Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Supportive Care in Cancer 3/2023

Open Access 01.03.2023 | Research

Humour interventions for patients in palliative care—a randomized controlled trial

verfasst von: Lisa Linge-Dahl, Rainer Kreuz, Mieke Stoffelen, Sonja Heintz, Willibald Ruch, Eckart von Hirschhausen, Lukas Radbruch

Erschienen in: Supportive Care in Cancer | Ausgabe 3/2023

Abstract

Purpose

The effect of humour on end-of-life patients could be beneficial and is worth investigating. However, data on humour interventions for patients in palliative care are scarce. This study evaluated the effects of a humour intervention in a palliative care setting.

Methods

A two-step intervention was developed based on the humour habits programme by McGhee. Patients were assisted to remember funny episodes from their past and recognize humorous aspects of the present and encouraged to produce humour. The intervention and control group completed questionnaires on life satisfaction, cheerfulness, symptom burden, and perceived stress and if possible gave saliva samples to investigate oxytocin levels. The study was a randomized controlled monocentre study on patients treated in a palliative care ward. Participants had to be conscious and alert enough to complete data collection. Overall, 55 patients were included and randomized to the intervention or control group.

Results

Parameters in the control group did not change significantly. In the intervention group, seriousness, bad mood, and stress were reduced. Cheerfulness increased significantly after the intervention. However, the methodologically complex intervention setting was too exhausting for the majority of patients.

Conclusion

Patients who were able to participate benefited from the effects of the intervention on multiple levels. For future research simple interventions, biomarkers for well-being and assessments by staff or proxies are needed to include patients with reduced cognitive and physical performance status at the end of their lives.

Trial registration

DRKS00028978 German Registry of Clinical Studies.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s00520-023-07606-9.

Key statements

• Humour in palliative care has rarely been investigated systematically.
• It is known to be commonly used and researched especially amongst staff.
• This study shows that humour interventions are well applicable for patients who receive palliative care.
• High attrition rates underline that lengthy questionnaires and data collection are unsuitable for this setting.
• Nonetheless, distress, seriousness, bad mood, and cheerfulness showed significant changes in the intervention group.
• Universally applicable qualitative evaluation tools should be developed to enable a higher level of complete datasets.

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Background

Humour has been investigated in various contexts in the past, but a range of diverging definitions has been used in these studies. Ruch [1] defined the perception that something is funny as prerequisite for the occurrence of humour. Martin and Ford [2] defined humour as a broad, multifaceted term that represents anything that people say or do and that others perceive as funny and tends to make them laugh but also included thoughts and the emotional response such as enjoyment and mirth to humorous stimuli. They stated that humour essentially is a way for people to interact in a playful manner. In the expression of humour, eight comic styles have been defined [3], including lighter (fun, humour, nonsense, and wit) and darker styles (irony, satire, sarcasm, and cynicism). The darker styles were associated with a potentially negative-critical effect.
Humour interventions in patients with palliative diagnosis have rarely been implemented or systematically evaluated. Recent systematic reviews have summarized the available evidence from Pinna et al. [4] and Linge-Dahl et al. [5] showing that humour serves different purposes, such as forming relationships (e.g. between patient and health care professional) or dealing with circumstances, and have mostly been researched in health care professionals. The few studies evaluating the effect of humour from the patients’ perspective reported only unstructured qualitative data. Pinna et al. [4] also distinguished between humour before and after the diagnosis of terminal illness and emphasized that there were also situations in which humour should not be used, such as coma or when people are on the verge of death. Patients suffering from certain pneumonic illnesses such as COPD might risk hyperinflation during intensive laughter [6], so these patients should not be included in humour studies. Kontos et al. [7] studied the effects of humour interventions in dementia care homes in Australia and demonstrated reduced agitation and aggression in residents. Adamle and Ludwick [8] illustrated that humour during the interaction with the patient was also frequently observed in hospices and was mainly initiated by the patients themselves. Based on the current state of literature [4, 5, 7], no commonly used styles of humour in palliative care can be defined. We developed and pilot tested an adapted version of the five-step humour training for psychiatric patients based on McGhee [9], as this programme is supported by research and clinical applications. In addition, it comes with a well-tested manual that has been applied in various areas. Based on previous studies, we chose an outcome measure premised on the state-trait model of cheerfulness [10]. The experiences and promising results of humour studies in paediatrics [11, 12] led to the inclusion of a biomarker parameter in this study.

Objectives

We hoped to improve the foundation of knowledge of suitable evaluation instruments for interventions in a palliative care setting. We investigated the effect of a humour intervention on life satisfaction, cheerfulness, seriousness, bad mood, symptom burden, level of stress, and oxytocin in saliva. We hypothesized that the intervention would reduce levels of stress and symptom burden and improve mood and cheerfulness in comparison to a control group without the intervention.

