Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2012

Open Access 01.12.2012 | Research article

Internet-delivered attention bias modification training in individuals with social anxiety disorder - a double blind randomized controlled trial

verfasst von: Per Carlbring, Maria Apelstrand, Helena Sehlin, Nader Amir, Andreas Rousseau, Stefan G Hofmann, Gerhard Andersson

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2012

Abstract

Background

Computerized cognitive bias modification for social anxiety disorder has in several well conducted trials shown great promise with as many as 72% no longer fulfilling diagnostic criteria after a 4 week training program. To test if the same program can be transferred from a clinical setting to an internet delivered home based treatment the authors conducted a randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial.

Methods

After a diagnostic interview 79 participants were randomized to one of two attention training programs using a probe detection task. In the active condition the participant was trained to direct attention away from threat, whereas in the placebo condition the probe appeared with equal frequency in the position of the threatening and neutral faces.

Results

Results were analyzed on an intention-to-treat basis, including all randomized participants. Immediate and 4-month follow-up results revealed a significant time effect on all measured dimensions (social anxiety scales, general anxiety and depression levels, quality of life). However, there were no time x group interactions. The lack of differences in the two groups was also mirrored by the infinitesimal between group effect size both at post test and at 4-month follow-up.

Conclusion

We conclude that computerized attention bias modification may need to be altered before dissemination for the Internet.

Trial registration

ISRCTN01715124
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1471-244X-12-66) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

Seven of the eight authors declare that there is no conflict of interest. However, Dr Amir has founded a company that market online anxiety relief products.

Authors’ contribution

All authors contributed to the design of this study. PC drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to the further writing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Background

A recent development in the treatment of anxiety disorders is attention bias modification [1], which derives from basic research on information processing of threat relevant stimuli in various conditions such as social anxiety disorder [2]. There is an extensive literature on the link between attention and anxiety [3] and this link appears to be causal [4, 5]. Attentional bias for threat in social anxiety is commonly measured using the probe detection task [68]. In the probe detection task [9] (for a review, see)[10], participants see a pair of faces at two different spatial locations on a screen. One of the faces is threatening, the other face is neutral. After the offset of these faces, a probe appears replacing the threatening face (congruent presentation) or neutral face stimulus (incongruent presentation). Faster responses to detect probes replacing threat faces than probes replacing neutral faces is used as an index of attentional bias toward threat relevant information.
Moreover, there is evidence that successful treatment for social phobia may lead to a normalization of attention bias for threat [11]. This finding is consistent with the view that attention bias to threat-relevant information plays a role in the maintenance of social phobia. Based on these preliminary findings, MacLeod and colleagues [12] reported the first study to train attention and examine its effect on anxiety. These researchers found that participants in the Attend Threat condition showed faster response latencies for detecting probes following threat words than neutral words. Participants in the Attend Neutral condition showed the opposite pattern of results. Moreover, this training extended to word pairs containing novel threat-relevant words and was not confined to specifically trained word pairs. Finally, participants in the Attend Threat condition responded more negatively to the experimental stressor than did those in the Attend Neutral condition. Extending this research, investigators have examined the effect of attention training in clinical populations. Recent reviews [4, 5, 13] suggest that attention training can be effective in in reducing anxiety in clinical and non-clinical population.
In a meta-analysis on the effects of attention bias modification by Hakamata and coworkers [14], 10 randomized controlled trials were identified including 467 participants. The average between group effect size was d = 0.61 on anxiety measures, and effects could be established up to 4 months after treatment termination. However, effect sizes differed widely between the studies, but a strength in this research is the possibility to have an active placebo control condition [5].
Attention bias modification is a potentially effective computerized treatment that most often has been delivered in a laboratory setting with subclinical samples e.g., [15]. The training can sometimes be as short as one session with a total of 128 stimulus pairings [16]. The treatment involves repeatedly redirecting attention away from socially relevant threat cues in order to induce preferential selective processing of neutral (non-threat) stimuli [17]. Up until recently few studies on attention bias modification have targeted clinical populations of people suffering from social anxiety. One exception is a study in which 44 participants with generalized social phobia were randomized to one of two computerized attention bias modification programs [17]. Both conditions used a combination of faces expressing neutral and/or disgust emotions. After practicing twice a week during 4 weeks – equaling approximately 160 minutes in total – half of the participants in the treatment group as compared to 14% in the placebo group no longer met criteria for social phobia. In addition, there were significant effects on all outcome measures such as lower levels of anxiety, emotional distress, depression and better functioning in work, social and family life. The training was made at a university clinic, which theoretically could be a barrier for help seeking since eye-contact and talking to authority figures is a core problem for some of the sufferers. Hence, if the treatment could be administered via the internet that could open the door for millions of people in need. Another study that also found positive outcomes was carried out by Schmidt and coworkers [18]. At termination, 72% of patients in the active treatment condition, relative to 11% of patients in the control condition, no longer met criteria for social phobia. The results were maintained at 4-month follow-up. Finally, McEvoy and Perini [19] have investigated whether or not supplementing cognitive behavioral group therapy (CBT) with attention training could potentiate greater changes. It was tentatively concluded that while supplementing CBT with attention training did not improve outcomes, increasing attention control during CBT was associated with symptom relief.
Another recent development in the treatment of social anxiety disorder is the possibility to deliver CBT over the Internet [20]. A large number of trials have found that CBT delivered over the Internet can be as effective as face-to-face CBT, even in direct comparison [21, 22], and effects have been documented by at least four independent research groups in Australia [23, 24], Spain [25], Sweden [26, 27], and Switzerland [28, 29]. Given the two recent developments and the fact that attention bias modification is delivered via computer we decided to investigate if this novel treatment could be delivered via the Internet with no physical contact with the study participants. This has been done once in a recent trial by Boettcher and co-workers [30]. In fact, they could not replicate the promising results from the American studies. The aim of the present study was to test if attention bias modification delivered via the Internet was superior to a placebo condition (random attention training) in a group of participants with diagnosed social anxiety disorder recruited from the community. We hypothesized that the treatment would be better than the control condition and that it would be possible to present attention bias modification via the Internet.

