Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Neuroscience Bulletin 5/2018

Open Access 01.10.2018 | Original Article

Separate Neural Networks for Gains and Losses in Intertemporal Choice

verfasst von: Yang-Yang Zhang, Lijuan Xu, Zhu-Yuan Liang, Kun Wang, Bing Hou, Yuan Zhou, Shu Li, Tianzi Jiang

Erschienen in: Neuroscience Bulletin | Ausgabe 5/2018

Abstract

An important and unresolved question is how human brain regions process information and interact with each other in intertemporal choice related to gains and losses. Using psychophysiological interaction and dynamic causal modeling analyses, we investigated the functional interactions between regions involved in the decision-making process while participants performed temporal discounting tasks in both the gains and losses domains. We found two distinct intrinsic valuation systems underlying temporal discounting in the gains and losses domains: gains were specifically evaluated in the medial regions, including the medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortices, and losses were evaluated in the lateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. In addition, immediate reward or punishment was found to modulate the functional interactions between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and distinct regions in both the gains and losses domains: in the gains domain, the mesolimbic regions; in the losses domain, the medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and insula. These findings suggest that intertemporal choice of gains and losses might involve distinct valuation systems, and more importantly, separate neural interactions may implement the intertemporal choices of gains and losses. These findings may provide a new biological perspective for understanding the neural mechanisms underlying intertemporal choice of gains and losses.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s12264-018-0267-x) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Yang-Yang Zhang, Lijuan Xu and Zhu-Yuan Liang contributed equally to this work.

Introduction

Decisions about daily life, such as education, diet, investment, and saving, all involve intertemporal choices, which refer to decisions between smaller/sooner and larger/later rewards or punishments [1]. People naturally devalue rewards in accordance with the delay in receiving them, which is referred to as “temporal discounting” [25]. Economists assume that people evaluate delayed losses in a similar way by discounting the value of outcomes according to the delay. This assumption implies that losses may recruit the same mechanisms as gains. However, several studies have reported that the aversion to losses declines more slowly than the attractiveness of gains as the delay increases, suggesting different neural correlations in intertemporal choice of gains and losses [612]. These findings challenge the view that gains- and losses-related temporal discounting can be condensed into a single discount function [13]. Therefore, whether a single neural process underlies the intertemporal choices in gains and losses in particular, and whether the domain of gains and losses differentially alters interactions within decision-related regions of the brain remain unclear.
Recent findings suggest that interactions between brain regions might play a critical role in intertemporal choice of gains and losses. McClure et al. [14, 15] originally found that temporal discounting is associated with two neural systems: the mesolimbic midbrain dopamine system is activated when making choices of immediate rewards, whereas the fronto-parietal system is activated when making choices independent of delay. Afterwards, Kable and Glimcher [16] found that a common valuation system appears to evaluate the subjective values of delayed monetary rewards, regardless of the delay time. However, Xu et al. found that, accompanied by the smaller/sooner choices in the losses domain than in the gains domain, different mesolimbic regions are activated in choices that involve immediate gains and losses, although lateral prefrontal regions are commonly activated in both gains and losses [17]. Consistent with this finding, a small number of studies highlighted that the nature of the interaction between these systems or regions is essential for understanding the neural mechanisms of intertemporal choice [1820]. For example, we used the same clusters as in the Xu et al. study [17], and found an asymmetrical effect of approach/avoidance motivation on the functional connectivity in both the gain and loss domains [18]. However, the effective connectivity among these regions (the influence exerted by one region on another) and how the effective connectivity differed in the gains and losses domains have still not been investigated.
In the neuroscience field, two methods are commonly used to evaluate functional integration, i.e., the effective connectivity, between brain regions: psychophysiological interaction (PPI) and dynamic causal modeling (DCM). PPI identifies regions that show altered connectivity in response to the influence of a defined seed region, this connectivity being modulated by the current experimental context [21]. DCM is a nonlinear systems identification procedure that uses Bayesian estimates of parameters to make inferences about the coupling among areas and how that coupling is influenced by changes in the experimental context [22, 23]. DCM together with PPI could make inferences about the directionality of the effects and about the most likely functional architecture. Thus, these analyses would contribute to investigating whether distinct neural networks exist and how the dynamic interactions within these networks are altered between intertemporal choice in the gains and losses domains.
In the present study, using PPI and DCM analyses, we further analyzed our previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data to investigate the functional interactions of the brain regions involved in intertemporal choice in the gains and losses domains. We first identified the critical regions that showed activation patterns covarying with seed regions depending on experimental context using PPI analysis [21] and then inferred the causal architecture among those regions using DCM analysis [22]. Of the seed regions, we particularly focused on the ventral striatum and the medial cortical regions (including the orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortices), which have been implicated in reward valuation and the computation of goal values [2429]; the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, which exerts cognitive control of decision-making processes [2935]; and the anterior cingulate and insula, which are involved in responding to aversive stimuli, the evaluation and representation of negative emotional states, and even pain [17, 3641].

Methods

Participants

Twenty healthy, right-handed Chinese graduate students [10 females; 25.0 ± 1.7 years old (range, 22–29 years, no gender differences in age (P = 0.74) or educational level (P = 0.12)] were recruited from the Institute of Automation, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing. All the participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Beijing MRI Center for Brain Research, and all participants provided written informed consent. Two participants, however, were excluded because of excessive head motion (absolute displacement with regard to the reference scan exceeded 2 mm).

