Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2012

Open Access 01.12.2012 | Research article

Impaired pitch identification as a potential marker for depression

verfasst von: Michael Schwenzer, Eva Zattarin, Michael Grözinger, Klaus Mathiak

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2012

Abstract

Background

Impaired auditory performance has been considered as marker for depression. The present study tested whether pitch perception is affected in depression and whether the impairment is task-specific or reflects global dysfunction.

Methods

Twelve depressive in-patients and 12 non-depressive participants, half of the sample women, volunteered. The participants performed pitch identification using a four-choice reaction task, pitch contour perception, and pitch discrimination.

Results

During pitch identification but not during pitch contour perception or pitch discrimination, depressive patients responded less accurate than non-depressive participants (F = 3.3, p = 0.047). An analysis of covariates revealed that only female but not male depressive patients identified pitches poorly (Z = −2.2, p = 0.025) and inaccurate pitch identification correlated with high scores in the Beck Depression Inventory in women (r = −0.8, p = 0.001) but not in men (r = −0.1, p = 0.745). Patients did not differ from controls in reaction time or responsiveness.

Conclusions

Impaired pitch perception in depression is task-specific. Therefore, cognitive deficits in depression are circumscribed and not global. Reduced pitch identification in depression was associated with female sex. We suggest that impaired pitch identification merits attention as a potential marker for depression in women.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1471-244X-12-32) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MS conceptualized the study, analyzed the data, and drafted the manuscript together with KM. EZ recruited healthy participants and performed the entire experiment. MG recruited the patients and provided substantial improvements of the manuscript. KM participated in the statistical analysis and drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Background

Cognitive deficits in depression arise among others in the auditory domain [1]. Thus, some authors considered impaired auditory processing as a potential marker for depression [2, 3]. There is a controversy, however, whether cognitive dysfunctions in depression are specific [4] or general [5, 6]. Variable findings of correlations between depression and pitch perception may indicate the involvement of specific impairments: depressive patients responded less [1], similarly [7], or even more accurately [8] than healthy controls in pitch perception tasks. The variable outcomes suggest that heterogeneous processes may be differentially affected during a depressive episode. The present study aimed to confirm that a specific pitch perception skill - pitch identification - and not pitch perception in general is impaired in depressive patients.
Three different skills contribute to pitch perception: pitch identification, pitch contour perception, and pitch discrimination [9, 10]. There are reasons to hypothesize that depression impairs specifically pitch identification: Pitch identification was assessed using a choice reaction task [11], and visual choice reaction was poor in depression [6, 12, 13]. Moreover, pitch identification activated the intraparietal sulcus which was associated with numerosity encoding [11], and non-verbal encoding strategies were impaired in depressive patients [14]. In contrast, little data support that depression affects pitch contour perception or pitch discrimination.
The test design can bias outcomes in a clinical study. For instance, too easily distinguishable pitches could level out differences due to a ceiling effect. A ceiling effect may explain why Knott et al. found no impairment of auditory perception in depression [7]. Indeed, depressive patients performed poorly only during effortful tasks [15]. A second reason for variable outcomes may be a speed-accuracy tradeoff in depressive patients as described by Kalb et al. [8]. Thirdly, reduced performance may reflect low motivation to respond, i.e. poor ‘responsiveness’ [1, 16]. To minimize misinterpretations due to such confounding factors, auditory tests in depression should employ difficult tasks and monitor speed-accuracy tradeoff and responsiveness.
The present investigation applied pitch identification, pitch contour perception, and pitch discrimination tasks which reflected different auditory skills and neurophysiologic processes [10, 11, 17]. The tests were adjusted to high task difficulty by selection of small pitch variations such that the tests were sensitive to impairments [10, 15]. To detect a potential speed-accuracy tradeoff, reaction times were recorded. Moreover, we considered the sum of correct and incorrect responses as measure of responsiveness.
Based on the quoted literature the hypothesis was tested, that auditory accuracy during pitch identification but not during pitch contour perception and pitch discrimination is impaired in depressive patients. This finding was expected to emerge independent of potential covariates, i.e. speed-accuracy tradeoff, responsiveness, motivation as well as sex, age, musical experience, education, and smoking.