Methods

Participants and recruitment

The general procedure was similar to our previous randomized controlled trials of internet-delivered self-help for social anxiety disorder [27, 30, 31]. Participants were recruited by media advertisements during the winter of 2009. A web page was created which included an outline of the study as well as general information about social anxiety disorder, the good results of the attention modification program used in the USA [17, 18], ethical issues, internet security and a description of the study personnel. Participants filled out an application form and a computerized screening battery consisting of the Social Phobia Screening Questionnaire (SPSQ; [32]), the self-rated version of the Montgomery and Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS–S; [33]), the remaining outcome measures (see instruments below), and a few additional questions regarding current and past treatments. To be included, participants had to meet the following criteria: (a) a DSM–IV diagnosis of social anxiety disorder according to the SPSQ; (b) scoring below 31 on the MADRS–S depression scale and below 4 on the suicide item of this scale (to prevent the inclusion of individuals in strong need of specialist consultation); (c) not undergoing any other psychological treatment during the study period; (d) if prescribed drugs for anxiety or depression, the dosage had to be constant for 2 months before the treatment onset and kept constant throughout the study; (e) being at least 18 years old; (f) living in Sweden; (g) having access to a computer with internet connection; (h) not having a having a significant vision impairment, (i) not admitting another serious or dominant disorder (e.g. psychosis, substance misuse) that could be expected to influence the outcome of the study; j) having a primary diagnosis of social anxiety disorder according to the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders (SCID–I; [34]). The last criterion was evaluated by a telephone interview in which the diagnostic questions from the social anxiety disorder section of the SCID–I were posed. When a person failed to meet the inclusion criteria, an individual encrypted message was sent with advice on how and where to seek more appropriate help.
As evident from the CONSORT flowchart (Figure 1), of the 112 individuals who were assessed for eligibility 79 were subsequently included and randomized to attention modification training or a placebo group. Demographic data on the included participants are presented in Table 1.
Table 1
Demographic description of the participants at pre‒treatment
  
Treatment (n=40)
Placebo (n=39)
Total (n=79)
Gender
Female
26 (65.0%)
28 (71.8%)
54 (68.4%)
 
Male
14 (35.0%)
11 (28.2%)
25 (31.7%)
Age
Mean (SD)
35.1 (13.3)
38.0 (12.0)
36.5 (12.7)
Marital status
Married/living together
26 (65.0%)
18 (46.2%)
44 (55.7%)
 
Single
11 (27.5%)
16 (41.0%)
27 (34.2%)
 
Other
3 (7.5%)
5 (12.8%)
8 (10.1%)
Children
Mean (SD)
1.0 (1.0)
0.5 (0.8)
0.8 (1.0)
Registered sick
No
36 (90.0%)
36 (92.3%)
72 (91.1%)
 
Yes
4 (10.0%)
3 (7.7%)
7 (8.9%)
Social phobia
Generalized
29 (72.5%)
30 (76.9%)
59 (74.7%)
 