Experimental Task

A temporal discounting task that was previously employed by McClure et al. (2004) was used in our study. Specifically, we revised the original task by using a symmetric pattern of gains and losses domains, which allowed for the extraction of BOLD signal patterns associated with each single trial, and a direct comparison was then performed between gain- and loss-related brain activities. The temporal discounting task included two parts: a temporal discounting task involving gains (G-TD) and a temporal discounting task involving losses (L-TD). In both parts, the participants were asked to choose from immediate and delayed options that varied across trials. In each trial, the participants were simultaneously presented with a smaller/sooner (SS) option (e.g., “¥40 today”) and a larger/later (LL) option (e.g., “¥60 in one month”). The SS option was available at different times [selected from the set (today, two weeks from now, and one month from now)]. The delay between the LL options was either two weeks or one month. The amount of the monetary reward in the SS options ranged from ¥13 to ¥110 and was randomly drawn from a Gaussian distribution with a mean of ¥50 and a standard deviation of ¥25, with integer conversion. The percentage difference in amounts between the two rewards was selected from the set {5%, 10%, 15%, 25%, 35%, and 50%}. Participants were asked to select one option by pressing a button that corresponded to the location of the chosen option on the screen. Both tasks had identical form, with the exception that a minus sign before a monetary amount indicated that the money would be lost in the L-TD.
The procedure of the temporal discounting task is shown in Fig. 1 (also in our previous study [17]). In each trial, a 2-s fixation period was presented, which indicated the next trial. The selected result was visible for 2 s after the participant’s response, followed by a black screen for 10 s. In the instructions about the L-TD, an initial offer of ¥150, which corresponded to the maximum amount a participant could lose, was made available for each participant. Participants received the corresponding payment at the specified time in one trial which was randomly selected from each task to ensure incentive compatibility.
Besides the temporal discounting task, we also assessed the participants’ approach and avoidance personality in a separate session after the scanning, using two subscales of Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire [42, 43]: the Impulsive and the Neuroticism-Anxiety subscales. As reported in our previous work [18], the mean scores of approach personality and avoidance personality of all 18 participants were 2.33 ± 1.46 and 6.67 ± 3.68, respectively. No gender differences were found for approach (P = 0.76) and avoidance personality (P = 0.26).

fMRI Data Acquisition and Preprocessing

Imaging was performed using a 3.0T Siemens MR scanner (Erlangen, Germany) at the Beijing MRI Center for Brain Research. Functional data were acquired in 26 axial slices using an echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR/TE = 2000/30 ms, flip angle = 90°, field of view = 19.2 cm, matrix = 64 × 64, thickness = 3 mm, gap = 1 mm). High-resolution T1-weighted anatomical images were collected in the two functional runs. The first run was the G-TD, and the second was the L-TD.
The analysis was conducted using SPM8 software (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK). The first five images were discarded from the analysis to allow for magnetic saturation effects. Functional images were corrected for differences in slice acquisition timing, corrected for head motion, normalized to a standard EPI template, and then spatially smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full-width-at-half-maximum.

Whole-Brain Analysis

The whole-brain analysis was performed as in our previous work [17]. Our aim was to identify regions activated during G-TD and L-TD. The whole-brain analysis was first conducted at the individual level using the voxel-wise general linear model. We separately created the motion parameters and two task-related regressors for each participant: one for trials in which the early option was available immediately, the other for all trials. Then, individual T-contrasts generated for each regressor were entered into a second-level group analysis with the one-sample t-test. The regions activated in each task then served as the regions of interest (ROIs) for the subsequent analysis. The statistical threshold was set to P < 0.05 (uncorrected). For more details of the analysis and results of whole-brain analysis, refer to Xu et al. [17].

Psychophysiological Interaction Analysis

After identifying the regions involved in both G-TD and L-TD in the whole-brain analysis, we used PPI analysis to capture the interactions between these regions in relation to the experimental conditions [21]. PPI analysis allows the detection of regionally specific responses in one brain region in terms of the interaction between input from another region and a cognitive/sensory process [21]. At this stage of analysis, we used an a priori, hypothesis-driven method similar to those used in other areas of research [4446].
For each participant, the regions showing a selective increase in activation in response to choices that involved immediate options in the whole-brain analysis were selected as seed regions for the PPI analysis. The selected seed regions for each individual were the medial orbitofrontal cortex (MOFC), medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and the ventral striatum in G-TD, and the PCC, MPFC, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and insula in L-TD.
To perform PPI analyses, the first Eigen variate time series from a sphere of 12-mm radius centered on the most significant voxel from the seed regions were extracted. For each seed region, the effect of the interaction term was computed as the element-by-element product of the seed region time series and a vector coding the contrast of interest (1 for the immediate condition and –1 for the delayed condition). The individual contrast images were then entered into the second level to perform a random effects analysis (using a one-sample t test). A stringent random effects model with a priori defined regions and a statistical threshold of P < 0.005 (uncorrected) was used. For inference purposes, the regions reported in supplementary Tables S1 and S2 are corrected for multiple comparisons based on Monte Carlo simulation using the AFNI AlphaSim program [47]. A combined threshold of 0.01 and a cluster size >74 resampled voxels determined by Monte Carlo simulation were used to correct for multiple comparisons at a statistical threshold of P < 0.05.
The PPI analysis was an important first step in exploring the functional interactions of the activated regions in the TD task. Based on the regions detected in the PPI analysis, we used DCM analysis to further explore the effects of the stimulus input, the causal direction, and the bilinear modulatory effect of the experimental conditions in the intertemporal choice.

Dynamic Causal Modeling Analysis

To identify the brain regions that responded to the input from all the choices and to determine how the connection strengths were modulated by the immediate condition, we used DCM to examine the directional influence between the regions that were detected using the PPI analysis. A series of subject-specific dynamic causal models were separately constructed for the G-TD and the L-TD.
For the G-TD, fMRI time-courses were extracted from the MPFC, MOFC, ventral striatum, and bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortices (dlPFC) (Fig. 2A) that had been identified in the whole-brain and the PPI analyses. Four possible models were defined in which the stimulus inputs (i.e., the presentation of choices regardless of the delay) were connected to different parts of the network: (1) a model with stimulus inputs to all the five regions; (2) a model with stimulus inputs to the medial-striatal regions, including the MPFC, MOFC, and ventral striatum; (3) a model with stimulus inputs to the bilateral dlPFC only; and (4) a model with stimulus inputs to the medial frontal regions, including the MPFC and MOFC (Fig. S1). All the models assumed bidirectional intrinsic connections among these five regions. Because PPI analysis indicated that the interactions within the medial-striatal network and between the dlPFC and the medial-striatal network were enhanced in the immediate condition, we set the immediate condition to modulate the connections within the medial-striatal network and the connections between the dlPFC and the medial-striatal network.
For the L-TD, fMRI time-series were extracted from four regions (the MPFC, ACC, insula, and right dlPFC) (Fig. 2B) that were identified in the whole-brain and PPI analyses. The left dlPFC that came from the mirror image of the right dlPFC (48, 30, 24) was also taken into account, considering that the left dlPFC plays an essential role of cognitive control in intertemporal choice [48, 49]. Four possible models were defined in which the driving inputs were connected with different parts of the network: (1) a model with stimulus inputs to all the five regions; (2) a model with stimulus inputs to the MPFC-cingulate-insula regions; (3) a model with stimulus inputs to the bilateral dlPFC only; and (4) a model with stimulus inputs to the MPFC only (Fig. S2). All the models assumed bidirectional intrinsic connections among these five regions and assumed that the immediate condition modulated the connections within the MPFC-cingulate-insula network and the connections between the dlPFC and the MPFC-cingulate-insula network.
For each seed region in both tasks, fMRI time-series were extracted from a 12-mm sphere centered on maximum coordinates. The maximum coordinates of the MPFC, MOFC, and ventral striatum in the G-TD, and the MPFC, ACC, and insula in the L-TD were based on the whole-brain analysis, and those of the bilateral dlPFC were based on the PPI analysis (listed in Tables S3 and S4).
After constructing a series of DCMs for each participant, we compared these DCMs to separately obtain optimal models for the G-TD and the L-TD using Bayesian model selection. Once the optimal model was selected, the participant-specific immediate-related modulatory parameters were entered into group analysis with a one-sample t test. This allowed us to summarize the consistent findings from the subject-specific DCMs using classical statistics. Because our study was hypothesis-driven, the results at a threshold of P < 0.05 (uncorrected and Bonferroni corrected) are reported.

