Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Brain Structure and Function 2/2013

Open Access 01.03.2013 | Original Article

Cytoarchitectonical analysis and probabilistic mapping of two extrastriate areas of the human posterior fusiform gyrus

verfasst von: Julian Caspers, Karl Zilles, Simon B. Eickhoff, Axel Schleicher, Hartmut Mohlberg, Katrin Amunts

Erschienen in: Brain Structure and Function | Ausgabe 2/2013

Abstract

The human extrastriate visual cortex comprises numerous functionally defined areas, which are not identified in the widely used cytoarchitectonical map of Brodmann. The ventral part of the extrastriate cortex is particularly devoted to the identification of visual objects, faces and word forms. We analyzed the region immediately antero-lateral to hOc4v in serially sectioned (20 μm) and cell body-stained human brains using a quantitative observer-independent cytoarchitectonical approach to further identify the anatomical organization of the extrastriate cortex. Two novel cytoarchitectonical areas, FG1 and FG2, were identified on the posterior fusiform gyrus. The results of ten postmortem brains were then registered to their MRI volumes (acquired before histological processing), 3D reconstructed, and spatially normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute reference brain. Finally, probabilistic maps were generated for each cytoarchitectonical area by superimposing the areas of the individual brains in the reference space. Comparison with recent functional imaging studies yielded that both areas are located within the object-related visual cortex. FG1 fills the gap between the retinotopically mapped area VO-1 and a posterior fusiform face patch. FG2 is probably the correlate of this face patch.

Introduction

The human visual cortex can be divided into the primary visual or striate cortex (V1 or BA17) (Brodmann 1909) and the adjoining extrastriate cortex. The extrastriate cortex covers the largest part of the occipital lobe and extends into the posterior parietal and temporal regions. Here, two processing pathways have been described, the dorsal pathway or “where”-stream for spatial localization and visually guided action, and the ventral pathway or “what”-stream, which is involved in object, color and shape recognition (Mishkin and Ungerleider 1982; Ungerleider and Haxby 1994; Eickhoff et al. 2008). During the last decades, a high degree of functional heterogeneity within the ventral visual cortex has been revealed by functional imaging, giving evidence for several functionally specialized areas, e.g. the fusiform face area (FFA, Kanwisher et al. 1997; Grill-Spector et al. 2004; Kanwisher and Yovel 2006; Weiner and Grill-Spector 2010), the parahippocampal place area (PPA, Epstein et al. 1999; Epstein 2008), the visual word-form area (VWFA, Cohen et al. 2000; Wandell et al. 2012) or the extrastriate body area (EBA, Downing et al. 2001; Peelen and Downing 2005; Weiner and Grill-Spector 2011b). Combined functional and cytoarchitectonical studies of extrastriate areas co-registered in the same reference space are rare (Wohlschläger et al. 2005; Wilms et al. 2010).
Conventionally, four criteria have been used to distinguish the visual areas: (1) retinotopy, (2) functional properties, (3) histology and (4) intracortical connections (Clarke and Miklossy 1990; Felleman and van Essen 1991; Sereno et al. 1995; Zilles and Clarke 1997; Tootell et al. 2003; Kolster et al. 2010). The classical anatomical maps from the beginning of the last century (Brodmann 1909; von Economo and Koskinas 1925; Sarkisov et al. 1949) have several disadvantages when attempting to establish functional–structural relationships: Most classical anatomical maps only show a principal tripartition of the visual cortex with V1 (BA17/OC), V2 (BA18/OB) followed by a single large area (BA19/OA) (Brodmann 1909; von Economo and Koskinas 1925) (Fig. 1). Functionally and also histologically, however, it becomes evident that BA19/OA comprises a variety of functional and cytoarchitectonical areas within its ventral and dorsal parts (Zeki 1969; van Essen 1979; Braak 1980; Tootell et al. 1996; Zilles and Clarke 1997; Orban et al. 2004; Malikovic et al. 2007; Rottschy et al. 2007). Moreover, all classical anatomical maps are reported as schematic 2D hand drawings of the cortical surface, which rarely provide information about cortical areas within the sulci and contain no information about stereotaxic location or interindividual variability of cortical areas. Finally, these maps are established on the basis of subjective criteria for the definition of cortical borders.
These shortcomings have prompted the development of probabilistic cytoarchitectonical maps (Amunts and Zilles 2001; Zilles et al. 2002; Zilles and Amunts 2010). The maps are based on the observer independent, statistically testable analysis of postmortem brains (Schleicher et al. 2005, 2009), and contain information about stereotaxic position and intersubject variability of cortical areas in the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) reference space (Evans et al. 1992). The maps help to probabilistically identify the cytoarchitectonical correlate of neuroimaging activations seen in functional MRI, positron emission tomography (PET), magneto- and electroencephalography (MEG/EEG) using standard analyses packages (Eickhoff et al. 2005; Zilles and Amunts 2010). As part of this atlasing project, probabilistic maps of the visual cortex have already been defined for the primary visual area hOc1 (BA17) (Amunts et al. 2000), for secondary hOc2 (BA18) (Amunts et al. 2000), and for tertiary hOc3v, hOc4v (Rottschy et al. 2007) and hOc5 (Malikovic et al. 2007).
Since cytoarchitectonical maps of the ventral visual areas anterior to hOc4v are presently not available, the aim of our study was to investigate the structural organization of this region on the posterior fusiform gyrus.

Materials and methods

Histological processing

Ten human postmortem brains (Amunts et al. 2000), five males and five females (Table 1), were obtained from the body donor program of the Institute of Anatomy, University of Düsseldorf. One brain came from a subject with transitory motor disturbance (brain 3), all other donors had no history of neurological or psychiatric diseases. Handedness of the subjects was unknown. We may assume that most of the subjects were right-handed supposing a prevalence of left-handedness of <10 % in the general population (Annett 1973). The brains were removed from the skulls within 8–24 h after death and fixated in 4 % formalin or Bodian’s fixative for at least 6 months. To document brain size and shape before the inevitable distortions by histological processing occurred, each brain was scanned using a T1 weighted 3D-FLASH sequence (flip angle 40°, TR = 40 ms, TE = 5 ms) implemented on a Siemens 1.5T scanner (Erlangen, Germany). Subsequently, the complete brains were embedded in paraffin and serially cut into coronal sections of 20 μm thickness. Every 15th section was mounted on glass slides and silver stained for cell bodies (Merker 1983) to achieve a high contrast between darkly stained neuronal perikarya and unstained neuropil. Every fourth stained section, i.e. every 60th section of the series of sections, was examined, resulting in a distance of 1.2 mm between the analyzed sections (Fig. 2).
Table 1
List of the ten postmortem brains
Case
Age (years)
Gender
Cause of death
Fresh weight (g)
1
79
F
Carcinoma of the bladder
1,350
2
56
M
Rectal carcinoma
1,270
3
69
M
Vascular disease
1,360
4
75
M
Acute glomerulonephritis
1,349
5
59
F
Cardiorespiratory insufficiency
1,142
6
54
M
Cardiac infarction
1,622
7
37
M
Cardiac arrest
1,437
8
72
F
Renal arrest
1,216
9
79
F
Cardiorespiratory insufficiency
1,110
10
85
F
Mesenteric infarction
1,046

Detection of cortical borders

Cytoarchitectonical analysis was performed using a quantitative method for observer independent and statistically testable detection of cortical borders (Zilles et al. 2002; Schleicher et al. 2005, 2009). First, rectangular regions of interest covering the posterior fusiform gyrus and neighboring cortex were defined in the histological sections and digitized using a microscope with a scanning stage (KS400; Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany) and a CCD camera (Sony, Tokyo, Japan; resolution 1.01 × 1.01 μm²/pixel) (Fig. 2b). The digitized sections were then transformed into gray level index (GLI) images, in which pixel values represent the volume fraction of cell bodies in the corresponding square measuring fields of 20 × 20 μm² each (Schleicher et al. 2000, 2005). If the GLI amounts to 20 %, for example, 80 % of the volume in a measuring field are occupied by neuropil (dendrites, axons, processes of glial cells and blood vessels).
Equidistant GLI profiles were extracted along curvilinear trajectories oriented perpendicular to the cortical layers and running from an interactively defined outer contour between layer I and layer II to an inner contour between layer VI and the white matter (Fig. 2c). These profiles represent the course of the regional cell density from superficial (outer contour) to deep (inner contour). To compensate for variations in cortical thickness, we resampled each profile with linear interpolation to a standard length corresponding to a cortical thickness of 100 %. The shape of each GLI profile was quantified by a vector consisting of ten features based on central moments, which were also used for previous cytoarchitectonical studies (e.g. Amunts et al. 2000; Amunts and Zilles 2001; Zilles et al. 2002). These features were: the mean GLI value, the position of the center of gravity on the profile curve (cortical depth), the standard deviation of the mean GLI (indicating the variability of the GLI throughout all layers), skewness and kurtosis of the profile curve and the respective features from the profile’s first derivative (Schleicher et al. 1999).
Differences in shape between GLI profiles indicate differences in cytoarchitecture and were quantified as the Mahalanobis distance (Mahalanobis et al. 1949; Bartels 1979) between the respective feature vectors of neighboring blocks of profiles (Schleicher et al. 2005) at every position along the cortical ribbon (Fig. 3). To assure reliability, the procedure is repeated for different block sizes ranging from 8 to 24 profiles per block. Areal borders are expected at positions where the distance function shows local maxima corresponding to a great dissimilarity in laminar pattern between adjacent blocks of profiles. These maxima were detected and their significance evaluated by the Hotelling’s T² test with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (Fig. 3b). Cortical borders were confirmed, if they were consistently present at the same position across several block sizes, and if the positions were found at comparable sites in adjacent sections.

