Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2006

Open Access 01.12.2006 | Research article

Event-related potentials when identifying or color-naming threatening schematic stimuli in spider phobic and non-phobic individuals

verfasst von: Iris-Tatjana Kolassa, Frauke Musial, Stephan Kolassa, Wolfgang HR Miltner

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2006

Abstract

Background

Previous studies revealed increased parietal late positive potentials (LPPs) in response to spider pictures in spider phobic individuals. This study searched for basic features of fear-relevant stimuli by investigating whether schematic spider images are sufficient to evoke differential behavioral as well as differential early and late ERP responses in spider phobic, social phobic (as a clinical control group), and non-phobic control participants.

Methods

Behavioral and electrophysiological correlates of the processing of schematic spider and flower images were investigated while participants performed a color (emotional Stroop) and an object identification task. Stimuli were schematic pictures of spiders and flowers matched with respect to constituting visual elements.

Results

Consistent with previous studies using photographic spider pictures, spider phobic persons showed enhanced LPPs when identifying schematic spiders compared to schematic flowers. In addition, spider phobic individuals showed generally faster responses than the control groups. This effect was interpreted as evidence for an increased general behavioral hypervigilance in this anxiety disorder group. Furthermore, both phobic groups showed enhanced P100 amplitudes compared to controls, which was interpreted as evidence for an increased (cortical) hypervigilance for incoming stimuli in phobic patients in general. Finally, all groups showed faster identification of and larger N170 amplitudes in response to schematic spider than flower pictures. This may reflect either a general advantage for fear-relevant compared to neutral stimuli, or might be due to a higher level of expertise in processing schematic spiders as compared to the more artificially looking flower stimuli.

Conclusion

Results suggest that schematic spiders are sufficient to prompt differential responses in spider-fearful and spider-non-fearful persons in late ERP components. Early ERP components, on the other hand, seem to be modified by anxiety status per se, which is consistent with recent theories on general hypervigilance in the anxiety disorder spectrum.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1471-244X-6-38) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

ITK designed the study, acquired the data, performed the data analysis and statistics, and wrote the manuscript. FM developed the stimuli for the study and participated in conceiving the study. SK programmed the paradigm and assisted in statistical analysis and writing of the manuscript. WHRM conceived the study, acquired funding and provided infrastructure. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Background

Spiders are genuinely feared stimuli for individuals with spider phobia, but they are also considered fear-relevant (ancestral) stimuli for humans in general, and it has been hypothesized that such stimuli are detected and processed preferentially to other stimuli [14]. In support of this hypothesis, Öhman, Flykt, and Esteves [5] reported faster detection of fear-relevant stimuli among neutral stimuli than vice versa in a visual search task (however, see [6] for a critical comment on the study). Furthermore, phobic persons detected feared stimuli even faster than fear-relevant stimuli they did not fear [5]. Thus, it has been suggested that phobic individuals are characterized by an attentional bias towards their feared object, i.e., phobics' attention is drawn involuntarily and automatically to the feared object, and they process it with high selectivity and priority ([79], for an overview see [9]).
However, in a visual search paradigm Miltner et al. [6] observed no threat advantage in spider phobic and control persons for spiders when participants' task was to search for a spider target stimulus among a crowd of neutral stimuli displayed in a 4 × 4 grid pattern search array. Instead, they found delayed responses in spider phobic individuals to a neutral target (mushroom) in a crowd of neutral objects (flowers) in the presence of a spider distractor. In the same series of studies, by measuring eye movements they observed that phobic individuals first moved their eyes toward the spider distractor before focusing on the neutral target, delaying phobics' target detection times. These findings might suggest that an attentional bias is present in phobic persons only if the feared stimulus is not the focus of attention (cf. also [10]).
One paradigm that allows the investigation of the processing of threatening stimuli when the threatening information itself is not task-relevant is the emotional Stroop paradigm. The emotional Stroop paradigm is a modified version of the original Stroop task [11] and is commonly used to assess attentional biases in anxiety disorders or high trait anxiety (for an overview see [9]). In this paradigm, the difference in color-naming latencies between emotionally relevant and emotionally neutral words or pictures is measured. The phenomenon that anxious persons show prolonged response latencies when color-naming feared stimuli has been called emotional Stroop interference (see [9]). Numerous studies have reported emotional interference in animal phobic individuals ([1217]; but see [18]). The emotional Stroop paradigm can be regarded as an implicit task, i.e. the emotional content of the stimulus is not the focus of attention, as compared to an explicit task in which subjects explicitly identify the emotional content of the stimulus. It has been shown that task conditions affect brain activation to emotional stimuli [10, 1921], and thus implicit and explicit tasks should be compared when investigating the processing of fear-related stimuli.
Although spiders are commonly regarded as fear-relevant stimuli, it remains largely unclear which perceptual properties make a spider fear-relevant. It has been postulated that specific feature detectors with high sensitivity to elementary threat features exist, "programmed" either genetically or by conditioning (cf. [1, 22]). These detectors screen incoming information for specific threat cues (e.g. high intensity or biologically prepared stimulus characteristics). As soon as a specific threat feature has been detected, the arousal system is activated still preattentively, and the stimulus is selected for preferential treatment by succeeding stages of stimulus elaboration [1]. However, as Öhman et al. ([5], p. 475) remark, "such elementary threat features [. . .] still remain to be specified".
This study searched for basic features of fear-relevant stimuli by investigating whether schematic spider pictures are sufficient to evoke differential responses in spider phobic and spider-non-phobic individuals, in order to provide first insights regarding the question which properties constitute the fear-relevance of a spider. The advantages of schematic stimuli are obvious: schematic stimuli reduce the depicted object to its essential features and are therefore simple and unequivocal. Schematic stimuli show less variance: in the case of spider stimuli there is no confoundation with spider species, hairiness, size, or camera angle. Finally, it is easier to design a control stimulus matched with respect to color, size, and spatial frequency: if one shifts the angles of the legs of a schematic spider image a schematic flower picture results.
Preceding studies using real spider pictures reported that spider phobic individuals exhibited larger parietal late positive potentials (LPPs) in response to and a faster identification of spiders than birds or flowers as well as larger LPPs and faster reaction times in response to spider pictures than controls and social phobic persons ([18]; compare also [23]). Spider-non-fearful persons also showed a trend towards larger LPPs for spiders than flowers. The enhanced LPPs in response to spiders in the spider phobic group in particular were interpreted as a correlate of increased perceptual processing of these stimuli. ERP studies consistently found larger LPPs for emotional, both pleasant and unpleasant, compared to neutral stimuli [2435].
Furthermore, if the attentional bias observed in persons with specific phobia is caused by abnormalities in the early visual processing stream, ERP studies should offer evidence for such abnormal processes in early visual components such as the occipital P100 and the posterior occipito-temporal N170 component – in support of this view, Straube, Mentzel, and Miltner [36] observed increased activation of extrastriate visual cortex in social phobic persons compared to non-phobic controls when processing faces in an fMRI study. The P100 is known to be an attention-sensitive component (for a review see [37]). The N170, although generally assumed to indicate face processing mechanisms [38, 39], also seems to be modulated by perceptual expertise in discriminating objects [4042]. For example, Tanaka and Curran [42] reported larger N170 amplitudes when experts categorized objects in their domain of expertise relative to objects outside their domain of expertise. Because spider phobics might be experts in the processing of spiders, they could thus be expected to show larger N170 amplitudes for spiders than neutral objects.
Following the experimental design of Kolassa et al. [18], this study used a modified emotional Stroop task (implicit task) and an object identification task (explicit task): in the former participants identified the color of red or blue schematic spiders or flowers, in the latter they classified the stimulus category. Participants were individuals with spider phobia, social phobic persons as an additional clinical control group, and non-phobic controls. In the color identification task, we expected longer response latencies for the color identification of spiders compared to flowers in spider phobics, i.e. emotional Stroop interference. Furthermore, in the object identification task, a faster identification of spiders in general and in particular by spider phobics was expected. Regarding ERP data, larger LPPs in response to spiders in general and in spider phobic individuals in particular were expected. Furthermore, we expected spider phobic participants to respond to spiders with larger early visual ERPs, i.e., P100 and N170 components, reflecting attentional bias and expertise effects, respectively.