Results

In our previous report [17], we found a significantly larger percentage of SS choices in the L-TD than in the G-TD, suggesting a significant reduction in the delay discounting for future losses compared with future gains. In addition, we found activation of the PCC, MOFC, MPFC, and ventral striatum in the G-TD as well as the PCC, MPFC, ACC, and insula in the L-TD. Having separately identified the regions that exhibited characteristic responses in G-TD and L-TD, we used effective connectivity analysis to characterize the regional integrations in intertemporal choices of gains and losses.

G-TD

In our previous report [17], we found significant activation across all decision epochs of gains in the frontal-parietal network, including the dlPFC, PPC, and lateral orbitofrontal cortex. We also found enhanced activation in the PCC and the medial-striatal network (including the MOFC, MPFC, and ventral striatum) for choices in which money was available immediately. We focused on the lateral prefrontal cortices and the medial-striatal network in the PPI analysis because these regions have been implicated in cognitive control of the decision-making process [3035] and subjective value evaluation [2428]. PPI analysis revealed greater functional coupling within the medial-striatal network and between the lateral dlPFC and the medial-striatal network (including the MOFC, MPFC, and ventral striatum) when comparing the immediate condition with the delayed condition (Fig. 3, Table S1). Specifically, compared with all delayed options, the immediate options were accompanied by increased functional connectivity between the MOFC and left DLPFC (x = –48, y = 45, z = 3, t-score = 3.53), between the MPFC and right DLPFC (x = 45, y = 42, z = 30, t-score = 3.51), and between the ventral striatum and right DLPFC (x = 45, y = 42, z = 30, t-score = 4.23). The ventral striatum also showed a positive covariation with the medial frontal cortex, including the MOFC (x = 15, y = 60, z = -6, t-score = 3.71) and MPFC (x = 3, y = 60, z = 12, t-score = 4.12), when comparing the immediate condition with the delayed condition.
The DCM analysis revealed a preference for model 4 in which all choices (regardless of delay) posited a significant influence on the MPFC and MOFC (Figs. 4A and 6; also see Fig. S3). In this model, the immediate condition enhanced the connectivity from the bilateral dlPFC and the medial frontal regions, including the MPFC and MOFC. The modulating effect of the immediate condition on the connection from the ventral striatum to the dlPFC was also positive, which indicated that the effective connectivity from the ventral striatum to the dlPFC was even stronger. It should be noted that the immediate condition led to a significant “inhibitory” modulation in the activation of the pathway from the MOFC to the MPFC. Furthermore, the immediate condition also reduced the connectivity from the MOFC to the dlPFC, which indicated that competition between these regions occurred under the immediate condition.

L-TD

In our previously published findings, decision-related activation was found in the frontal-parietal network (including the dlPFC, lateral orbitofrontal cortex, and PPC), thalamus and right striatum across all decision epochs of losses [17]. We also found that the ACC, insula, MPFC, superior frontal gyrus, and PCC showed significantly enhanced activation when money was lost immediately. In the PPI analysis, we focused on the lateral prefrontal cortices and the MPFC-cingulate-insula network, including the MPFC, ACC and insula, because these regions have been implicated in aversive stimuli and negative emotional states [36, 37, 39, 50]. PPI analysis revealed greater functional coupling within the MPFC-cingulate-insula network and between the dlPFC and MPFC-cingulate-insula network upon comparing the immediate condition with the delayed condition (Fig. 5, Table S2). Specifically, the immediate condition enhanced the functional connectivity between the MPFC and ACC (x = 6, y = 30, z = 27, t-score = 3.23), the functional connectivity between the right insula and ACC (x = 3, y = 33, z = 27, t-score = 4.54), and between the right insula and right dlPFC (x = 48, y = 30, z = 24, t-score = 5.35).
The DCM analysis revealed a preference for model 3 in which all the choices (regardless of delay) posited a significant influence on the bilateral dlPFC (Figs. 4B and 6; also see Fig. S4). In this optimal model, the immediate condition enhanced the connectivity from the bilateral dlPFC to the MPFC-cingulate-insula network (Fig. 4B). The immediate condition also enhanced local connectivity within the MPFC-cingulate-insula network, with stronger connectivity from insula to MPFC and ACC, and bidirectional connectivity between MPFC and ACC.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate whether dynamic interactions within intertemporal decision-related brain regions were different between gains and losses domains, and whether they were modulated by the immediate condition in which money was available immediately. Our previous studies identified multiple regions involved in temporal discounting and preliminarily established whether the underlying functional interactions between these regions showed differences between the gains and losses domains [17, 18]. The present study revealed two main findings. First, we found that gains were specifically evaluated in the medial regions, including the MPFC and MOFC, whereas losses were evaluated in the lateral regions (the dlPFC). Second, the immediate condition consistently modulated the functional coupling between the dlPFC and the medial-striatal regions in the G-TD, and between the dlPFC and the MPFC-cingulate-insula regions in the L-TD.

Where Do the Choice Stimuli Enter?