Methods

Participants

Twelve patients suffering from unipolar depression and 12 healthy controls volunteered. Half of each sample was women. The depressive participants were in-patients of the RWTH Aachen hospital. Depression was diagnosed according to ICD-10 [18] by at least two experienced psychiatrists including MG. Table 1 displays the diagnosis for each patient. None of the patients had hallucinations. Eleven patients were medicated (5 x mirtazapine, 3 x venlafaxine, and 3 x atypical antipsychotics) for at least four weeks; one patient was not medicated. The healthy participants had no mental disorder according to a standardized clinical interview [19] and were free of medication. Both groups comprised three musicians and nine non-musicians each. All participants were right-handed as determined by scores above 67 in the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [20]. All participants were naïve to the auditory tasks and had not taken part in a similar study before. The participants reported not to be hard of hearing. In the practicing trials of each auditory test, the participants were asked whether they could hear each tone, which was the case. All participants volunteered on the base of written informed consent and were paid. The medical ethics committee of the Aachen University approved the study.
Table 1
Characteristics of depressive patients and non-depressive controls
ICD-Diagnosis
BDIa
sex
age
musician
educationb
smoker
Depressive patients
F32.1
19
f
36
no
low
yes
F32.1
23
f
47
no
low
yes
F32.2
23
f
62
yes
high
no
F33.1
27
f
48
yes
high
yes
F33.1
27
f
63
no
middle
no
F33.2
29
f
55
no
low
yes
F32.1
15
m
24
yes
high
no
F32.2
31
m
27
no
high
yes
F33.2
34
m
36
no
high
yes
F33.3
11
m
52
no
high
yes
F33.3
34
m
40
no
low
yes
F33.3
17
m
55
no
high
no
M ± SD
24 ± 7
6/6
45 ± 12
3/9
4/1/7
8/4
Controls
 
4
f
47
no
middle
yes
 
8
f
51
no
middle
yes
 
8
f
60
yes
middle
no
 
1
f
50
yes
high
yes
 
0
f
62
no
middle
no
 
1
f
58
no
low
yes
 
3
m
22
yes
high
no
 
5
m
23
no
high
yes
 
1
m
39
no
high
no
 
2
m
56
no
middle
yes
 
0
m
42
no
middle
yes
 
1
m
52
no
high
yes
M ± SD
3 ± 3
6/6
46 ± 13
3/9
1/6/5
8/4
a BDI = scores in the Beck Depression Inventory.
b low = no secondary modern school; middle = secondary modern school education; high = matriculation standard.

