Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Clinical Autonomic Research 2/2007

Open Access 01.04.2007 | REVIEW ARTICLE

Reduced brain perfusion and cognitive performance due to constitutional hypotension

verfasst von: Stefan Duschek, Rainer Schandry

Erschienen in: Clinical Autonomic Research | Ausgabe 2/2007

Abstract

This review article includes a systematic evaluation of the empirical data concerning deficits in mental ability, brain perfusion, and cerebral functioning due to chronically low blood pressure. A number of studies have provided strong evidence for reduced cognitive performance in hypotension, particularly in the domains of attention and memory. EEG studies have demonstrated that the hypotension-related poorer mental ability is also reflected in diminished cortical activity. Contrary to convention, more recent research has suggested a deficient regulation of cerebral blood flow in persons with low blood pressure. In addition to reduced tonic brain perfusion, studies demonstrated insufficient adjustment of blood flow to cognitive requirements. Altogether, these findings suggest that more attention should be allocated to chronic hypotension in both research and clinical practice.

Introduction

Chronically low blood pressure is accompanied by a variety of complaints including fatigue, reduced drive, faintness, dizziness, headaches, palpitations, and increased pain sensitivity [14]. In addition, hypotensive individuals report cognitive impairment, above all deficits in attention and memory. Nevertheless, it is generally the case that in research, as well as in clinical practice, relatively little importance is ascribed to hypotension. One reason for this is that, despite mental symptoms, cerebral dysfunction generally is not taken into account [1]. This is a consequence of the current doctrine that low systemic blood pressure is compensated by autoregulatory processes which prevent reduced blood perfusion of the brain [5, 6].
Some recent findings challenge this doctrine: reduced cognitive performance in hypotension has been demonstrated by neuropsychological testing, and EEG studies have revealed diminished cortical activity. Moreover, the assumption of unimpaired brain perfusion in hypotension no longer holds. In the present review the necessity of a reappraisal concerning hypotension is discussed in light of the relationship between blood pressure and cerebral functioning.

Constitutional hypotension

Hypotension has been defined by the World Health Organization (WHO) [4] as a low blood pressure with a systolic reading below 110 mmHg in males and below 100 mmHg in females, regardless of diastolic blood pressure. The concept of constitutional hypotension refers to a chronic condition of inappropriately reduced blood pressure independent of the presence of further pathological factors. Both secondary hypotension (e.g. due to blood loss or medication) and orthostatic hypotension (caused by circulatory problems when assuming an upright position) are distinguished from the constitutional form [1, 6].
Constitutional hypotension is relatively widespread in the general population. In a representative German sample of more than 7,000 subjects approximately 3% fulfilled the criteria of the WHO [c.f. 1]. In contrast to elevated blood pressure, which constitutes a significant risk factor for cardiovascular disease, hypotension is commonly not regarded as a severe condition. Nevertheless, its impact on personal well-being and quality of life has been shown in several epidemiological studies. For instance, Wessely et al. [7], as well as Pilgrim et al. [3] demonstrated impaired physical and mental health in the case of low blood pressure. Reduced health-related quality of life [8] and a heightened degree of depressiveness [9] were also reported by hypotensive individuals.
Chronic hypotension has been identified as a major risk factor in pregnancy [10]. Moreover, several studies focusing on the elderly population have reported associations between low blood pressure and the prevalence and incidence of Alzheimer’s and vascular dementia [c.f. 1114]. Whilst a few longitudinal studies examining this relationship have been published [15, 16], the causal role of low blood pressure in degenerative brain disorders has thus far not been proven [e.g. 13].1
Concerning the etiology of hypotension, reduced liquid intake and low body weight may be considered [c.f. 1, 17]. A dysregulation of the autonomous nervous system may additionally be of significance. Various findings on reduced electrodermal activity [18] and increased heart rate variability [19] reflect diminished sympathetic tone and habitually heightened parasympathetic activity in persons with low blood pressure values. This is corroborated by findings on the effects of sympathomimetic drugs [20, 21]. In mixed samples of persons with constitutional and orthostatic hypotension, α and β adrenergic substances were shown to lead to blood pressure elevation accompanied by a reduction of subjective symptoms. A malfunction of the arterial baroreceptor system was postulated as a further etiological factor [22]. Responding to mechanical stretch of the vessel walls, the baroreceptors form part of a negative feedback loop (“baroreflex”) compensating phasic blood pressure fluctuations [23]. Increased sensitivity of the baroreceptor system may result in stabilizing blood pressure at a lower level. In addition to findings in animals [24], this hypothesis is supported by a still unpublished study of our own group which yielded increased baroreflex sensitivity in the case of low blood pressure.