Our results from the DCM analysis revealed a systematic difference in the evaluation of intertemporal choices between the gains domain and the losses domain. This suggested that there might be distinct mechanisms underlying the evaluation of intertemporal choice in the domains of gains and losses.
Specifically, in the G-TD, the DCM analysis indicated that the choice stimuli input was connected to the medial frontal cortices, including the MPFC and MOFC, which evaluated the subjective values of all gains, regardless of delay. These results are consistent with a substantial body of research implying that the mesolimbic regions are involved in the processing of anticipated rewards [27, 5153]. The MOFC is involved in coding the relative values of different reward stimuli [24, 54, 55] and in updating the incentive value of outcomes in response to devaluation [56]. Given the role of the MPFC in integrating various kinds of reward value information [16, 26, 35, 57, 58], the MPFC and MOFC might integrate the anticipated subjective utility of future gains.
In the L-TD, the DCM analysis indicated that the choice stimuli input was connected with the bilateral dlPFC which had been implicated in prospective processing and future planning [30, 59]. These results are consistent with the arguments that decision makers might experience “savor” from anticipating future gains when a positive outcome is delayed [60, 61] and that they might experience “dread” from anticipating future losses when the outcome is negative [7, 38, 50]. Our finding that all choice stimuli were connected to the dlPFC might suggest that individuals experienced a strong feeling of dread as a result of anticipating the subjective utility associated with losses. Together with our previous reports [17, 18], participants preferred more SS losses than SS gains, indicating that the fear of future losses was a greater motivation than the pleasure associated with future gains. Therefore, the dlPFC might exert greater cognitive control to compete with the strong emotional tendency to avoid larger losses.
At the same time, the difference of DCM results between the gains and losses domains, especially the differential role of the dlPFC, raised a question about the role of the dlPFC in gains and losses. There are several potential explanations. First, there might be multiple valence-dependent valuation signals and neural processes which evaluate the subjective utility of gains and losses. The finding that choice stimuli enter on different regions in G-TD and L-TD might indicate different valuation processing of positive and negative outcomes. Specifically, the medial regions might be more sensitive to gains, whereas the dlPFC may be particularly sensitive to losses. This potential explanation is consistent with previous studies which found that lateral areas, including the dlPFC, are significantly activated in negative utility processing [17, 55, 62]. Another potential explanation is that losing money is correlated with negative motivation. Although the findings that greater preference for SS losses than gains might suggest that losses involve negative emotions, this does not indicate that such patterns of intertemporal choice of losses are irrational. Individuals might need more cognitive control to override the tendency to delay the acquisition of smaller losses in order to avoid larger losses. Therefore, activation in the dlPFC might suggest such an attempt to override the prepotent aversion to delay upcoming losses [48].

How Do Brain Regions Interact to Arrive at a Decision?

Our findings might clarify several issues in the brain networks of intertemporal choice.
First, in the G-TD, our findings revealed a “two-system” involvement in the brain networks of intertemporal choice: the mesolimbic regions (i.e., the MPFC and MOFC) and the lateral cortical regions (i.e., the dlPFC). In contrast to McClure et al.’s [14, 15] notion that delayed and immediate rewards are separated, our DCM analysis of gains indicated that the mesolimbic regions respond to both delayed and immediate rewards by showing that all choice stimuli inputs were connected to the MPFC and MOFC. The dlPFC, in our findings, modulated neural responses to rewards, indicating its role in cognitive control of the delay discounting of gains. This finding is consistent with a recent study conducted by Hare et al. [35], which found that the medial frontal regions evaluate the common valuation signal, whereas the dlPFC plays a critical role in self-control by modulating the value signal encoded in the medial frontal regions. Thus, in the context of gains, the dlPFC might be involved in inhibiting the tendency to choose the SS rewards [13].
Second, our results might indicate distinct roles of the MOFC and MPFC in receiving a common value signal. This result is consistent with a recent study which reported dissociable roles of the MOFC and MPFC in the rat [63], suggesting that the MOFC plays a direct role during decision-making that extends simple outcome monitoring and representation, while the MPFC is mainly associated with representing rewards. Specifically, we found that the reciprocal connections from the MOFC to the dlPFC and the MPFC were both negative in G-TD, indicating an inhibitory effect of MOFC in intertemporal choice. Several studies have indicated that the MOFC might be involved in updating the incentive value of outcomes in humans [56] and that the MOFC might even be the critical region that mediates impulsive choice in animals [64]. Still, lesion studies have reported contradictory results on the directional roles of the MOFC in impulsive choices: either a decrease after the development of lesions in the MOFC [56, 65, 66] or an increase in impulsive choices [64, 67]. Our results suggest a unidirectional inhibitory role of the MOFC in impulsive choices. Together with the above research, our results indicate that the functional interactions between the MOFC and the MPFC convey motivational incentives based on the subjective value of gains by regulating the differential engagement of the MPFC, while the functional interactions from the MOFC to the dlPFC convey impulsivity information for inhibiting behavior.
Third, our findings in the L-TD revealed that intertemporal choices of losses recruited the integration of the neural circuitry of negative emotion-related regions and the lateral prefrontal cortex. This notion is supported by our findings that the immediate condition enhanced the forward connectivity from the bilateral dlPFC to the MPFC-cingulate-insula network and the connectivity within the MPFC-cingulate-insula network. Compared with gains, losses provoke emotional responses associated with fear or dread [24]. Neuroimaging studies have shown that the insula is activated in response to the negative emotions induced by social exclusion [68], unfairness [36], a disgusting odor [69], or the feeling of dread induced by a delayed unpleasant outcome [50]. Other studies have shown that the ACC responds to a variety of negative utilities or expected values which are derived from the consumption of goods and services [50, 7072]. We also found that the immediate condition enhanced the functional interactions between the insula, ACC, and MPFC, which indicated that immediate losses might invoke stronger negative emotions. The forward connectivity from the dlPFC to these emotion-related regions was also enhanced in the immediate condition, suggesting that the participants attempted to override the prepotent aversion to delay the upcoming losses. Above all, our results indicate that the dlPFC plays a key role in biasing the emotion-related network.

Limitations

It should be noted that our study has several inherent limitations from the original study [17]. The relatively small sample size of our study might bias the selection of the seed regions, which mainly depends on statistical thresholding in the whole-brain analysis. In the fMRI analysis, we did not include the age and gender as covariates to control their effects. In addition, the experimental design in which the gain and loss trials were separately presented in different blocks might cause a contextual effect. Future studies should improve the experimental design by presenting the gain and loss trials in a randomized way within the same block, or draw from paradigms that directly examine the processes involved in risky decision making [73].