Measurements

Auditory performance

A personal computer equipped with a custom program presented the pitch identification, pitch contour perception, and pitch discrimination tasks [10]. The computer soundcard produced sine wave tones (100 ms with 10 ms onset/offset ramps, 52 dB). The tones were played after a silent period varying randomly between 100 and 900 ms. In each task, different types of trials occurred in pseudo-randomized order with the constraint that the type changed between consecutive trials. The participants listened to the tones via headphones that absorbed ambient noise and responded on a 4-button keypad. The program recorded each response within 1.8 s after stimulation. Each trial lasted 3 s regardless of the variable silent period and the response time. The program registered each response and the reaction time of correct responses.
During the pitch identification task, one of four frequencies (800, 832, 852, and 872 Hz) were played in each of 60 trials. The frequencies did not represent a standardized, e.g. chromatic, linear or logarithmic scale, to avoid a bias due to the recognition of regularities. The participants memorized each frequency before the measurement and were instructed to identify the tones independent from preceding trials. The participants categorized the pitches on the 4-button keypad. The keys indicated rising frequencies from left to right, i.e. pressing the index finger after 800 Hz, pressing the middle finger after 832 Hz etc.
During pitch contour perception, the participants should detect a pitch ascent within a melody of descending pitches. In 60 out of 120 trials (resp. melodies), a pitch ascent occurred. Each melody consisted of a dichotic sequence of four tones in which the sequence of pitches differed between ears. For instance, pitches were 683, 724, 645, 215 Hz at the left ear, and 1149, 1149, 966, 304 Hz at the right ear listed in temporal succession; in this example the participant should notice the pitch ascent from 683 to 724 Hz at the left ear. Four basic melodies were generated; left and right side were balanced additionally and varied with respect to pitch ascent vs. no pitch ascent. Considering all melodies, the tonal range was between 304 and 3068 Hz, the pitch ascended up to three semitones, the descents of pitches were up to 20 semitones between subsequent tones. We chose such a complex presentation to achieve a high task difficulty and pitch contour perception should clearly differ from the pitch identification and pitch discrimination tasks. The participants had to press a fixed button of the 4-button keypad whenever the pitch rose; unused keys were covered to reduce distraction. The participants were instructed to avoid false positive responses.
In the pitch discrimination task, two succeeding tones had the same frequency or the second tone was higher than the first tone, i.e. an ascent from 1000 to 1004, 1008, 1012, or 1016 Hz. The frequency ascended in 60 out of 120 trials whereas in the other half of trials the frequency remained 1000 Hz in both succeeding tones. Like during pitch contour perception, participants should press the fixed button of the 4-button keypad when the frequency increased and avoid false positive responses.

Assessment of personal data

All participants completed the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI [21]; German version [22]). For assessing self-rated motivation, the participants stated on a scale from 1 through 10, how much they made an effort to achieve a good performance in the testing. For the assessment of musical experience, the investigator inquired how often the participant played an instrument or sang in a choir, how many years he played the instrument or sang, and if he took lessons. Since all German pupils received music lessons in school, only practice or lessons in addition to regular school education was considered. Playing an instrument or singing in a choir at least once per week during a period of two years distinguished musicians from non-musicians in this study.

Procedure

After informed consent and the clinical interviews, the participants completed the BDI and the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory. Pitch identification, pitch contour perception, and pitch discrimination were accomplished in randomized order. The investigator introduced each pitch perception test, played each kind of trial, and demonstrated the expected responses. In each task, the participants performed 60 practice trials before assessment. After the auditory assessment, the investigator enquired self-rated motivation.

Statistics

The analysis considered the number of correct responses as measure of accuracy. A correct response meant pressing the correct key when a response was expected. Pressing a false key when a response was expected or pressing any key when no response was expected were valued incorrect responses. Trials without pressing a key were ignored. The average reaction time of correct responses allowed analyzing the speed-accuracy tradeoff. The sum of correct and incorrect responses should indicate responsiveness and may be an indicator of drive together with the motivation rating.
The Shapiro-Wilk test assessed Gaussian normal distribution of data as a prerequisite for parametric analysis. An ANOVA tested the hypothesis that pitch perception is impaired in depressive compared to non-depressive participants. The interaction between the group factor (depressives vs. controls) and the repeated measurement factor of pitch perception (pitch identification, pitch contour perception, pitch discrimination) as well as post-hoc t-tests should confirm a task-specific effect. Not normally distributed data and covariates with cells with six or fewer participants were analyzed using the non-parametric Mann–Whitney U-test or a χ²-test for more than two categories. Pearson’s correlation coefficients and regression analysis allowed further data exploration. The significance level was p < 0.050. A Bonferroni correction was applied.