Methods

Sample/study design

We implemented a parallel study design with two groups with equal randomization. A pilot test used a more elaborate study setting with extensive questionnaires and quantitative sensory pain threshold testing [13]. As the recruitment rate was extremely poor and due to ethical concerns, the setting had to be adapted and simplified for the main study. The pilot test and the methodological development will be reported in detail elsewhere.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

All participants were being treated in the palliative care ward of the University Hospital Bonn in Germany. Patients had to be conscious and alert and understand the German language well enough to complete the questionnaires. They had to provide informed consent to participate in the study.
Patients were excluded if they were unconscious or severely fatigued. Potential test persons with multi-resistant infections could not provide saliva samples due to laboratory restrictions.
All included patients were randomized to intervention or control group using a simple randomization list (using the random number generation function in Excel). The study was not blinded nor allocation concealed as the ethics committee had requested to include information on the specific burden related to participation in the study for each group. One of the authors (LLD), who is a researcher in the Department of Palliative Medicine, but not involved in clinical care, enrolled and assigned the patients to one of the groups. The power calculation resulted in overall 240 patients to achieve a medium effect of d = 0.50 in the State-Trait-Cheerfulness Questionnaire (power = 0.70, Cohen’s d), with 120 in the intervention and 120 in the control group. Patients in the control and intervention group were tested on different days to avoid any inferences between the groups. The primary outcome was the mood of patients (State-Trait-Cheerfulness-Inventory). Secondary outcomes were burden of symptoms, distress, life satisfaction, and oxytocin level in saliva.

Instruments

The set of questionnaires included the State-Trait-Cheerfulness-Inventory (STCI-T and -S) [10, 14], the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) [15], the stress thermometer [16], the Minimal Documentation System for patients in palliative care (MIDOS) [17, 18], and a few psychometric variables (age, gender, illness). The ECOG performance status was derived from the patient files [ECOG, 19]. The German self-rating version of all questionnaires was used.
Cheerfulness was measured using the STCI-T and -S [10, 14, 20, 21]. The STCI-T and -S, which are rated on 4-point Likert scale (from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”), consist of the three scales cheerfulness, seriousness, and bad mood, which are built from sum scores of 10 (STCI-T) and 6 (STCI-S) items, respectively. Mean values of 25.75 (SD 6.87) for cheerfulness, 24.28 (SD 6.03) for seriousness, and 15.20 (SD 6.31) for bad mood have been described in healthy subjects for the STCI-T [20]. The STCI-S evaluates the mood in the current situation, while the STCI-T investigates enduring personality traits [10, 14, 20, 21].
Symptom burden and well-being were assessed with the MIDOS [17, 18]. MIDOS is a short instrument with 8 items on physical and psychological symptoms and one item on general well-being, using categorical scales.
Life satisfaction was measured with the SWLS [15] with the sum score of 5 items, each rated on a 7-point Likert scale.
The distress thermometer consists of a scale from 0 to 10 where participants can mark their level of stress in the current situation [16].
Saliva samples were collected by a study nurse by having the patient chew on a cotton wool roll (Salivette® Sarstedt) for at least 60 s. Then the concentration of oxytocin in saliva [11] was analysed.
The researcher assisted the patients in completing the questionnaires. Depending on their performance level, she read the questions aloud or supervised independent completion.

Intervention

The “humorous visit” was offered to the patients of the intervention group. Two professional hospital clowns who were dressed in the bright style of Mr. Bean (as clown outfits and the red nose seemed unsuitable for the setting) visited the patients one or two times. The training was performed by Laura Fernandez. The primary goal was to find the connection between the clown characters “Robert “ and”Lilly”, their joy, their humour, their differences, and their abilities. This was followed by exercises to “be in the moment”, “to get in touch”, and to find a playful or calm way from there on. Improvisation was one tool for training and to establish a trustful understanding between the clown actors. As both clown actors play instruments, making music together became not only an important part for the interventions, but also a nice warm-up for the clown actors to re-focus on their goals every week before the intervention. The intervention was based on McGhee’s humour habits programme, which was adapted by Falkenberg et al. [9] to a five-session training—memory of a funny episode during childhood (finding one’s preferred humour style), providing humour according to that style to the participant, finding humorous aspects in the current situation, producing humour, and applying humour in everyday life. The content of these 5 group sessions was condensed to two tailored humorous visits per patient. The coaches used various props (colourful cloths, a hand puppet, heating jacket tubes, musical instruments), but mostly they communicated and used their own and the patient’s imagination to build humorous interactions with the materials in the room (cushions, curtains, whiteboard, a wheelchair, etc.). Both coaches were educated as hospital clowns and play at least one instrument; one studied at a circus school and is a trained actress; the other studied at a clown school and is a certified social worker.
Entering the patient’s room, the humour coaches explored the mood of the patient and tried to find an adequate vibe to communicate. They asked a couple of questions concerning the biography and important life events to find out the patient’s preferred humour style. They then tried to find humorous aspects of the current situation using equipment in the room or making up a funny song about something the patient had mentioned. If the patients were still at the palliative care ward in the following week, they prepared a second visit focusing on aspects that were dear to the patient. As planning into the future is limited for patients in palliative care, they sometimes acted out unfulfilled wishes (such as a concert with songs of a specific singer or a cruise) in a caring and humorous manner.
The control group filled out the questionnaires twice and then provided saliva samples as well 1 day before the intervention group.