Cognitive deficits in constitutional hypotension

Table 1 presents the most relevant studies devoted to cognitive functioning in hypotension. In addition to the main results, the table includes information concerning the respective samples and assessment instruments.
Table 1
Studies on cognitive performance in hypotension (SBP, systolic blood pressure; DBP, diastolic blood pressure)
Authors
Main focus
Samples
Assessment instruments
Main results
Richter-Heinrich et al. [18]
Attentional performance in hypotension and hypertension
30 hypotensives (SBP < 106 mmHg); 40 hypertensives (SBP > 140 mmHg); 31 normotensive controls; age 16–40 years
Konzentrations-Verlaufs-Test (course of concentration test) [25]
Reduced attentional performance in the case of both lowered and elevated blood pressure
Stegagno et al. [26]
Attentional, memory and arithmetic performance in hypotension
17 female hypotensives (SBP < 100 mmHg); 19 normotensive controls; mean age 23 years
Reaction times to acoustic stimuli; digit span [27]; recall of word lists [28]; serial subtractions
Prolonged reaction times, poorer performance on the verbal memory and arithmetic tasks in hypotension, no effect for digit span
Costa et al. [29]
Attentional and memory performance in hypotension
26 female hypotensives (SBP < 105, DBP < 65 mmHg), 22 normotensive controls; mean age 29 years
Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test (attentional strain test) [30]; Zahlen-Verbindungs-Test (trail making test) [31]; recall of word lists [28]
Poorer attentional and memory performance on each of the three tests in hypotension
Morris et al. [32]
Relationship between blood pressure and cognitive performance in elderly persons
Representative population sample covering the total spectrum of blood pressure (n = 5,816; age over 64 years)
East Boston Memory Test [33]; Symbol Digit Modalities Test [34]; Mini-Mental State Examination [35]
Weak U-shaped relationship between blood pressure and cognitive performance
Weisz et al. [22]
Attentional performance in hypotension
25 female hypotensives (mean SBP = 102 mmHg); 25 normotensive controls; age 19–44 years
Attentional and Cognitive Efficiency Battery [36]
Reduced performance on a subtest assessing cognitive flexibility in hypotension
Duschek et al. [37]
Attentional performance in moderate hypotension
26 borderline hypotensives (mean SBP = 112 mmHg); 29 normotensive controls; mean age 26 years
Reaction times to acoustic stimuli; Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test (attentional strain test) [30]; Zahlen-Verbindungs-Test (trail making test) [31]
Prolonged reaction times and poorer performance on the “Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test” in moderate hypotension, no effects for the “Zahlen-Verbindungs-Test”
Duschek et al. [38]
Attentional and working memory performance in hypotension with motor performance and mood controlled
40 hypotensives (SBP < 105 mmHg in women, SBP < 110 mmHg in men); 40 normotensive controls; age 19–45 years
Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung (battery for the assessment of attention) [39]; Motorische Leistungsserie (motor performance series) [40]; Befindlichkeitsskala (mood scale) [41]
Poorer performance on six tests assessing tonic and phasic alertness, selective, divided and sustained attention as well as working memory in hypotensives with fine motor performance and mood controlled
A pioneering study investigating the relationship between blood pressure and cognitive abilities was conducted by Richter-Heinrich et al. [18]. They showed reduced performance of individuals with both lowered and elevated blood pressure on a test of concentration. A more comprehensive assessment was carried out by Stegagno et al. [26], in which poorer performances of hypotensive subjects on a verbal memory test and on an arithmetic task, as well as prolonged reaction times to acoustic stimuli were observed. Costa et al. [29] found reduced scores on two standard German paper–pencil tests assessing selective attention and cognitive speed. Moreover, as in the Stegagno et al. [26] study, impaired verbal memory performance was documented.
Weisz et al. [22] reported that female hypotensive subjects performed significantly worse than controls on a computer-based test measuring attentional flexibility [36]. Subjects with only a moderately decreased blood pressure were assessed by Duschek et al. [37]. In this sample, reduced attentional performance and prolonged reaction times were again found as compared to normotensive controls.
Duschek et al. [38] investigated the relationship between low blood pressure and attentional abilities through the application of a multidimensional diagnostic approach. They presented their subjects with a battery of six computer-based tasks [39] focusing on tonic and phasic alertness, selective, divided and sustained attention, as well as working memory. Additionally, in order to control for possible confounders, a test battery examining fine motor abilities [40] and a mood questionnaire [41] were presented. Reduced performance of hypotensives was evident in each of the six cognitive tests. The significant differences between hypotensive and control subjects persisted even when the effects of motor performance and mood were controlled.
Altogether, the existing data provide strong evidence for cognitive deficits related to constitutional hypotension, especially in the domains of attention and memory. The reduced performance seems to be a direct consequence of low blood pressure rather than an effect of impaired well-being related to this state. Up to now there have been no empirical data concerning specific effects of hypotension-related deficits on everyday life. Nonetheless, these findings raise concern regarding the impact of the deficits on attention-demanding activities (e.g. professional or academic), including public health-related functions (e.g. traffic safety) [42].
It would appear that both extremes of the blood pressure spectrum are accompanied by a decrease in cognitive ability. A number of studies concerned with elevated blood pressure showed reduced performance on various cognitive tests [43, 44]. This is in line with epidemiological studies reporting an inverted U-shaped relationship between blood pressure and cognitive performance in elderly persons [c.f. 11, 13, 32, 45, 46]. Therefore, it may further be hypothesized that there is a relatively small range of normal blood pressure in which the brain exerts its optimal function.

Neuroelectrophysiological correlates of hypotension-related cognitive deficits

Cognitive deficits in low blood pressure states are also reflected in decreased cortical activity. In accordance with earlier results [22, 29], Duschek et al. [47] found the amplitude of the contingent negative variation (CNV) to be reduced in a hypotensive sample defined according to the WHO criteria [4] (c.f. Fig. 1).
The CNV is an evoked potential which is generally viewed as a neuroelectrophysiological correlative of attentional processing [48]. This is supported by pharmacological studies showing that the effects of various stimulant and sedative drugs on attention involve an increase or decrease, respectively, of the CNV amplitude [49, 50]. Moreover, attentional deficits due to brain lesions and psychiatric diseases are associated with a reduced CNV [51, 52]. In the Duschek et al. [47] study, the amplitude of the CNV proved to be negatively correlated with reaction time, which in turn was prolonged in the hypotensive group. This highlights that the CNV constitutes a brain electrical correlate of an aspect of cognitive functioning which is affected in hypotension.
The relationship between blood pressure and cortical activity is also reflected in a negative correlation between blood pressure and α wave activity in the spontaneous EEG [47]. This suggests that low blood pressure is associated with a reduced tonic cortical arousal [53]. On the behavioral level, higher degrees of α activity are known to be associated with generally diminished vigilance and preparedness to react [54].
Considering the physiological processes mediating the relationship between blood pressure and brain function, it must be taken into account that blood pressure can influence cortical activation processes via afferent projections [47, 5557]. The brain continuously receives information about the state of the cardiovascular system by means of viscero-afferent fibers [23]. These afferent signals enter the brain via brainstem nuclei. From there, ascending pathways continue via hypothalamic and thalamic regions to cortical areas such as the anterior cingulate, the insula and the prefrontal lobe [58]. The latter areas, in particular the prefrontal cortex and the anterior cingulate are of crucial importance in the regulation of cortical arousal, as well as for attentional processes [59, 60]. Thus, these brain structures may represent functional interfaces between cardiovascular activity and attention [42].
Also neurochemical mediators should be regarded in the relationship between blood pressure and cortical activity. Noradrenaline is involved in the control of cortical arousal and attentional processes, as well as in the genesis of the CNV [59, 61]. Catecholamines are also of great influence in the regulation of blood pressure [62]. Bearing this in mind, one could speculate on the neurochemical level about a specific role of noradrenaline linking cardiovascular and cortical activation.