Results

Most data approximated a normal distribution (S-W > 0.93, p > 0.110) but the sum of correct and false responses in pitch identification (S-W = 0.57, p = 0.001) and self-rated motivation scores (S-W = 0.90, p = 0.025) yielded a ceiling effect. A task-specific impairment emerged (interaction depression x tasks: F(2,44) = 3.3, G-G = 0.98, η² = 0.13, p = 0.047). Post-hoc tests revealed that depressive patients identified pitch less accurately than controls without impairment in pitch contour perception and pitch discrimination (Table 2); during pitch identification, depressive patients yielded 24% fewer correct responses than non-depressive participants. Depression did not slow-down response times (interaction depression x tasks: F(2,44) = 0.4, G-G = 0.96, p = 0.620). No significant speed-accuracy tradeoff emerged during pitch identification (r(24) = −0.19, p = 0.368) and pitch contour perception (r(24) = −0.22, p = 0.297) but during pitch discrimination (r(24) = −0.47, p = 0.020). Responsiveness did not differ between depressive and non-depressive participants (interaction groups x tasks F(2,22) = 0.5, G-G = 0.96, p = 0.575; main difference between groups F(1,11) = 0.1, p = 0.793). Thus, responsiveness cannot explain differential accuracy scores. Self-rated motivation was identical in depressive and non-depressive participants (Z = −0.1, p = 0.910).
Table 2
Pitch perception and motivation in depressive patients and non-depressive controls
 
Depressives N = 12
Controls N = 12
Difference
t/Za
p
 
M ± SD
M ± SD
M ± SD
  
Accuracy
     
Pitch identification
23.7 ± 10.6
31.4 ±    6.0
−7.6 ± 12.0
−2.1
0.040*
Pitch contour percept.
27.5 ± 10.1
23.9 ± 11.4
3.5 ± 17.3
0.8
0.426
Pitch discrimination
26.6 ± 13.7
25.3 ± 11.5
1.3 ± 18.2
0.2
0.799
Reaction time (ms)
     
Pitch identification
1013 ± 234
954 ± 125
58 ± 290
0.7
0.453
Pitch contour percept.
932 ± 224
930 ± 232
1 ± 341
0.1
0.987
Pitch discrimination
864 ± 230
794 ± 175
69 ± 291
0.8
0.413
Responsiveness
     
Pitch identification
51.7 ± 12.9
58.0 ±   1.9
−6.2 ± 13.8
−0.2a
0.785
Pitch contour percept.
48.0 ± 16.7
40.0 ± 17.9
8.0 ± 28.1
1.1
0.266
Pitch discrimination
44.9 ± 19.1
42.6 ± 20.9
2.2 ± 32.3
0.2
0.786
Self-rated motivation
7.9 ±   1.4
8.0 ±   1.5
−0.1 ±   1.9
−0.2a
0.859
Accuracy = correct responses. Responsiveness = correct plus incorrect responses. Degree of freedom = 22. a non-parametric Mann–Whitney U-test because of a ceiling effect. *p < 0.050.
The analysis of further covariates revealed a sex effect. Female but not male depressive patients performed poor in pitch identification (female vs. male depressive patients: Z = −2.2, p = 0.025; female patients vs. female controls: -14.5 ± 9.5 with 45% fewer correct responses, one-tailed Z = −2.4, p = 0.031; male patients vs. male controls: -0.8 ± 10.6, one-tailed Z = −0.1, p > 0.500). Differential correlations of BDI depression scores with pitch identification reflect this sex effect: A high BDI score predicted inaccurate pitch identification only in women (r(12) = −0.82, p = 0.001; β = −0.73,  = 0.68) but not in men (r(12) = −0.10, p = 0.745; β = −0.06,  = 0.01; see Figure 1). Types of drugs did not differ between sexes (χ² = 0.4, p = .649). The difference in body weight failed significance (women: 70.6 ± 15.0 kg, men: 86.0 ± 14.0 kg, Z = 1.4, p = 0.180) thus an interaction between sex and relative dosage appears unlikely. Finally, impaired pitch identification was not significantly associated with age (r(12) = 0.28, p = 0.379), musical experience (Z = −0.6, p = 0.564), education (χ² = 5.4, p = 0.062 with a tendency to a lower effect in high-educated participants), or smoking (Z = 1.1, p = 0.299). Thus, depression and no other factor led to impaired pitch identification in women.