Procedure

Data collection was performed according to the scheme displayed in Table 1. The control group was evaluated with the same routine except the interventions. The researcher documented field notes during the interventions, which were supplemented by questionnaires that the humour coaches completed after the humorous visits. The field note logs included start and end times and time stamps.
Table 1
Procedure of data collection intervention group
 
Procedure (time)
Assessment instruments
Day 0
Briefing (10)
Informed written consent
Day 1
Assessment of psychological parameters (questionnaires) (15–25)
State-Trait-Cheerfulness (STCI-S and T), life satisfaction (SWLS), stress
 
Assessment of medical parameters (5–10)
Burden of symptoms (MIDOS), oxytocin level, ECOG
 
1st humour intervention (20–30)
Standardized non-participant observation (notes, “questionnaire” humour coaches, start and ending time)
 
Assessment of the impact of the intervention (5–10)
STCI-S − oxytocin level
Day 3
Assessment of psychological parameters (questionnaires) (15–25)
STCI-S and T, SWLS, stress − MIDOS, oxytocin level
 
2nd humour intervention (20–30)
Same as 1st humour intervention
 
Assessment of the impact of the intervention (5–10)
STCI-S, SWLS, stress + MIDOS, oxytocin level

Analyses

We implemented SPSS statistics for quantitative and MAXQDA for qualitative analyses. For pre- and post-workshop comparisons, t-tests were performed on mean values of the grouped data of all participants of the intervention and control group that completed the questionnaires. We included patients who had not more than two missing values in the main outcome variables STCI-S and SWLS in the evaluation. An ANOVA was used to compare pre-post values for both groups. We also compared means of the STCI-T, ECOG, and SWLS sum scores as well the MIDOS results of both groups to analyse potential differences in personality to agree to take part in a humour intervention.
An inductive-deductive approach was used to analyse the qualitative data. The inductively defined codes were condensed with additional codes until saturation was reached. The details of these analyses will be reported elsewhere.

Results

Sample

Overall 984 patients were scanned for eligibility from October 2017 to April 2019, and 140 patients were recruited for the study. However, only 55 patients completed the questionnaires and were included in the evaluation (27 were in the control group and 28 in the intervention group; see Fig. 1). Gender was well distributed with 27 women and 28 men (intervention group 16 ♀/12 ♂, control group 11 ♀/16 ♂). Age ranged from 29 to 99 years with a median of 64.48 (SD 14.09). All but 7 patients had an oncological diagnosis. ECOG performance status at admission was 2.91 (SD 0.95; min 0–max 4). In addition to the 55 patients included in the evaluation, another 68 patients received the humour intervention even though they were not able to complete the questionnaires. No patient reported adverse events or additional emotional burden from the humour intervention or data collection.

Missing values

Only five of the 28 patients who received a second intervention were able to fill out the complete questionnaires again before and after the intervention to make an evaluation of quantitative data possible. Oxytocin in saliva could only be derived from 9 patients of the intervention and 9 of the control group, and thus, oxytocin data were not included in the analysis.

Group comparisons

There were no significant differences in the pre-test scores for life satisfaction (t (48) = 0.70, p > 0.001) between intervention (M = 20.24, SD = 7.94) and control group (M = 18.72, SD = 7.24). Bad mood was slightly but not significantly higher in the control group (t (46) =  − 0.57) with mean values M = 22.50 (SD = 9.39) in the control and M = 21.13 (SD = 7.01) in the intervention group. The magnitude of the differences in the means (mean difference = 0.63, 95% CI: − 3.51 to 4.75) was very small for life satisfaction and also small for the nine other parameters that were investigated (see Table 2). The statistical parameters show that the intervention and control group were highly similar before the intervention in all investigated features. ANOVA analysis showed no significant differences in between groups (see Table 3).
Table 2
Pre-test group differences
  
Test for equality of means
 
M
SD
T
df
p
Life satisfaction
Intervention
20.24
7.94
0.70
48
0.483
Control
18.72
7.24
Stress
Intervention
5.11
2.95
 − 0.86
38
0.397
Control
5.84
2.44
Symptom burden
Intervention
19.41
6.28
 − 0.32
30
0.751
Control
20.07
5.12
ECOG
Intervention
2.95
1.12
0.42
52
0.674
Control
2.84
0.77
Cheerfulness trait
Intervention
32.22
6.77
0.30
43
0.761
Control
31.59
6.96
Seriousness trait
Intervention
30.38
6.08
 − 0.29
45
0.769
Control
30.91
6.39
Bad mood trait
Intervention
21.13
7.01
 − 0.57
46
0.568
Control
22.50
9.39
Cheerfulness state
Intervention
11.53
4.81
0.04
53
0.969
Control
11.48
4.62
Seriousness state
Intervention
16.96
4.11
0.13
53
0.895
Control
16.81
4.23
Bad mood state
Intervention
13.24
5.56
1.45
53
0.152
Control
11.22
4.67
Table 3
Univariate analysis for variances between intervention and control
 