Methods

Pilot Study

In a pilot study, 43 students of the University of Jena rated schematic spider and flower pictures as to their affective valence and arousal with the Self-Assessment Manikin Scale (SAM; [43]). Fourteen of these participants were diagnosed with spider phobia (5 male, 9 female; mean age M = 22.3 years, SD = 3.8), 12 with social phobia (7 male, 5 female; mean age M = 26.1 years, SD = 3.9), and 17 were healthy controls (7 male, 10 female; mean age M = 23.1 years, SD = 3.7). Participants in the pilot study were recruited, screened and diagnosed by the same procedure as participants in the main experiment.
ANOVAs revealed significantly more unpleasant, F(1,40) = 50.39, p < .0001, ηp 2 = .56, and arousing, F(1,40) = 36.71, p < .0001, ηp 2 = .48, ratings for pictures displaying schematic spiders than schematic flowers (see Table 1). Furthermore, main effects of Group for valence, F(2,40) = 5.50, p = .008, ηp 2 = .22, and arousal ratings, F(2,40) = 8.08, p = .001, ηp 2 = .29, were observed. As expected, individuals with spider phobia rated schematic spider pictures as significantly more unpleasant, interaction of Group × Object, F(2,40) = 7.91, p = .001, ηp 2 = 28, and arousing, F(2,40) = 11.46, p = .0001, ηp 2 = .36, than controls and persons with social phobia. Thus, the pictures used in this study were suitable to elicit specific responses in each group.
Table 1
Valence and arousal ratings
 
Controls
Spider Phobics
Social Phobics
Rating
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
Valence
      
   Spider
5.46
1.65
3.30
1.52
4.59
0.69
   Flower
6.05
1.45
5.29
1.27
5.39
0.77
Arousal
      
   Spider
2.74
1.42
5.30
1.63
3.27
1.31
   Flower
2.24
1.10
3.28
1.25
2.93
1.38
Mean (M) valence and arousal ratings and standard deviations (SD) for schematic spider and flower stimuli for each group. The SAM scale [23, 43, 79] ranges from 1 to 9, with 1 = highly unpleasant/low arousing and 9 = highly pleasant/highly arousing.

Participants

Fifty-six individuals (mean age 23 years, SD = 3.5, age range 19–32 yrs) participated in the study: 18 spider phobic individuals (9 male, 9 female), 19 social phobic patients (10 male, 9 female), and 19 controls (10 male, 9 female). Groups did not differ in mean age. Fifty-three participants were right-handed and 3 left-handed as measured by the Edinburgh handedness questionnaire [44]. All participants were students of the University of Jena recruited by newspaper advertisement, on campus bulletin boards and in lectures. All procedures were approved by the ethics committee of the University of Jena. Participants provided informed consent and were paid 6 € an hour for their participation. As additional compensation participants with spider phobia were offered a one-day spider phobia therapy [45] and individuals with social phobia received a 10-session group training of social skills after the experiment [46].
Prior to the experiment proper, participants were screened with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I; [47]). Spider phobic and social phobic individuals were included in the study if they fulfilled DSM-IV criteria [48] of spider phobia and social phobia respectively, but did not suffer from any other current or previous disorders according to DSM-IV. Controls were accepted for participation if they had no current or past mental disorders according to DSM-IV. All participants were free of any psychotropic medication. Prior to the experiment, all participants completed German versions of the Spider Questionnaire (SPQ; [49]), the Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory (SPAI; [50]), the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; [51]), and the Trait Anxiety Questionnaire of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; [52, 53]). See Table 2 for questionnaire values.
Table 2
Questionnaire values
 
Control Group
Spider Phobics
Social Phobics
 
Questionnaire
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
One-way ANOVA
SPQ
2.47
1.78
20.61
2.66
2.58
1.95
F(2,53) = 430.97, p < .001*‡
SPAI
33.53
16.89
44.41
15.51
126.81
18.17
F(2,53) = 171.72, p < .001
BDI
2.68
2.71
4.94
4.76
9.42
7.09
F(2,53) = 8.52, p = .001
STAI-T
30.79
5.92
33.50
8.05
50.47
6.61
F(2,53) = 45.27, p < .001*‡
Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) for each group. The German scores of the Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory (SPAI) were transformed into the original scores [80]. STAI-T, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory – Trait Version; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; SPQ, Spider Questionnaire.
* Control group differs from spider phobics (p < .005). Control group differs from social phobics (p < .05). ‡ Spider phobics differ from social phobics (p < .05)
Social phobic individuals scored on average higher on the BDI than controls and individuals with spider phobia, but the scores were not in the clinically significant range, M = 9.42, SD = 7.09. The high comorbidity of social phobia with depression is well established [5456].

Paradigm

The experiment consisted of two blocks, each preceded by a training phase. In each block, 60 pictures of schematic spiders and flowers (30 of each) were presented for 1 s with a variable interstimulus interval of 2–3 s. Of each category 15 stimuli were colored red and the other 15 blue. Four different types of schematic flower and spider pictures were used (see Figure 1): flower stimuli varied with regard to the size of the flower's interior and the angularity of the outline petals; spider pictures differed in body size and angularity of spider legs (similar stimuli were also used by Vuilleumier and Schwartz [57]). Flowers differed from spiders only insofar as four legs of the spiders were reflected about a diagonal axis.
In one block participants' task was to identify the color (blue or red) of the stimulus, whereas in the other block they were instructed to categorize the object (spider or flower). Answers were given by pressing one of two buttons on a button box with the index finger of the dominant hand while EEG was recorded. Participants were asked to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible. The presentation order of the two blocks as well as the assignment of buttons was counterbalanced across participants. The order of the stimuli in each block was pseudorandomized: no more than 4 times did the same color and no more than 4 times did the same object category occur in a row. The occurrence of identical objects of the same color was allowed only 2 times in a row.

Assessment and Analysis of EEG

During the testing session participants sat in a comfortable chair in a sound-attenuated room. Stimuli were presented on a 20 inch Sony monitor (resolution 800 × 600) placed 1.1 m in front of the subject. EEG was recorded with a 62-electrode montage according to the international 10-10 system [58] with additional non-standard electrodes (AF1, AF2, PO1, PO3) at frontal and occipital sites spaced equally between the standard electrodes. Cz served as a reference electrode. Electrode impedances were kept below 5 kΩ. Vertical and horizontal electrooculograms (VEOG and HEOG) were measured for off-line correction of eye movements and blink artifacts. EEG data were recorded continuously in AC mode at a rate of 500 Hz.
The EEG raw data were filtered (low pass = 30 Hz, 24 dB/oct; high pass = 0.1 Hz, 24 dB/oct), segmented (200 ms pre- to 900 ms poststimulus), corrected for blinks [59], and screened for artifacts with the software Brain Vision Analyzer 1.04 (Brain Products GmbH, Germany). Trials containing artifacts (amplitude deviations of ± 150 μV) were rejected. Altogether 2.8% of trials were excluded due to artifacts (3.0% Identify Color Spiders; 2.5% Identify Object Spiders; 3.1% Identify Color Flowers; 2.7% Identify Object Flowers). Artifact-free EEG epochs were averaged for each subject, condition and electrode. All epochs were aligned to the prestimulus baseline from -200 to 0 ms and rereferenced to an average reference.
Time intervals chosen for peak detection were based on visual inspection of the data and grand means and agreed well with those suggested by a temporal principal component analysis (PCA). Using varimax rotation, 9 components were extracted from ERPs. Factor 2 explained 16.2% of total variance and showed a broad parietal distribution with a maximum at Pz. This factor peaked at 330 ms and showed a turning point at about 370 ms after which a second prolonged positivity followed. Subjects loading high on this factor showed a pronounced positivity in this latency range in the ERP. Factor 3 explained 6.4% of total variance, was maximal on electrodes P7 and P8, and peaked at 150 ms. Subjects loading high on this factor showed a negative component on P7 and P8 electrodes in this latency range. Factor 4 explained 4.2% of total variance and had a pronounced occipital maximum at 80 ms. Subjects loading high on this component showed a pronounced positivity in this latency range. Because the other factors are of no relevance in this context, they are not further discussed here.
Factor peak-latency and topography characteristics thus associate Factor 2 with the late parietal potentials (LPPs), Factor 3 with the wave labeled N170 and Factor 4 with P100. Since it is well known that multiple positive components over parietal areas are frequently observed in response to emotional stimuli (e.g., [18, 26, 30]), and since it is possible that the PCA was not able to disentangle such multiple components due to substantial overlap, we decided for LPPs to analyze mean amplitudes in the time intervals 270–370 ms (P300) and 380–480 ms (P400) for electrodes P3, Pz, and P4. P100 and N170 peaks were detected bilaterally at electrodes O1 and O2 (50–100 ms) and electrodes P7 and P8 (100–200 ms), respectively.