Computation of probabilistic cytoarchitectonical maps

3D reconstructions of the histological volumes were computed using the following three datasets: (i) the previously ascertained 3D-MRI scan, (ii) images of the paraffin block face obtained during sectioning for the precise alignment of the histological sections and (iii) the digitized images of the cell body-stained sections (Amunts et al. 2004).
The defined borders of the cortical areas were interactively traced on the corresponding sections of the 3D-reconstructed brains. The histological volumes were then spatially normalized by registration to the stereotaxic space of the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) (Evans et al. 1992) using a combination of affine transformations and nonlinear elastic registration (Hömke 2006). To keep the anterior commissure as the origin of the coordinate system and for consistency with previous cytoarchitectonical studies (e.g. Amunts et al. 2005; Rottschy et al. 2007; Caspers et al. 2008; Scheperjans et al. 2008; Kurth et al. 2010), data were shifted by 4 mm caudally (y-axis) and 5 mm dorsally (z-axis) to the ‘anatomical MNI-space’ (Amunts et al. 2005). Corresponding areas of the ten brains were superimposed and a probabilistic map was generated for each area (Zilles et al. 2002). These maps indicate for each voxel of the reference brain the relative frequency with which a respective area was found at that position (Eickhoff et al. 2007; Zilles and Amunts 2010).
The probability maps of all areas in the ventral visual cortex were visualized as a continuous, non-overlapping map of this region by generating a maximum probability map (MPM). The MPM was computed by comparing the probabilities of all areas in each single voxel and by assigning to each voxel the cytoarchitectonical area with the highest probability (Eickhoff et al. 2005, 2006). If two or more areas showed equal probabilities in a single voxel, this voxel was assigned to the area with the highest average probability of the directly adjacent voxels.

Analysis of volumes

The volumes of the cytoarchitectonical areas were calculated for each hemisphere separately based on area measurements in the individual histological sections, section thickness and distance between the measured sections as well as the shrinkage factor of each brain. Shrinkage factors were determined as the ratio between the fresh volumes of the brains and their volumes after histological processing (Amunts et al. 2005). The volumetric data were then analyzed for interindividual and interhemispheric differences using a repeated measurement analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the following design: between-subject factor: gender; within-subject factors: area and side; blocking factor: subject.

Results

Two cytoarchitectonically distinct areas, FG1 and FG2, were identified antero-lateral to hOc4v (Rottschy et al. 2007) on the posterior fusiform gyrus (Fig. 4). The term “FG” was used for “fusiform gyrus”. This allows a neutral, macroanatomical nomenclature. The different areas of the fusiform gyrus were labeled by numbers instead of more descriptive labels like “medial” or “lateral”. This avoids future inconsistencies, as the total number of cytoarchitectonical fusiform areas and their relative spatial arrangement are not yet known. Since the cytoarchitectonical areas described here were not previously identified, any assignment to areas of Brodmann (1909) or von Economo and Koskinas (1925) was not meaningful. Asterisks behind the names of two other areas (col.s.* and l.ot.s.*) indicate that these areas were not completely mapped in the present observation.
The more medial area FG1 was located immediately lateral to the rostral portion of area hOc4v. It was found on the medial half of the posterior part of the fusiform gyrus and extended on the lateral bank of the collateral sulcus. FG2 was located lateral to FG1 on the lateral half of the fusiform gyrus and on the lateral occipitotemporal sulcus. Rarely, minor parts of FG2 reached onto the inferior temporal gyrus. FG2 extended more rostrally than FG1. The rostral part of FG2 covered nearly the full width of the fusiform gyrus.

Cytoarchitecture

Both areas belong to the homotypical isocortex with an inner granular layer IV but with further distinctive cytoarchitectonical features:
FG1 was characterized mainly by a marked columnar arrangement of small pyramidal cells and a lower cell density in layer IV (Fig. 5) when compared to neighboring areas. Layer II showed a rather low density of cells and smooth transition into a layer III of low to moderate cell density. The sizes of pyramidal cells in layer III were small with no superficial to deep increase. These pyramidal cells were arranged in clearly visible cell columns, a feature that was unique to FG1 compared to the adjacent areas. Layer IV of FG1 was thin, but clearly delineable from adjacent layers IIIc and V. Pyramidal cells in layer V were small and showed a columnar arrangement as well. The cell sparse layer VI had no conspicuous border with lower layer V. The border between cortex and white matter was blurred.
FG2 showed large pyramidal cells in lower layer III and a prominent layer IV as characteristic features (Fig. 5). A columnar arrangement of pyramidal cells was not found, neither in layer III nor in layer V. Layer II of FG2 had a rather high cell density with a clearly delineable border to the cell sparse layer IIIa. Large, typical pyramidal cells of layer III were primarily found in sublayers IIIb and IIIc with a slight increase of cell sizes in deep sublayer IIIc. This sublayer often merged with the broad and cell dense layer IV. Layer V consisted of equally distributed pyramidal cells, which were smaller than those in lower layer III but larger than those of FG1. Layer V was clearly delineable from the cell dense layer VI, which again had a distinct border to the white matter.
To demonstrate the variability of these cytoarchitectonical features of FG1 and FG2 along the rostro-caudal extent, Fig. 6 illustrates three representative sections of a single brain. The intersubject variability of cytoarchitectonical features is illustrated by sections of three different brains (Fig. 7). Figures 6 and 7 highlight that the main characteristic features of each area, e.g. visible or not visible columnar arrangement of pyramidal cells, prominent or inconspicuous subdivisions of layer III, and width and cell density in layer IV, could be found as distincitive features at all sectioning levels and in all brains studied.
FG1 and FG2 differed not only by their cytoarchitecture (Fig. 8a) but could also be separated from adjoining cortical areas (see Table 2 for an overview):
Table 2
Cytoarchitectonical features of FG1, FG2 and the adjoining areas
  
hOc4v
col.s.*
FG1
FG2
l.ot.s.*
II
Cell density
●●●
●●
●●
●●●
●●
III
Pyramidal size
●●
●●
●●
●●●
 
Pyramidal arrangement
Superf. to deep increase
Deep IIIc
Cell columns
IIIb and IIIc
IIIc
IV
Width
●●
●●
●●●
●●
 
Cell density
●●●
●●
●●●
V
Cell density
●●
●●
●●
VI
Cell density
●●●
●●
●●●
Feature intensity is coded with black dots: ●●● high, ●● medium and ● low. The area on the collateral sulcus medially adjoining FG1 is labeled with col.s.*, the area laterally adjoining FG2 is labeled with l.ot.s*. An asterisk indicates that these areas are not mapped in detail
Posteriorly, FG1 immediately adjoined hOc4v, which was characterized by a more cell dense layer II with a well-defined border to layer III (Fig. 8b). This latter layer showed larger pyramidal cells with a characteristic superficial to deep increase of cell sizes and no clear arrangement in cell columns. Layer IV provided the most pronounced distinction as it was prominent and had a higher cell density in hOc4v when compared to FG1. Cells in layer V were also larger in hOc4v but not densely packed. Layer VI of hOc4v again showed a higher cell density and a better defined border to the white matter.
In the more rostral parts, FG1 was followed medially by a new, not further analyzed area col.s.*, which was found anterior to hOc4v (Fig. 8c). col.s.* showed medium-sized and densely packed pyramidal cells in layer III, particularly in sublayer IIIc, with no columnar arrangement. Layer IV of col.s.* was more cell dense and slightly thicker than that of FG1, layer V showed a higher cell density, and layer VI had a distinct border to the white matter.
The likewise not yet completely mapped area l.ot.s.* was found lateral to FG2. Area l.ot.s.* extended into the lateral occipitotemporal sulcus and on the inferior occiptal and inferior temporal gyrus. l.ot.s.* showed cell sparse sublayers IIIa and IIIb with large pyramidal cells only in lower IIIc (Fig. 8d). Layer IV of this area, was less prominent than in FG2 and difficult to delineate from layer III. The cell density of layer V in l.ot.s.* was lower than in FG2, and the border between layer V and layer VI was blurred.