Conclusions

In summary, the results of this study provide evidence that separate neural networks underlie the intertemporal choice of gains and losses. We found two distinct valuation systems for gains and losses. In addition, compared with delayed choices, immediate options modulated functional integration between the lateral prefrontal cortex and mesolimbic regions in gains, and functional integration between the lateral prefrontal cortex and emotional regions in losses. These results indicate that valence might exert its influence via distinct mechanisms in the gain and loss domains. In particular choices, especially when one option is immediately available, the subjective values of the options might be modulated by different regions between gains and losses. Above all, the separate neural networks for gains and losses enrich our understanding of the neural mechanisms in intertemporal choice.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (71471171, 71071150, 91432302, 31620103905, 31471005, and 71761167001), the Science Frontier Program of the Chinese Academy of Sciences (QYZDJ-SSW-SMC019), the Shenzhen Peacock Plan (KQTD2015033016104926), the Guangdong Pearl River Talents Plan Innovative and Entrepreneurial Team (2016ZT06S220), and the CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology (Y5CX052003). We also would like to thank Yan-Mei Li, Jie-Hong Xu, and Yan Sun for their help in experimental design; Zhendong Liu for helping with data acquisition; Yongfu Hao for help with the artwork; and Drs. Edmund F. and Rhoda E. Perozzi for their assistance with English.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Neurologie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes, den Premium-Inhalten der neurologischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Neurologie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Anhänge