Statistical Analysis

For data analysis, linear mixed effects models [60] were implemented using SAS 9.1 (SAS Institute Inc.). Linear mixed models are particularly well suited for repeated measurement designs within the same individual that can lead to positive correlations between measurements. In all analyses of variance (ANOVAs), Subjects served as a random effect [61], whereas all other factors were fixed effects. Significant effects in an ANOVA were further analyzed by calculating relevant contrasts, where rejection of the null hypothesis was controlled by Holm's sequential rejection algorithm [62]. Original p-values that remained significant after α-correction are reported below. As a measure of effect size, partial eta squared (ηp 2) is reported.
In the reaction time analysis, trials were excluded in which no reaction-time button-press occurred, the answer was wrong, or the response time was below 200 ms. A 3 × 2 × 2 ANOVA with between factor Group and repeated measures factors Task (identify color, identify object) and Object (spider, flower) was performed.
Mean amplitudes in the time intervals 270–370 ms (P300) and 380–480 ms (P400) were analyzed by means of a 3 × 2 × 2 × 3 ANOVA with between factor Group and repeated measures factors Task, Object, and Laterality (left, central, right). P100 and N170 amplitudes were also analyzed with a 3 × 2 × 2 × 2 ANOVA with between factor Group and repeated measures factors Task, Object, and Laterality (left, right). Three individuals (two controls, one social phobic) had to be excluded from the analysis of LPPs due to severe occipito-parietal alpha activity. For the analysis of early visual components, three individuals (one social phobic, two spider phobics) were excluded because they showed no detectable P100 and N170 components.

Results

Table 3 shows an overview of significant statistical results.
Table 3
Overview of statistical results
 
F value
p value
Reaction times
  
   Group
F(2,53) = 3.67
p = .03
   Task
F(1,53) = 13.52
p = .0006
   Object
F(1,53) = 9.36
p = .004
   Task × Object
F(1,53) = 15.02
p = .0003
P100
  
   Group
F(2,50) = 3.32
p = .04
   Laterality
F(1,52) = 4.39
p = .04
   Object × Laterality
F(1,208) = 4.75
p = .03
N170
  
   Object
F(1,50) = 16.14
p = .0002
   Task × Object
F(1,208) = 5.09
p = .03
P300
  
   Task
F(1,50) = 19.50
p < .0001
   Object
F(1,50) = 5.91
p = .02
   Task × Object
F(1,364) = 22.62
p < .0001
   Group × Task × Object
F(4,364) = 7.62
p < .0001
   Laterality
F(2,104) = 16.91
p < .0001
P400
  
   Object
F(1,50) = 10.06
p = .003
   Task × Object
F(1,364) = 8.33
p = .004
   Group × Task × Object
F(4,364) = 5.42
p = .0003
   Laterality
F(2,104) = 20.87
p <.0001

Reaction Times

Participants failed to respond in 1.1% of all trials, and incorrect responses were observed in 1.2% of all trials. Groups did not differ in total mistakes (omissions & incorrect choices), Kruskal-Wallis χ2(2, N = 56) = 1.7, p = .43.
Participants identified the color of an object faster than the object itself, main effect of Task, F(1,53) = 13.52, p = .0006, ηp 2 = .20. Furthermore, a main effect of Object, F(1,53) = 9.36, p = .004, ηp 2 = .15, and an interaction of Task × Object, F(1,53) = 15.02, p = .0003, ηp 2 = .22, were observed. All groups showed similar color identification latencies for schematic spiders and flowers. Thus, spider phobic individuals showed no emotional Stroop interference when identifying the color of schematic spiders (see Table 4). However, in the object identification task, all groups identified schematic spiders faster than schematic flowers, p < .0001.
Table 4
Reaction times
Condition
Control Group
Spider Phobics
Social Phobics
 
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
Color identification
      
Spider
522.77
100.45
479.40
74.20
501.24
75.51
Flower
523.13
99.64
478.11
69.81
501.69
72.08
Object identification
      
Spider
540.62
72.72
469.99
76.43
541.51
54.00
Flower
563.93
64.25
501.84
55.72
561.00
56.92
Mean response latencies (M) and standard deviances (SD) for the color and object identification of schematic spiders and flowers for each group.
Furthermore, a main effect of Group, F(2,53) = 3.67, p = .03, ηp 2 = .12, was observed. Subsequent contrasts showed that individuals with spider phobia responded faster than controls and social phobic persons, p = .01.

Analysis of P100 amplitude

ANOVA revealed a main effect of Group, F(2,50) = 3.32, p = .04, ηp 2 = .12. Larger P100 amplitudes for social and spider phobic persons were observed compared to controls, p = .01 (see Figure 2). P100 amplitudes were larger over left compared to right sites, main effect of Laterality, F(1,52) = 4.39, p = .04, ηp 2 = .09. However, as the interaction Object × Laterality revealed, F(1,208) = 4.75, p = .03, ηp 2 = .10, this difference was only present for schematic flowers, p = .007, and not for schematic spiders.

Analysis of N170 amplitude

Larger N170 amplitudes for spiders than flowers were observed, main effect of Object, F(1,50) = 16.14, p = .0002, ηp 2 = .24 (see Figure 3). This effect was modulated by task, interaction Task × Object, F(1,208) = 5.09, p = .03, ηp 2 = .08. In both tasks, spiders led to larger N170 amplitudes than flowers. However, this effect was more pronounced in the color identification task, p < .0001, than in the object identification task, p = .04. No significant main effect of or interaction with group was observed.

Analysis of P300 amplitude

Color identification led to larger P300 amplitudes than object identification, main effect of Task, F(1,50) = 19.5, p < .0001, ηp 2 = .28. Spiders led to larger P300 amplitudes than flowers, main effect of Object, F(1,50) = 5.91, p = .02, ηp 2 = .11. However, this effect was only present in the object, but not in the color identification task, interaction of Task × Object, F(1,364) = 22.62, p < .0001, ηp 2 = .11. Furthermore, this effect was only significant in the spider phobic group, as the interaction of Group × Task × Object, F(4,364) = 7.62, p < .0001, ηp 2 = .14, revealed, although it was present as a tendency also in the control groups (see Figure 4). Subsequent contrasts confirmed larger P300 amplitudes in spider phobic persons in response to schematic spiders than flowers in the object identification task, p = .005. In addition, there was a tendency towards smaller P300 amplitudes in spider phobic persons when identifying the color of a spider which, however, was not significant.
A main effect of laterality, F(2,104) = 16.91, p < .0001, ηp 2 = .25, showed larger P300 amplitudes over central compared to left or right sites, both p < .0001, as well as over right compared to left sites, p = .002.