Non‒generalized
11 (27.5%)
9 (23.1%)
20 (25.3%)
Avoidant personality disorder (axis‒II)
Yes
14 (35.0%)
17 (43.6%)
31 (39.2%)
 
No
26 (65.0%)
22 (56.4%)
48 (60.8%)
Medication
None
24 (60.0%)
21 (53.9%)
45 (57.0%)
 
Earlier
8 (20.0%)
9 (23.1%)
17 (21.5%)
 
Present
8 (20.0%)
9 (23.1%)
17 (21.5%)
Depression
Mean (SD)
14.6 (7.1%)
14.7 (6.4%)
14.7 (6.7%)
AUDIT
Mean (SD)
4.8 (3.5%)
4.3 (3.6%)
4.5 (3.5%)
Note: No significant differences existed between groups according to chi‒2 and independent t‒tests with Bonferroni correction for multiple tests.

Measures

The following social anxiety scales constituted the outcome measures in the study: the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale self-report version (LSAS-SR; [35, 36]), the Social Phobia Scale (SPS), the Social Interaction Anxiety Scale (SIAS; [35]), and the Social Phobia Screening Questionnaire (SPSQ; [32]). The LSAS-SR was the primary outcome measure. In addition, the following secondary measures were used to assess general anxiety and quality of life: the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; [37]), and the Quality of Life Inventory (QOLI; [38]). In the screening phase the Montgomery Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS-SR; [33]) and the Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test; AUDIT [39]) was used. The outcome measures have good psychometric properties even when administered via the Internet [40, 41]).

Treatment and placebo

Participants were either assigned to the real attention modification program or to a placebo version. Everything was identical in both conditions except for the location of the probe. Hence, in both conditions a trial began with a fixation cross (“+”) presented in the center of the screen for 500 ms. Immediately following termination of the fixation cue, the web based flash program in full screen mode presented two faces of the same person, one face on the top and one on the bottom, with each pair displaying one of two combinations of emotions. Either neutral-disgust, or neutral-neutral. After presentation of the faces for 500 ms, a probe appeared in the location of one of the two faces. Participants were instructed to indicate whether the probe was the letter E or F by pressing the corresponding arrow on the keyboard using their dominant hand. The probe remained on the screen until a response was given, after which the next trial began. During each session, 160 trials with various combinations of probe type (E/F), probe position (top/bottom), face type (neutral/disgust) and person (four male/four female were presented). There are a total of 8 persons showing 2 different facial expressions; 4 male and 4 female showing disgust or neutral. In the real condition the probe was always presented (100% of the trials) at the location of the neutral face if there also was a disgust face present (n = 128 trials). In contrast, in the placebo condition the location of the probe could not be predicted since the probe appeared with equal frequency in the position of the disgust face and the neutral face. The remaining 32 trials were neutral-neutral with the probe randomly presented at the top/bottom. For a more detailed description of the two conditions see Amir et al.[17]), since this study used the same procedures with the exception that the material was presented via the Internet.
Participants were encouraged to do the training on Tuesdays and Thursdays. They received an email and a SMS reminding them to do the training on the training days. If a session was missed a reminder was sent the following day. The participants could only do the training between 5 AM and 11 PM, and there should always be least one day between the sessions.

Procedure and design

The participants were divided into two groups; treatment or control by an online true random-number service independent of the investigators. The study protocol was approved by the regional ethics committee, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants by surface mail.
All self-report scales were administered before the start of the treatment. During the treatment LSAS-SR once a week (Sundays). Following the four weeks of training LSAS-SR, SPS, SIAS, SPSQ, MADRS-S and QOLI was readministered. Immediately following the training phase, a clinical global impression of improvement (CGI-I) was mapped on a 7-point scale CGI; [42]) after a telephone interview by a blind assessor who had no earlier contact with the participants and no knowledge of to which group they had been randomly allocated. Finally, to check if the effects were stable over time a follow-up was conducted 4 months after the post-treatment assessment, conducted online by the respective participant.

Analysis

In accordance with the intention-to-treat principle, all participants were asked to complete post-treatment and follow-up assessments, regardless of how many training sessions they had completed. Independent t-tests and chi-2 were used to check if randomization had resulted in a balanced distribution across both conditions. As evident from Figure 1, not all randomized participants provided complete datasets. The lowest response rate was 94.9% (75/79) at 4 month follow-up. In order to handle missing data in an intention-to-treat approach, we used a mixed models approach with a first-order autoregressive covariance structure as suggested by Gueorguieva and Krystal [43]. The first-order autoregressive covariance structure has the property that observations on the same subject that are closer in time are more highly correlated than measurements at times that are farther apart. Effect sizes (Cohen’s d) were calculated with the estimated means from the mixed model and by the following formula for converting standard error to standard deviation: SD = SE*(sqrt(n)).