Cerebral blood perfusion in constitutional hypotension

It is generally assumed that in healthy individuals processes of autoregulation keep the cerebral blood flow constant within a wide range of arterial pressure. In order to ensure stable perfusion, cerebral resistance vessels constrict during increases and dilate during reductions in systemic blood pressure [5]. Autoregulation is considered to be a protective mechanism which prevents brain ischemia during blood pressure decrease, and guards against capillary damage and edema formation during periods of elevated blood pressure.
Under normal conditions the limits within which cerebral blood flow is assumed to be constant are approximately between a mean arterial pressure (MAP) of 60 and 150 mmHg [5, 63, 64]. If blood pressure is outside of this range, cerebral blood flow rises or falls with respective increases or decreases in blood pressure. Slight reductions of blood pressure below the lower limit of autoregulation can be compensated by an increase of the extraction coefficient of oxygen from the blood. Further reductions are accompanied by symptoms such as pallor and dizziness and ultimately lead to irreversible brain damage [5].
MAP in individuals with constitutional hypotension usually does not decrease beyond the assumed lower MAP limit of autoregulation (60 mmHg). In light of this, low blood pressure in these subjects should be compensated, and cerebral blood flow should not be affected. This assumption was challenged by Duschek and Schandry [65]. They recorded blood flow velocities by means of transcranial Doppler sonography in both middle cerebral arteries (MCA) in hypotensive individuals, defined according to the WHO [4], and in normotensive control subjects. Contrary to the current doctrine, MCA blood flow at rest was found to be substantially bilaterally reduced in hypotensives (c.f. Fig. 2).
The perfusion territory of the MCA includes subcortical areas, large fractions of the frontal and parietal lobes, as well as the temporal lobes [66]. Thus, the latter finding demonstrated that, despite autoregulation, blood pressure in subjects with constitutional hypotension is not sufficient in maintaining the perfusion of a large part of the brain at the level of normotensive individuals [see also 67].
In explaining this unexpected result, it must be noted that the determination of the lower limit of autoregulation was exclusively based on the experimental manipulation of blood pressure employing pharmacological methods, as well as head up-tilt [6871]. However, such a transient reduction of blood pressure is most certainly not comparable to conditions of chronic hypotension. Additionally, in all of these studies global cerebral blood flow was assessed by means of the oxygen difference method according to Lennox and Gibbs [72], which may be less sensitive to perfusion changes than Doppler sonography.
Nevertheless, more recent findings suggest values for the lower MAP limit of autoregulation to be considerably higher than 60 mmHg [7375]. In a critical review of the literature, Drummond [76] postulated an average lower limit of no less than 70 mmHg. Moreover, the limit seems to vary strongly across individuals. Waldemar et al. [77], for instance, reported an inter-individual range between 53 and 103 mmHg. In accordance with Duschek and Schandry’s [65] data, the doctrine of stable cerebral perfusion down to the limit of a MAP of 60 mmHg can no longer be supported by the current state of research.
A further important aspect of cerebral hemodynamics concerns the continuous adjustment of brain perfusion to current requirements. Due to the close coupling of neural activity and brain metabolism, cerebral activation processes are accompanied by changes in cerebral blood flow [78]. Neurovascular coupling is based on the contraction and dilation of small resistance vessels, resulting from the changing metabolic demands of neuron populations in the vicinity [64].
With regards to hypotension-related cognitive deficits, the extent of blood flow adjustment to mental activity was also tested in the Duschek and Schandry [65] study. Subjects were presented with a simple attentional task (motor reactions to visual stimuli cued by acoustic signals). During task execution blood flow velocities in the MCA of both hemispheres were recorded by means of functional transcranial Doppler sonography (for technical details see [66]).
As expected, mental activity was accompanied by a substantial increase of MCA perfusion. In control subjects, however, this increase was approximately 70% stronger than in hypotensives (c.f. Fig. 3). It would seem that blood pressure in the latter group was not sufficient to enable adjustment of brain perfusion to cognitive demands, as seen in normotensives. Moreover, hypotensive participants showed prolonged reaction times, and a positive correlation was found between reaction speed and the extent of blood flow increase. This finding corroborates the significance of hemodynamic adjustment for optimal cognitive functioning.

Final comments

There is strong evidence suggesting that chronically low blood pressure is accompanied by diminished cognitive performance, primarily involving attention and memory [26, 29, 38]. Recent research has provided an insight into the psychophysiological mechanisms of action underlying these deficits: EEG studies have demonstrated that the weaker cognitive performance is associated with reduced cortical activity [22, 29, 47]. Furthermore, deficient regulation of cerebral blood flow must be assumed in hypotension. In addition to diminished tonic brain perfusion, reduced adjustment of brain perfusion to cognitive demands was documented [65]. It stands to reason that, as a consequence of this situation, a diminished metabolic supply of the brain tissue accounts for the cognitive deficits.
The cognitive deficits can widely affect the every day life of hypotensive persons, and more attention should therefore be allocated to this topic within basic and clinical research, as well as in clinical practice [1, 17]. This finally leads to the issue of the treatment of hypotension. Empirical knowledge concerning the effectiveness of antihypotensive therapy continues to be scant. A variety of treatment strategies such as physical training, increase of liquid intake and pharmacological measures have been suggested [1]. In two clinical trials the administration of sympathomimetics was found to result in a reduction of subjective hypotensive symptoms [20, 21]. Recent data of our own group suggest that cognitive performance may also be enhanced by pharmacological blood pressure elevation [79]. Based on a placebo controlled design, the vasopressor agent midodrine was shown to raise cerebral blood perfusion as well as performance on a test assessing selective attention [30] in hypotensive subjects, defined according to the WHO [4]. The results of these pioneering studies are promising. Further research would appear worthwhile and should aim to establish precise guidelines which enable chronic hypotension to be efficiently and effectively dealt with in practice.
Open Access This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License ( https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by-nc/​2.​0 ), which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

Neuer Inhalt

Print-Titel

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Fußnoten
1
The cited findings [3, 79, 1116] were revealed in large epidemiological studies. All included samples across the total blood pressure spectra and did not explicitly define hypotension according to the WHO criteria [4].
 