Analysis of P400 amplitude

Larger P400 amplitudes were observed in response to schematic spiders than flowers, main effect of Object, F(1,50) = 10.06, p = .003, ηp 2 = .17. However, this effect was only present in the object identification task but not in the color identification task, interaction of Task × Object, F(1,364) = 8.33, p = .004, ηp 2 = .05. Although this effect was as a tendency present in all groups, it was most pronounced in individuals with spider phobia (compare Figure 4), interaction of Group × Task × Object, F(4,364) = 5.42, p = .0003, ηp 2 = .11. Subsequent contrasts confirmed that schematic spiders led to larger P400 amplitudes in individuals with spider phobia than schematic flowers when the task was to identify the object, p < .0001, whereas in the control groups the effect was not significant after α-adjustment. In the color identification task spider phobic individuals again showed a tendency towards smaller P400 amplitudes in response to schematic spiders than flowers, which, however, was not significant.
P400 amplitudes were maximal at central electrodes, main effect of Laterality, F(2,104) = 20.87, p < .0001, ηp 2 = .29. Subsequent contrasts revealed larger P400 amplitudes at central sites compared to left, p < .0001, and right hemispheric sites, p < .0001.

Discussion

The analysis of behavioral data showed that persons with spider phobia responded faster than both control groups when identifying schematic spiders in the object identification task. However, it must be noted that individuals with spider phobia responded generally faster than the control groups, while all groups identified schematic spiders faster than flowers. In contrast to the original hypotheses, no emotional Stroop interference was observed in spider phobic individuals when identifying the color of schematic spiders.
Analysis of ERPs revealed that both social phobic and spider phobic individuals showed larger P100 amplitudes compared to non-phobic controls. Furthermore, all groups, whether spider phobic or not, showed larger N170 amplitudes in response to schematic spiders than flowers. Finally, individuals with spider phobia showed enhanced LPPs (P300 and P400), in response to spiders compared to flowers in the object identification task.

Behavioral Data

Facilitation for schematic spiders

All groups identified schematic spiders faster than schematic flowers, supporting Öhman's theory [1] that spiders are fear-relevant (ancestral) stimuli which are processed with high selectivity and priority by all human beings, whether phobic or not. However, in a similar study design with "real" spider pictures, Kolassa et al. [18] found no facilitated responses for spiders in non-spider-fearful individuals. Thus, the question arises why spider-non-fearful persons show facilitated identification of schematic but not photographic spider images. It is possible that the cause for the different findings lies in the experimental design of the studies. On the one hand, the task performance may have been easier when using schematic as compared to veridical pictures, and on the other hand, whereas the present study used only two types of stimuli (schematic spiders and flowers), Kolassa et al. [18] used three types of trial-unique stimuli (photographic spider, bird, and flower images). These differences in task difficulty are also reflected in the smaller standard deviations in the present compared to the Kolassa et al. [18] study. Because data were less noisy it is possible that even small effects reached significance in the present study.

Hypervigilance in spider phobic individuals

Persons with spider phobia responded generally faster than both control groups, presumably due to a general "behavioral" hypervigilance in phobic persons. According to Beck et al. [[63], p. 31], "The [anxious] patient is hypervigilant, constantly scanning the environment for signs of impending disaster or personal harm." According to Eysenck [6466], there are two ways in which individuals high in trait anxiety show hypervigilance: general hypervigilance or distractability is demonstrated by a propensity to attend to any task-irrelevant stimuli presented, and specific hypervigilance is demonstrated by a tendency to attend selectively to threat-related rather than neutral stimuli. But why should such a general behavioral hypervigilance be present in spider phobic but not in social phobic persons? One explanation for the lack of similarly faster responses of social phobic persons might be the larger depression and general anxiety-related psychopathology in this patient group, which reciprocally diminishes fear reactivity. Increasing psychopathology along the anxiety disorder spectrum (specific phobia → social phobia → panic disorder with agoraphobia → generalized anxiety disorder) is a well-established finding [6769]. Lang et al. [69] showed that individuals suffering from specific phobia are most reactive to specific cues in the environment, e.g., their startle reflex was most pronounced in a startle probe modulation task involving imagery of social and survival threat. However, defensive reflexes were diminished with increasing generalized anxiety and depression. It has been shown that this reflex pattern is not specific to fear cues that are related to phobics' clinical problems [68, 70]. Lang et al. [69] suggested that generalized anxiety and comorbid depression additively attenuate startle potentiation to imagined threat in anxiety disordered patients.

Absence of Stroop interference

This study found no emotional Stroop interference in spider phobic persons when identifying the color of schematic spiders, which is consistent with a preceding study by Kolassa et al. [18] that also found no emotional interference in a similar experimental paradigm with spider phobic individuals. As discussed by Kolassa et al. [18], several explanations might account for this finding: a two-choice color identification task might be too simple for interference to occur, or else emotional interference might be smaller or even absent when using a manual response mode instead of a vocal one (compare [71, 72]). Furthermore, different formats of the emotional Stroop task might not be psychometrically equivalent (card vs. computer, blocked vs. randomized designs; compare [7377]. Finally, whether pictorial or linguistic stimuli are used seems to affect the magnitude of emotional Stroop interference: No study so far reported larger interference for pictorial than for linguistic stimuli [13, 78]. Because pictures of spiders seem to be more ecologically valid stimuli for spider phobic persons than spider-related words, one would have expected the opposite effect. Possibly, linguistic stimuli increase task difficulty, thus enhancing interference.

Early Visual Components: P100 and N170

Phobic persons displayed larger P100 amplitudes than non-phobic individuals regardless of the processed stimulus material or task, in accordance with a reanalysis of our previous study [18] in which originally P100 and N170 amplitudes were not analyzed.
Because the P100 is thought to be attention-sensitive (for a review see [12, 37]), the present data may be indicative of a "cortical" hypervigilance in phobic individuals [6466] and stronger attentional processing of incoming stimuli in phobic than in non-phobic persons. Both phobic groups were in situations they fear: persons with social phobia fear performance situations such as the experimental setting, while spider phobic persons viewed pictures of their feared object.
The processing of schematic spiders elicited larger N170 amplitudes than the processing of schematic flowers. This also is in accordance with the reanalysis of our previous study [18]: spiders led to largest N170 amplitudes, flowers to lowest with birds in between and with all contrasts highly significant. So far, the literature on the N170 component mainly focuses on face processing mechanisms (e.g., [38, 39]). More recently, it has been proposed that the amplitude of the N170 depends on the perceptual expertise in discriminating objects [4042]. Thus, the enhanced N170 for spiders in this study might be interpreted on the one hand as being due to a higher level of expertise in processing schematic spiders as compared to the more artificially looking flower stimuli, on the other hand it might reflect a general advantage for fear-relevant compared to neutral stimuli. Nevertheless, it remains intriguing that spider phobic persons do not exhibit an additional expert effect on the N170 over and beyond the control groups, as found both in the present study as well as in the reanalysis of the data of our previous study. Although the debate cannot be resolved at this point, a new interpretation of the N170 in addition to face processing and expertise effects might be called for.

Parietal Late Positive Potentials (LPPs)

The larger P300 and P400 amplitudes in response to schematic spiders compared to schematic flowers in the spider phobic group when the task was to identify the object fit the well-documented finding that parietal LPPs are influenced by the emotionality of presented stimuli (e.g. [24, 27, 28, 35]). Furthermore, the findings are in accordance with previous studies that found larger parietal LPPs in spider phobic persons when viewing veridical spider pictures [18, 23]. Thus, the data suggest that schematic spiders trigger meaning-related evaluative processes in the brains of spider phobic persons similar to those observed when processing real spider pictures.
Whereas enhanced LPPs in response to spiders were present in both tasks in spider phobic individuals in our previous study [18, 23], in the present study such an effect was only present in the object identification task, i.e. when attention was directed explicitly at the spider stimulus instead of at its color. Indeed, there was even a tendency towards smaller amplitudes in response to schematic spiders than flowers in persons with spider phobia in the color identification task. The present results might suggest that spider phobics try to avoid detailed processing of spider pictures if the task does not require object processing, and that it might be easier to avoid processing of schematic spiders than real spiders when only their color has to be identified, by processing low-level visual elements of schematic pictures, conceptually remaining on the level of a basic arrangement of lines. On the other hand, photographic stimuli as used in our previous study may not be so easily reducible to abstract basic elements, engaging resources in processing the stimulus category even in color identification and thus showing the effects of differential processing of phobic stimuli by spider phobics.