Volumes and stereotaxic location

The statistical analysis of the histological volumes of FG1 and FG2 (Table 3) revealed a significant size difference between both areas (F = 25.547, p = 0.01) in favor of FG2. There were no significant effects of the factors side (F = 2.990, p = 0.122), gender (F = 1.784, p = 0.218) and no significant effects of the interactions between area and side (F = 0.011, p = 0.920), between area and gender (F = 0.254, p = 0.628), between side and gender (F = 0.380, p = 0.555) and between area, side and gender (F = 0.001, p = 0.981).
Table 3
Histological volumes (mm³) of the areas in a sample of ten brains, corrected for shrinkage
 
Mean
SD
FG1
 Left hemisphere
1,091
333
 Right hemisphere
886
322
FG2
 Left hemisphere
1,617
554
 Right hemisphere
1,430
575
Volumes did not differ between the hemispheres (p > 0.05)
The coordinates for the centers of gravity of areas FG1 and FG2 in anatomical MNI space are given in Table 4. A slight difference of the anterior–posterior location was noted as both areas were located more rostrally in the right hemisphere. This interhemispheric shift has already been reported for other striate and extrastriate areas in previous studies (Amunts et al. 2000; Rottschy et al. 2007), and reflects the marked asymmetry in position of the occipital poles in the MNI single-subject template.
Table 4
Coordinates of the centers of gravity in anatomical MNI space for the probability maps  (PMap) and the maximum probability map (MPM) of areas FG1 and FG2
 
x
y
z
FG1
 Left hemisphere
  PMap
−32.3
−76.3
−8.8
  MPM
−29.7
−75.6
−8.6
 Right hemisphere
  PMap
33.6
−73.6
−10.1
  MPM
33.4
−73.1
−10.8
FG2
 Left hemisphere
  PMap
−42.7
−72.3
−12.6
  MPM
−41.2
−74.3
−12.3
 Right hemisphere
  PMap
42.0
−70.7
−13.3
  MPM
42.1
−72.1
−13.7

Probability map and maximum probability map

Probability maps (PMaps) were generated for each FG area by superimposing the spatially normalized representations of the individual subjects (Fig. 9). The PMaps reflect the probability of observing the respective area in single voxels of the MNI space in our sample of postmortem brains. The probability maps for FG1 and FG2 barely showed regions with high probabilities for one area (overlap of 9 or 10 brains), but broad regions with low probabilities (overlap of 1 or 2 brains) in the periphery of each of the FG areas. This reflects a high intersubject variability of both areas, which is much higher compared to early visual areas hOc1 and hOc2 (Amunts et al. 2000).
Consequently, there was substantial overlap between the probabilistic maps of FG1 and FG2 and also adjoining area hOc4v. To assign the most likely area to each single voxel, an MPM of the visual cortex (comprising areas hOc1, hOc2, hOc3v, hOc4v, FG1 and FG2) was computed (Fig. 10). The MPM represents a contiguous, non-overlapping parcellation of this region. It hence bears a close conceptual resemblance to presentations in classical brain maps. Importantly, however, the MPM does not show the parceling of an exemplary or “typical” hemisphere as do previous architectonic brain maps (Brodmann 1909; von Economo and Koskinas 1925), but reflects the most likely area based on a sample of ten brains and represented in each voxel of the reference space.

Discussion

This study reports the microscopical features and stereotaxic locations of two previously unknown cytoarchitectonical areas, FG1 and FG2, of the ventral human visual cortex. Our mapping study used quantitative, statistically testable cytoarchitectonical criteria (Schleicher et al. 2005), to overcome uncertainties caused by pure visual inspection (Brodmann 1909; von Economo and Koskinas 1925; Sarkisov et al. 1949) and intersubject variability.
FG1 is located on the medial half of the posterior fusiform gyrus and extends rostrally on the lateral bank of the collateral sulcus. FG2 is located lateral to FG1 on the lateral half of the fusiform gyrus reaching into the lateral occipitotemporal sulcus.
An interpretation of the cytoarchitectonical areas described here should be based on a comparison with previously proposed cytoarchitectonical maps and results of recent functional imaging research. It should be noted, however, that a simple comparison of center of gravity coordinates is problematic because of tremendous divergences between the coordinates in the different functional imaging reports [e.g. Bartels and Zeki (2004) report the center of the FFA at L: [−44, −46, −24]/R: [44, −46, −26], while Spiridon et al. (2006) report L: [−50.1, −69.2, −7.5]/R: [31.3, −55.8, −5.9], both labeled “Talairach space”). The divergences may be caused by the usage of different reference spaces, since the terms “MNI space” or “Talairach space” are not sufficiently clear definitions in many studies. Thus, the stereotaxic coordinates of the same cortical site can vary considerably between the different studies even if the same definition, e.g. “MNI space” was used, depending on the precise position of the CA-CP-line (CA = commissura anterior, CP = commissura posterior) and other aspects as shown in detail by Lancaster et al. (2007). Due to this remarkable lack of comparability between studies using seemingly identical reference space, the purely coordinate-based correlation between cytoarchitectonical and functional imaging data coming from different studies is problematic, particularly if positions of cortical areas have to be compared. In this case, minimal differences in the position of the CA-CP-line lead to rather large deviations in location of the rather remote cortical areas. Therefore, a comparison of stereotaxic coordinates has to be critically weighted against a combination of topographic descriptions, neighborhood relations and reported illustrations.

Comparison with previous architectonic maps

Classical anatomical maps of the human brain show a tripartition of the visual cortex (Brodmann 1909; von Economo and Koskinas 1925; Sarkisov et al. 1949). Brodmann (1909) mentioned that his temporal area BA37, which adjoins BA19 in its ventral parts, is a transitional area between temporal and occipital regions. Von Economo and Koskinas (1925) found a roughly comparable area to BA37 adjoining their area OA, which can be seen as an equivalent to BA19. They describe the cytoarchitecture of this adjoining area as highly heterogeneous but were not able to delimit clearly defined subregions of this area. Because they saw primarily cytoarchitectonical characteristics of the parietal lobe, they assigned this area to the parietal region and labeled it “area parietalis temporooccipitalis” or PH. Whereas the classical architectonical definitions of the primary and secondary visual cortex (BA17 and BA18) are fully agreed by most observers (Amunts et al. 2000), it became evident by functional neuroimaging and non-human primate data that BA19/OA should be subdivided into multiple functionally and putatively also architectonically distinct regions (Zeki 1969; van Essen 1979; Braak 1980; Tootell et al. 1996). It seems conceivable that FG1 and FG2 are situated within the border region between BA19 and BA37 or OA and PH, respectively.
Detailed maps of the occipital and adjacent temporo-parietal lobe were published by Braak (1977) in his pigmentoarchitectonical study. His report included drawings of identified areas on the cortical surface as well as coronal sections, which makes a comparison less difficult. FG1 and FG2 topographically fit to Braak’s (1977) “area peristriata densopyramidalis”. Its description as a “well-developed bitaeniate cortex with conspicuous pIIIc” (“bitaeniate” = “double-striped”) is likewise in accordance with our observations on the cytoarchitecture of area FG2. Braak (1977), however, identified a single area in this region. Our results indicate clear cytoarchitectonical differences between two areas and hence a sub-parcellation of “area peristriata densopyramidalis” into FG1 and FG2.