Squares
df
Mean Squares
F
p
Life satisfaction
Between groups
28.87
1
28.87
0.50
0.483
In groups
2769.60
48
57.70
Stress
Between groups
5.27
1
5.26
0.74
0.397
In groups
272.46
38
7.16
Symptom burden
Between groups
3.42
1
3.47
0.10
0.751
In groups
1001.04
30
33.37
ECOG
Between groups
0.16
1
0.16
0.18
0.674
In groups
48.36
52
0.92
Cheerfulness state
Between groups
0.03
1
0.03
0.00
0.969
In groups
1180.55
53
22.27
Seriousness state
Between groups
0.30
1
0.30
0.02
0.895
In groups
927.04
53
17.48
Bad mood state
Between groups
55.90
1
55.90
2.12
0.152
In groups
1400.58
53
26.42
In the control group, none of the investigated parameters changed significantly between pre- and post-measurement (see Table 4). For example, the score of state seriousness showed no significant change in between the pre- (M = 16.90, SD = 4.48) and post-measurements (M = 16.76, SD = 4.54), t(20) = 0.37. The mean change in the test scores was 0.13 with a 95% confidence interval ranging from − 0.63 to 0.91.
Table 4
Mean values pre- and post-measures in the control group
  
M
SD
t
df
p
Life satisfaction
Before
19.78
6.92
0.00
17
1.000
After
19.78
6.87
Stress
Before
5.56
2.55
 − 0.20
16
0.835
After
5.59
2.51
Symptom burden
Before
20.67
5.98
0.96
8
0.366
After
20.11
6.40
Cheerfulness
Before
11.14
4.33
0.76
20
0.452
After
11.00
4.30
Seriousness
Before
16.90
4.48
0.37
20
0.706
After
16.76
4.54
Bad mood
Before
11.00
4.62
 − 1.27
20
0.219
After
11.57
5.06
As expected, the t-test for paired samples for the pre- and post-measures in the intervention group found four significant effects (see Table 5). The scores of distress, cheerfulness, seriousness, and bad mood changed significantly between pre- and post-measurements (Table 5).
Table 5
Mean values pre- and post-measures in the intervention group
  
M
SD
t
df
p
Life satisfaction
Before
19.94
7.35
1.63
15
0.123
After
18.56
7.09
Stress
Before
5.55
2.90
2.40
10
0.037*
After
3.41
2.63
Symptom burden
Before
19.80
7.35
1.72
9
0.120
After
18.30
5.89
Cheerfulness
Before
11.49
4.23
 − 4.06
19
0.001**
After
15.80
5.40
Seriousness
Before
16.45
4.60
2.90
19
0.009*
After
13.10
4.02
Bad mood
Before
13.35
5.51
3.11
19
0.006*
After
9.95
4.43
**p ≤ 0.001; *p ≤ 0.05.

Qualitative data

Field notes were documented for all patients in the intervention group by the researcher. The field notes were coded and afterwards categorized into condition, contact, situation and life, expression of emotion, positive aspects, and symptoms. In the category condition, the code “deep breath” was coded most frequently. Frequent topics for contact were “participation”, “reception”, and “thank you/expression of gratitude”. “Reported memories” were predominant in situation and life. Expressions of emotion were very versatile, but signs of emotion were frequently coded. The category positive aspects included the highest number of codes including “smile”, “laugh”, “I like/that was great”, “joke”, and “applause”. Symptom codes were related to fatigue and exhaustion. During the coding of the data, new codes were added during the first half of the protocols, after which saturation occurred and the existing codes were sufficient for the analysis of the protocols. Exemplary quotes can be accessed in a table in the supplementary files.