Results

Most participants (74 of 79) completed all 8 training session for a mean of 7.8. Tables 1 and 2 show that randomization resulted in a balanced distribution across both conditions at pre-treatment. However, no interaction effects were identified suggesting that the treatment did not outperform the placebo condition. This is echoed in the between-group effect size for the main outcome measure (LSAS-SR: Cohen’s d = 0.07; CI95% -0.29 to 0.23). Furthermore, the non-significant planned pair wise comparisons suggested that the treatment was without added value when controlling for repeated measurements and spontaneous remission by the use of a control condition. However, with the exception of the Quality of Life Inventory there were significant overall time effects on all measures for both groups. But it should be noted that the within-group effect sizes were small (treatment d = 0.15; placebo d = 0.19).
Table 2
Immediate and four months follow‒up results with intention‒to‒treat analysis using mixedeffect model estimating means and (standard deviation) for all participants
 
Treatment (n=40)
Placebo (n=39)
Pairwise comparison
Time (F)
Interaction (F)
 
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
   
Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale, self‒report
6.7***
1.7
Week 0
73.8
(35.1)
73.0
(35.6)
NS
  
Week 1
71.3
(35.1)
74.3
(35.6)
NS
  
Week 2
67.4
(35.2)
66.4
(35.6)
NS
  
Week 3
64.8
(35.4)
64.1
(35.6)
NS
  
Week 4
66.0
(35.5)
60.5
(35.6)
NS
  
4 months
68.6
(35.6)
66.1
(35.8)
NS
  
Social Phobia Screening Questionnaire
32.9***
0.6
Pre
31.9
(14.6)
33.3
(14.8)
NS
  
Post
26.7
(14.8)
26.9
(14.8)
NS
  
4 months
29.2
(14.9)
28.5
(15.0)
NS
  
Social Phobia Scale
10.1***
0.3
Pre
40.3
(25.4)
40.4
(25.7)
NS
  
Post
35.9
(25.7)
34.2
(25.7)
NS
  
4 months
36.6
(25.8)
35.4
(26.0)
NS
  
Social Interaction Anxiety Scale
5.1**
0.3
Pre
50.6
(24.2)
53.1
(24.5)
NS
  
Post
47.5
(24.5
48.7
(24.5)
NS
  
4 months
47.6
(24.6)
49.0
(24.8)
NS
  
Beck Anxiety Inventory
9.6***
0.5
Pre
17.7
(12.8)
17.2
(12.9)
NS
  
Post
15.3
(13.0)
13.4
(12.9)
NS
  
4 months
16.2
(13.0)
14.1
(13.1)
NS
  
Quality of Life Inventory
2.0
0.2
Pre
0.8
(2.5)
0.4
(2.5)
NS
  
Post
1.1
(2.5)
0.6
(2.5)
NS
  
4 months
1.0
(2.5)
0.6
(2.5)
NS
  
*** p<.001; ** p<.01.

Clinical significance

The CGI-I rating at post-treatment (n = 79) showed the following non-significant results for the treatment group vs. placebo group: very much improved (5.0% vs. 2.6%), much improved (2.5% vs. 20.5%), small improvement (32.5% vs. 35.9%), unchanged (57.5% vs. 38.5%) and small deterioration (2.5% vs. 2.6%) (chi-2(4, N = 79) = 7.5; p = 0.112). In addition to the self-report measures and the blind CGI-I rating a SCID interview was conducted with the results that three (7.5%) in the treatment group and two (5.1%) in the placebo group no longer met DSM-IV criteria for social phobia.

Treatment adherence

Of the 79 participants who commenced treatment 74 (93.7%) completed all 8 training sessions as scheduled. However, five (6.3%) dropped out after only finishing M = 4.8 (SD = 1.0) sessions.
An analysis of the proportion of correct trials revealed no difference between the groups. The overall average was M = 98.7% correct indications of the letters presented. In addition, there was no difference in the average response time per trial M = 733 ms (SD = 290 ms).