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Frederick S, Loewenstein G, O’Donoghue T. Time discounting and time preference: a critical review. J Econ Lit 2002, 40: 351–401.CrossRef Frederick S, Loewenstein G, O’Donoghue T. Time discounting and time preference: a critical review. J Econ Lit 2002, 40: 351–401.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Samuelson P. A note on measurement of utility. Rev Econ Stud 1937, 4: 155–161.CrossRef Samuelson P. A note on measurement of utility. Rev Econ Stud 1937, 4: 155–161.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Ainslie G. Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol Bull 1975, 82: 463–496.CrossRefPubMed Ainslie G. Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol Bull 1975, 82: 463–496.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Ainslie G. Breakdown of Will. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 2001.CrossRef Ainslie G. Breakdown of Will. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 2001.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang Z, Liang S, Yu S, Xie T, Wang B, Wang J, et al. Distinct roles of dopamine receptors in the lateral thalamus in a rat model of decisional impulsivity. Neurosci Bull 2017, 33: 413–422.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wang Z, Liang S, Yu S, Xie T, Wang B, Wang J, et al. Distinct roles of dopamine receptors in the lateral thalamus in a rat model of decisional impulsivity. Neurosci Bull 2017, 33: 413–422.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Thaler R. Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency. Econ Lett 1981, 8: 201.CrossRef Thaler R. Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency. Econ Lett 1981, 8: 201.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Loewenstein G. Anticipation and the valuation of delayed consumption. Econ J 1987, 97: 666–684.CrossRef Loewenstein G. Anticipation and the valuation of delayed consumption. Econ J 1987, 97: 666–684.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat MacKeigan LD, Larson LN, Draugalis JR, Bootman JL, Burns LR. Time preference for health gains versus health losses. Pharmacoeconomics 1993, 3: 374–386.CrossRefPubMed MacKeigan LD, Larson LN, Draugalis JR, Bootman JL, Burns LR. Time preference for health gains versus health losses. Pharmacoeconomics 1993, 3: 374–386.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Murphy JG, Vuchinich RE, Simpson CA. Delayed reward and cost discounting. Psychol Rec 2001, 51: 571–588. Murphy JG, Vuchinich RE, Simpson CA. Delayed reward and cost discounting. Psychol Rec 2001, 51: 571–588.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Read D. Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing, 2004: 424–443.CrossRef Read D. Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing, 2004: 424–443.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Estle SJ, Green L, Myerson J, Holt DD. Differential effects of amount on temporal and probability discounting of gains and losses. Mem Cognit 2006, 34: 914–928.CrossRefPubMed Estle SJ, Green L, Myerson J, Holt DD. Differential effects of amount on temporal and probability discounting of gains and losses. Mem Cognit 2006, 34: 914–928.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Mitchell SH, Wilson VB. The subjective value of delayed and probabilistic outcomes: outcome size matters for gains but not for losses. Behav Process 2010, 83: 36–40.CrossRefPubMed Mitchell SH, Wilson VB. The subjective value of delayed and probabilistic outcomes: outcome size matters for gains but not for losses. Behav Process 2010, 83: 36–40.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Kalenscher T, Pennartz CM. Is a bird in the hand worth two in the future? The neuroeconomics of intertemporal decision-making. Prog Neurobiol 2008, 84: 284–315.CrossRefPubMed Kalenscher T, Pennartz CM. Is a bird in the hand worth two in the future? The neuroeconomics of intertemporal decision-making. Prog Neurobiol 2008, 84: 284–315.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 2004, 306: 503–507.CrossRefPubMed McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 2004, 306: 503–507.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat McClure SM, Ericson KM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. Time discounting for primary rewards. J Neurosci 2007, 27: 5796–5804.CrossRefPubMed McClure SM, Ericson KM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. Time discounting for primary rewards. J Neurosci 2007, 27: 5796–5804.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Xu L, Liang ZY, Wang K, Li S, Jiang T. Neural mechanism of intertemporal choice: from discounting future gains to future losses. Brain Res 2009, 1261: 65–74.CrossRefPubMed Xu L, Liang ZY, Wang K, Li S, Jiang T. Neural mechanism of intertemporal choice: from discounting future gains to future losses. Brain Res 2009, 1261: 65–74.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang YY, Xu L, Rao LL, Zhou L, Zhou Y, Jiang T, et al. Gain-loss asymmetry in neural correlates of temporal discounting: an approach-avoidance motivation perspective. Sci Rep 2017, 6: 31902.CrossRef Zhang YY, Xu L, Rao LL, Zhou L, Zhou Y, Jiang T, et al. Gain-loss asymmetry in neural correlates of temporal discounting: an approach-avoidance motivation perspective. Sci Rep 2017, 6: 31902.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Li N, Ma N, Liu Y, He XS, Sun DL, Fu XM, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity predicts impulsivity in economic decision-making. J Neurosci 2013, 33: 4886–4895.CrossRefPubMed Li N, Ma N, Liu Y, He XS, Sun DL, Fu XM, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity predicts impulsivity in economic decision-making. J Neurosci 2013, 33: 4886–4895.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Hare T, Hakimi S, Rangel A. Activity in dlPFC and its effective connectivity to vmPFC are associated with temporal discounting. Front Neurosci 2014, 8: 50. Hare T, Hakimi S, Rangel A. Activity in dlPFC and its effective connectivity to vmPFC are associated with temporal discounting. Front Neurosci 2014, 8: 50.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR, Morris J, Rolls E, Dolan RJ. Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage 1997, 6: 218–229.CrossRefPubMed Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR, Morris J, Rolls E, Dolan RJ. Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage 1997, 6: 218–229.CrossRefPubMed
22.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Penny WD, Stephan KE, Mechelli A, Friston KJ. Comparing dynamic causal models. Neuroimage 2004, 22: 1157–1172.CrossRefPubMed Penny WD, Stephan KE, Mechelli A, Friston KJ. Comparing dynamic causal models. Neuroimage 2004, 22: 1157–1172.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Breiter HC, Aharon I, Kahneman D, Dale A, Shizgal P. Functional imaging of neural responses to expectancy and experience of monetary gains and losses. Neuron 2001, 30: 619–639.CrossRefPubMed Breiter HC, Aharon I, Kahneman D, Dale A, Shizgal P. Functional imaging of neural responses to expectancy and experience of monetary gains and losses. Neuron 2001, 30: 619–639.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C. Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2001, 4: 95–102.CrossRefPubMed O’Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C. Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2001, 4: 95–102.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Doherty J, Critchley H, Deichmann R, Dolan RJ. Dissociating valence of outcome from behavioral control in human orbital and ventral prefrontal cortices. J Neurosci 2003, 23: 7931–7939.CrossRefPubMed O’Doherty J, Critchley H, Deichmann R, Dolan RJ. Dissociating valence of outcome from behavioral control in human orbital and ventral prefrontal cortices. J Neurosci 2003, 23: 7931–7939.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Hare TA, O’Doherty J, Camerer CF, Schultz W, Rangel A. Dissociating the role of the orbitofrontal cortex and the striatum in the computation of goal values and prediction errors. J Neurosci 2008, 28: 5623–5630.CrossRefPubMed Hare TA, O’Doherty J, Camerer CF, Schultz W, Rangel A. Dissociating the role of the orbitofrontal cortex and the striatum in the computation of goal values and prediction errors. J Neurosci 2008, 28: 5623–5630.CrossRefPubMed
28.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Peters J, Büchel C. The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability. Trends Cogn Sci 2011, 15: 227–239.CrossRefPubMed Peters J, Büchel C. The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability. Trends Cogn Sci 2011, 15: 227–239.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci 2001, 24: 167–202.CrossRefPubMed Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci 2001, 24: 167–202.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Paulus MP, Hozack N, Frank L, Brown GG. Error rate and outcome predictability affect neural activation in prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate during decision-making. Neuroimage 2002, 15: 836–846.CrossRefPubMed Paulus MP, Hozack N, Frank L, Brown GG. Error rate and outcome predictability affect neural activation in prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate during decision-making. Neuroimage 2002, 15: 836–846.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Sanfey AG, Loewenstein G, McClure SM, Cohen JD. Neuroeconomics: cross-currents in research on decision-making. Trends Cogn Sci 2006, 10: 108–116.CrossRefPubMed Sanfey AG, Loewenstein G, McClure SM, Cohen JD. Neuroeconomics: cross-currents in research on decision-making. Trends Cogn Sci 2006, 10: 108–116.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Gold JI, Shadlen MN. The neural basis of decision making. Annu Rev Neurosci 2007, 30: 535–574.CrossRefPubMed Gold JI, Shadlen MN. The neural basis of decision making. Annu Rev Neurosci 2007, 30: 535–574.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Hare TA, Camerer CF, Rangel A. Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science 2009, 324: 646–648.CrossRefPubMed Hare TA, Camerer CF, Rangel A. Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science 2009, 324: 646–648.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Sanfey AG, Rilling JK, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD. The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science 2003, 300: 1755–1758.CrossRefPubMed Sanfey AG, Rilling JK, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD. The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science 2003, 300: 1755–1758.CrossRefPubMed
37.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Berns GS, Laibson D, Loewenstein G. Intertemporal choice–toward an integrative framework. Trends Cogn Sci 2007, 11: 482–488.CrossRefPubMed Berns GS, Laibson D, Loewenstein G. Intertemporal choice–toward an integrative framework. Trends Cogn Sci 2007, 11: 482–488.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Caria A, Sitaram R, Veit R, Begliomini C, Birbaumer N. Volitional control of anterior insula activity modulates the response to aversive stimuli: a real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 2010, 68: 425–432.CrossRefPubMed Caria A, Sitaram R, Veit R, Begliomini C, Birbaumer N. Volitional control of anterior insula activity modulates the response to aversive stimuli: a real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 2010, 68: 425–432.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang K, Zhu Y, Zhu Y, Wu S, Liu H, Zhang W, et al. Molecular, functional, and structural imaging of major depressive disorder. Neurosci Bull 2016, 32: 273–285.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Zhang K, Zhu Y, Zhu Y, Wu S, Liu H, Zhang W, et al. Molecular, functional, and structural imaging of major depressive disorder. Neurosci Bull 2016, 32: 273–285.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Reddan MC, Wager TD. Modeling pain using fMRI: from regions to biomarkers. Neurosci Bull 2018, 34: 208–215.CrossRefPubMed Reddan MC, Wager TD. Modeling pain using fMRI: from regions to biomarkers. Neurosci Bull 2018, 34: 208–215.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Zuckerman M, Kuhlman DM, Joireman J, Teta P, Kraft M. A comparison of three structural models for personality: the big three, the big five, and the alternative five. J Personal Soc Psychol 1993, 65: 757–768.CrossRef Zuckerman M, Kuhlman DM, Joireman J, Teta P, Kraft M. A comparison of three structural models for personality: the big three, the big five, and the alternative five. J Personal Soc Psychol 1993, 65: 757–768.