Comparison with functional imaging data

Retinotopy is a common organizational principle of early visual areas in the primate visual cortex (e.g. Sereno et al. 1995; DeYoe et al. 1996; Engel et al. 1997). Utilizing this principle, retinotopic mapping has emerged to the gold standard in functional imaging of these early visual areas and yielded robust results for the borders of areas V1, V2 and V3v (Tootell et al. 1998; Wade et al. 2002; Wohlschläger et al. 2005).
However, there has been an intense debate during the last decade concerning the anterior border of human area V4. One view proposed an area V4v, representing the (contralateral) upper-quarterfield, followed anteriorly by a color-selective area, containing a complete hemifield representation termed V8 (Hadjikhani et al. 1998; Tootell and Hadjikhani 2001). This arrangement was challenged by others, who postulated V3v to be the most anterior quarterfield representation, which is then followed by a hemifield representation selective for color perception. They labeled this area the V4-complex, containing V4 and an anterior non-retinotopic subarea V4α. The authors did not see any evidence for a quarterfield representation like in V4v (Bartels and Zeki 1998, 2000; Zeki 2001). A third scheme contained an upper field representation, analogous to V4v, followed anteriorly by its matching lower field termed human V4 (hV4). The authors argued for yet another color-selective hemifield representation, VO-1 anterior to it (Wade et al. 2002; Wandell et al. 2005; Brewer et al. 2005).
It should be mentioned that the hV4/VO-1 model does not contradict the V4-complex model, but can be harmonized with it, while the V8 model cannot (Wade et al. 2002). Furthermore, the hV4/VO-1 model was later confirmed by other laboratories (Kastner et al. 2001; Larsson et al. 2006; Arcaro et al. 2009; Kolster et al. 2010). Our study shows two distinct cytoarchitectonical areas FG1 and FG2 antero-lateral to the cytoarchitectonical area hOc4v (Rottschy et al. 2007), which overlaps with retinotopically defined hV4 (Wilms et al. 2010). The descriptions of VO-1 as an area in the collateral sulcus and on the medial fusiform gyrus (Brewer et al. 2005; Liu and Wandell 2005) would topographically fit to FG1. However, the apparently closer proximity of VO-1 to the collateral sulcus and its location directly anterior to hV4 as well as the reported coordinates (Kastner et al. 2001; Brewer et al. 2005; Liu and Wandell 2005; Arcaro et al. 2009) suggest that VO-1 is more medial than FG1 and probably correlates with the medially adjoining cytoarchitectonical area col.s.* (Fig. 8c).
Recently, another retinotopic region has been discovered lateral to hV4, i.e. the phPIT cluster (Kolster et al. 2010), which refers to macaque areas PITd and PITv on the posterior inferotemporal gyrus (Felleman and van Essen 1991). The phPIT cluster is located on the inferior temporal gyrus at the posterior end of the lateral occipitotemporal sulcus and consists of two hemifield representations, phPITd and phPITv, which share their foveal representation and vertical meridians. The lateral of our identified cytoarchitectonical areas, FG2, is located on the lateral bank of the posterior fusiform gyrus and within the posterior lateral occipitotemporal sulcus. FG2 rarely reaches the inferior temporal gyrus. Thus, the topography and the center of gravity coordinates indicate that phPIT is located posterior and dorsal to FG2, and most probably overlaps with the laterally adjoining cortex (Fig. 8d).
Taken together, the comparison with current retinotopic literature indicates that FG1 and FG2 do not correspond to any hitherto identified retinotopic area. Instead, both cytoarchitectonical areas seem to fill in the “non-retinotopic gap” that is spanned between VO-1 medially and phPITv laterally (see Kolster et al. 2010, Fig. 16A). A minor peripheral overlap between these retinotopic and our cytoarchitectonical areas cannot be completely ruled out. However, such a mismatch between cytoarchitectonically and retinotopically defined cortical units would be in contrast to the correlation of both parcellation approaches in other cortical areas, e.g. in V1 and V2 (Wohlschläger et al. 2005).
Besides its retinotopic organization, the human ventral visual cortex contains a number of apparently category-specific functional modules for visual object processing. Some regions that respond more strongly to visual objects than to scrambled images were identified around the posterior fusiform gyrus and the inferior occipital gyrus. They are called the “lateral occipital complex” (LOC, Malach et al. 1995, 2002; Kanwisher et al. 1996; Grill-Spector et al. 2001). Bilateral LOC-activations receive input from both hemifields (Grill-Spector et al. 1998) and are correlated with recognition performance of objects (Grill-Spector et al. 2000). Conventionally, the LOC comprises two entities, the dorsal “LO” and the ventral and anterior “pFs” on the posterior and mid-fusiform gyrus, which differ in their response to size and position changes of presented objects (Grill-Spector et al. 1999). A later scheme assigned pFs to an object-selective cluster “VOT” of the ventral occipitotemporal cortex directly adjoining early retinotopic visual areas (Malach et al. 2002). In consideration of the pertinent reports of the LOC, a direct correspondence of our areas FG1 and FG2 to a LOC-cluster seems to be unlikely, as they are located between both classical clusters, ventral to LO and posterior to pFs. Indeed, the center of gravity of FG2 is in close proximity to the coordinates of the “branching point” between both LOC-clusters described by Malach et al. (1995). Thus, an overlap of the margins of LOC with FG2 is possible. Based on the VOT-scheme predicting object-responsive areas directly adjacent to early visual areas (Malach et al. 2002), it can be hypothesized that FG1 and FG2 are both situated within the higher order object-related cortex.
Additional to the LOC and partially overlapping with it, some areas selective for specific objects have been identified within the ventral occipitotemporal cortex including the prototypical example of the “fusiform face area” (FFA). First hints on the existence of the FFA were derived from studies on subjects with prosopagnosia. These subjects showed a bilateral or right-hemispheric lesion in the ventral occipitotemporal cortex (Damasio et al. 1982; De Renzi 1986; Landis et al. 1986; Sergent and Signoret 1992). Later PET studies revealed a distinct activation on the fusiform gyrus during face perception tasks in healthy volunteers (Sergent et al. 1992; Haxby et al. 1994). The face specificity and location of the FFA could be demonstrated at higher spatial resolution in numerous fMRI studies (Clark et al. 1996; Puce et al. 1996; Kanwisher et al. 1997; Tong et al. 1998; Halgren et al. 1999; Hasson et al. 2001; Grill-Spector et al. 2004; Kanwisher and Yovel 2006). In addition, a response to images of headless bodies was reported on the fusiform gyrus in the vicinity of the FFA (Cox et al. 2004; Peelen and Downing 2005). High resolution fMRI identified this activation as adjacent yet distinct from the FFA in the fusiform body area (FBA, Schwarzlose et al. 2005). Most functional investigations locate the FFA on the lateral bank of the posterior or mid-fusiform gyrus, about 1–2 cm anterior to the here identified cytoarchitectonical area FG2 (e.g. Gauthier et al. 2000; Hasson et al. 2001; Levy et al. 2001; Avidan et al. 2003; Rossion et al. 2003; Grill-Spector et al. 2004; Bartels and Zeki 2004; Peelen and Downing 2005). However, some reports show peak activations much closer to FG2 (Puce et al. 1996; Kanwisher et al. 1997; Halgren et al. 1999; Ishai et al. 1999). This discrepancy might be explained by a recent investigation of the fusiform gyrus demonstrating a subdivision of the FFA into a posterior and an anterior part (Weiner and Grill-Spector 2010, 2011a), which was also suggested by others (Pinsk et al. 2009; Mei et al. 2010). The posterior face patch named pFus-faces accurately matches the location of our lateral cytoarchitectonical area FG2 situated on the lateral bank of the posterior fusiform gyrus antero-lateral to hV4 and, hence, might be its functional correlate.
Another functional, category-specific area, which is located within the lateral occipitotemporal sulcus extending onto the lateral fusiform gyrus is the visual word-form area (VWFA), which responds specifically to words and letter strings. First hints came from patients suffering from lesions in the ventral occipitotemporal cortex and respective neuropsychological deficits, i.e. pure alexia (Damasio and Damasio 1983; Binder and Mohr 1992). This functionally defined area could later be localized by PET (Petersen et al. 1990; Petersen and Fiez 1993), MEG (Tarkiainen et al. 1999) and fMRI (Wagner et al. 1998; Cohen et al. 2000; Hasson et al. 2002; Dehaene et al. 2002), although the functional specificity of VWFA was also controversially discussed (Price and Devlin 2003). Similar to our area FG2, the VWFA was first described to be located on the lateral fusiform gyrus (Cohen et al. 2000; Dehaene et al. 2002), but more recent findings show that the large portion of VWFA lies within the fundus of the lateral occipitotemporal sulcus and about 1 cm anterior to FG2 (Cohen and Dehaene 2004; Baker et al. 2007; Ben-Shachar et al. 2007; Wandell et al. 2012). An overlap of FG2 with language-related visual areas is possible, since FG2 largely extends into the lateral occipitotemporal sulcus and distances of FG2 to the center of gravity of VWFA are quite small (e.g. Cohen et al. 2002; Vigneau et al. 2005; Dehaene et al. 2010; Mei et al. 2010). Moreover, the processing of words seems to continuously extend from early visual areas to the ventral occipitotemporal locations in a hierarchical manner (Dehaene et al. 2005; Szwed et al. 2011), where FG2 could possibly be involved at an intermediate stage.
Our results indicate that FG1 and FG2 are symmetrically found in all ten brains, showing the same cytoarchitectonical features on both sides and no significant left–right differences in volume. By contrast, the majority of reports show a right lateralization of the FFA that apparently depends on handedness (Willems et al. 2010), while the VWFA is most often left lateralized (e.g. Cohen and Dehaene 2004). This does not exclude a possible overlap of the functional and our cytoarchitectonical areas, since different functional manifestations can be implemented on the same cytoarchitectonical basis. Furthermore, recent investigations imply that lateralization for faces (Weiner and Grill-Spector 2010) and words (Ben-Shachar et al. 2007) is possibly much less pronounced as hitherto assumed.
Thus, our cytoarchitectonical areas FG1 and FG2 probably lie within the object-related higher order visual cortex on the posterior fusiform gyrus. Furthermore, FG2 likely comprises a posterior fusiform face-selective patch. However, distinct functional correlates in this region are rare. This issue might also be affected by the fMRI artifacts evoked by the transverse sinus, a venous vessel, which commonly proceeds directly inferior to the cortex we investigated here (Winawer et al. 2010). Appropriately, Winawer et al. (2010) denote this region as ‘No man’s land’. A more specific-functional characterization of FG1 and FG2 and the relation to object, face and visual word processing remains a topic for future work. It can certainly be encouraged by further improved functional imaging techniques and comprehensive approaches including cytoarchitectonical, retinotopic and category-related investigations. For now, we provide probability maps that can be used to relate functional measurements to our cytoarchitectonical delineations and, hence, can help to understand the functional role of this region rostrally adjoining the early ventral visual cortex.