Change in attention bias

To get a measure of attention bias we followed the suggested procedure from the previous Amir study [17]. That included first eliminating response latencies for inaccurate trials. This procedure resulted in the elimination of 1% of the trials. Change in attention bias was subsequently calculated by subtracting response latencies of trials where the probe preplaced a neutral face of a disgust-neutral face pair from trial where both faces were neutral. This bias has also been used by other investigator to index disengagement [44, 45]. In addition, response latencies less than 50 ms and greater than 1200 ms were considered outliers and were also eliminated from the analysis. These ranges were determined based on the inspection of the data using box plots and resulted in eliminating 1% of the trials. We submitted the participants’ performance on the Attention Task during each session to a 2 × 8 (Group [treatment, control] × Time [session 1–8]) analysis of variance (ANOVA). This analysis did not reveal a significant interaction of Group X Time (F(7, 69) = 1.15, p = .327). However, when analyzing the change scores on LSAS-SR there was a significant, albeit weak, positive correlation, indicating that the higher bias a participant had at the end of the treatment the more that participant improved (r = .32, p < 0.006). We calculated the correlation between session 1 bias score and change in LSAS. This correlation was only significant in the active group (r = 0.32, p < 005 active; r = −0.14, p = 0.37 placebo group).
To assess whether participants remained blind to their respective experimental condition, we asked the participants at post treatment whether they thought they had received the active or the placebo intervention. Of the participants who provided responses (n = 76) 18.9% (7/37) in the treatment group and 28.2% (11/39) in the placebo condition thought they had been randomized to the treatment program (Chi2(1, N = 76) = 0.9, p = .42).

Discussion

This was a double blind randomized controlled trial with the aim to transfer the promising results of the Amir et al.[17] and Schmidt et al.[18] studies to the Internet. Results did not confirm our hypothesis that Internet-delivered attention bias modification would be superior to a placebo condition. However, there was a significant time effect, but that is probably best explained by spontaneous remission since both the treatment and the placebo group displayed similar gains. In addition, the within group effect sizes were small. This is the second randomized trial not being able to replicate the initial promising results. However, the first non-replication by Boettcher and coworkers was not identical to the original studies since different stimuli were used [30]. Specifically, the two original studies used the emotional facial expressions from Matsumoto and Ekman [46], while Boettcher and colleges used the NimStim faces [47]. The procedure in the present study mirrored the Amir study exactly including the same faces, number and spacing of the trials, and the location and duration of the stimuli. One obvious difference is that participants in the present trial did the training in a home setting as opposed to a university clinic ([17, 18]). When looking at the proportion of correctly identified probes there was no difference and the marginally higher reaction time, as compared to Amir et al.[17] is explained by how elimination of individual trials were defined. The present study only excluded 3 SD above the individual trial mean (apx 1600 ms), while Amir et al.[17] simply excluded values higher than 1200 ms. Moreover, the method of delivery (i.e., internet based flash program) vs. personal computer delivery was a difference between current study and previous research.
An explanation of the null findings could be that this study also included participants with non-generalized social phobia. However, the mean scores on LSAS-SR were almost identical (73.4 in this trial compared to 74.5 in the Amir trial). In addition, when running the analysis with the NGSP and GSP groups separated, no differences in the treatment effect emerged.
When the participants who received the real treatment where asked to predict condition the absolute majority (81%) thought they had been randomized to the placebo group. One would think that such a low level of positive expectation would influence the trial. However, that cannot explain the lack of effect since the Amir et al.[17] and Schmidt et al.[18] papers reported almost identical numbers (78% and 94% respectively). Hence, there is a problem with the rationale and believability (cf.)[48]. However, the results when delivered on site have been impressive. Possibly attention training at home is negatively influenced by the fact that the person is sitting calmly in the comfort of his or her home. Perhaps there is an interaction between the active attention training and the mild anxiety that participants with social anxiety probably feel when they come to a university clinic to do their training (cf.)[49]. Although we instructed the participants to schedule the training when there would not be disturbed, there is a possibility that short breaks or non-focus reduces the effectiveness. Hence, the control over the procedures and the setting before, during and after training was suboptimal as compared to a lab. There could of course also be cultural differences. For example, in the case of panic disorder applied relaxation seems to work well in Sweden, but maybe not in the United Kingdom and USA [50, 51]. The bias measure used in this study can be criticized. It could be argued that a task, in which the probe sometimes appears in the position of the disgust face and sometimes in the position of the neutral face, would be more accurate. However, Koster and coworkers [52] proposed that the probe detection task may be modified such that vigilance for threat and disengagement from threat may be assessed by including baseline trials, i.e., trial with two neutral faces. Using this new measure of bias, Koster et al., found that individuals with anxiety have had difficulty in disengaging their attention from highly threatening pictures. This measure of bias has been used but other investigators to assess the specific componets of attentional bias in anxiety [5355]. The largest limitation is that participants were included without checking if they indeed had an initial attentional bias [1]. This is a serious flaw since not all patients with presenting with anxiety show bias [5]. However, the presence of a bias was not an inclusion criterion in the Amir et al.[17] or the Schmidt et al.[18] studies. Since there were no interaction effects in terms of change in bias between the two groups it can be concluded that not only was the internet-based treatment in the present study unable to modify attentional bias. In fact, there was the unexpected finding that larger bias was correlated with higher improvement. However, it should be noted that this was only a weak association, and could very well be explained as a random replicable finding. Indeed, as Heeren and coworkers have shown, engagement towards non-threat faces does not account for the positive treatment effects [56]. It should be noted that the intervention employed only trains attention away from negativity. There could be added benefits of instead of training what is essentially avoidance, to also or instead train active selection of positive mood-supporting information [57]. Perhaps the placebo condition, with equal frequency in the position of the threatening and neutral faces, is all that is needed to accomplish this disengagement from threat. Hence, future studies could, if done in an ethical way, test to include a condition of negative training.