Discussion

Pitch identification was selectively impaired in depression. Pitch identification differed from other pitch-related skills, and this supports a model of circumscribed cognitive deficits rather than a global impairment. The choice reaction task used for assessing pitch identification activated the intraparietal sulcus [11] and not memory-related mesio-temporal or frontal regions [23]. The intraparietal sulcus was associated with the encoding of numerosity [24]. Conceivably, numerosity contributed to poor performance of depressive patients during effortful tasks in the study of Hamar et al. [15]. These authors increased effort by applying a higher number of stimuli. In view of these findings, we suggest that poor pitch identification reflects a reduced mental representation of numerous alternatives in depression.
We could not confirm a bias from speed-accuracy tradeoff, responsiveness, and motivation. Though several studies reported a slowing in depression (e.g. [6]), choice reaction time remains a controversial issue: neither the present study nor Chase et al. [25] found a deceleration. Possibly, speed becomes less important in difficult tasks [26]. Malone and Hemsley [1] hypothesized that demand characteristics of the task enhance responsiveness, which was not supported in the current study. Depressive patients performed worse during the pitch identification task, which demanded a response in each trial. The total number of responses did not differ between depressive and non-depressive participants. A previous study did not find a generally reduced responsiveness as well because false positive responses and omissions counterbalanced each other [27]. Finally, the data did not support low motivation as origin of cognitive impairment [16] but a distinction of neuropsychological deficits from motivation in depression [28].
Inaccurate pitch identification emerged in women but not in men. This sex effect was unexpected. In view of the low number of participants, this finding is exploratory and needs a replication. Another study reported hearing impairment in male depressive patients [29] – though on a very basic sensory level and not directly comparable to the pitch identification task. The test which presented the lowest pitch differences in the present study – pitch discrimination – did not distinguish depressive and non-depressive participants. Therefore it is unconceivable that sex interacted with pitch identification due to different perception thresholds. Poor pitch identification in depressive women resembled the outcome after serotonergic medication in healthy (male) participants [17]. Serotonergic neurotransmission and specific auditory impairments in depression may be associated; in this regard, differences between women and men have not been thoroughly investigated so far.

Conclusions

Auditory processing has been considered as marker of depression reflecting an underlying serotonergic dysfunction [30]. In the present study, depression in women impaired pitch identification; the effect was task-specific, not biased from reaction time and motivational factors, and persisted despite treatment. Thus, we suggest that inaccurate pitch identification merits attention as a potential marker for depression in women.