Conclusions

The present study detected two novel, cytoarchitectonically distinct areas on the human posterior fusiform gyrus, using a quantitative microscopical approach. The delineation of the extrastriate areas is used to provide probabilistic maps in standardized 3D-stereotaxic space. The maps allow a comparison of functional imaging data on object, face and word-form recognition with its putative cytoarchitectonical correlates. Comparisons between our map and previous functional imaging studies suggest that FG1 and FG2 are situated in a not yet retinotopically mapped gap between VO-1 and phPITv whereas FG2 is the cytoarchitectonical correlate of the posterior face patch pFus-faces.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge funding by the Initiative and Networking Fund of the Helmholtz Association within the Helmholtz Alliance on Systems Biology (Human Brain Model; K.Z., S.B.E.), the DFG (IRTG 1328, S.B.E., K.A.) and the Helmholtz Alliance for Mental Health in an Aging Society (HelMA; K.Z., K.A.).

Open Access

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License which permits any use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and the source are credited.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 2.0 International License (https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Neurologie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes, den Premium-Inhalten der neurologischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Neurologie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Weitere Produktempfehlungen anzeigen
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Amunts K, Zilles K (2001) Advances in cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human cerebral cortex. Neuroimaging Clin N Am 11(2):151–169PubMed Amunts K, Zilles K (2001) Advances in cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human cerebral cortex. Neuroimaging Clin N Am 11(2):151–169PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Amunts K, Weiss PH, Mohlberg H, Pieperhoff P, Eickhoff S, Gurd JM, Marshall JC, Shah NJ, Fink GR, Zilles K (2004) Analysis of neural mechanisms underlying verbal fluency in cytoarchitectonically defined stereotaxic space—the roles of Brodmann areas 44 and 45. Neuroimage 22(1):42–56. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.031 PubMedCrossRef Amunts K, Weiss PH, Mohlberg H, Pieperhoff P, Eickhoff S, Gurd JM, Marshall JC, Shah NJ, Fink GR, Zilles K (2004) Analysis of neural mechanisms underlying verbal fluency in cytoarchitectonically defined stereotaxic space—the roles of Brodmann areas 44 and 45. Neuroimage 22(1):42–56. doi:10.​1016/​j.​neuroimage.​2003.​12.​031 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Amunts K, Kedo O, Kindler M, Pieperhoff P, Mohlberg H, Shah NJ, Habel U, Schneider F, Zilles K (2005) Cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human amygdala, hippocampal region and entorhinal cortex: intersubject variability and probability maps. Anat Embryol (Berl) 210(5–6):343–352. doi:10.1007/s00429-005-0025-5 CrossRef Amunts K, Kedo O, Kindler M, Pieperhoff P, Mohlberg H, Shah NJ, Habel U, Schneider F, Zilles K (2005) Cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human amygdala, hippocampal region and entorhinal cortex: intersubject variability and probability maps. Anat Embryol (Berl) 210(5–6):343–352. doi:10.​1007/​s00429-005-0025-5 CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Avidan G, Levy I, Hendler T, Zohary E, Malach R (2003) Spatial vs. object specific attention in high-order visual areas. Neuroimage 19(2 Pt 1):308–318 Avidan G, Levy I, Hendler T, Zohary E, Malach R (2003) Spatial vs. object specific attention in high-order visual areas. Neuroimage 19(2 Pt 1):308–318
Zurück zum Zitat Baker CI, Liu J, Wald LL, Kwong KK, Benner T, Kanwisher N (2007) Visual word processing and experiential origins of functional selectivity in human extrastriate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104(21):9087–9092. doi:10.1073/pnas.0703300104 PubMedCrossRef Baker CI, Liu J, Wald LL, Kwong KK, Benner T, Kanwisher N (2007) Visual word processing and experiential origins of functional selectivity in human extrastriate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104(21):9087–9092. doi:10.​1073/​pnas.​0703300104 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bartels PH (1979) Numerical evaluation of cytologic data: II. Comparison of profiles. Anal Quant Cytol 1(2):77–83PubMed Bartels PH (1979) Numerical evaluation of cytologic data: II. Comparison of profiles. Anal Quant Cytol 1(2):77–83PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Bartels A, Zeki S (2000) The architecture of the colour centre in the human visual brain: new results and a review. Eur J Neurosci 12(1):172–193PubMedCrossRef Bartels A, Zeki S (2000) The architecture of the colour centre in the human visual brain: new results and a review. Eur J Neurosci 12(1):172–193PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Binder JR, Mohr JP (1992) The topography of callosal reading pathways. A case-control analysis. Brain 115:1807–1826PubMedCrossRef Binder JR, Mohr JP (1992) The topography of callosal reading pathways. A case-control analysis. Brain 115:1807–1826PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Braak H (1977) The pigment architecture of the human occipital lobe. Anat Embryol (Berl) 150(2):229–250CrossRef Braak H (1977) The pigment architecture of the human occipital lobe. Anat Embryol (Berl) 150(2):229–250CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Braak H (1980) Architectonics of the human telencephalic cortex. Springer, BerlinCrossRef Braak H (1980) Architectonics of the human telencephalic cortex. Springer, BerlinCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brodmann K (1909) Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Großhirnrinde. Barth, Leipzig Brodmann K (1909) Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Großhirnrinde. Barth, Leipzig
Zurück zum Zitat Clark VP, Keil K, Maisog JM, Courtney S, Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV (1996) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human visual cortex during face matching: a comparison with positron emission tomography. Neuroimage 4(1):1–15. doi:10.1006/nimg.1996.0025 PubMedCrossRef Clark VP, Keil K, Maisog JM, Courtney S, Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV (1996) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human visual cortex during face matching: a comparison with positron emission tomography. Neuroimage 4(1):1–15. doi:10.​1006/​nimg.​1996.​0025 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Clarke S, Miklossy J (1990) Occipital cortex in man: organization of callosal connections, related myelo- and cytoarchitecture, and putative boundaries of functional visual areas. J Comp Neurol 298(2):188–214. doi:10.1002/cne.902980205 PubMedCrossRef Clarke S, Miklossy J (1990) Occipital cortex in man: organization of callosal connections, related myelo- and cytoarchitecture, and putative boundaries of functional visual areas. J Comp Neurol 298(2):188–214. doi:10.​1002/​cne.​902980205 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen L, Dehaene S, Naccache L, Lehericy S, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Henaff MA, Michel F (2000) The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain 123:291–307PubMedCrossRef Cohen L, Dehaene S, Naccache L, Lehericy S, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Henaff MA, Michel F (2000) The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain 123:291–307PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen L, Lehericy S, Chochon F, Lemer C, Rivaud S, Dehaene S (2002) Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the visual word form area. Brain 125(Pt 5):1054–1069PubMedCrossRef Cohen L, Lehericy S, Chochon F, Lemer C, Rivaud S, Dehaene S (2002) Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the visual word form area. Brain 125(Pt 5):1054–1069PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Damasio AR, Damasio H, Van Hoesen GW (1982) Prosopagnosia: anatomic basis and behavioral mechanisms. Neurology 32(4):331–341PubMedCrossRef Damasio AR, Damasio H, Van Hoesen GW (1982) Prosopagnosia: anatomic basis and behavioral mechanisms. Neurology 32(4):331–341PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat De Renzi E (1986) Prosopagnosia in two patients with CT scan evidence of damage confined to the right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 24(3):385–389PubMedCrossRef De Renzi E (1986) Prosopagnosia in two patients with CT scan evidence of damage confined to the right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 24(3):385–389PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dehaene S, Le Clec HG, Poline JB, Le Bihan D, Cohen L (2002) The visual word form area: a prelexical representation of visual words in the fusiform gyrus. NeuroReport 13(3):321–325PubMedCrossRef Dehaene S, Le Clec HG, Poline JB, Le Bihan D, Cohen L (2002) The visual word form area: a prelexical representation of visual words in the fusiform gyrus. NeuroReport 13(3):321–325PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dehaene S, Pegado F, Braga LW, Ventura P, Nunes Filho G, Jobert A, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Kolinsky R, Morais J, Cohen L (2010) How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science 330(6009):1359–1364. doi:10.1126/science.1194140 PubMedCrossRef Dehaene S, Pegado F, Braga LW, Ventura P, Nunes Filho G, Jobert A, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Kolinsky R, Morais J, Cohen L (2010) How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science 330(6009):1359–1364. doi:10.​1126/​science.