Conclusions

We conclude that attention bias modification may need to be further investigated before dissemination for the Internet.

Acknowledgement

This study was sponsored by grants from the Swedish Council for Working and Life Research (FAS 2009–0222).
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

Seven of the eight authors declare that there is no conflict of interest. However, Dr Amir has founded a company that market online anxiety relief products.

Authors’ contribution

All authors contributed to the design of this study. PC drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to the further writing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Bar-Haim Y: Research Review: attention bias modification (ABM): a novel treatment for anxiety disorders. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2010, 51 (8): 859-870. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02251.x.CrossRef Bar-Haim Y: Research Review: attention bias modification (ABM): a novel treatment for anxiety disorders. J Child Psychol Psyc. 2010, 51 (8): 859-870. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02251.x.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Heinrichs N, Hofmann SG: Information processing in social phobia: a critical review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2001, 21 (5): 751-770. 10.1016/S0272-7358(00)00067-2.CrossRefPubMed Heinrichs N, Hofmann SG: Information processing in social phobia: a critical review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2001, 21 (5): 751-770. 10.1016/S0272-7358(00)00067-2.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat MacLeod C: Cognitive bias modification procedures in the management of mental disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2012, 25 (2): 114-120. 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32834fda4a.CrossRefPubMed MacLeod C: Cognitive bias modification procedures in the management of mental disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2012, 25 (2): 114-120. 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32834fda4a.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Hallion LS, Ruscio AM: A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychol Bull. 2011, 137 (6): 940-958.CrossRefPubMed Hallion LS, Ruscio AM: A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychol Bull. 2011, 137 (6): 940-958.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Beard C: Cognitive bias modification for anxiety: current evidence and future directions. Expert Rev Neurother. 2011, 11 (2): 299-311. 10.1586/ern.10.194.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Beard C: Cognitive bias modification for anxiety: current evidence and future directions. Expert Rev Neurother. 2011, 11 (2): 299-311. 10.1586/ern.10.194.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Mogg K, Bradley BP: Selective orienting of attention to masked threat faces in social anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 2002, 40 (12): 1403-1414. 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00017-7.CrossRefPubMed Mogg K, Bradley BP: Selective orienting of attention to masked threat faces in social anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 2002, 40 (12): 1403-1414. 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00017-7.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Mogg K, Philippot P, Bradley BP: Selective attention to angry faces in clinical social phobia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2004, 113 (1): 160-165.CrossRefPubMed Mogg K, Philippot P, Bradley BP: Selective attention to angry faces in clinical social phobia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2004, 113 (1): 160-165.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Pishyar R, Harris LM, Menzies RG: Attentional bias for words and faces in social anxiety. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2004, 17 (1): 23-36. 10.1080/10615800310001601458.CrossRef Pishyar R, Harris LM, Menzies RG: Attentional bias for words and faces in social anxiety. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2004, 17 (1): 23-36. 10.1080/10615800310001601458.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat MacLeod C, Mathews A, Tata P: Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 1986, 95 (1): 15-20.CrossRefPubMed MacLeod C, Mathews A, Tata P: Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 1986, 95 (1): 15-20.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Mogg K, Bradley BP: A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 1998, 36 (9): 809-848. 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00063-1.CrossRefPubMed Mogg K, Bradley BP: A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 1998, 36 (9): 809-848. 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00063-1.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Mattia JI, Heimberg RG, Hope DA: The revised stroop color-naming task in social phobics. Behav Res Ther. 1993, 31 (3): 305-313. 10.1016/0005-7967(93)90029-T.CrossRefPubMed Mattia JI, Heimberg RG, Hope DA: The revised stroop color-naming task in social phobics. Behav Res Ther. 1993, 31 (3): 305-313. 10.1016/0005-7967(93)90029-T.