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Duschek S, Schandry R (2006) Antriebsschwäche und beeinträchtigte Hirnleistungen: neue Perspektiven zum chronisch niedrigen Blutdruck. Deut Med Wochenschr 131:272–277CrossRef Duschek S, Schandry R (2006) Antriebsschwäche und beeinträchtigte Hirnleistungen: neue Perspektiven zum chronisch niedrigen Blutdruck. Deut Med Wochenschr 131:272–277CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Duschek S, Schandry R (2006) Increased pain sensitivity in low blood pressure. Abstract of the 5th Congress of the Spanish Society of Psychophysiology (SEPF). Granada, Spain, September 28–30, 2006. J Psychophysiol 20:235 Duschek S, Schandry R (2006) Increased pain sensitivity in low blood pressure. Abstract of the 5th Congress of the Spanish Society of Psychophysiology (SEPF). Granada, Spain, September 28–30, 2006. J Psychophysiol 20:235
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Pilgrim JA, Stansfeld S, Marmot M (1992) Low blood pressure, low mood? Brit Med J 304:75–78PubMed Pilgrim JA, Stansfeld S, Marmot M (1992) Low blood pressure, low mood? Brit Med J 304:75–78PubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat WHO (1978) Arterial hypertension. Technical Report Series No. 628. World Health Organisation, Genova WHO (1978) Arterial hypertension. Technical Report Series No. 628. World Health Organisation, Genova
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Chillon JM, Baumbach GL (1995) Autoregulation of cerebral blood flow. In: Welch KMA, Caplan LR, Reis DJ, Siesjö BK, Weir B (eds) Primer on cerebrovascular diseases. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 51–54 Chillon JM, Baumbach GL (1995) Autoregulation of cerebral blood flow. In: Welch KMA, Caplan LR, Reis DJ, Siesjö BK, Weir B (eds) Primer on cerebrovascular diseases. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 51–54
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Tonkin A (2004) Low blood pressure and low energy: (how) are they related? J Hypertens 22:671–673PubMedCrossRef Tonkin A (2004) Low blood pressure and low energy: (how) are they related? J Hypertens 22:671–673PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Wessely S, Nickson J, Cox B (1990) Symptoms of low blood pressure: a population study. Brit Med J 301:362–365PubMed Wessely S, Nickson J, Cox B (1990) Symptoms of low blood pressure: a population study. Brit Med J 301:362–365PubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosengren A, Tibblin G, Wilhelmsen L (1993) Low systolic blood pressure and self perceived wellbeing in middle aged men. Brit Med J 306:243–246PubMed Rosengren A, Tibblin G, Wilhelmsen L (1993) Low systolic blood pressure and self perceived wellbeing in middle aged men. Brit Med J 306:243–246PubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Barrett-Connor E, Palinkas LA (1994) Low blood pressure and depression in men: a population based study. Brit Med J 308:446–449PubMed Barrett-Connor E, Palinkas LA (1994) Low blood pressure and depression in men: a population based study. Brit Med J 308:446–449PubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Ng PH, Walters WA (1992) The effect of chronic maternal hypotension during pregnancy. Aust NZ J Obstet Gyn 32:14–16 Ng PH, Walters WA (1992) The effect of chronic maternal hypotension during pregnancy. Aust NZ J Obstet Gyn 32:14–16
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Hestad K, Kveberg B, Engedal K (2005) Low blood pressure is a better predictor of cognitive deficits than the apolipoprotein e4 allele in the oldest old. Acta Neurol Scand 111:323–328PubMedCrossRef Hestad K, Kveberg B, Engedal K (2005) Low blood pressure is a better predictor of cognitive deficits than the apolipoprotein e4 allele in the oldest old. Acta Neurol Scand 111:323–328PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Morris MC, Scherr PA, Hebert LE, Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Glynn RJ, Evans DA (2000) The cross-sectional association between blood pressure and Alzheimer’s disease in a biracial community population of older persons. J Gerontol A-Biol 55:M130–M136 Morris MC, Scherr PA, Hebert LE, Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Glynn RJ, Evans DA (2000) The cross-sectional association between blood pressure and Alzheimer’s disease in a biracial community population of older persons. J Gerontol A-Biol 55:M130–M136
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Qiu C, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L (2005) The age-dependent relation of blood pressure to cognitive function and dementia. Lancet Neurol 4:487–499PubMedCrossRef Qiu C, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L (2005) The age-dependent relation of blood pressure to cognitive function and dementia. Lancet Neurol 4:487–499PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Zuccala G, Onder G, Pedone C, Carosella L, Pahor M, Bernabei R, Cocchi A (2001) Hypotension and cognitive impairment: selective association in patients with heart failure. Neurology 57:1986–1992PubMed Zuccala G, Onder G, Pedone C, Carosella L, Pahor M, Bernabei R, Cocchi A (2001) Hypotension and cognitive impairment: selective association in patients with heart failure. Neurology 57:1986–1992PubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Heijer T, Skoog I, Oudkerk M, de Leeuw FE, de Groot JC, Hofman A, Breteler MM (2003) Association between blood pressure levels over time and brain atrophy in the elderly. Neurobiol Aging 24:307–313PubMedCrossRef Heijer T, Skoog I, Oudkerk M, de Leeuw FE, de Groot JC, Hofman A, Breteler MM (2003) Association between blood pressure levels over time and brain atrophy in the elderly. Neurobiol Aging 24:307–313PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruitenberg A, Skoog I, Ott A, Aevarsson O, Witteman JC, Lernfelt B, van Harskamp F, Hofman A, Breteler M (2001) Blood pressure and risk of dementia. Results from the Rotterdam study and the Gothenburg H-70 study. Dement Geriatr Cogn 12:33–39CrossRef Ruitenberg A, Skoog I, Ott A, Aevarsson O, Witteman JC, Lernfelt B, van Harskamp F, Hofman A, Breteler M (2001) Blood pressure and risk of dementia. Results from the Rotterdam study and the Gothenburg H-70 study. Dement Geriatr Cogn 12:33–39CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Duschek S, Schandry R (2005) Subjektive Beschwerden und kognitive Minderleistungen bei essentieller Hypotonie. VT&VM 26:5–31 Duschek S, Schandry R (2005) Subjektive Beschwerden und kognitive Minderleistungen bei essentieller Hypotonie. VT&VM 26:5–31