​1194140 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat DeYoe EA, Carman GJ, Bandettini P, Glickman S, Wieser J, Cox R, Miller D, Neitz J (1996) Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93(6):2382–2386PubMedCrossRef DeYoe EA, Carman GJ, Bandettini P, Glickman S, Wieser J, Cox R, Miller D, Neitz J (1996) Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93(6):2382–2386PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Engel SA, Glover GH, Wandell BA (1997) Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. Cereb Cortex 7(2):181–192PubMedCrossRef Engel SA, Glover GH, Wandell BA (1997) Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. Cereb Cortex 7(2):181–192PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Epstein R, Harris A, Stanley D, Kanwisher N (1999) The parahippocampal place area: recognition, navigation, or encoding? Neuron 23(1):115–125PubMedCrossRef Epstein R, Harris A, Stanley D, Kanwisher N (1999) The parahippocampal place area: recognition, navigation, or encoding? Neuron 23(1):115–125PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Evans AC, Marrett S, Neelin P, Collins L, Worsley K, Dai W, Milot S, Meyer E, Bub D (1992) Anatomical mapping of functional activation in stereotactic coordinate space. Neuroimage 1(1):43–53PubMedCrossRef Evans AC, Marrett S, Neelin P, Collins L, Worsley K, Dai W, Milot S, Meyer E, Bub D (1992) Anatomical mapping of functional activation in stereotactic coordinate space. Neuroimage 1(1):43–53PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Felleman DJ, van Essen DC (1991) Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 1(1):1–47PubMedCrossRef Felleman DJ, van Essen DC (1991) Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 1(1):1–47PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gauthier I, Tarr MJ, Moylan J, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Anderson AW (2000) The fusiform “face area” is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. J Cogn Neurosci 12(3):495–504PubMedCrossRef Gauthier I, Tarr MJ, Moylan J, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Anderson AW (2000) The fusiform “face area” is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. J Cogn Neurosci 12(3):495–504PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Hendler T, Edelman S, Itzchak Y, Malach R (1998) A sequence of object-processing stages revealed by fMRI in the human occipital lobe. Hum Brain Mapp 6(4):316–328PubMedCrossRef Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Hendler T, Edelman S, Itzchak Y, Malach R (1998) A sequence of object-processing stages revealed by fMRI in the human occipital lobe. Hum Brain Mapp 6(4):316–328PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Edelman S, Avidan G, Itzchak Y, Malach R (1999) Differential processing of objects under various viewing conditions in the human lateral occipital complex. Neuron 24(1):187–203PubMedCrossRef Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Edelman S, Avidan G, Itzchak Y, Malach R (1999) Differential processing of objects under various viewing conditions in the human lateral occipital complex. Neuron 24(1):187–203PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Hendler T, Malach R (2000) The dynamics of object-selective activation correlate with recognition performance in humans. Nat Neurosci 3(8):837–843. doi:10.1038/77754 PubMedCrossRef Grill-Spector K, Kushnir T, Hendler T, Malach R (2000) The dynamics of object-selective activation correlate with recognition performance in humans. Nat Neurosci 3(8):837–843. doi:10.​1038/​77754 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Grill-Spector K, Kourtzi Z, Kanwisher N (2001) The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition. Vision Res 41(10–11):1409–1422PubMedCrossRef Grill-Spector K, Kourtzi Z, Kanwisher N (2001) The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition. Vision Res 41(10–11):1409–1422PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Grill-Spector K, Knouf N, Kanwisher N (2004) The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification. Nat Neurosci 7(5):555–562. doi:10.1038/nn1224 PubMedCrossRef Grill-Spector K, Knouf N, Kanwisher N (2004) The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification. Nat Neurosci 7(5):555–562. doi:10.​1038/​nn1224 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hadjikhani N, Liu AK, Dale AM, Cavanagh P, Tootell RB (1998) Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8. Nat Neurosci 1(3):235–241. doi:10.1038/681 PubMedCrossRef Hadjikhani N, Liu AK, Dale AM, Cavanagh P, Tootell RB (1998) Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8. Nat Neurosci 1(3):235–241. doi:10.​1038/​681 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Halgren E, Dale AM, Sereno MI, Tootell RB, Marinkovic K, Rosen BR (1999) Location of human face-selective cortex with respect to retinotopic areas. Hum Brain Mapp 7(1):29–37PubMedCrossRef Halgren E, Dale AM, Sereno MI, Tootell RB, Marinkovic K, Rosen BR (1999) Location of human face-selective cortex with respect to retinotopic areas. Hum Brain Mapp 7(1):29–37PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hasson U, Levy I, Behrmann M, Hendler T, Malach R (2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle for human high-order object areas. Neuron 34(3):479–490PubMedCrossRef Hasson U, Levy I, Behrmann M, Hendler T, Malach R (2002) Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle for human high-order object areas. Neuron 34(3):479–490PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Haxby JV, Horwitz B, Ungerleider LG, Maisog JM, Pietrini P, Grady CL (1994) The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: a PET-rCBF study of selective attention to faces and locations. J Neurosci 14(11 Pt 1):6336–6353PubMed Haxby JV, Horwitz B, Ungerleider LG, Maisog JM, Pietrini P, Grady CL (1994) The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: a PET-rCBF study of selective attention to faces and locations. J Neurosci 14(11 Pt 1):6336–6353PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hömke L (2006) A multigrid method for anisotrophic PDE’s in elastic image registration. Numer Linear Algebra Appl 13(2–3):215–229CrossRef Hömke L (2006) A multigrid method for anisotrophic PDE’s in elastic image registration. Numer Linear Algebra Appl 13(2–3):215–229CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ishai A, Ungerleider LG, Martin A, Schouten JL, Haxby JV (1999) Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96(16):9379–9384PubMedCrossRef Ishai A, Ungerleider LG, Martin A, Schouten JL, Haxby JV (1999) Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96(16):9379–9384PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kanwisher N, Chun MM, McDermott J, Ledden PJ (1996) Functional imaging of human visual recognition. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 5(1–2):55–67PubMedCrossRef Kanwisher N, Chun MM, McDermott J, Ledden PJ (1996) Functional imaging of human visual recognition. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 5(1–2):55–67PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM (1997) The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J Neurosci 17(11):4302–4311PubMed Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM (1997) The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J Neurosci 17(11):4302–4311PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Kastner S, De Weerd P, Pinsk MA, Elizondo MI, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG (2001) Modulation of sensory suppression: implications for receptive field sizes in the human visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 86(3):1398–1411PubMed Kastner S, De Weerd P, Pinsk MA, Elizondo MI, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG (2001) Modulation of sensory suppression: implications for receptive field sizes in the human visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 86(3):1398–1411PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Lancaster JL, Tordesillas-Gutierrez D, Martinez M, Salinas F, Evans A, Zilles K, Mazziotta JC, Fox PT (2007) Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template. Hum Brain Mapp 28(11):1194–1205. doi:10.1002/hbm.20345 PubMedCrossRef Lancaster JL, Tordesillas-Gutierrez D, Martinez M, Salinas F, Evans A, Zilles K, Mazziotta JC, Fox PT (2007) Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template. Hum Brain Mapp 28(11):1194–1205. doi:10.​1002/​hbm.​20345 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Landis T, Cummings JL, Christen L, Bogen JE, Imhof HG (1986) Are unilateral right posterior cerebral lesions sufficient to cause prosopagnosia? Clinical and radiological findings in six additional patients. Cortex 22(2):243–252PubMed Landis T, Cummings JL, Christen L, Bogen JE, Imhof HG (1986) Are unilateral right posterior cerebral lesions sufficient to cause prosopagnosia? Clinical and radiological findings in six additional patients. Cortex 22(2):243–252PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Levy I, Hasson U, Avidan G, Hendler T, Malach R (2001) Center-periphery organization of human object areas. Nat Neurosci 4(5):533–539. doi:10.1038/87490 PubMed Levy I, Hasson U, Avidan G, Hendler T, Malach R (2001) Center-periphery organization of human object areas. Nat Neurosci 4(5):533–539. doi:10.​1038/​87490 PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Mahalanobis PC, Majumdar DN, Rao CR (1949) Anthropometric survey of the United Provinces, 1941: a statistical study. Sankya 9:89–324 Mahalanobis PC, Majumdar DN, Rao CR (1949) Anthropometric survey of the United Provinces, 1941: a statistical study. Sankya 9:89–324
Zurück zum Zitat Malach R, Reppas JB, Benson RR, Kwong KK, Jiang H, Kennedy WA, Ledden PJ, Brady TJ, Rosen BR, Tootell RB (1995) Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92(18):8135–8139PubMedCrossRef Malach R, Reppas JB, Benson RR, Kwong KK, Jiang H, Kennedy WA, Ledden PJ, Brady TJ, Rosen BR, Tootell RB (1995) Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92(18):8135–8139PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Malach R, Levy I, Hasson U (2002) The topography of high-order human object areas. Trends Cogn Sci 6(4):176–184PubMedCrossRef Malach R, Levy I, Hasson U (2002) The topography of high-order human object areas. Trends Cogn Sci 6(4):176–184PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Malikovic A, Amunts K, Schleicher A, Mohlberg H, Eickhoff SB, Wilms M, Palomero-Gallagher N, Armstrong E, Zilles K (2007) Cytoarchitectonic analysis of the human extrastriate cortex in the region of V5/MT+: a probabilistic, stereotaxic map of area hOc5. Cereb Cortex 17(3):562–574. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj181 PubMedCrossRef Malikovic A, Amunts K, Schleicher A, Mohlberg H, Eickhoff SB, Wilms M, Palomero-Gallagher N, Armstrong E, Zilles K (2007) Cytoarchitectonic analysis of the human extrastriate cortex in the region of V5/MT+: a probabilistic, stereotaxic map of area hOc5. Cereb Cortex 17(3):562–574. doi:10.​1093/​cercor/​bhj181 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Merker B (1983) Silver staining of cell bodies by means of physical development. J Neurosci Methods 9(3):235–241PubMedCrossRef Merker B (1983) Silver staining of cell bodies by means of physical development. J Neurosci Methods 9(3):235–241PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mishkin M, Ungerleider LG (1982) Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys. Behav Brain Res 6(1):57–77PubMedCrossRef Mishkin M, Ungerleider LG (1982) Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys. Behav Brain Res 6(1):57–77PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Petersen SE, Fox PT, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME (1990) Activation of extrastriate and frontal cortical areas by visual words and word-like stimuli. Science 249(4972):1041–1044PubMedCrossRef Petersen SE, Fox PT, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME (1990) Activation of extrastriate and frontal cortical areas by visual words and word-like stimuli. Science 249(4972):1041–1044PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pinsk MA, Arcaro M, Weiner KS, Kalkus JF, Inati SJ, Gross CG, Kastner S (2009) Neural representations of faces and body parts in macaque and human cortex: a comparative FMRI study. J Neurophysiol 101(5):2581–2600. doi:10.1152/jn.91198.2008 PubMedCrossRef Pinsk MA, Arcaro M, Weiner KS, Kalkus JF, Inati SJ, Gross CG, Kastner S (2009) Neural representations of faces and body parts in macaque and human cortex: a comparative FMRI study. J Neurophysiol 101(5):2581–2600. doi:10.​1152/​jn.​91198.