Discussions and conclusions

Limitations

There was major attrition in this study, leading to many incomplete datasets and only very few patients that were treated according to protocol, even though we had shortened and simplified assessment instruments and intervention following a pilot testing. Most randomized patients did not consent to participate in the study due to feeling fatigued or being sedated, and most of those who participated were not available for a second humour intervention as they had been discharged or transferred to another place of care. Therefore an intention-to-treat analysis was not feasible. With almost zero questionnaire data available from these patients, imputation was not possible, and an elevation of N in the existing analyses would have distorted the standard error and painted an overly positive picture of the effect. We were not able to meet the power analyses calculated for the study plan with this inadequate sample size. Considering the high degree of attrition, we decided to stop the study after 18 months of recruitment. In consequence, we could only evaluate the results of the first humour intervention in this article in a smaller as planned sample. Ultimately, only 14% of the patients treated in the palliative care unit in the 18-month recruitment period were found to be eligible for the study, and only 39% of those patients (6% of the total patient number) participated and were included in the analysis. Oxytocin in saliva could only be sampled from 9 patients of each group and thus was not included in the analysis.
The control group did not receive an alternative intervention, due to feasibility reasons. This inactive control group setting creates the risk of performance bias. Lack of an active control also prevented adequate blinding and allocation concealment. These limitations may limit the transferability of the results. However, inclusion rates did not differ significantly between the intervention and control group, indicating a low risk of allocation bias. Methodologically, it would have been useful to set a cut-off value for cheerfulness to rule out bias by higher levels of cheerfulness in the persons who agreed to participate in the study. However, since levels of state cheerfulness did not show significant differences between intervention and control group, it can at least be assured that there was no bias due to allocation. The effect of cheerfulness on the willingness to participate might not be specific to humour interventions though, as a higher level of depression has been shown to limit the willingness to participate in any kind of study [22]. Thus, this limitation would not produce a specific bias of this study but rather seems to be a general phenomenon due to the elaborated precaution of persons with symptoms of depression.
The comparison of control and intervention group at the start of data collection showed that there was no significant difference between groups. Even though allocation was not concealed, these data seem to ensure comparability of both groups.
Ethical aspects of collecting complex data with severely ill patients at the end of their lives need to be discussed [23]. The SWLS questions for example were found to be distressing by more than half of the patients surveyed. Asking them how happy they are with their lives at the moment after receiving a palliative diagnosis seemed inappropriate.
In the original study plan, we had included a semi-structured interview for day 5 of the data collection. This was only possible in very few cases due to patient discharge, fatigue, and exhaustion of patients after the data collection and interventions.

Discussion

Although we had already simplified the intervention after the pilot test and reduced the number of intervention appointments, still only a very small proportion of palliative care patients were eligible, and even fewer were able to provide sufficient data from the first intervention. This phenomenon of high attrition rates has been reported in palliative care previously [21, 24]. Attrition has even been identified as a major problem in palliative care research [24, 25]. We aimed to reduce attrition with a combination of patient-reported outcome questionnaires with physiological parameters [11, 26]. Using the level of oxytocin in saliva as biomarker has been evaluated critically in the past [27] because of its strong concentration fluctuations and potentially not measurable amount in saliva. The radioimmunoassay (RIA) method [28], which has been used in this study, can measure even very small amounts of oxytocin. However, even though saliva samples do not place a huge burden on patients, these assessments were possible only in a small minority of the study patients. Many patients suffered from xerostomia or were not able to chew on the swab for 60 s due to nausea. Half of the samples did not contain enough liquid for analysis.
Even though the analysis of variance did not show a significant effect of the intervention in the pre- and post-comparison, the comparison of pre- and post-data between the intervention and control group presented some promising results despite the small sample size. Whereas there were no significant changes in the control group, the perceived level of stress, seriousness, and bad mood were reduced in the intervention group, and cheerfulness increased. The positive effects of the humour intervention were supported in the qualitative analysis of the field notes. However, Bland and Altman [29] warned to use baseline as parameter for comparison. Recruitment of a bigger sample would be desirable in order to evaluate analysis of variance in the pre- and post-data between groups.
In the intervention group, life satisfaction was slightly lower after the intervention, in contrast to the positive findings for level of stress, seriousness, bad mood, and cheerfulness. A possible explanation could be that completing the questionnaires might have had a negative effect on patients’ life satisfaction. This effect has been reported in literature in the past [30], and there has been even a questionnaire created to measure negative effects [31]. However, neither the intervention group nor the control group showed this effect. More research is needed to evaluate this discrepancy.
Our study confirmed that short and simple assessment instruments are a mandatory precondition for palliative care research. However, evaluation of the effectiveness of humour interventions in patients with far-advanced disease might require proxy-reported instead of patient-reported outcome measures and observational assessment instruments as well as suitable biomarkers. Again, problems with sampling have to be considered, such as the high frequency of dry mouth or swallowing problems which can interfere with saliva sampling.

Conclusion

Major problems with attrition led to a smaller as planned sample size in our intervention study. However, we found some promising results for a positive effect of the humour interventions for patients in palliative care. Further research could be planned for the outpatient and home care setting, recruiting patients less advanced in the disease trajectory and thus with less physical or cognitive impairment compared to those on a palliative care unit. However, standardized training of clowns for this kind of humour intervention would be a necessary prerequisite for such a roll-out.

Acknowledgements

The study has been registered at the German Registry of Clinical Studies under the number DRKS00028978. We thank Martin Mücke and Sarit Quirin for proofreading the manuscript and giving valuable advice, as well as the medical staff of the palliative care unit of the University Hospital Bonn for cooperating and supporting our study at all times. Most importantly we thank the patients who agreed to take part in this study.

Declarations

The Ethics Committee of the University Hospital Bonn examined and approved the questionnaires and methodology of the study under the number 003/16 on the10th of February 2016. It works in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki Declarations. All participants had to provide informed written consent to be able to participate in the study.
All participants also agreed to the publication of data in anonymized form.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Key statements

• Humour in palliative care has rarely been investigated systematically.
• It is known to be commonly used and researched especially amongst staff.
• This study shows that humour interventions are well applicable for patients who receive palliative care.
• High attrition rates underline that lengthy questionnaires and data collection are unsuitable for this setting.
• Nonetheless, distress, seriousness, bad mood, and cheerfulness showed significant changes in the intervention group.
• Universally applicable qualitative evaluation tools should be developed to enable a higher level of complete datasets.