Conclusion

Individuals with spider phobia showed larger LPPs when their task was to identify schematic spiders, suggesting that these stimuli prompted phobia-specific responses in these individuals. Results indicate that schematic spiders are sufficient to evoke differential processing in spider-fearful compared to non-spider-fearful persons. Furthermore, all groups, whether spider phobic or not, exhibited differential behavioral and ERP responses in response to schematic spiders compared to schematic flowers, suggesting that spider stimuli are special stimuli all individuals, whether phobic or not. Finally, consistent with current models on hypervigilance in the anxiety disorder spectrum, this study found behavioral and electrophysiological evidence for an enhanced hypervigilance in individuals with spider phobia.

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by grant Mi265/6-1 of the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft) awarded to WHRM and a doctoral grant of the German National Academic Foundation (Studienstiftung des deutschen Volkes) to ITK. Thanks are due to Alexander Mohr, Sandra Bergmann, and Katharina Stößel for their assistance in data acquisition. Furthermore, we are very grateful for Dr. Willi Nagl's support in the statistical analysis of the data. ITK is now at Department of Psychology, Clinical & Neuropsychology, University of Konstanz, Germany. SK is now at SAF AG, Tägerwilen, Switzerland.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

ITK designed the study, acquired the data, performed the data analysis and statistics, and wrote the manuscript. FM developed the stimuli for the study and participated in conceiving the study. SK programmed the paradigm and assisted in statistical analysis and writing of the manuscript. WHRM conceived the study, acquired funding and provided infrastructure. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Öhman A: Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena: clinical phenomenology, evolutionary perspectives and information processing mechanisms. Handbook of Emotions. Edited by: Lewis M and Haviland JM. 1993, New York, Guilford Press, 511-536. Öhman A: Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena: clinical phenomenology, evolutionary perspectives and information processing mechanisms. Handbook of Emotions. Edited by: Lewis M and Haviland JM. 1993, New York, Guilford Press, 511-536.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Öhman A, Mineka S: Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear learning. Psychological Review. 2001, 108: 483-522. 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483.CrossRefPubMed Öhman A, Mineka S: Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear learning. Psychological Review. 2001, 108: 483-522. 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Seligman MEP: Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy. 1971, 2: 307-320. 10.1016/S0005-7894(71)80064-3.CrossRef Seligman MEP: Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy. 1971, 2: 307-320. 10.1016/S0005-7894(71)80064-3.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Mineka S, Öhman A: Phobias and preparedness: The selective, automatic, and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Psychiatry. 2002, 52: 927-937. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01669-4.CrossRefPubMed Mineka S, Öhman A: Phobias and preparedness: The selective, automatic, and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Psychiatry. 2002, 52: 927-937. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01669-4.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Öhman A, Flykt A, Esteves F: Emotion Drives Attention: Detecting the Snake in the Grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2001, 130: 466-478. 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466.CrossRef Öhman A, Flykt A, Esteves F: Emotion Drives Attention: Detecting the Snake in the Grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2001, 130: 466-478. 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Miltner WHR, Krieschel S, Hecht H, Trippe RH, Weiss T: Eye movements and behavioral responses to threatening and non-threatening stimuli during visual search in phobic and non-phobic subjects. Emotion. 2004, 4: 323-339. 10.1037/1528-3542.4.4.323.CrossRefPubMed Miltner WHR, Krieschel S, Hecht H, Trippe RH, Weiss T: Eye movements and behavioral responses to threatening and non-threatening stimuli during visual search in phobic and non-phobic subjects. Emotion. 2004, 4: 323-339. 10.1037/1528-3542.4.4.323.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Öhman A: The orienting response, attention and learning: An information processing perspective. The Orienting Reflex in Humans. Edited by: Kimmel HD, van Olst EH and Orlebeke JF. 1979, Hillsdale NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 443-471. Öhman A: The orienting response, attention and learning: An information processing perspective. The Orienting Reflex in Humans. Edited by: Kimmel HD, van Olst EH and Orlebeke JF. 1979, Hillsdale NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 443-471.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Öhman A, Esteves F, Flykt A, Soares JJF: Gateways to consciousness: Emotion, attention, and electrodermal activity. Progress in Electrodermal Research. Edited by: Roy JC, Boucsein W, Fowles DC and Gruzelier JH. 1993, New York, Plenum, 137-157.CrossRef Öhman A, Esteves F, Flykt A, Soares JJF: Gateways to consciousness: Emotion, attention, and electrodermal activity. Progress in Electrodermal Research. Edited by: Roy JC, Boucsein W, Fowles DC and Gruzelier JH. 1993, New York, Plenum, 137-157.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Williams J, Mark G, Watts FN, MacLeod C, Mathews A: Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders. 1997, Chichester, Wiley Williams J, Mark G, Watts FN, MacLeod C, Mathews A: Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders. 1997, Chichester, Wiley
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Straube T, Kolassa IT, Glauer M, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WHR: Effect of task conditions on brain responses to threatening faces in social phobics: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry. 2004, 56: 921-930. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.024.CrossRefPubMed Straube T, Kolassa IT, Glauer M, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WHR: Effect of task conditions on brain responses to threatening faces in social phobics: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry. 2004, 56: 921-930. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.024.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Stroop JR: Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1935, 18: 643-662.CrossRef Stroop JR: Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1935, 18: 643-662.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Kindt M, Brosschot JF: Phobia-related cognitive bias for pictorial and linguistic stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997, 106: 644-648. 10.1037/0021-843X.106.4.644.CrossRefPubMed Kindt M, Brosschot JF: Phobia-related cognitive bias for pictorial and linguistic stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997, 106: 644-648. 10.1037/0021-843X.106.4.644.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Lavy E, van den Hout M: Selective Attention evidenced by pictorial and linguistic Stroop tasks. Behavior Therapy. 1993, 24: 645-657. 10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80323-5.CrossRef Lavy E, van den Hout M: Selective Attention evidenced by pictorial and linguistic Stroop tasks. Behavior Therapy. 1993, 24: 645-657. 10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80323-5.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Lavy EH, van den Hout M, Arntz A: Attentional bias and spider phobia: conceptual and clinical issues. Behaviour Research & Therapy. 1993, 31: 17-24. 10.1016/0005-7967(93)90038-V.CrossRef Lavy EH, van den Hout M, Arntz A: Attentional bias and spider phobia: conceptual and clinical issues. Behaviour Research & Therapy. 1993, 31: 17-24. 10.1016/0005-7967(93)90038-V.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin M, Horder P, Gregory V: Integral bias in naming of phobia-related words. Cognition & Emotion. 1992, 6: 479-486.CrossRef Martin M, Horder P, Gregory V: Integral bias in naming of phobia-related words. Cognition & Emotion. 1992, 6: 479-486.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin M, Jones GV: Integral bias in the cognitive processing of emotionally linked pictures. British Journal of Psychology. 1995, 86: 419-435.CrossRefPubMed Martin M, Jones GV: Integral bias in the cognitive processing of emotionally linked pictures. British Journal of Psychology. 1995, 86: 419-435.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Watts FN, McKenna FP, Sharrock R, Trezise L: Colour naming of phobia-related words. British Journal of Psychology. 1986, 77: 97-108.CrossRefPubMed Watts FN, McKenna FP, Sharrock R, Trezise L: Colour naming of phobia-related words. British Journal of Psychology. 1986, 77: 97-108.