Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Experimental Brain Research 2/2007

01.11.2007 | Research Article

Specificity of practice results from differences in movement planning strategies

verfasst von: Isabelle Mackrous, Luc Proteau

Erschienen in: Experimental Brain Research | Ausgabe 2/2007

Einloggen, um Zugang zu erhalten

Abstract

Withdrawing visual feedback after practice of a manual aiming task results in a severe decrease in aiming accuracy. This decrease in accuracy is such that participants are often less accurate than controls who are beginning practice of the task without visual feedback. These results have been interpreted as evidence that motor learning is specific to the sources of afferent information optimizing performance, because it could be processed at the exclusion of other sources of afferent information. The goal of the present study was to test this hypothesis. To reach our goal we evaluated whether online visual feedback prevented kinesthetic information to be used for: (1) eliminating movement anisotropy resulting from difference in limb inertia when aiming in different directions and (2) creating an internal model of limb mechanics. Participants practiced a manual aiming task with or without visual feedback and with knowledge of results. After this acquisition phase, participants performed two transfer tests. The first transfer test was performed without visual feedback and/or knowledge of results. The second transfer test was similar to the first one but participants initiated their movements from a different starting base. The results showed strong specificity effects in that withdrawing visual feedback resulted in large pointing bias and variability. However, the results of the two transfer tests showed that the processing of visual feedback did not prevent the processing of kinesthetic information used to eliminate movement anisotropy or to create an internal model of limb mechanics. Rather, specificity of practice effects resulted from participants using the same motor plan in transfer as they did in acquisition even though they had no longer access to visual feedback to modulate their movement online. These results indicate that during acquisition participants adopted different movement planning strategies depending on the source of afferent information available.
Fußnoten
1
Note that no such bias was observed when participants were trained to initiate their movements from different starting bases in the presence of knowledge of results (Lhuisset and Proteau 2004).
 