​2008 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Puce A, Allison T, Asgari M, Gore JC, McCarthy G (1996) Differential sensitivity of human visual cortex to faces, letterstrings, and textures: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 16(16):5205–5215PubMed Puce A, Allison T, Asgari M, Gore JC, McCarthy G (1996) Differential sensitivity of human visual cortex to faces, letterstrings, and textures: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 16(16):5205–5215PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Rossion B, Schiltz C, Crommelinck M (2003) The functionally defined right occipital and fusiform “face areas” discriminate novel from visually familiar faces. Neuroimage 19(3):877–883PubMedCrossRef Rossion B, Schiltz C, Crommelinck M (2003) The functionally defined right occipital and fusiform “face areas” discriminate novel from visually familiar faces. Neuroimage 19(3):877–883PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rottschy C, Eickhoff SB, Schleicher A, Mohlberg H, Kujovic M, Zilles K, Amunts K (2007) Ventral visual cortex in humans: cytoarchitectonic mapping of two extrastriate areas. Hum Brain Mapp 28(10):1045–1059. doi:10.1002/hbm.20348 PubMedCrossRef Rottschy C, Eickhoff SB, Schleicher A, Mohlberg H, Kujovic M, Zilles K, Amunts K (2007) Ventral visual cortex in humans: cytoarchitectonic mapping of two extrastriate areas. Hum Brain Mapp 28(10):1045–1059. doi:10.​1002/​hbm.​20348 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sarkisov SA, Filimonoff IN, Preobrashenskaya NS (1949) Cytoarchitecture of the human cortex cerebri. Medgiz, Moscow Sarkisov SA, Filimonoff IN, Preobrashenskaya NS (1949) Cytoarchitecture of the human cortex cerebri. Medgiz, Moscow
Zurück zum Zitat Scheperjans F, Eickhoff SB, Hömke L, Mohlberg H, Hermann K, Amunts K, Zilles K (2008) Probabilistic maps, morphometry, and variability of cytoarchitectonic areas in the human superior parietal cortex. Cereb Cortex 18(9):2141–2157. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm241 PubMedCrossRef Scheperjans F, Eickhoff SB, Hömke L, Mohlberg H, Hermann K, Amunts K, Zilles K (2008) Probabilistic maps, morphometry, and variability of cytoarchitectonic areas in the human superior parietal cortex. Cereb Cortex 18(9):2141–2157. doi:10.​1093/​cercor/​bhm241 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schleicher A, Amunts K, Geyer S, Morosan P, Zilles K (1999) Observer-independent method for microstructural parcellation of cerebral cortex: a quantitative approach to cytoarchitectonics. Neuroimage 9(1):165–177. doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0385 PubMedCrossRef Schleicher A, Amunts K, Geyer S, Morosan P, Zilles K (1999) Observer-independent method for microstructural parcellation of cerebral cortex: a quantitative approach to cytoarchitectonics. Neuroimage 9(1):165–177. doi:10.​1006/​nimg.​1998.​0385 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schleicher A, Amunts K, Geyer S, Kowalski T, Schormann T, Palomero-Gallagher N, Zilles K (2000) A stereological approach to human cortical architecture: identification and delineation of cortical areas. J Chem Neuroanat 20(1):31–47PubMedCrossRef Schleicher A, Amunts K, Geyer S, Kowalski T, Schormann T, Palomero-Gallagher N, Zilles K (2000) A stereological approach to human cortical architecture: identification and delineation of cortical areas. J Chem Neuroanat 20(1):31–47PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schleicher A, Palomero-Gallagher N, Morosan P, Eickhoff SB, Kowalski T, de Vos K, Amunts K, Zilles K (2005) Quantitative architectural analysis: a new approach to cortical mapping. Anat Embryol (Berl) 210(5–6):373–386. doi:10.1007/s00429-005-0028-2 CrossRef Schleicher A, Palomero-Gallagher N, Morosan P, Eickhoff SB, Kowalski T, de Vos K, Amunts K, Zilles K (2005) Quantitative architectural analysis: a new approach to cortical mapping. Anat Embryol (Berl) 210(5–6):373–386. doi:10.​1007/​s00429-005-0028-2 CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sereno MI, Dale AM, Reppas JB, Kwong KK, Belliveau JW, Brady TJ, Rosen BR, Tootell RB (1995) Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Science 268(5212):889–893PubMedCrossRef Sereno MI, Dale AM, Reppas JB, Kwong KK, Belliveau JW, Brady TJ, Rosen BR, Tootell RB (1995) Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Science 268(5212):889–893PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sergent J, Signoret JL (1992) Varieties of functional deficits in prosopagnosia. Cereb Cortex 2(5):375–388PubMedCrossRef Sergent J, Signoret JL (1992) Varieties of functional deficits in prosopagnosia. Cereb Cortex 2(5):375–388PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sergent J, Ohta S, MacDonald B (1992) Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. A positron emission tomography study. Brain 115:15–36PubMedCrossRef Sergent J, Ohta S, MacDonald B (1992) Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. A positron emission tomography study. Brain 115:15–36PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tarkiainen A, Helenius P, Hansen PC, Cornelissen PL, Salmelin R (1999) Dynamics of letter string perception in the human occipitotemporal cortex. Brain 122:2119–2132PubMedCrossRef Tarkiainen A, Helenius P, Hansen PC, Cornelissen PL, Salmelin R (1999) Dynamics of letter string perception in the human occipitotemporal cortex. Brain 122:2119–2132PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tong F, Nakayama K, Vaughan JT, Kanwisher N (1998) Binocular rivalry and visual awareness in human extrastriate cortex. Neuron 21(4):753–759PubMedCrossRef Tong F, Nakayama K, Vaughan JT, Kanwisher N (1998) Binocular rivalry and visual awareness in human extrastriate cortex. Neuron 21(4):753–759PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tootell RB, Hadjikhani N (2001) Where is ‘dorsal V4’ in human visual cortex? Retinotopic, topographic and functional evidence. Cereb Cortex 11(4):298–311PubMedCrossRef Tootell RB, Hadjikhani N (2001) Where is ‘dorsal V4’ in human visual cortex? Retinotopic, topographic and functional evidence. Cereb Cortex 11(4):298–311PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tootell RB, Dale AM, Sereno MI, Malach R (1996) New images from human visual cortex. Trends Neurosci 19(11):481–489PubMedCrossRef Tootell RB, Dale AM, Sereno MI, Malach R (1996) New images from human visual cortex. Trends Neurosci 19(11):481–489PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tootell RB, Mendola JD, Hadjikhani NK, Liu AK, Dale AM (1998) The representation of the ipsilateral visual field in human cerebral cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95(3):818–824PubMedCrossRef Tootell RB, Mendola JD, Hadjikhani NK, Liu AK, Dale AM (1998) The representation of the ipsilateral visual field in human cerebral cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95(3):818–824PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tootell RB, Tsao D, Vanduffel W (2003) Neuroimaging weighs in: humans meet macaques in “primate” visual cortex. J Neurosci 23(10):3981–3989PubMed Tootell RB, Tsao D, Vanduffel W (2003) Neuroimaging weighs in: humans meet macaques in “primate” visual cortex. J Neurosci 23(10):3981–3989PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV (1994) ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol 4(2):157–165PubMedCrossRef Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV (1994) ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol 4(2):157–165PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat von Economo C, Koskinas GN (1925) Die Cytoarchitektonik der Hirnrinde des Erwachsenen Menschen. Springer, Berlin von Economo C, Koskinas GN (1925) Die Cytoarchitektonik der Hirnrinde des Erwachsenen Menschen. Springer, Berlin
Zurück zum Zitat Wagner AD, Schacter DL, Rotte M, Koutstaal W, Maril A, Dale AM, Rosen BR, Buckner RL (1998) Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science 281(5380):1188–1191PubMedCrossRef Wagner AD, Schacter DL, Rotte M, Koutstaal W, Maril A, Dale AM, Rosen BR, Buckner RL (1998) Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science 281(5380):1188–1191PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Weiner KS, Grill-Spector K (2011a) Neural representations of faces and limbs neighbor in human high-level visual cortex: evidence for a new organization principle. Psychol Res. doi:10.1007/s00426-011-0392-x PubMed Weiner KS, Grill-Spector K (2011a) Neural representations of faces and limbs neighbor in human high-level visual cortex: evidence for a new organization principle. Psychol Res. doi:10.​1007/​s00426-011-0392-x PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Wohlschläger AM, Specht K, Lie C, Mohlberg H, Wohlschläger A, Bente K, Pietrzyk U, Stöcker T, Zilles K, Amunts K, Fink GR (2005) Linking retinotopic fMRI mapping and anatomical probability maps of human occipital areas V1 and V2. Neuroimage 26(1):73–82. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.021 PubMedCrossRef Wohlschläger AM, Specht K, Lie C, Mohlberg H, Wohlschläger A, Bente K, Pietrzyk U, Stöcker T, Zilles K, Amunts K, Fink GR (2005) Linking retinotopic fMRI mapping and anatomical probability maps of human occipital areas V1 and V2. Neuroimage 26(1):73–82. doi:10.​1016/​j.​neuroimage.​2005.​01.​021 PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zeki SM (1969) Representation of central visual fields in prestriate cortex of monkey. Brain Res 14(2):271–291PubMedCrossRef Zeki SM (1969) Representation of central visual fields in prestriate cortex of monkey. Brain Res 14(2):271–291PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zilles K, Clarke S (1997) Architecture, connectivity and transmitter receptors of human extrastriate visual cortex: comparison with non-human primates. In: Rockland KS, Kaas JH, Peters A (eds) Cerebral cortex, vol 12. Plenum Press, New York, London, pp 673–742 Zilles K, Clarke S (1997) Architecture, connectivity and transmitter receptors of human extrastriate visual cortex: comparison with non-human primates. In: Rockland KS, Kaas JH, Peters A (eds) Cerebral cortex, vol 12. Plenum Press, New York, London, pp 673–742
Zurück zum Zitat Zilles K, Schleicher A, Palomero-Gallagher N, Amunts K (2002) Quantitative analysis of cyto- and receptor architecture of the human brain. In: Mazziotta JC, Toga A (eds) Brain mapping: the methods, 2nd edn. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 573–602CrossRef Zilles K, Schleicher A, Palomero-Gallagher N, Amunts K (2002) Quantitative analysis of cyto- and receptor architecture of the human brain. In: Mazziotta JC, Toga A (eds) Brain mapping: the methods, 2nd edn. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 573–602CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Cytoarchitectonical analysis and probabilistic mapping of two extrastriate areas of the human posterior fusiform gyrus
verfasst von
Julian Caspers
Karl Zilles
Simon B. Eickhoff
Axel Schleicher
Hartmut Mohlberg
Katrin Amunts
Publikationsdatum
01.03.2013
Verlag
Springer-Verlag
Erschienen in
Brain Structure and Function / Ausgabe 2/2013
Print ISSN: 1863-2653
Elektronische ISSN: 1863-2661
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0411-8