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Anhänge

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruch W (2008) Psychology of humor. In: Raskin V (ed) The primer of humor research. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin, pp 17–100CrossRef Ruch W (2008) Psychology of humor. In: Raskin V (ed) The primer of humor research. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin, pp 17–100CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin RA, Ford TE (2018) The psychology of humor: an integrative approach. Acedemic Press as an imprint of Elsevier, London, United Kingdom Martin RA, Ford TE (2018) The psychology of humor: an integrative approach. Acedemic Press as an imprint of Elsevier, London, United Kingdom
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruch W, Heintz S, Platt T, Wagner L, Proyer R (2018) Broadening humor: comic styles differentially tap into temperament, character, and ability. Front Psychol 9(6) Ruch W, Heintz S, Platt T, Wagner L, Proyer R (2018) Broadening humor: comic styles differentially tap into temperament, character, and ability. Front Psychol 9(6)
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Pinna M, Mahtani-Chugani V, Sánchez Correas M, Sanz Rubiales A (2018) The use of humor in palliative care: a systematic literature review. Am J Hosp Palliat Med:1–13. Pinna M, Mahtani-Chugani V, Sánchez Correas M, Sanz Rubiales A (2018) The use of humor in palliative care: a systematic literature review. Am J Hosp Palliat Med:1–13.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Linge-Dahl L, Heintz S, Ruch W, Radbruch L (2018) Humor assessment and interventions in palliative care: a systematic review. Front Psychol 9 Linge-Dahl L, Heintz S, Ruch W, Radbruch L (2018) Humor assessment and interventions in palliative care: a systematic review. Front Psychol 9
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Lebowitz K, Suh S, Diaz P (2011) Emery CF 2014 Effects of humor and laughter on psychological functioning, quality of life, health status, and pulmonary disease: a preliminary investigation. Heart Lung 40(4):310–9CrossRefPubMed Lebowitz K, Suh S, Diaz P (2011) Emery CF 2014 Effects of humor and laughter on psychological functioning, quality of life, health status, and pulmonary disease: a preliminary investigation. Heart Lung 40(4):310–9CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Kontos P, Miller K-L, Colobong R, Lazgare L, Binns M, Low L-F et al (2016) Elder-clowning in long-term dementia care: results of a pilot study. J Am Geriatr Soc 64(2):347–353CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kontos P, Miller K-L, Colobong R, Lazgare L, Binns M, Low L-F et al (2016) Elder-clowning in long-term dementia care: results of a pilot study. J Am Geriatr Soc 64(2):347–353CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Adamle K, Ludwick R (2005) Humor in hospice care: who, where, and how much? Am J Hosp Pall Med 22(4):287–290CrossRef Adamle K, Ludwick R (2005) Humor in hospice care: who, where, and how much? Am J Hosp Pall Med 22(4):287–290CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Falkenberg I, McGhee P, Wild B (2013) Humorfähigkeiten trainieren (Training Humour Skills). Stuttgart, Germany: Schattauer Falkenberg I, McGhee P, Wild B (2013) Humorfähigkeiten trainieren (Training Humour Skills). Stuttgart, Germany: Schattauer
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruch W, Zweyer K (2001) Heiterkeit und Humor: Ergebnisse der Forschung. In: Hirsch R, Bruder J, Radebold H, (eds). Heiterkeit und Humor im Alter. Schriftenreihe der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Gerontopsychiatrie und Psychotherapie. Bornheim-Sechtem: Chudeck-Druck Ruch W, Zweyer K (2001) Heiterkeit und Humor: Ergebnisse der Forschung. In: Hirsch R, Bruder J, Radebold H, (eds). Heiterkeit und Humor im Alter. Schriftenreihe der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Gerontopsychiatrie und Psychotherapie. Bornheim-Sechtem: Chudeck-Druck
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Scheel T, Hoeppner D, Grotevendt A, Barthlen W (2017) Clowns in paediatric surgery: less anxiety and more oxytocin? A pilot study. Klin Padiatr 229(5):274–280CrossRefPubMed Scheel T, Hoeppner D, Grotevendt A, Barthlen W (2017) Clowns in paediatric surgery: less anxiety and more oxytocin? A pilot study. Klin Padiatr 229(5):274–280CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Lopes-Júnior LC, Pereira-da-Silva G, Silveira DSC, Veronez LC, Santos JC, Alonso JB et al (2018) The effect of clown intervention on self-report and biomarker measures of stress and fatigue in pediatric osteosarcoma inpatients: a pilot study. Integr Cancer Ther 17(3):928–940CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lopes-Júnior LC, Pereira-da-Silva G, Silveira DSC, Veronez LC, Santos JC, Alonso JB et al (2018) The effect of clown intervention on self-report and biomarker measures of stress and fatigue in pediatric osteosarcoma inpatients: a pilot study. Integr Cancer Ther 17(3):928–940CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Mücke M, Cuhls H, Radbruch L, Baron R, Maier C, Tolle T et al (2014) Quantitative sensory testing. Schmerz. 28(6):635–46 (quiz 47-8)CrossRefPubMed Mücke M, Cuhls H, Radbruch L, Baron R, Maier C, Tolle T et al (2014) Quantitative sensory testing. Schmerz. 28(6):635–46 (quiz 47-8)CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruch W, Hofmann J (2012) A temperament approach to humor". In: (Ed.) PG, editor. Humor and health promotion Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers, pp 79–112 Ruch W, Hofmann J (2012) A temperament approach to humor". In: (Ed.) PG, editor. Humor and health promotion Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers, pp 79–112
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Glaesmer H, Grande G, Braehler E, Roth M (2011) The German version of the Satisfaction with Life Scale – psychometric properties and population based norms. Eur J Psychol Assess 27(2):127–132CrossRef Glaesmer H, Grande G, Braehler E, Roth M (2011) The German version of the Satisfaction with Life Scale – psychometric properties and population based norms. Eur J Psychol Assess 27(2):127–132CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Mehnert A, Müller D, Lehmann C, Koch U (2006) Die deutsche Versiondes NCCN Distress-Thermometers - Empirische Prüfung eines Screening-Instruments zur Erfassung psychosozialer Belastung bei Krebspatienten. Z Psychiatr Psychol Psychother 54(3):213–223 Mehnert A, Müller D, Lehmann C, Koch U (2006) Die deutsche Versiondes NCCN Distress-Thermometers - Empirische Prüfung eines Screening-Instruments zur Erfassung psychosozialer Belastung bei Krebspatienten. Z Psychiatr Psychol Psychother 54(3):213–223