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat MacLeod C, Rutherford E, Campbell L, Ebsworthy G, Holker L: Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002, 111 (1): 107-123.CrossRefPubMed MacLeod C, Rutherford E, Campbell L, Ebsworthy G, Holker L: Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002, 111 (1): 107-123.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Macleod C, Holmes EA: Cognitive bias modification: An intervention approach worth attending to. Am J Psychiatry. 2012, 169 (2): 118-120. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11111682.CrossRefPubMed Macleod C, Holmes EA: Cognitive bias modification: An intervention approach worth attending to. Am J Psychiatry. 2012, 169 (2): 118-120. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11111682.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Hakamata Y, Lissek S, Bar-Haim Y, Britton JC, Fox NA, Leibenluft E, Ernst M, Pine DS: Attention bias modification treatment: a meta-analysis toward the establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. 2010, 68 (11): 982-990. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.021.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hakamata Y, Lissek S, Bar-Haim Y, Britton JC, Fox NA, Leibenluft E, Ernst M, Pine DS: Attention bias modification treatment: a meta-analysis toward the establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. 2010, 68 (11): 982-990. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.021.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
15.
Zurück zum Zitat MacLeod C, Mathews A: Cognitive bias modification approaches to anxiety. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2012, 8: 189-217. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143052.CrossRefPubMed MacLeod C, Mathews A: Cognitive bias modification approaches to anxiety. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2012, 8: 189-217. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143052.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Amir N, Weber G, Beard C, Bomyea J, Taylor CT: The effect of a single-session attention modification program on response to a public-speaking challenge in socially anxious individuals. J Abnorm Psychol. 2008, 117 (4): 860-868.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Amir N, Weber G, Beard C, Bomyea J, Taylor CT: The effect of a single-session attention modification program on response to a public-speaking challenge in socially anxious individuals. J Abnorm Psychol. 2008, 117 (4): 860-868.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Amir N, Beard C, Taylor CT, Klumpp H, Elias J, Burns M, Chen X: Attention training in individuals with generalized social phobia: A randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2009, 77 (5): 961-973.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Amir N, Beard C, Taylor CT, Klumpp H, Elias J, Burns M, Chen X: Attention training in individuals with generalized social phobia: A randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2009, 77 (5): 961-973.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmidt NB, Richey JA, Buckner JD, Timpano KR: Attention Training for Generalized Social Anxiety Disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009, 118 (1): 5-14.CrossRefPubMed Schmidt NB, Richey JA, Buckner JD, Timpano KR: Attention Training for Generalized Social Anxiety Disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009, 118 (1): 5-14.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat McEvoy PM, Perini SJ: Cognitive behavioral group therapy for social phobia with or without attention training: a controlled trial. J Anxiety Disord. 2009, 23 (4): 519-528. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.10.008.CrossRefPubMed McEvoy PM, Perini SJ: Cognitive behavioral group therapy for social phobia with or without attention training: a controlled trial. J Anxiety Disord. 2009, 23 (4): 519-528. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.10.008.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Carlbring P, Andersson G: Internet and psychological treatment. How well can they be combined. Comput Hum Behav. 2006, 22 (3): 545-553. 10.1016/j.chb.2004.10.009.CrossRef Carlbring P, Andersson G: Internet and psychological treatment. How well can they be combined. Comput Hum Behav. 2006, 22 (3): 545-553. 10.1016/j.chb.2004.10.009.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Andrews G, Davies M, Titov N: Effectiveness randomized controlled trial of face to face versus Internet cognitive behaviour therapy for social phobia. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2011, 45: 337-340. 10.3109/00048674.2010.538840.CrossRefPubMed Andrews G, Davies M, Titov N: Effectiveness randomized controlled trial of face to face versus Internet cognitive behaviour therapy for social phobia. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2011, 45: 337-340. 10.3109/00048674.2010.538840.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Hedman E, Andersson G, Ljotsson B, Andersson E, Ruck C, Mortberg E, Lindefors N: Internet-based cognitive behavior therapy vs. cognitive behavioral group therapy for social anxiety disorder: a randomized controlled non-inferiority trial. PLoS One. 2011, 6 (3): e18001-10.1371/journal.pone.0018001.