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Kolassa IT, Musial F, Mohr A, Trippe RH, Miltner WHR: Electrophysiological correlates of threat processing in spider phobics. Psychophysiology. 2005, 42: 520-530.PubMed Kolassa IT, Musial F, Mohr A, Trippe RH, Miltner WHR: Electrophysiological correlates of threat processing in spider phobics. Psychophysiology. 2005, 42: 520-530.PubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Critchley H, Daly E, Phillips M, Brammer M, Bullmore E, Williams S, Van Amelsvoort T, Robertson D, David A, Murphy D: Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Human Brain Mapping. 2000, 9: 93-105. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(200002)9:2<93::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-Z.CrossRefPubMed Critchley H, Daly E, Phillips M, Brammer M, Bullmore E, Williams S, Van Amelsvoort T, Robertson D, David A, Murphy D: Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Human Brain Mapping. 2000, 9: 93-105. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(200002)9:2<93::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-Z.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Keightley ML, Winocur G, Graham SJ, Mayberg HS, Hevenor SJ, Grady CL: An fMRI study investigating cognitive modulation of brain regions associated with emotional processing of visual stimuli. Neuropsychologia. 2003, 41: 585-596. 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00199-9.CrossRefPubMed Keightley ML, Winocur G, Graham SJ, Mayberg HS, Hevenor SJ, Grady CL: An fMRI study investigating cognitive modulation of brain regions associated with emotional processing of visual stimuli. Neuropsychologia. 2003, 41: 585-596. 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00199-9.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Lange K, Williams LM, Young AW, Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Williams SC, Gray JA, Phillips ML: Task instructions modulate neural responses to fearful facial expressions. Biological Psychiatry. 2003, 53: 226-232. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01455-5.CrossRefPubMed Lange K, Williams LM, Young AW, Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Williams SC, Gray JA, Phillips ML: Task instructions modulate neural responses to fearful facial expressions. Biological Psychiatry. 2003, 53: 226-232. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01455-5.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Öhman A, Dimberg U, Öst LG: Animal and social phobias: biological constraints on learned fear responses. Theoretical Issues in Behavior Therapy. Edited by: Reiss S and Bootzin RR. 1985, Orlando, FL, Academic Press Öhman A, Dimberg U, Öst LG: Animal and social phobias: biological constraints on learned fear responses. Theoretical Issues in Behavior Therapy. Edited by: Reiss S and Bootzin RR. 1985, Orlando, FL, Academic Press
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Miltner WHR, Trippe RH, Krieschel S, Gutberlet I, Hecht H, Weiss T: Event-related brain potentials and affective responses to threat in spider/snake-phobic and non-phobic subjects. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2005, 57: 43-52. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.01.012.CrossRefPubMed Miltner WHR, Trippe RH, Krieschel S, Gutberlet I, Hecht H, Weiss T: Event-related brain potentials and affective responses to threat in spider/snake-phobic and non-phobic subjects. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2005, 57: 43-52. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.01.012.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Cuthbert BN, Schupp HT, Bradley MM, Birbaumer N, Lang PJ: Brain potentials in affective picture processing: Covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report. Biological Psychology. 2000, 52: 95-111. 10.1016/S0301-0511(99)00044-7.CrossRefPubMed Cuthbert BN, Schupp HT, Bradley MM, Birbaumer N, Lang PJ: Brain potentials in affective picture processing: Covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report. Biological Psychology. 2000, 52: 95-111. 10.1016/S0301-0511(99)00044-7.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Diedrich O, Naumann E, Maier S, Becker G, Bartussek D: A frontal positive slow wave in the ERP associated with emotional slides. Journal of Psychophysiology. 1997, 11: 71-84. Diedrich O, Naumann E, Maier S, Becker G, Bartussek D: A frontal positive slow wave in the ERP associated with emotional slides. Journal of Psychophysiology. 1997, 11: 71-84.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnston VS, Miller DR, Burleson MH: Multiple P3s to emotional stimuli and their theoretical significance. Psychophysiology. 1986, 23: 684-694.CrossRefPubMed Johnston VS, Miller DR, Burleson MH: Multiple P3s to emotional stimuli and their theoretical significance. Psychophysiology. 1986, 23: 684-694.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Keil A, Müller MM, Gruber T, Stolarova M, Wienbruch C, Elbert T: Effects of emotional arousal in the cerebral hemispheres: A study of oscillatory brain activity and event-related potentials. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2001, 112: 2057-2068. 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00654-X.CrossRefPubMed Keil A, Müller MM, Gruber T, Stolarova M, Wienbruch C, Elbert T: Effects of emotional arousal in the cerebral hemispheres: A study of oscillatory brain activity and event-related potentials. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2001, 112: 2057-2068. 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00654-X.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Keil A, Bradley MM, Hauk O, Rockstroh B, Elbert T, Lang PJ: Large-scale neural correlates of affective picture processing. Psychophysiology. 2002, 39: 641-649. 10.1017/S0048577202394162.CrossRefPubMed Keil A, Bradley MM, Hauk O, Rockstroh B, Elbert T, Lang PJ: Large-scale neural correlates of affective picture processing. Psychophysiology. 2002, 39: 641-649. 10.1017/S0048577202394162.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Laurian S, Bader M, Lanares J, Oros L: Topography of event-related potentials elicited by visual stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 1991, 10: 231-238. 10.1016/0167-8760(91)90033-T.CrossRefPubMed Laurian S, Bader M, Lanares J, Oros L: Topography of event-related potentials elicited by visual stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 1991, 10: 231-238. 10.1016/0167-8760(91)90033-T.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Mini A, Palomba D, Angrilli A, Bravi S: Emotional information processing and visual evoked brain potentials. Perceptual & Motor Skills. 1996, 83: 143-152.CrossRef Mini A, Palomba D, Angrilli A, Bravi S: Emotional information processing and visual evoked brain potentials. Perceptual & Motor Skills. 1996, 83: 143-152.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Palomba D, Angrilli A, Mini A: Visual evoked potentials, heart rate responses and memory to emotional pictorial stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 1997, 27: 55-67. 10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00751-4.CrossRefPubMed Palomba D, Angrilli A, Mini A: Visual evoked potentials, heart rate responses and memory to emotional pictorial stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 1997, 27: 55-67. 10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00751-4.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Radilová J: The late positive components of visual evoked responses sensitive to emotional factors. Activitas Nervosa Superior. 1982, Suppl. 3: 334-337.PubMed Radilová J: The late positive components of visual evoked responses sensitive to emotional factors. Activitas Nervosa Superior. 1982, Suppl. 3: 334-337.PubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Radilová J, Figar S, Radil T: Sexual arousal and visual perception. Activitas Nervosa Superior. 1983, 25: 168-170. Radilová J, Figar S, Radil T: Sexual arousal and visual perception. Activitas Nervosa Superior. 1983, 25: 168-170.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Radilová J, Figar S, Radil T: Emotional states influence the visual evoked potentials. Activitas Nervosa Superior. 1984, 26: 159-160. Radilová J, Figar S, Radil T: Emotional states influence the visual evoked potentials. Activitas Nervosa Superior. 1984, 26: 159-160.