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2013

Brain Structure and Function 2/2013 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Schützt Olivenöl vor dem Tod durch Demenz?

10.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Konsumieren Menschen täglich 7 Gramm Olivenöl, ist ihr Risiko, an einer Demenz zu sterben, um mehr als ein Vierten reduziert – und dies weitgehend unabhängig von ihrer sonstigen Ernährung. Dafür sprechen Auswertungen zweier großer US-Studien.

Bluttest erkennt Parkinson schon zehn Jahre vor der Diagnose

10.05.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Ein Bluttest kann abnorm aggregiertes Alpha-Synuclein bei einigen Menschen schon zehn Jahre vor Beginn der motorischen Parkinsonsymptome nachweisen. Mit einem solchen Test lassen sich möglicherweise Prodromalstadien erfassen und die Betroffenen früher behandeln.

Darf man die Behandlung eines Neonazis ablehnen?

08.05.2024 Gesellschaft Nachrichten

In einer Leseranfrage in der Zeitschrift Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology möchte ein anonymer Dermatologe bzw. eine anonyme Dermatologin wissen, ob er oder sie einen Patienten behandeln muss, der eine rassistische Tätowierung trägt.

Wartezeit nicht kürzer, aber Arbeit flexibler

Psychotherapie Medizin aktuell

Fünf Jahren nach der Neugestaltung der Psychotherapie-Richtlinie wurden jetzt die Effekte der vorgenommenen Änderungen ausgewertet. Das Hauptziel der Novellierung war eine kürzere Wartezeit auf Therapieplätze. Dieses Ziel wurde nicht erreicht, es gab jedoch positive Auswirkungen auf andere Bereiche.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.