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang W, Wu YX, Peng ZG, Lu SW, Yu L, Wang GP, et al. Test of sensation seeking in a Chinese sample. Personal Individ Differ 2000, 28: 169–179.CrossRef Wang W, Wu YX, Peng ZG, Lu SW, Yu L, Wang GP, et al. Test of sensation seeking in a Chinese sample. Personal Individ Differ 2000, 28: 169–179.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Das P, Kemp AH, Liddell BJ, Brown KJ, Olivieri G, Peduto A, et al. Pathways for fear perception: modulation of amygdala activity by thalamo-cortical systems. Neuroimage 2005, 26: 141–148.CrossRefPubMed Das P, Kemp AH, Liddell BJ, Brown KJ, Olivieri G, Peduto A, et al. Pathways for fear perception: modulation of amygdala activity by thalamo-cortical systems. Neuroimage 2005, 26: 141–148.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Williams LM, Das P, Liddell BJ, Kemp AH, Rennie CJ, Gordon E. Mode of functional connectivity in amygdala pathways dissociates level of awareness for signals of fear. J Neurosci 2006, 26: 9264–9271.CrossRefPubMed Williams LM, Das P, Liddell BJ, Kemp AH, Rennie CJ, Gordon E. Mode of functional connectivity in amygdala pathways dissociates level of awareness for signals of fear. J Neurosci 2006, 26: 9264–9271.CrossRefPubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Yoshino A, Okamoto Y, Onoda K, Yoshimura S, Kunisato Y, Demoto Y, et al. Sadness enhances the experience of pain via neural activation in the anterior cingulate cortex and amygdala: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 2010, 50: 1194–1201.CrossRefPubMed Yoshino A, Okamoto Y, Onoda K, Yoshimura S, Kunisato Y, Demoto Y, et al. Sadness enhances the experience of pain via neural activation in the anterior cingulate cortex and amygdala: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 2010, 50: 1194–1201.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Forman SD, Cohen JD, Fitzgerald M, Eddy WF, Mintun MA, Noll DC. Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magn Reson Med 1995, 33: 636–647.CrossRefPubMed Forman SD, Cohen JD, Fitzgerald M, Eddy WF, Mintun MA, Noll DC. Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magn Reson Med 1995, 33: 636–647.CrossRefPubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Luhmann CC, Chun MM, Yi DJ, Lee D, Wang XJ. Neural dissociation of delay and uncertainty in intertemporal choice. J Neurosci 2008, 28: 14459–14466.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Luhmann CC, Chun MM, Yi DJ, Lee D, Wang XJ. Neural dissociation of delay and uncertainty in intertemporal choice. J Neurosci 2008, 28: 14459–14466.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Figner B, Knoch D, Johnson E, Krosch A, Lisanby S, Fehr E, et al. Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice. Nat Neurosci 2010, 13: 538–539.CrossRefPubMed Figner B, Knoch D, Johnson E, Krosch A, Lisanby S, Fehr E, et al. Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice. Nat Neurosci 2010, 13: 538–539.CrossRefPubMed
50.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Elliott R, Newman JL, Longe OA, Deakin JF. Differential response patterns in the striatum and orbitofrontal cortex to financial reward in humans: a parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 2003, 23: 303–307.CrossRefPubMed Elliott R, Newman JL, Longe OA, Deakin JF. Differential response patterns in the striatum and orbitofrontal cortex to financial reward in humans: a parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 2003, 23: 303–307.CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Knutson B, Cooper JC. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward prediction. Curr Opin Neurol 2005, 18: 411–417.CrossRefPubMed Knutson B, Cooper JC. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward prediction. Curr Opin Neurol 2005, 18: 411–417.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Chib VS, Rangel A, Shimojo S, O’Doherty JP. Evidence for a common representation of decision values for dissimilar goods in human ventromedial prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 2009, 29: 12315–12320.CrossRefPubMed Chib VS, Rangel A, Shimojo S, O’Doherty JP. Evidence for a common representation of decision values for dissimilar goods in human ventromedial prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 2009, 29: 12315–12320.CrossRefPubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Doherty JP. Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2004, 14: 769–776.CrossRefPubMed O’Doherty JP. Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2004, 14: 769–776.CrossRefPubMed
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu X, Powell DK, Wang H, Gold BT, Corbly CR, Joseph JE. Functional dissociation in frontal and striatal areas for processing of positive and negative reward information. J Neurosci 2007, 27: 4587–4597.CrossRefPubMed Liu X, Powell DK, Wang H, Gold BT, Corbly CR, Joseph JE. Functional dissociation in frontal and striatal areas for processing of positive and negative reward information. J Neurosci 2007, 27: 4587–4597.CrossRefPubMed
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW. Contrasting roles of basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in impulsive choice. J Neurosci 2004, 24: 4718–4722.CrossRefPubMed Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW. Contrasting roles of basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in impulsive choice. J Neurosci 2004, 24: 4718–4722.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Knutson B, Taylor J, Kaufman M, Peterson R, Glover G. Distributed neural representation of expected value. J Neurosci 2005, 25: 4806–4812.CrossRefPubMed Knutson B, Taylor J, Kaufman M, Peterson R, Glover G. Distributed neural representation of expected value. J Neurosci 2005, 25: 4806–4812.CrossRefPubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Plassmann H, O’Doherty J, Rangel A. Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactions. J Neurosci 2007, 27: 9984–9988.CrossRefPubMed Plassmann H, O’Doherty J, Rangel A. Orbitofrontal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic transactions. J Neurosci 2007, 27: 9984–9988.CrossRefPubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Tanaka SC, Doya K, Okada G, Ueda K, Okamoto Y, Yamawaki S. Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops. Nat Neurosci 2004, 7: 887–893.CrossRefPubMed Tanaka SC, Doya K, Okada G, Ueda K, Okamoto Y, Yamawaki S. Prediction of immediate and future rewards differentially recruits cortico-basal ganglia loops. Nat Neurosci 2004, 7: 887–893.CrossRefPubMed
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Marshall A. Principles of Economics. 2nd ed. London: Macmillian, 1981. Marshall A. Principles of Economics. 2nd ed. London: Macmillian, 1981.
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Loewenstein G, Thaler RH. Anomalies: intertemporal choice. J Econ Perspect 1989, 3: 181–193.CrossRef Loewenstein G, Thaler RH. Anomalies: intertemporal choice. J Econ Perspect 1989, 3: 181–193.CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu X, Hairston J, Schrier M, Fan J. Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav R 2011, 35: 1219–1236.CrossRef Liu X, Hairston J, Schrier M, Fan J. Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav R 2011, 35: 1219–1236.CrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Dalley JW, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW. Double dissociation between serotonergic and dopaminergic modulation of medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex during a test of impulsive choice. Cereb Cortex 2006, 16: 106–114.CrossRefPubMed Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Dalley JW, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW. Double dissociation between serotonergic and dopaminergic modulation of medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex during a test of impulsive choice. Cereb Cortex 2006, 16: 106–114.CrossRefPubMed
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Rudebeck PH, Walton ME, Smyth AN, Bannerman DM, Rushworth MF. Separate neural pathways process different decision costs. Nat Neurosci 2006, 9: 1161–1168.CrossRefPubMed Rudebeck PH, Walton ME, Smyth AN, Bannerman DM, Rushworth MF. Separate neural pathways process different decision costs. Nat Neurosci 2006, 9: 1161–1168.CrossRefPubMed
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Kheramin S, Body S, Mobini S, Ho MY, Velazquez-Martinez DN, Bradshaw CM, et al. Effects of quinolinic acid-induced lesions of the orbital prefrontal cortex on inter-temporal choice: a quantitative analysis. Psychopharmacology 2002, 165: 9–17.CrossRefPubMed Kheramin S, Body S, Mobini S, Ho MY, Velazquez-Martinez DN, Bradshaw CM, et al. Effects of quinolinic acid-induced lesions of the orbital prefrontal cortex on inter-temporal choice: a quantitative analysis. Psychopharmacology 2002, 165: 9–17.CrossRefPubMed
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Winstanley CA, Baunez C, Theobald DE, Robbins TW. Lesions to the subthalamic nucleus decrease impulsive choice but impair autoshaping in rats: the importance of the basal ganglia in Pavlovian conditioning and impulse control. Eur J Neurosci 2005, 21: 3107–3116.CrossRefPubMed Winstanley CA, Baunez C, Theobald DE, Robbins TW. Lesions to the subthalamic nucleus decrease impulsive choice but impair autoshaping in rats: the importance of the basal ganglia in Pavlovian conditioning and impulse control. Eur J Neurosci 2005, 21: 3107–3116.CrossRefPubMed
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Mobini S, Body S, Ho MY, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E, Deakin JF, et al. Effects of lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex on sensitivity to delayed and probabilistic reinforcement. Psychopharmacology 2002, 160: 290–298.CrossRefPubMed Mobini S, Body S, Ho MY, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E, Deakin JF, et al. Effects of lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex on sensitivity to delayed and probabilistic reinforcement. Psychopharmacology 2002, 160: 290–298.CrossRefPubMed
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD, Williams KD. Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science 2003, 302: 290–292.CrossRefPubMed Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD, Williams KD. Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science 2003, 302: 290–292.CrossRefPubMed
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Wicker B, Keysers C, Plailly J, Royet JP, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G. Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 2003, 40: 655–664.CrossRefPubMed Wicker B, Keysers C, Plailly J, Royet JP, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G. Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 2003, 40: 655–664.CrossRefPubMed
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Peyron R, Laurent B, Garcia-Larrea L. Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis. Neurophysiol Clin 2000, 30: 263–288.CrossRefPubMed Peyron R, Laurent B, Garcia-Larrea L. Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis. Neurophysiol Clin 2000, 30: 263–288.CrossRefPubMed
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Gehring WJ, Willoughby AR. The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science 2002, 295: 2279–2282.CrossRefPubMed Gehring WJ, Willoughby AR. The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science 2002, 295: 2279–2282.CrossRefPubMed
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Yeung N, Sanfey AG. Independent coding of reward magnitude and valence in the human brain. J Neurosci 2004, 24: 6258–6264.CrossRefPubMed Yeung N, Sanfey AG. Independent coding of reward magnitude and valence in the human brain. J Neurosci 2004, 24: 6258–6264.CrossRefPubMed
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhou L, Zhang YY, Li S, Liang ZY. New paradigms for the old question: Challenge the expectation rule held by risky decision-making theories. J Pac Rim Psychol 2018, 12: E17.CrossRef Zhou L, Zhang YY, Li S, Liang ZY. New paradigms for the old question: Challenge the expectation rule held by risky decision-making theories. J Pac Rim Psychol 2018, 12: E17.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Separate Neural Networks for Gains and Losses in Intertemporal Choice
verfasst von
Yang-Yang Zhang
Lijuan Xu
Zhu-Yuan Liang
Kun Wang
Bing Hou
Yuan Zhou
Shu Li
Tianzi Jiang
Publikationsdatum
01.10.2018
Verlag
Springer Singapore
Erschienen in
Neuroscience Bulletin / Ausgabe 5/2018
Print ISSN: 1673-7067
Elektronische ISSN: 1995-8218
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12264-018-0267-x