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Schupp HT, Cuthbert BN, Bradley MM, Cacioppo JT, Ito T, Lang PJ: Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology. 2000, 37: 257-261. 10.1017/S0048577200001530.CrossRefPubMed Schupp HT, Cuthbert BN, Bradley MM, Cacioppo JT, Ito T, Lang PJ: Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology. 2000, 37: 257-261. 10.1017/S0048577200001530.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Straube T, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WHR: Common and distinct brain activation to threat and safety signals in social phobia. Neuropsychobiology. 2005, 52: 163-168. 10.1159/000087987.CrossRefPubMed Straube T, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WHR: Common and distinct brain activation to threat and safety signals in social phobia. Neuropsychobiology. 2005, 52: 163-168. 10.1159/000087987.CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Mangun GR: Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. Psychophysiology. 1995, 32: 4-18.CrossRefPubMed Mangun GR: Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. Psychophysiology. 1995, 32: 4-18.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Bentin S, Allison T, Puce A, Perez E, McCarthy G: Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1996, 8: 551-565.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bentin S, Allison T, Puce A, Perez E, McCarthy G: Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1996, 8: 551-565.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Eimer M: The face-specific N170 component reflects late stages in the structural encoding of faces. NeuroReport. 2000, 11: 2319-2324.CrossRefPubMed Eimer M: The face-specific N170 component reflects late stages in the structural encoding of faces. NeuroReport. 2000, 11: 2319-2324.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Gauthier I, Curran T, Curby KM, Collins D: Perceptual interference supports a non-modular account of face processing. Nature Neuroscience. 2003, 6: 428-432. 10.1038/nn1029.CrossRefPubMed Gauthier I, Curran T, Curby KM, Collins D: Perceptual interference supports a non-modular account of face processing. Nature Neuroscience. 2003, 6: 428-432. 10.1038/nn1029.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Rossion B, Gauthier I, Goffaux V, Tarr MJ, Crommelinck M: Expertise training with novel objects leads to left-lateralized facelike electrophysiological responses. Psychological Science. 2002, 13: 250-257. 10.1111/1467-9280.00446.CrossRefPubMed Rossion B, Gauthier I, Goffaux V, Tarr MJ, Crommelinck M: Expertise training with novel objects leads to left-lateralized facelike electrophysiological responses. Psychological Science. 2002, 13: 250-257. 10.1111/1467-9280.00446.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Tanaka JW, Curran T: A neural basis for expert object recognition. Psychological Science. 2001, 12: 43-47. 10.1111/1467-9280.00308.CrossRefPubMed Tanaka JW, Curran T: A neural basis for expert object recognition. Psychological Science. 2001, 12: 43-47. 10.1111/1467-9280.00308.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Lang PJ: Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral assessment: Computer applications. Technology in Mental Health Care Delivery Systems. Edited by: Sidowski JB, Johnson JH and Williams TA. 1980, Norwood, NJ, Ablex Publishing, 119-137. Lang PJ: Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral assessment: Computer applications. Technology in Mental Health Care Delivery Systems. Edited by: Sidowski JB, Johnson JH and Williams TA. 1980, Norwood, NJ, Ablex Publishing, 119-137.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Oldfield RC: The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971, 9: 97-113. 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4.CrossRefPubMed Oldfield RC: The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971, 9: 97-113. 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Öst LG: One-session treatment for specific phobias. Behaviour Research & Therapy. 1989, 27: 1-7. 10.1016/0005-7967(89)90113-7.CrossRef Öst LG: One-session treatment for specific phobias. Behaviour Research & Therapy. 1989, 27: 1-7. 10.1016/0005-7967(89)90113-7.CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Hinsch R, Pfingsten U: Gruppentraining sozialer Kompetenzen (GSK) [Group training of social skills]. 2002, Weinheim, Beltz, 4th Hinsch R, Pfingsten U: Gruppentraining sozialer Kompetenzen (GSK) [Group training of social skills]. 2002, Weinheim, Beltz, 4th
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Wittchen HU, Wunderlich U, Gruschwitz S, Zaudig M: Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV [Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV]. 1997, Göttingen, Hogrefe Wittchen HU, Wunderlich U, Gruschwitz S, Zaudig M: Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV [Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV]. 1997, Göttingen, Hogrefe
48.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 1994, Washington, DC, Author, 4th American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 1994, Washington, DC, Author, 4th
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Klorman R, Weerts TC, Hastings JE, Melamed BG, Lang PJ: Psychometric description of some specific fear questionnaires. Behavior Therapy. 1974, 5: 401-409. 10.1016/S0005-7894(74)80008-0.CrossRef Klorman R, Weerts TC, Hastings JE, Melamed BG, Lang PJ: Psychometric description of some specific fear questionnaires. Behavior Therapy. 1974, 5: 401-409. 10.1016/S0005-7894(74)80008-0.CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Fydrich T: Soziale Phobie und Angst Inventar. Diagnostische Verfahren in der Psychotherapie [Diagnostic methods in psychotherapy]. Edited by: Brähler E, Schumacher J and Strauß B. 2002, Göttingen, Hogrefe, 335-338. Fydrich T: Soziale Phobie und Angst Inventar. Diagnostische Verfahren in der Psychotherapie [Diagnostic methods in psychotherapy]. Edited by: Brähler E, Schumacher J and Strauß B. 2002, Göttingen, Hogrefe, 335-338.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Hautzinger M, Bailer M, Worall H, Keller F: Beck-Depressions-Inventar (BDI): Testhandbuch. 1995, Bern, Hans Huber, 2nd Hautzinger M, Bailer M, Worall H, Keller F: Beck-Depressions-Inventar (BDI): Testhandbuch. 1995, Bern, Hans Huber, 2nd
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Laux L, Glanzmann P, Schaffner P, Spielberger CD: Das State-Trait-Angstinventar (STAI-G). 1981, Weinheim, Beltz Laux L, Glanzmann P, Schaffner P, Spielberger CD: Das State-Trait-Angstinventar (STAI-G). 1981, Weinheim, Beltz
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE: The State Trait Anxiety Inventory. 1970, Palo Alto, CA, Consulting Psychologists Press Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE: The State Trait Anxiety Inventory. 1970, Palo Alto, CA, Consulting Psychologists Press
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Merikangas KR, Angst J, Eaton W, Canino G, Rubio-Stipec M, Wacker H, Wittchen HU, Andrade L, Essau C, Whitaker A, Kraemer H, Robins L, Kupfer DJ: Comorbidity and boundaries of affective disorders with anxiety disorders and substance abuse: results of an international task force. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 1996, (30): 58-67. Merikangas KR, Angst J, Eaton W, Canino G, Rubio-Stipec M, Wacker H, Wittchen HU, Andrade L, Essau C, Whitaker A, Kraemer H, Robins L, Kupfer DJ: Comorbidity and boundaries of affective disorders with anxiety disorders and substance abuse: results of an international task force. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 1996, (30): 58-67.
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Schneier FR, Johnson J, Hornig CD, Liebowitz MR, Weissman MM: Social phobia: Comorbidity and morbidity in an epidemiological sample. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1992, 45: 282-288.CrossRef Schneier FR, Johnson J, Hornig CD, Liebowitz MR, Weissman MM: Social phobia: Comorbidity and morbidity in an epidemiological sample. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1992, 45: 282-288.CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Stein MB, Fuetsch M, Müller N, Höfler M, Lieb R, Wittchen HU: Social anxiety disorder and the risk of depression: A prospective community study of adolescents and young adults. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001, 58: 251-256. 10.1001/archpsyc.58.3.251.CrossRefPubMed Stein MB, Fuetsch M, Müller N, Höfler M, Lieb R, Wittchen HU: Social anxiety disorder and the risk of depression: A prospective community study of adolescents and young adults. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001, 58: 251-256. 10.1001/archpsyc.58.3.251.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Vuilleumier P, Schwartz S: Beware and be aware: capture of spatial attention by fear-related stimuli in neglect. Neuroreport. 2001, 12: 1119-1122. 10.1097/00001756-200105080-00014.CrossRefPubMed Vuilleumier P, Schwartz S: Beware and be aware: capture of spatial attention by fear-related stimuli in neglect. Neuroreport. 2001, 12: 1119-1122. 10.1097/00001756-200105080-00014.CrossRefPubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Chatrian GE, Lettici E, Nelson PL: Modified nomenclature for the 10% electrode system. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology. 1998, 5: 183-186.CrossRef Chatrian GE, Lettici E, Nelson PL: Modified nomenclature for the 10% electrode system. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology. 1998, 5: 183-186.CrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Gratton G, Coles MGH, Donchin E: A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology. 1983, 55: 468-484. 10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9.CrossRef Gratton G, Coles MGH, Donchin E: A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology. 1983, 55: 468-484. 10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9.CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Laird NM, Ware JH: Random-Effects Models for Longitudinal Data. Biometrics. 1982, 38: 963-974. 10.2307/2529876.CrossRefPubMed Laird NM, Ware JH: Random-Effects Models for Longitudinal Data. Biometrics. 1982, 38: 963-974. 10.2307/2529876.CrossRefPubMed