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Richter-Heinrich E, Borys M, Sprung H, Läuter J (1971) Psychophysiologische Reaktionsprofile von Hypo- und Hypertonikern. Deut Gesundheitswes 32:1481–1489 Richter-Heinrich E, Borys M, Sprung H, Läuter J (1971) Psychophysiologische Reaktionsprofile von Hypo- und Hypertonikern. Deut Gesundheitswes 32:1481–1489
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Cadalbert B (1997) Die Psychophysiologie des niedrigen Blutdrucks: Kreislaufregulation, Lebensgewohnheiten und Beschwerden. Peter Lang, Frankfurt a.M Cadalbert B (1997) Die Psychophysiologie des niedrigen Blutdrucks: Kreislaufregulation, Lebensgewohnheiten und Beschwerden. Peter Lang, Frankfurt a.M
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Bismarck M, Rust G (1982) Erste Erfahrungen mit dem Antihypotonikum Thomasin (=Etilefrin) unter ambulanten Bedingungen (eine Doppelblindstudie). Z Ärztliche Fortb 76:153–156 Bismarck M, Rust G (1982) Erste Erfahrungen mit dem Antihypotonikum Thomasin (=Etilefrin) unter ambulanten Bedingungen (eine Doppelblindstudie). Z Ärztliche Fortb 76:153–156
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Schandry R (1999) Die Verbesserung der subjektiven Befindlichkeit bei orthostatischer Hypotonie unter dem Einfluss blutdrucksteigernder Therapie. Med Welt 50:160–165 Schandry R (1999) Die Verbesserung der subjektiven Befindlichkeit bei orthostatischer Hypotonie unter dem Einfluss blutdrucksteigernder Therapie. Med Welt 50:160–165
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Weisz N, Schandry R, Jacobs A, Mialet J, Duschek S (2002) Early contingent negative variation of the EEG and attentional flexibility are reduced in hypotension. Int J Psychophysiol 45:253–260PubMedCrossRef Weisz N, Schandry R, Jacobs A, Mialet J, Duschek S (2002) Early contingent negative variation of the EEG and attentional flexibility are reduced in hypotension. Int J Psychophysiol 45:253–260PubMedCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Dembowsky K, Seller H (1995) Arterial baroreceptor reflexes. In: Vaitl D, Schandry R (eds) From the heart to the brain: the psychophysiology of circulation–brain interaction. Europäischer Verlag der Wissenschaften, Frankfurt a.M., pp 35–60 Dembowsky K, Seller H (1995) Arterial baroreceptor reflexes. In: Vaitl D, Schandry R (eds) From the heart to the brain: the psychophysiology of circulation–brain interaction. Europäischer Verlag der Wissenschaften, Frankfurt a.M., pp 35–60
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Igler FO, Donegan JH, Hoo KC, Korns ME, Kampine JP (1981) Chronic localized hypotension and resetting of carotid sinus baroreceptors. Electrophy- siological and histological studies in the dog. Circ Res 49:649–654PubMed Igler FO, Donegan JH, Hoo KC, Korns ME, Kampine JP (1981) Chronic localized hypotension and resetting of carotid sinus baroreceptors. Electrophy- siological and histological studies in the dog. Circ Res 49:649–654PubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Abels D (1954) K-V-T. Konzentrations-Verlaufs-Test. Hogrefe, Göttingen Abels D (1954) K-V-T. Konzentrations-Verlaufs-Test. Hogrefe, Göttingen
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Stegagno L, Angrilli A, Costa M, Palomba D (1996) Deficit cognitivi e ipotensione arteriosa: Un indagine cronopsycofisiologica. G Ital Psicol 23:837–859 Stegagno L, Angrilli A, Costa M, Palomba D (1996) Deficit cognitivi e ipotensione arteriosa: Un indagine cronopsycofisiologica. G Ital Psicol 23:837–859
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D (1962) Wechsler memory scale. Forma I. Adattazione Italaliana di E. Cimino. Organizzazioni Speciali, Firence Wechsler D (1962) Wechsler memory scale. Forma I. Adattazione Italaliana di E. Cimino. Organizzazioni Speciali, Firence
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Baddeley A (1982) Your memory. A user’s guide. Multimedia Publications, London Baddeley A (1982) Your memory. A user’s guide. Multimedia Publications, London
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Costa M, Stegagno L, Schandry R, Bitti PER (1998) Contingent negative variation and cognitive performance in hypotension. Psychophysiology 35:737–744PubMedCrossRef Costa M, Stegagno L, Schandry R, Bitti PER (1998) Contingent negative variation and cognitive performance in hypotension. Psychophysiology 35:737–744PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Brickenkamp R (1994) Test d2, Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test. Hogrefe, Göttingen Brickenkamp R (1994) Test d2, Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test. Hogrefe, Göttingen
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Oswald WD, Roth E (1987) Der Zahlen-Verbindungs-Test (ZVT) – Ein sprachfreier Intelligenz-Test zur Messung der “kognitiven Leistungsgeschwindigkeit”. Hogrefe, Göttingen Oswald WD, Roth E (1987) Der Zahlen-Verbindungs-Test (ZVT) – Ein sprachfreier Intelligenz-Test zur Messung der “kognitiven Leistungsgeschwindigkeit”. Hogrefe, Göttingen