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 5/2018

Neuroscience Bulletin 5/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Schützt Olivenöl vor dem Tod durch Demenz?

10.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Konsumieren Menschen täglich 7 Gramm Olivenöl, ist ihr Risiko, an einer Demenz zu sterben, um mehr als ein Viertel reduziert – und dies weitgehend unabhängig von ihrer sonstigen Ernährung. Dafür sprechen Auswertungen zweier großer US-Studien.

Bluttest erkennt Parkinson schon zehn Jahre vor der Diagnose

10.05.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Ein Bluttest kann abnorm aggregiertes Alpha-Synuclein bei einigen Menschen schon zehn Jahre vor Beginn der motorischen Parkinsonsymptome nachweisen. Mit einem solchen Test lassen sich möglicherweise Prodromalstadien erfassen und die Betroffenen früher behandeln.

Darf man die Behandlung eines Neonazis ablehnen?

08.05.2024 Gesellschaft Nachrichten

In einer Leseranfrage in der Zeitschrift Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology möchte ein anonymer Dermatologe bzw. eine anonyme Dermatologin wissen, ob er oder sie einen Patienten behandeln muss, der eine rassistische Tätowierung trägt.

Wartezeit nicht kürzer, aber Arbeit flexibler

Psychotherapie Medizin aktuell

Fünf Jahren nach der Neugestaltung der Psychotherapie-Richtlinie wurden jetzt die Effekte der vorgenommenen Änderungen ausgewertet. Das Hauptziel der Novellierung war eine kürzere Wartezeit auf Therapieplätze. Dieses Ziel wurde nicht erreicht, es gab jedoch positive Auswirkungen auf andere Bereiche.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.