Acknowledgement

The German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft DFG MA2631/4-1) provided financial support.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MS conceptualized the study, analyzed the data, and drafted the manuscript together with KM. EZ recruited healthy participants and performed the entire experiment. MG recruited the patients and provided substantial improvements of the manuscript. KM participated in the statistical analysis and drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Malone JR, Hemsley DR: Lowered responsiveness and auditory signal detectability during depression. Psychol Med. 1977, 7: 717-722. 10.1017/S0033291700006383.CrossRefPubMed Malone JR, Hemsley DR: Lowered responsiveness and auditory signal detectability during depression. Psychol Med. 1977, 7: 717-722. 10.1017/S0033291700006383.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Hegerl U, Gallinat J, Juckel G: Event-related potentials. Do they reflect central serotonergic neurotransmission and do they predict clinical response to serotonin agonists?. J Affect Disord. 2001, 62: 93-100. 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00353-0.CrossRefPubMed Hegerl U, Gallinat J, Juckel G: Event-related potentials. Do they reflect central serotonergic neurotransmission and do they predict clinical response to serotonin agonists?. J Affect Disord. 2001, 62: 93-100. 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00353-0.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Karaaslan F, Gonul AS, Oguz A, Erdnic E, Esel E: P300 changes in major depressive disorders with and without psychotic features. J Affect Disord. 2003, 73: 283-287. 10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00477-3.CrossRefPubMed Karaaslan F, Gonul AS, Oguz A, Erdnic E, Esel E: P300 changes in major depressive disorders with and without psychotic features. J Affect Disord. 2003, 73: 283-287. 10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00477-3.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Purcell R, Maruff P, Kyrios M, Pantelis C: Neurophysiological function in young patients with unipolar major depression. Psychol Med. 1997, 27: 1277-1285. 10.1017/S0033291797005448.CrossRefPubMed Purcell R, Maruff P, Kyrios M, Pantelis C: Neurophysiological function in young patients with unipolar major depression. Psychol Med. 1997, 27: 1277-1285. 10.1017/S0033291797005448.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Landrø NI, Stiles TC, Sletvold H: Neuropsychological function in nonpsychotic unipolar major depression. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 2001, 14: 233-240.PubMed Landrø NI, Stiles TC, Sletvold H: Neuropsychological function in nonpsychotic unipolar major depression. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 2001, 14: 233-240.PubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Veiel HO: A preliminary profile of neuropsychological deficits associated with major depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1997, 19: 587-603. 10.1080/01688639708403745.CrossRefPubMed Veiel HO: A preliminary profile of neuropsychological deficits associated with major depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1997, 19: 587-603. 10.1080/01688639708403745.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Knott V, Lapierre Y, Griffiths L, de Lugt D, Bakish D: Event-related potentials and selective attention in major depressive illness. J Affect Disord. 1991, 23: 43-48. 10.1016/0165-0327(91)90034-P.CrossRefPubMed Knott V, Lapierre Y, Griffiths L, de Lugt D, Bakish D: Event-related potentials and selective attention in major depressive illness. J Affect Disord. 1991, 23: 43-48. 10.1016/0165-0327(91)90034-P.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Kalb R, Dörner M, Kalb S: Opposite effects of depression and antidepressants on processing speed and error rate. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2006, 30: 244-250. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.10.009.CrossRefPubMed Kalb R, Dörner M, Kalb S: Opposite effects of depression and antidepressants on processing speed and error rate. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2006, 30: 244-250. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.10.009.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Dowling WJ: Scale and contour: two components of a theory of memory for melodies. Psychol Rev. 1978, 85: 341-354.CrossRef Dowling WJ: Scale and contour: two components of a theory of memory for melodies. Psychol Rev. 1978, 85: 341-354.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwenzer M, Mathiak K: Differential skills of perception of frequency. Percept Mot Skills. 2007, 105: 236-242.PubMed Schwenzer M, Mathiak K: Differential skills of perception of frequency. Percept Mot Skills. 2007, 105: 236-242.PubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Azorin JM, Benhaΐm P, Hasbroucq T, Possamaΐ CA: Stimulus preprocessing and response selection in depression: a reaction time study. Acta Psychol. 1995, 89: 95-100. 10.1016/0001-6918(94)00024-B.CrossRef Azorin JM, Benhaΐm P, Hasbroucq T, Possamaΐ CA: Stimulus preprocessing and response selection in depression: a reaction time study. Acta Psychol. 1995, 89: 95-100. 10.1016/0001-6918(94)00024-B.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Thomas P, Goudemand M, Rousseaux M: Attentional resources in major depression. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999, 249: 79-85. 10.1007/s004060050070.CrossRefPubMed Thomas P, Goudemand M, Rousseaux M: Attentional resources in major depression. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999, 249: 79-85. 10.1007/s004060050070.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Behnken A, Schöning S, Gerss J, Konrad C, de Jong-Meyer R, Zwanzger P, Arolt V: Persistent non-verbal memory impairment in remitted major depression – caused by encoding deficits?. J Affect Disord. 2010, 122: 144-148. 10.1016/j.jad.2009.07.010.CrossRefPubMed Behnken A, Schöning S, Gerss J, Konrad C, de Jong-Meyer R, Zwanzger P, Arolt V: Persistent non-verbal memory impairment in remitted major depression – caused by encoding deficits?. J Affect Disord. 2010, 122: 144-148. 10.1016/j.jad.2009.07.010.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Hammar Å, Lund A, Hugdahl K: Selective impairment in effortful information processing in major depression. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003, 9: 954-959.CrossRefPubMed Hammar Å, Lund A, Hugdahl K: Selective impairment in effortful information processing in major depression. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003, 9: 954-959.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Scheurich A, Fellgiebel A, Schermuly I, Bauer S, Wölfges R, Müller MJ: Experimental evidence for a motivational origin of cognitive impairment in major depression. Psychol Med. 2008, 38: 237-246.CrossRefPubMed Scheurich A, Fellgiebel A, Schermuly I, Bauer S, Wölfges R, Müller MJ: Experimental evidence for a motivational origin of cognitive impairment in major depression. Psychol Med. 2008, 38: 237-246.