2
This definition of movement initiation differed from that used during data collection. In addition to the normal impact of the filtering procedure on the time scale, it will result in apparently longer movement times in comparison to the movement time bandwidth accepted during data collection.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Bédard P, Proteau L (2005) Movement planning of video and of manual aiming movements. Spat Vis 18:275–296PubMedCrossRef Bédard P, Proteau L (2005) Movement planning of video and of manual aiming movements. Spat Vis 18:275–296PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Charvin M, Proteau L (1996) Developmental differences in the processing of afferent information for motor control. Dev Neuropsychol 2:347–387 Charvin M, Proteau L (1996) Developmental differences in the processing of afferent information for motor control. Dev Neuropsychol 2:347–387
Zurück zum Zitat Chicoine A, Lassonde M, Proteau L (1992) Developmental aspects of sensorimotor integration. Dev Neuropsychol 8:381–394CrossRef Chicoine A, Lassonde M, Proteau L (1992) Developmental aspects of sensorimotor integration. Dev Neuropsychol 8:381–394CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Desmurget M, Grafton S (2000) Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cogn Sci 4:423–431PubMedCrossRef Desmurget M, Grafton S (2000) Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cogn Sci 4:423–431PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Elliott D, Allard F (1985) The utilization of visual feedback information during rapid pointing movements. Q J Exp Psychol 37:407–425 Elliott D, Allard F (1985) The utilization of visual feedback information during rapid pointing movements. Q J Exp Psychol 37:407–425
Zurück zum Zitat Elliott D, Calvert R (1990) The influence of uncertainty and premovement visual information on manual aiming. Can J Exp Psychol 44:501–511 Elliott D, Calvert R (1990) The influence of uncertainty and premovement visual information on manual aiming. Can J Exp Psychol 44:501–511
Zurück zum Zitat Elliott D, Carson RG, Goodman D, Chua R (1991) Discrete vs. continuous visual control of manual aiming movements. Hum Mov Sci 10:393–418CrossRef Elliott D, Carson RG, Goodman D, Chua R (1991) Discrete vs. continuous visual control of manual aiming movements. Hum Mov Sci 10:393–418CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol 47:381–391PubMedCrossRef Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol 47:381–391PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ghilardi MF, Gordon J, Ghez C (1995a) Deafferented subjects do not compensate for workspace anisotropies of limb movements. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 95:73P–73P (71) Ghilardi MF, Gordon J, Ghez C (1995a) Deafferented subjects do not compensate for workspace anisotropies of limb movements. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 95:73P–73P (71)
Zurück zum Zitat Ghilardi MF, Gordon J, Ghez C (1995b) Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas. J Neurophysiol 73:2535–2539PubMed Ghilardi MF, Gordon J, Ghez C (1995b) Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas. J Neurophysiol 73:2535–2539PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Cooper SE, Ghez C (1994) Accuracy of planar reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 99:112–130PubMedCrossRef Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Cooper SE, Ghez C (1994) Accuracy of planar reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 99:112–130PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hansen S, Glazebrook CM, Anson JG, Weeks DJ, Elliott D (2006) The influence of advance information about target location and visual feedback on movement planning and execution. Can J Exp Psychol 60:200–208PubMed Hansen S, Glazebrook CM, Anson JG, Weeks DJ, Elliott D (2006) The influence of advance information about target location and visual feedback on movement planning and execution. Can J Exp Psychol 60:200–208PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Harris CM, Wolpert DM (1998) Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394:780–784PubMedCrossRef Harris CM, Wolpert DM (1998) Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394:780–784PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Khan MA, Franks IM (2003) Online versus offline processing of visual feedback in the production of component submovements. J Mot Behav 35:285–295PubMedCrossRef Khan MA, Franks IM (2003) Online versus offline processing of visual feedback in the production of component submovements. J Mot Behav 35:285–295PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Khan MA, Franks IM, Goodman D (1998) The effect of practice on the control of rapid aiming movements: evidence for an interdependency between programming and feedback processing. Q J Exp Psychol 51:425–443CrossRef Khan MA, Franks IM, Goodman D (1998) The effect of practice on the control of rapid aiming movements: evidence for an interdependency between programming and feedback processing. Q J Exp Psychol 51:425–443CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Khan MA, Franks IM, Elliott D, Lawrence GP, Chua R, Bernier PM, Hansen S, Weeks DJ (2006) Inferring online and offline processing of visual feedback in target-directed movements from kinematic data. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30:1106–1121PubMedCrossRef Khan MA, Franks IM, Elliott D, Lawrence GP, Chua R, Bernier PM, Hansen S, Weeks DJ (2006) Inferring online and offline processing of visual feedback in target-directed movements from kinematic data. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30:1106–1121PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lhuisset L, Proteau L (2004) Planning and control of straight-ahead and angled planar movements in adults and young children. Can J Exp Psychol 58:245–258PubMed Lhuisset L, Proteau L (2004) Planning and control of straight-ahead and angled planar movements in adults and young children. Can J Exp Psychol 58:245–258PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Meyer DE, Abrams RA, Kornblum S, Wright CE, Smith JE (1988) Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol Rev 95:340–370PubMedCrossRef Meyer DE, Abrams RA, Kornblum S, Wright CE, Smith JE (1988) Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol Rev 95:340–370PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Novak K, Miller L, Houk J (2002) The use of overlapping submovements in the control of rapid hand movements. Exp Brain Res 144:351–364PubMedCrossRef Novak K, Miller L, Houk J (2002) The use of overlapping submovements in the control of rapid hand movements. Exp Brain Res 144:351–364PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Novak K, Miller L, Houk J (2003) Features of motor performance that drive adaptation in rapid hand movements. Exp Brain Res 148:388–400PubMed Novak K, Miller L, Houk J (2003) Features of motor performance that drive adaptation in rapid hand movements. Exp Brain Res 148:388–400PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Proteau L (1992) On the specificity of learning and the role of visual information for movement control. In: Proteau L, Elliott D (eds) Vision and motor control, vol 85. North Holland, Amsterdam, pp 67–103CrossRef Proteau L (1992) On the specificity of learning and the role of visual information for movement control. In: Proteau L, Elliott D (eds) Vision and motor control, vol 85. North Holland, Amsterdam, pp 67–103CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Proteau L (2005) Visual afferent information dominates other sources of afferent information during mixed practice of a video-aiming task. Exp Brain Res 161:441–456PubMedCrossRef Proteau L (2005) Visual afferent information dominates other sources of afferent information during mixed practice of a video-aiming task. Exp Brain Res 161:441–456PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Proteau L, Carnahan H (2001) What causes specificity of practice in a manual aiming movement: vision dominance or transformation errors? J Mot Behav 33:226PubMed Proteau L, Carnahan H (2001) What causes specificity of practice in a manual aiming movement: vision dominance or transformation errors? J Mot Behav 33:226PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Proteau L, Cournoyer J (1990) Vision of the stylus in a manual aiming task: the effect of practice. Q J Exp Psychol 42A:811–828 Proteau L, Cournoyer J (1990) Vision of the stylus in a manual aiming task: the effect of practice. Q J Exp Psychol 42A:811–828
Zurück zum Zitat Proteau L, Marteniuk RG, Girouard Y, Dugas C (1987) On the type of information used to control and learn an aiming movement after moderate and extensive training. Hum Mov Sci 6:181–199CrossRef Proteau L, Marteniuk RG, Girouard Y, Dugas C (1987) On the type of information used to control and learn an aiming movement after moderate and extensive training. Hum Mov Sci 6:181–199CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Proteau L, Al Roujoula A, Messier J (2007) Online control of video-aiming movements: fast and efficient corrections of undetected experimentally-induced errors. Unpublished data Proteau L, Al Roujoula A, Messier J (2007) Online control of video-aiming movements: fast and efficient corrections of undetected experimentally-induced errors. Unpublished data
Zurück zum Zitat Robin C, Toussaint L, Blandin Y, Proteau L (2005) Specificity of learning in a video-aiming task: modifying the salience of dynamic visual cues. J Mot Behav 37:367–376PubMedCrossRef Robin C, Toussaint L, Blandin Y, Proteau L (2005) Specificity of learning in a video-aiming task: modifying the salience of dynamic visual cues. J Mot Behav 37:367–376PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rossetti Y, Desmurget M, Prablanc C (1995) Vectorial coding of movement: vision, proprioception, or both? J Neurophysiol 74:457–463PubMed Rossetti Y, Desmurget M, Prablanc C (1995) Vectorial coding of movement: vision, proprioception, or both? J Neurophysiol 74:457–463PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Soucy MC, Proteau L (2001) Development of multiple movement representations with practice: specificity versus flexibility. J Mot Behav 33:243–254PubMed Soucy MC, Proteau L (2001) Development of multiple movement representations with practice: specificity versus flexibility. J Mot Behav 33:243–254PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Tremblay L, Proteau L (1998) Specificity of practice: the case of powerlifting. Res Q Exerc Sport 69:284–289PubMed Tremblay L, Proteau L (1998) Specificity of practice: the case of powerlifting. Res Q Exerc Sport 69:284–289PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Virji-Babul N, Sainburg RL, Huang H, Ghez C (1997) Rate of learning novel limb dynamics depends on the nature of feedback signals. Soc Neurosci 85 Virji-Babul N, Sainburg RL, Huang H, Ghez C (1997) Rate of learning novel limb dynamics depends on the nature of feedback signals. Soc Neurosci 85
Metadaten
Titel
Specificity of practice results from differences in movement planning strategies
verfasst von
Isabelle Mackrous
Luc Proteau
Publikationsdatum
01.11.2007
Verlag
Springer-Verlag
Erschienen in
Experimental Brain Research / Ausgabe 2/2007
Print ISSN: 0014-4819
Elektronische ISSN: 1432-1106
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1031-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2007