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Morris MC, Scherr PA, Hebert LE, Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Glynn RJ, Evans DA (2002) Association between blood pressure and cognitive function in a biracial community population of older persons. Neuroepidemiology 21:123–130PubMedCrossRef Morris MC, Scherr PA, Hebert LE, Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Glynn RJ, Evans DA (2002) Association between blood pressure and cognitive function in a biracial community population of older persons. Neuroepidemiology 21:123–130PubMedCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Albert M (1993) Neuropsychological changes in healthy adult humans across the age range. Neurobiol Aging 14:623–625PubMedCrossRef Albert M (1993) Neuropsychological changes in healthy adult humans across the age range. Neurobiol Aging 14:623–625PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith A (1984) Symbol digit modalities test. Manual – Revised. Western Psychological, Los Angeles Smith A (1984) Symbol digit modalities test. Manual – Revised. Western Psychological, Los Angeles
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) Mini mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiat Res 12:189–198PubMedCrossRef Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) Mini mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiat Res 12:189–198PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Mialet JP, Jacobs A, Bisserbe JC (1995) Computerized attentional and cognitive efficiency battery: preliminary results. Eur J Psychol Assess 11(suppl. 1):111 Mialet JP, Jacobs A, Bisserbe JC (1995) Computerized attentional and cognitive efficiency battery: preliminary results. Eur J Psychol Assess 11(suppl. 1):111
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Duschek S, Weisz N, Schandry R (2003) Reduced cognitive performance and prolonged reaction time accompany moderate hypotension. Clin Auton Res 13:427–432PubMed Duschek S, Weisz N, Schandry R (2003) Reduced cognitive performance and prolonged reaction time accompany moderate hypotension. Clin Auton Res 13:427–432PubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Duschek S, Matthias E, Schandry R (2005) Essential hypotension is accompanied by deficits in attention and working memory. Behav Med 30:149–158PubMedCrossRef Duschek S, Matthias E, Schandry R (2005) Essential hypotension is accompanied by deficits in attention and working memory. Behav Med 30:149–158PubMedCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Zimmermann P, Fimm B (2002) Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung (TAP). Version 1.7. Handbuch Teile 1 und 2. Psytest, Herzogenrath Zimmermann P, Fimm B (2002) Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung (TAP). Version 1.7. Handbuch Teile 1 und 2. Psytest, Herzogenrath
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Schoppe KJ (1974) Das MLS-Gerät. Ein neuer Testapparat zur Messung feinmotorischer Leistungen. Diagnostica 20:43–46 Schoppe KJ (1974) Das MLS-Gerät. Ein neuer Testapparat zur Messung feinmotorischer Leistungen. Diagnostica 20:43–46
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Von Zerssen D (1976) Klinische Selbstbeurteilungs-Skalen aus dem Münchner Psychiatrischen Informations-System (PSYCHIS) München. Die Befindlichkeitsskala, Beltz, Weinheim Von Zerssen D (1976) Klinische Selbstbeurteilungs-Skalen aus dem Münchner Psychiatrischen Informations-System (PSYCHIS) München. Die Befindlichkeitsskala, Beltz, Weinheim
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Critchley HD, Mathias CJ (2003) Blood pressure, attention and cognition: drivers and air traffic controllers. Clin Auton Res 13:399–401PubMed Critchley HD, Mathias CJ (2003) Blood pressure, attention and cognition: drivers and air traffic controllers. Clin Auton Res 13:399–401PubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Shapiro D, Miller RE, King HE, Ginchereau EH, Fitzgibbon K (1982) Behavioral consequences of mild hypertension. Hypertension 4:355–360PubMed Shapiro D, Miller RE, King HE, Ginchereau EH, Fitzgibbon K (1982) Behavioral consequences of mild hypertension. Hypertension 4:355–360PubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Waldstein SR, Ryan CM, Manuck SB, Parkinson DK, Bromet EJ (1991) Learning and memory function in men with untreated blood pressure elevation. J Consult Clin Psychol 59:513–517PubMedCrossRef Waldstein SR, Ryan CM, Manuck SB, Parkinson DK, Bromet EJ (1991) Learning and memory function in men with untreated blood pressure elevation. J Consult Clin Psychol 59:513–517PubMedCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Pandav R, Dodge HH, DeKosky ST, Ganguli M (2003) Blood pressure and cognitive impairment in India and the United States: a cross-national epidemiological study. Arch Neurol 60:1123–1128PubMedCrossRef Pandav R, Dodge HH, DeKosky ST, Ganguli M (2003) Blood pressure and cognitive impairment in India and the United States: a cross-national epidemiological study. Arch Neurol 60:1123–1128PubMedCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Waldstein SR, Giggey PP, Thayer JF, Zonderman AB (2005) Nonlinear relations of blood pressure to cognitive function: the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Hypertension 45:374–379PubMedCrossRef Waldstein SR, Giggey PP, Thayer JF, Zonderman AB (2005) Nonlinear relations of blood pressure to cognitive function: the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Hypertension 45:374–379PubMedCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Duschek S, Meinhardt J, Schandry R (2006) Reduced cortical activity due to chronic low blood pressure: an EEG study. Biol Psychol 72:241–250PubMedCrossRef Duschek S, Meinhardt J, Schandry R (2006) Reduced cortical activity due to chronic low blood pressure: an EEG study. Biol Psychol 72:241–250PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Tecce JJ, Cattanach L (1993) Contingent negative variation. In: Niedermeyer E, Lopes da Silva F (ed) Electroencephalography: basic principles clinical applications, and related fields. Urban & Schwarzenberg, Baltimore, pp 657–679 Tecce JJ, Cattanach L (1993) Contingent negative variation. In: Niedermeyer E, Lopes da Silva F (ed) Electroencephalography: basic principles clinical applications, and related fields. Urban & Schwarzenberg, Baltimore, pp 657–679