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwenzer M, Heitkamp HC, Mathiak K: Differential effects of indoleamines on auditory choice reaction, selective attention, and pitch discrimination. Neurosci Lett. 2006, 398: 310-313. 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.01.016.CrossRefPubMed Schwenzer M, Heitkamp HC, Mathiak K: Differential effects of indoleamines on auditory choice reaction, selective attention, and pitch discrimination. Neurosci Lett. 2006, 398: 310-313. 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.01.016.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat World Health Organization: 10th revision of the international classification of diseases, ICD-10, Chapter V (F): Mental and behavioural disorders. 1995, World Health Organization, Genf World Health Organization: 10th revision of the international classification of diseases, ICD-10, Chapter V (F): Mental and behavioural disorders. 1995, World Health Organization, Genf
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Margraf J: Mini-DIPS. Diagnostisches Kurz-Interview bei psychischen Störungen. Handbuch, Interviewleitfaden. 1994, Springer, HeidelbergCrossRef Margraf J: Mini-DIPS. Diagnostisches Kurz-Interview bei psychischen Störungen. Handbuch, Interviewleitfaden. 1994, Springer, HeidelbergCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Oldfield RC: The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971, 9: 97-113. 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4.CrossRefPubMed Oldfield RC: The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971, 9: 97-113. 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J: An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961, 4: 561-571. 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004.CrossRefPubMed Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J: An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961, 4: 561-571. 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Hautzinger M, Bailer M, Worall H, Keller F: Beck-Depressions-Inventar BDI, Handanweisung. 1995, Verlag Hans Huber, Göttingen, 2 Hautzinger M, Bailer M, Worall H, Keller F: Beck-Depressions-Inventar BDI, Handanweisung. 1995, Verlag Hans Huber, Göttingen, 2
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Fairhall SL, Sharma S, Magnusson J, Murphy B: Memory related dysregulation of hippocampal function in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychol. 2010, 85: 499-503. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.002.CrossRefPubMed Fairhall SL, Sharma S, Magnusson J, Murphy B: Memory related dysregulation of hippocampal function in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychol. 2010, 85: 499-503. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.002.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Piazza M, Pinel P, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S: A magnitude code common to numerosities and number symbols in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron. 2007, 53: 293-305. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.022.CrossRefPubMed Piazza M, Pinel P, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S: A magnitude code common to numerosities and number symbols in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron. 2007, 53: 293-305. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.022.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Chase HW, Michael A, Bullmore ET, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW: Paradoxical enhancement of choice reaction time performance in patients with major depression. J Psychopharmacol. 2010, 24: 471-479. 10.1177/0269881109104883.CrossRefPubMed Chase HW, Michael A, Bullmore ET, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW: Paradoxical enhancement of choice reaction time performance in patients with major depression. J Psychopharmacol. 2010, 24: 471-479. 10.1177/0269881109104883.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Weckowicz TE, Tam CN, Mason J, Bay KS: Speed in test performance in depressed patients. J Abnorm Psychol. 1978, 87: 578-582.CrossRefPubMed Weckowicz TE, Tam CN, Mason J, Bay KS: Speed in test performance in depressed patients. J Abnorm Psychol. 1978, 87: 578-582.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith MJ, Brébion G, Banquet JP, Affilaire JF: Experimental evidence for two dimensions of cognitive disorders in depressives. J Psychiatr Res. 1994, 28: 401-411. 10.1016/0022-3956(94)90021-3.CrossRefPubMed Smith MJ, Brébion G, Banquet JP, Affilaire JF: Experimental evidence for two dimensions of cognitive disorders in depressives. J Psychiatr Res. 1994, 28: 401-411. 10.1016/0022-3956(94)90021-3.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Richards PM, Ruff RM: Motivational effects on neuropsychological functioning: comparison of depressed versus nondepressed individuals. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1989, 57: 396-402.CrossRefPubMed Richards PM, Ruff RM: Motivational effects on neuropsychological functioning: comparison of depressed versus nondepressed individuals. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1989, 57: 396-402.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Harada S, Nishiwaki Y, Michikawa T, Kikuchi Y, Iwasawa S, Nakano M, Ishigami A, Saito H, Takebayashi T: Gender difference in the relationships between vision and hearing impairments and negative well-being. Prev Med. 2008, 47: 433-437. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.06.011.CrossRefPubMed Harada S, Nishiwaki Y, Michikawa T, Kikuchi Y, Iwasawa S, Nakano M, Ishigami A, Saito H, Takebayashi T: Gender difference in the relationships between vision and hearing impairments and negative well-being. Prev Med. 2008, 47: 433-437. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.06.011.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Hegerl U, Juckel G: Identifying psychiatric patients with serotonergic dysfunctions by event-related potentials. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2000, 1: 112-118. 10.3109/15622970009150574.CrossRefPubMed Hegerl U, Juckel G: Identifying psychiatric patients with serotonergic dysfunctions by event-related potentials. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2000, 1: 112-118. 10.3109/15622970009150574.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Impaired pitch identification as a potential marker for depression
verfasst von
Michael Schwenzer
Eva Zattarin
Michael Grözinger
Klaus Mathiak
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2012
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2012
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-12-32

