rd ed. Munich: Pearson Education Deutschland GmbH; 2011. Bühner M. Einführung in die Test- und Fragebogenkonstruktion. 3rd ed. Munich: Pearson Education Deutschland GmbH; 2011.
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Schermelleh-Engel K, Moosbrugger H. Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods Psychol Res Online. 2003;8:23–74. Schermelleh-Engel K, Moosbrugger H. Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods Psychol Res Online. 2003;8:23–74.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat West SG, Finch JF, Curran PJ. Structural Equatation models with nonormal variables: problems and remedies. In: Hoyle RH, editor. Structural Equatation modeling: concepts, issues, and applications. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 1995. p. 56–75. West SG, Finch JF, Curran PJ. Structural Equatation models with nonormal variables: problems and remedies. In: Hoyle RH, editor. Structural Equatation modeling: concepts, issues, and applications. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 1995. p. 56–75.
Metadaten
Titel
Development and validity of the Value-based Stigma Inventory (VASI): a value-sensitive questionnaire for the assessment of mental health stigma
verfasst von
Sophia Rieckhof
Christian Sander
Sven Speerforck
Elke Prestin
Matthias C. Angermeyer
Georg Schomerus
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2021
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2021
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-021-03427-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2021

BMC Psychiatry 1/2021 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).