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Winer BJ, Brown DR, Michels KM: Statistical principles in experimental design. 1991, New York, McGraw-Hill, 3rd Winer BJ, Brown DR, Michels KM: Statistical principles in experimental design. 1991, New York, McGraw-Hill, 3rd
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Holm S: A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics. 1979, 6: 65-70. Holm S: A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics. 1979, 6: 65-70.
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Beck AT, Emery G, Greenberg RL: Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspective. 1985, New York, Basic Books Beck AT, Emery G, Greenberg RL: Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspective. 1985, New York, Basic Books
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Eysenck MW: Cognitive factors in clinical psychology: Potential relevance to therapy. New concepts in anxiety. Edited by: Briley M and File SE. 1991, London, Macmillan Press Eysenck MW: Cognitive factors in clinical psychology: Potential relevance to therapy. New concepts in anxiety. Edited by: Briley M and File SE. 1991, London, Macmillan Press
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Eysenck MW: Anxiety: The cognitive perspective. 1992, Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Eysenck MW: Anxiety: The cognitive perspective. 1992, Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Eysenck MW: Anxiety and cognition: A unified theory. 1997, East Sussex, UK, Psychology Press Eysenck MW: Anxiety and cognition: A unified theory. 1997, East Sussex, UK, Psychology Press
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown TA, Campbell LA, Lehman CL, Grisham JR, Mancil RB: Current and lifetime comorbidity of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders in a large clinical sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001, 110: 585-599. 10.1037/0021-843X.110.4.585.CrossRefPubMed Brown TA, Campbell LA, Lehman CL, Grisham JR, Mancil RB: Current and lifetime comorbidity of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders in a large clinical sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001, 110: 585-599. 10.1037/0021-843X.110.4.585.CrossRefPubMed
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Cuthbert BN, Lang PJ, Strauss C, Drobes D, Patrick CJ, Bradley MM: The psychophysiology of anxiety disorder: fear memory imagery. Psychophysiology. 2003, 40: 407-422. 10.1111/1469-8986.00043.CrossRefPubMed Cuthbert BN, Lang PJ, Strauss C, Drobes D, Patrick CJ, Bradley MM: The psychophysiology of anxiety disorder: fear memory imagery. Psychophysiology. 2003, 40: 407-422. 10.1111/1469-8986.00043.CrossRefPubMed
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Lang PJ, McTeague LM, Cuthbert BN: Fear, Anxiety, Depression and the Anxiety Disorder Spectrum: A psychophysiological analysis. Psychological Clinical Science: Recent Advances in Theory and Practice Integrative Perspectives in Honor of Richard M McFall. Edited by: Baker T, Bootzin R and Treat T. 2006, , Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Lang PJ, McTeague LM, Cuthbert BN: Fear, Anxiety, Depression and the Anxiety Disorder Spectrum: A psychophysiological analysis. Psychological Clinical Science: Recent Advances in Theory and Practice Integrative Perspectives in Honor of Richard M McFall. Edited by: Baker T, Bootzin R and Treat T. 2006, , Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Cook EWIII, Melamed BG, Cuthbert BN, McNeil DW, Lang PJ: Emotional imagery and the differential diagnosis of anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1988, 56: 734-740. 10.1037/0022-006X.56.5.734.CrossRefPubMed Cook EWIII, Melamed BG, Cuthbert BN, McNeil DW, Lang PJ: Emotional imagery and the differential diagnosis of anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1988, 56: 734-740. 10.1037/0022-006X.56.5.734.CrossRefPubMed
71.
Zurück zum Zitat MacLeod CM: Half a century research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin. 1991, 109: 163-203. 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163.CrossRefPubMed MacLeod CM: Half a century research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin. 1991, 109: 163-203. 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163.CrossRefPubMed
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Redding GM, Gerjets DA: Stroop effect: Interference and facilitation with verbal and manual responses. Perceptual & Motor Skills. 1977, 1: 11-17.CrossRef Redding GM, Gerjets DA: Stroop effect: Interference and facilitation with verbal and manual responses. Perceptual & Motor Skills. 1977, 1: 11-17.CrossRef
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Ballesteros S, Reales JM, Manga D: Effects of type of design (blocked vs. randomized) on Stroop and emotional Stroop tasks. Psicothema. 2000, 12: 60-63. Ballesteros S, Reales JM, Manga D: Effects of type of design (blocked vs. randomized) on Stroop and emotional Stroop tasks. Psicothema. 2000, 12: 60-63.
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Holle C, Neely JH, Heimberg RG: The effects of blocked versus random presentation and semantic relatedness of stimulus words on response to a modified Stroop task among social phobics. Cognitive Therapy & Research. 1997, 21: 681-697. 10.1023/A:1021860324879.CrossRef Holle C, Neely JH, Heimberg RG: The effects of blocked versus random presentation and semantic relatedness of stimulus words on response to a modified Stroop task among social phobics. Cognitive Therapy & Research. 1997, 21: 681-697. 10.1023/A:1021860324879.CrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Kindt M, Bierman D, Brosschot JF: Stroop versus Stroop: Comparison of a card format and a single-trial format of the standard color-word Stroop task and the emotional Stroop task. Personality & Individual Differences. 1996, 21: 415-422. 10.1016/0191-8869(96)00133-X.CrossRef Kindt M, Bierman D, Brosschot JF: Stroop versus Stroop: Comparison of a card format and a single-trial format of the standard color-word Stroop task and the emotional Stroop task. Personality & Individual Differences. 1996, 21: 415-422. 10.1016/0191-8869(96)00133-X.CrossRef
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Richards A, French CC, Johnson W, Naparstek J, Williams J: Effects of emotion manipulation and anxiety on performance of an emotional Stroop task. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1992, 83: 479-491.CrossRef Richards A, French CC, Johnson W, Naparstek J, Williams J: Effects of emotion manipulation and anxiety on performance of an emotional Stroop task. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1992, 83: 479-491.CrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Waters AJ, Feyerabend C: Determinants and effects of attentional bias in smokers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2000, 14: 111-120. 10.1037/0893-164X.14.2.111.CrossRefPubMed Waters AJ, Feyerabend C: Determinants and effects of attentional bias in smokers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2000, 14: 111-120. 10.1037/0893-164X.14.2.111.CrossRefPubMed
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Kindt M, Brosschot JF: Cognitive bias in spider-phobic children: Comparison of a pictorial and a linguistic spider Stroop. Journal of Psychopathology & Behavioral Assessment. 1999, 21: 207-220. 10.1023/A:1022873331067.CrossRef Kindt M, Brosschot JF: Cognitive bias in spider-phobic children: Comparison of a pictorial and a linguistic spider Stroop. Journal of Psychopathology & Behavioral Assessment. 1999, 21: 207-220. 10.1023/A:1022873331067.CrossRef
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Bradley MM, Lang PJ: Measuring emotion: the Self-Assessment Manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry. 1994, 25: 49-59. 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9.CrossRef Bradley MM, Lang PJ: Measuring emotion: the Self-Assessment Manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry. 1994, 25: 49-59. 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9.CrossRef
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Turner SM, Beidel DC, Dancu CV, Stanley MA: An empirically derived inventory to measure social fears and anxiety: The Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory. Psychological Assessment. 1989, 1: 35-40. 10.1037/1040-3590.1.1.35.CrossRef Turner SM, Beidel DC, Dancu CV, Stanley MA: An empirically derived inventory to measure social fears and anxiety: The Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory. Psychological Assessment. 1989, 1: 35-40. 10.1037/1040-3590.1.1.35.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Event-related potentials when identifying or color-naming threatening schematic stimuli in spider phobic and non-phobic individuals
verfasst von
Iris-Tatjana Kolassa
Frauke Musial
Stephan Kolassa
Wolfgang HR Miltner
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2006
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2006
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-6-38

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2006

BMC Psychiatry 1/2006 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).