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2012

BMC Psychiatry 1/2012 Zur Ausgabe

ADHS-Medikation erhöht das kardiovaskuläre Risiko

16.05.2024 Herzinsuffizienz Nachrichten

Erwachsene, die Medikamente gegen das Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-Hyperaktivitätssyndrom einnehmen, laufen offenbar erhöhte Gefahr, an Herzschwäche zu erkranken oder einen Schlaganfall zu erleiden. Es scheint eine Dosis-Wirkungs-Beziehung zu bestehen.

Typ-2-Diabetes und Depression folgen oft aufeinander

14.05.2024 Typ-2-Diabetes Nachrichten

Menschen mit Typ-2-Diabetes sind überdurchschnittlich gefährdet, in den nächsten Jahren auch noch eine Depression zu entwickeln – und umgekehrt. Besonders ausgeprägt ist die Wechselbeziehung laut GKV-Daten bei jüngeren Erwachsenen.

Darf man die Behandlung eines Neonazis ablehnen?

08.05.2024 Gesellschaft Nachrichten

In einer Leseranfrage in der Zeitschrift Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology möchte ein anonymer Dermatologe bzw. eine anonyme Dermatologin wissen, ob er oder sie einen Patienten behandeln muss, der eine rassistische Tätowierung trägt.

Spezielles Sportprogramm bei einer Reihe von psychischen Erkrankungen effektiv

08.05.2024 Psychotherapie Nachrichten

Sportliche Betätigung hilft nicht nur bei Depression, sondern auch in Gruppen von Patientinnen und Patienten mit unterschiedlichen psychischen Erkrankungen, wie Insomnie, Panikattacken, Agoraphobie und posttraumatischem Belastungssyndrom. Sie alle profitieren längerfristig.