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Coons HW, Peloquin LJ, Klorman R, Bauer LO, Ryan RM, Perlmutter RA, Salzman LF (1981) Effect of methylphenidate on young adult’s vigilance and event-related potentials. Electroen Clin Neuro 51:373–387CrossRef Coons HW, Peloquin LJ, Klorman R, Bauer LO, Ryan RM, Perlmutter RA, Salzman LF (1981) Effect of methylphenidate on young adult’s vigilance and event-related potentials. Electroen Clin Neuro 51:373–387CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Fattaposta F, Floris R, Sanarelli L, Terribile P, Tilia G (1984) Changes in slow cerebral potentials (CNV) subsequent to midazolam administration. Double-blind study. Riv Neurol 54:289–298PubMed Fattaposta F, Floris R, Sanarelli L, Terribile P, Tilia G (1984) Changes in slow cerebral potentials (CNV) subsequent to midazolam administration. Double-blind study. Riv Neurol 54:289–298PubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Chao LL, Meyerhoff DJ, Cardenas VA, Rothlind JC, Weiner MW (2003) Abnormal CNV in chronic heavy drinkers. Clin Neurophysiol 114:2081–2095PubMedCrossRef Chao LL, Meyerhoff DJ, Cardenas VA, Rothlind JC, Weiner MW (2003) Abnormal CNV in chronic heavy drinkers. Clin Neurophysiol 114:2081–2095PubMedCrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Claverie B, Brun A, Nizard A, Brenot P, Paty J (1984) Multiparametric outlines with CNV: applications to depressive syndromes. In: Karrer R, Cohen J, Tueting P (eds) Brain and information: event-related potentials. New York Academy of Sciences, New York, pp 556–564 Claverie B, Brun A, Nizard A, Brenot P, Paty J (1984) Multiparametric outlines with CNV: applications to depressive syndromes. In: Karrer R, Cohen J, Tueting P (eds) Brain and information: event-related potentials. New York Academy of Sciences, New York, pp 556–564
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Andreassi JL (2000) Human behavior and physiological response. Lawrence Erlbaum, London Andreassi JL (2000) Human behavior and physiological response. Lawrence Erlbaum, London
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Davidson RJ, Jackson DC, Larson CL (2000) Human electroencephalography. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson GG (eds) Handbook of psychophysiology. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 27–52 Davidson RJ, Jackson DC, Larson CL (2000) Human electroencephalography. In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson GG (eds) Handbook of psychophysiology. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 27–52
55.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Heims HC, Critchley HD, Martin NH, Jäger HR, Mathias CJ, Cipolotti L (2006) Cognitive functioning in orthostatic hypotension due to pure autonomic failure. Clin Auton Res 16:113–120PubMedCrossRef Heims HC, Critchley HD, Martin NH, Jäger HR, Mathias CJ, Cipolotti L (2006) Cognitive functioning in orthostatic hypotension due to pure autonomic failure. Clin Auton Res 16:113–120PubMedCrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Vaitl D, Gruppe H (1991) Baroreceptor stimulation and changes in EEG and vigilance. In: Perssons PB, Kirchheim HR (eds) Baroreceptor reflexes: integrative functions and clinical aspects. Springer, Berlin, pp 293–313 Vaitl D, Gruppe H (1991) Baroreceptor stimulation and changes in EEG and vigilance. In: Perssons PB, Kirchheim HR (eds) Baroreceptor reflexes: integrative functions and clinical aspects. Springer, Berlin, pp 293–313
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Craig AD (2003) Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13:500–505PubMedCrossRef Craig AD (2003) Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13:500–505PubMedCrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Gómez CM, Vaquero E, Vázquez-Marrufo M (2004) A neurocognitive model of short-term sensory and motor preparatory activity in humans. Psicológica 25:217–229 Gómez CM, Vaquero E, Vázquez-Marrufo M (2004) A neurocognitive model of short-term sensory and motor preparatory activity in humans. Psicológica 25:217–229
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Sturm W, de Simone A, Krause BJ, Specht K, Hesselmann V, Radermacher I, Herzog H, Tellmann L, Muller-Gartner HW, Willmes K (1999) Functional anatomy of intrinsic alertness: evidence for a fronto-parietal-thalamic-brainstem network in the right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 37:797–805PubMedCrossRef Sturm W, de Simone A, Krause BJ, Specht K, Hesselmann V, Radermacher I, Herzog H, Tellmann L, Muller-Gartner HW, Willmes K (1999) Functional anatomy of intrinsic alertness: evidence for a fronto-parietal-thalamic-brainstem network in the right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia 37:797–805PubMedCrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Posner MI, Petersen SE (1990) The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 13:25–42PubMedCrossRef Posner MI, Petersen SE (1990) The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 13:25–42PubMedCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Carlson NR (2004) Physiology of behavior. Pearson Education, Harlow Carlson NR (2004) Physiology of behavior. Pearson Education, Harlow
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Jennings JR (2003) Autoregulation of blood pressure and thought: preliminary results of an application of brain imaging to psychosomatic medicine. Psychosom Med 65:384–395PubMedCrossRef Jennings JR (2003) Autoregulation of blood pressure and thought: preliminary results of an application of brain imaging to psychosomatic medicine. Psychosom Med 65:384–395PubMedCrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Paulson OB (2002) Blood–brain barrier, brain metabolism and cerebral blood flow. Eur Neuropsychopharm 12:495–501CrossRef Paulson OB (2002) Blood–brain barrier, brain metabolism and cerebral blood flow. Eur Neuropsychopharm 12:495–501CrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Duschek S, Schandry R (2004) Cognitive performance and cerebral blood flow in essential hypotension. Psychophysiology 41:905–913PubMedCrossRef Duschek S, Schandry R (2004) Cognitive performance