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Stiel S, Matthes M, Bertram L, Ostgathe C, Elsner F, Radbruch L (2010) Validierung der neuen Fassung des Minimalen Dokumentationssystems (MIDOS) für Patienten in der Palliativmedizin. Schmerz 24:596–604CrossRefPubMed Stiel S, Matthes M, Bertram L, Ostgathe C, Elsner F, Radbruch L (2010) Validierung der neuen Fassung des Minimalen Dokumentationssystems (MIDOS) für Patienten in der Palliativmedizin. Schmerz 24:596–604CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Stiel S, Kues K, Krumm N, Radbruch L, Elsner F (2011) Assessment of quality of life in patients receiving palliative care: comparison of measurement tools and single item on subjective well-being. J Palliat Med 14:599–606CrossRefPubMed Stiel S, Kues K, Krumm N, Radbruch L, Elsner F (2011) Assessment of quality of life in patients receiving palliative care: comparison of measurement tools and single item on subjective well-being. J Palliat Med 14:599–606CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Oken MM, Creech RH, Tormey DC, Horton J, Davis TE, McFadden ET et al (1982) Toxicity and response criteria of the Eastern Cooperative Oncology Group. Am J Clin Oncol 5(6):649–656CrossRefPubMed Oken MM, Creech RH, Tormey DC, Horton J, Davis TE, McFadden ET et al (1982) Toxicity and response criteria of the Eastern Cooperative Oncology Group. Am J Clin Oncol 5(6):649–656CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruch W, Köhler G, van Thriel C (1996) Assessing the “humorous temperament”: construction of the facet and standard trait forms of the State-Trait-Cheerfulness-Inventory — STCI. In: Ruch W, editor. Measurement of the sense of humor [special issue]. 9: Humor: Int J Humor Res:303–39 Ruch W, Köhler G, van Thriel C (1996) Assessing the “humorous temperament”: construction of the facet and standard trait forms of the State-Trait-Cheerfulness-Inventory — STCI. In: Ruch W, editor. Measurement of the sense of humor [special issue]. 9: Humor: Int J Humor Res:303–39
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruch W, Köhler G, van Thriel C (1997) To be in good or bad humor: construction of the state form of the State-Trait-Cheerfulness-Inventory — STCI. Pers Individ Differ 22(4):477–491CrossRef Ruch W, Köhler G, van Thriel C (1997) To be in good or bad humor: construction of the state form of the State-Trait-Cheerfulness-Inventory — STCI. Pers Individ Differ 22(4):477–491CrossRef
22.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Rees E (2001) The ethics and practicalities of consent in palliative care research: an overview. Int J Palliat Nurs 7(10):489–492CrossRefPubMed Rees E (2001) The ethics and practicalities of consent in palliative care research: an overview. Int J Palliat Nurs 7(10):489–492CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Hui D, Glitza I, Chisholm G, Yennu S, Bruera E (2012) Attrition rates, reasons, and predictive factors in supportive care and palliative oncology clinical trials. Cancer 119(5):1098–105CrossRefPubMed Hui D, Glitza I, Chisholm G, Yennu S, Bruera E (2012) Attrition rates, reasons, and predictive factors in supportive care and palliative oncology clinical trials. Cancer 119(5):1098–105CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Jordhøy M, Kaasa S, Fayers P, Underland G, Ahlner-Elmqvist M (1999) Challenges in palliative care research; recruitment, attrition and compliance: experience from a randomized controlled trial. Palliat Med 13(4):299–310CrossRefPubMed Jordhøy M, Kaasa S, Fayers P, Underland G, Ahlner-Elmqvist M (1999) Challenges in palliative care research; recruitment, attrition and compliance: experience from a randomized controlled trial. Palliat Med 13(4):299–310CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Santos F, Pinto S, Pessalacia J, Luchesi B, Silva L, Marinho M (2021) Effects of clown activities on patients eligible for palliative care in primary health care. Rev Bras Enferm 74(5):1–8CrossRef Santos F, Pinto S, Pessalacia J, Luchesi B, Silva L, Marinho M (2021) Effects of clown activities on patients eligible for palliative care in primary health care. Rev Bras Enferm 74(5):1–8CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Horvat-Gordon M, Granger DA, Schwartz EB, Nelson VJ, Kivlighan KT (2005) Oxytocin is not a valid biomarker when measured in saliva by immunoassay. Physiol Behav 84(3):445–448CrossRefPubMed Horvat-Gordon M, Granger DA, Schwartz EB, Nelson VJ, Kivlighan KT (2005) Oxytocin is not a valid biomarker when measured in saliva by immunoassay. Physiol Behav 84(3):445–448CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat de Jong TR, Menon R, Bludau A, Grund T, Biermeier V, Klampfl SM et al (2015) Salivary oxytocin concentrations in response to running, sexual self-stimulation, breastfeeding and the TSST: The Regensburg Oxytocin Challenge (ROC) study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 62:381–388CrossRefPubMed de Jong TR, Menon R, Bludau A, Grund T, Biermeier V, Klampfl SM et al (2015) Salivary oxytocin concentrations in response to running, sexual self-stimulation, breastfeeding and the TSST: The Regensburg Oxytocin Challenge (ROC) study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 62:381–388CrossRefPubMed
29.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Herzog AR, Bachman JG (1981) Effects of questionnaire length on response quality. Public Opin Q 45:549–559CrossRef Herzog AR, Bachman JG (1981) Effects of questionnaire length on response quality. Public Opin Q 45:549–559CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Rozental A, Kottorp A, Boettcher J, Andersson G, Carlbring P (2016) Negative effects of psychological treatments: an exploratory factor analysis of the negative effects questionnaire for monitoring and reporting adverse and unwanted events. PLoS ONE 11(6):e0157503CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Rozental A, Kottorp A, Boettcher J, Andersson G, Carlbring P (2016) Negative effects of psychological treatments: an exploratory factor analysis of the negative effects questionnaire for monitoring and reporting adverse and unwanted events. PLoS ONE 11(6):e0157503CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Humour interventions for patients in palliative care—a randomized controlled trial
verfasst von
Lisa Linge-Dahl
Rainer Kreuz
Mieke Stoffelen
Sonja Heintz
Willibald Ruch
Eckart von Hirschhausen
Lukas Radbruch
Publikationsdatum
01.03.2023
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
Supportive Care in Cancer / Ausgabe 3/2023
Print ISSN: 0941-4355
Elektronische ISSN: 1433-7339
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00520-023-07606-9