Experimental Brain Research 2/2007 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Schützt Olivenöl vor dem Tod durch Demenz?

10.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Konsumieren Menschen täglich 7 Gramm Olivenöl, ist ihr Risiko, an einer Demenz zu sterben, um mehr als ein Viertel reduziert – und dies weitgehend unabhängig von ihrer sonstigen Ernährung. Dafür sprechen Auswertungen zweier großer US-Studien.

Bluttest erkennt Parkinson schon zehn Jahre vor der Diagnose

10.05.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Ein Bluttest kann abnorm aggregiertes Alpha-Synuclein bei einigen Menschen schon zehn Jahre vor Beginn der motorischen Parkinsonsymptome nachweisen. Mit einem solchen Test lassen sich möglicherweise Prodromalstadien erfassen und die Betroffenen früher behandeln.

Darf man die Behandlung eines Neonazis ablehnen?

08.05.2024 Gesellschaft Nachrichten

In einer Leseranfrage in der Zeitschrift Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology möchte ein anonymer Dermatologe bzw. eine anonyme Dermatologin wissen, ob er oder sie einen Patienten behandeln muss, der eine rassistische Tätowierung trägt.

Wartezeit nicht kürzer, aber Arbeit flexibler

Psychotherapie Medizin aktuell

Fünf Jahren nach der Neugestaltung der Psychotherapie-Richtlinie wurden jetzt die Effekte der vorgenommenen Änderungen ausgewertet. Das Hauptziel der Novellierung war eine kürzere Wartezeit auf Therapieplätze. Dieses Ziel wurde nicht erreicht, es gab jedoch positive Auswirkungen auf andere Bereiche.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.