and cerebral blood flow in essential hypotension. Psychophysiology 41:905–913PubMedCrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Duschek S, Schandry R (2003) Functional transcranial Doppler sonography as a tool in psychophysiological research. Psychophysiology 40:436–454PubMedCrossRef Duschek S, Schandry R (2003) Functional transcranial Doppler sonography as a tool in psychophysiological research. Psychophysiology 40:436–454PubMedCrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Duschek S, Schandry R (2006) Deficient adjustment of cerebral blood flow to cognitive activity due to chronically low blood pressure. Biol Psychol 72:311–317PubMedCrossRef Duschek S, Schandry R (2006) Deficient adjustment of cerebral blood flow to cognitive activity due to chronically low blood pressure. Biol Psychol 72:311–317PubMedCrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat McHenry LC, West JW, Cooper ES, Goldberg HI, Jaffe ME (1974) Cerebral autoregulation in man. Stroke 5:695–706PubMed McHenry LC, West JW, Cooper ES, Goldberg HI, Jaffe ME (1974) Cerebral autoregulation in man. Stroke 5:695–706PubMed
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Olesen J (1973) Quantitative evaluation of normal and pathologic cerebral blood flow regulation to perfusion pressure. Changes in man. Arch Neurol (Chicago) 28:143–148 Olesen J (1973) Quantitative evaluation of normal and pathologic cerebral blood flow regulation to perfusion pressure. Changes in man. Arch Neurol (Chicago) 28:143–148
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Strandgaard S (1976) Autoregulation of cerebral blood flow in hypertensive patients. The modifying influence of prolonged antihypertensive treatment on the tolerance to acute, drug-induced hypotension. Circulation 53:720–727PubMed Strandgaard S (1976) Autoregulation of cerebral blood flow in hypertensive patients. The modifying influence of prolonged antihypertensive treatment on the tolerance to acute, drug-induced hypotension. Circulation 53:720–727PubMed
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Strandgaard S, Olesen J, Skinhoj E, Lassen NA (1973) Autoregulation of brain circulation in severe arterial hypertension. Brit Med J 1:507–510PubMedCrossRef Strandgaard S, Olesen J, Skinhoj E, Lassen NA (1973) Autoregulation of brain circulation in severe arterial hypertension. Brit Med J 1:507–510PubMedCrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Lennox WG, Gibbs EL (1932) The blood flow in the brain and the leg of man and the changes induced by alternation of blood gases. J Clin Invest 11:1155–1177PubMedCrossRef Lennox WG, Gibbs EL (1932) The blood flow in the brain and the leg of man and the changes induced by alternation of blood gases. J Clin Invest 11:1155–1177PubMedCrossRef
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Larsen FS, Olsen KS, Hansen BA, Paulson OB, Knudsen GM (1994) Transcranial Doppler is valid for determination of the lower limit of cerebral blood flow autoregulation. Stroke 25:1985–1988PubMed Larsen FS, Olsen KS, Hansen BA, Paulson OB, Knudsen GM (1994) Transcranial Doppler is valid for determination of the lower limit of cerebral blood flow autoregulation. Stroke 25:1985–1988PubMed
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Olsen KS, Svenden LB, Larsen FS, Paulson OB (1995) Effect of labetalol on cerebral blood flow, oxygen metabolism and autoregulation in healthy humans. Br J Anaesth 75:51–54PubMed Olsen KS, Svenden LB, Larsen FS, Paulson OB (1995) Effect of labetalol on cerebral blood flow, oxygen metabolism and autoregulation in healthy humans. Br J Anaesth 75:51–54PubMed
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Olsen KS, Svendsen LB, Larsen FS (1996) Validation of transcranial near-infrared spectroscopy for evaluation of cerebral blood flow autoregulation. J Neurosurg Anaesth 8:280–285 Olsen KS, Svendsen LB, Larsen FS (1996) Validation of transcranial near-infrared spectroscopy for evaluation of cerebral blood flow autoregulation. J Neurosurg Anaesth 8:280–285
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Drummond JC (1997) The lower limit of autoregulation: time to revise our thinking? Anesthesiology 86:1431–143PubMedCrossRef Drummond JC (1997) The lower limit of autoregulation: time to revise our thinking? Anesthesiology 86:1431–143PubMedCrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Waldemar G, Schmidt JF, Andersen AR, Vorstrup S, Ibsen H, Paulson OB (1989) Angiotensin converting enzyme inhibition and cerebral blood flow autoregulation in normotensive and hypertensive man. J Hypertens 7:229–235PubMedCrossRef Waldemar G, Schmidt JF, Andersen AR, Vorstrup S, Ibsen H, Paulson OB (1989) Angiotensin converting enzyme inhibition and cerebral blood flow autoregulation in normotensive and hypertensive man. J Hypertens 7:229–235PubMedCrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuschinsky W (1991) Coupling of function, metabolism, and blood flow in the brain. Neurosurg Rev 14:163–168PubMedCrossRef Kuschinsky W (1991) Coupling of function, metabolism, and blood flow in the brain. Neurosurg Rev 14:163–168PubMedCrossRef
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Duschek S, Hadjamu M, Schandry R (2006) Enhancement of cerebral blood flow and cognitive performance due to pharmacological blood pressure elevation in chronic hypotension. Psychophysiology 44:(in press) Duschek S, Hadjamu M, Schandry R (2006) Enhancement of cerebral blood flow and cognitive performance due to pharmacological blood pressure elevation in chronic hypotension. Psychophysiology 44:(in press)
Metadaten
Titel
Reduced brain perfusion and cognitive performance due to constitutional hypotension
verfasst von
Stefan Duschek
Rainer Schandry
Publikationsdatum
01.04.2007
Erschienen in
Clinical Autonomic Research / Ausgabe 2/2007
Print ISSN: 0959-9851
Elektronische ISSN: 1619-1560
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10286-006-0379-7

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2007

Clinical Autonomic Research 2/2007 Zur Ausgabe