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2023

Supportive Care in Cancer 3/2023 Zur Ausgabe

Alphablocker schützt vor Miktionsproblemen nach der Biopsie

16.05.2024 alpha-1-Rezeptorantagonisten Nachrichten

Nach einer Prostatabiopsie treten häufig Probleme beim Wasserlassen auf. Ob sich das durch den periinterventionellen Einsatz von Alphablockern verhindern lässt, haben australische Mediziner im Zuge einer Metaanalyse untersucht.

Mammakarzinom: Senken Statine das krebsbedingte Sterberisiko?

15.05.2024 Mammakarzinom Nachrichten

Frauen mit lokalem oder metastasiertem Brustkrebs, die Statine einnehmen, haben eine niedrigere krebsspezifische Mortalität als Patientinnen, die dies nicht tun, legen neue Daten aus den USA nahe.

Labor, CT-Anthropometrie zeigen Risiko für Pankreaskrebs

13.05.2024 Pankreaskarzinom Nachrichten

Gerade bei aggressiven Malignomen wie dem duktalen Adenokarzinom des Pankreas könnte Früherkennung die Therapiechancen verbessern. Noch jedoch klafft hier eine Lücke. Ein Studienteam hat einen Weg gesucht, sie zu schließen.

Viel pflanzliche Nahrung, seltener Prostata-Ca.-Progression

12.05.2024 Prostatakarzinom Nachrichten

Ein hoher Anteil pflanzlicher Nahrung trägt möglicherweise dazu bei, das Progressionsrisiko von Männern mit Prostatakarzinomen zu senken. In einer US-Studie war das Risiko bei ausgeprägter pflanzlicher Ernährung in etwa halbiert.

Update Onkologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.