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hedman E, Andersson G, Ljotsson B, Andersson E, Ruck C, Mortberg E, Lindefors N: Internet-based cognitive behavior therapy vs. cognitive behavioral group therapy for social anxiety disorder: a randomized controlled non-inferiority trial. PLoS One. 2011, 6 (3): e18001-10.1371/journal.pone.0018001.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Titov N, Andrews G, Schwencke G, Drobny J, Einstein D: Shyness 1: distance treatment of social phobia over the Internet. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2008, 42 (7): 585-594. 10.1080/00048670802119762.CrossRefPubMed Titov N, Andrews G, Schwencke G, Drobny J, Einstein D: Shyness 1: distance treatment of social phobia over the Internet. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2008, 42 (7): 585-594. 10.1080/00048670802119762.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Titov N, Gibson M, Andrews G, McEvoy P: Internet treatment for social phobia reduces comorbidity. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2009, 43 (8): 754-759. 10.1080/00048670903001992.CrossRefPubMed Titov N, Gibson M, Andrews G, McEvoy P: Internet treatment for social phobia reduces comorbidity. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2009, 43 (8): 754-759. 10.1080/00048670903001992.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Botella C, Gallego MJ, Garcia-Palacios A, Guillen V, Banos RM, Quero S, Alcaniz M: An Internet-based self-help treatment for fear of public speaking: a controlled trial. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2010, 13 (4): 407-421. 10.1089/cyber.2009.0224.CrossRefPubMed Botella C, Gallego MJ, Garcia-Palacios A, Guillen V, Banos RM, Quero S, Alcaniz M: An Internet-based self-help treatment for fear of public speaking: a controlled trial. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2010, 13 (4): 407-421. 10.1089/cyber.2009.0224.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Andersson G, Carlbring P, Holmstrom A, Sparthan E, Furmark T, Nilsson-Ihrfelt E, Buhrman M, Ekselius L: Internet-based self-help with therapist feedback and in vivo group exposure for social phobia: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2006, 74 (4): 677-686.CrossRefPubMed Andersson G, Carlbring P, Holmstrom A, Sparthan E, Furmark T, Nilsson-Ihrfelt E, Buhrman M, Ekselius L: Internet-based self-help with therapist feedback and in vivo group exposure for social phobia: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2006, 74 (4): 677-686.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Carlbring P, Gunnarsdottir M, Hedensjo L, Andersson G, Ekselius L, Furmark T: Treatment of social phobia: randomised trial of internet-delivered cognitive-behavioural therapy with telephone support. The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science. 2007, 190: 123-128. 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.020107.CrossRef Carlbring P, Gunnarsdottir M, Hedensjo L, Andersson G, Ekselius L, Furmark T: Treatment of social phobia: randomised trial of internet-delivered cognitive-behavioural therapy with telephone support. The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science. 2007, 190: 123-128. 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.020107.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Berger T, Caspar F, Richardson R, Kneubuhler B, Sutter D, Andersson G: Internet-based treatment of social phobia: a randomized controlled trial comparing unguided with two types of guided self-help. Behav Res Ther. 2011, 49 (3): 158-169. 10.1016/j.brat.2010.12.007.CrossRefPubMed Berger T, Caspar F, Richardson R, Kneubuhler B, Sutter D, Andersson G: Internet-based treatment of social phobia: a randomized controlled trial comparing unguided with two types of guided self-help. Behav Res Ther. 2011, 49 (3): 158-169. 10.1016/j.brat.2010.12.007.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Berger T, Hohl E, Caspar F: Internet-based treatment for social phobia: a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Psychol. 2009, 65 (10): 1021-1035. 10.1002/jclp.20603.CrossRefPubMed Berger T, Hohl E, Caspar F: Internet-based treatment for social phobia: a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Psychol. 2009, 65 (10): 1021-1035. 10.1002/jclp.20603.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Boettcher J, Berger T, Renneberg B: Internet-Based Attention Training for Social Anxiety: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Cogn Ther Res. 2011, 1-15. Boettcher J, Berger T, Renneberg B: Internet-Based Attention Training for Social Anxiety: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Cogn Ther Res. 2011, 1-15.
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Furmark T, Carlbring P, Hedman E, Sonnenstein A, Clevberger P, Bohman B, Eriksson A, Hallen A, Frykman M, Holmstrom A, et al: Guided and unguided self-help for social anxiety disorder: randomised controlled trial. The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science. 2009, 195 (5): 440-447. 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.060996.CrossRef Furmark T, Carlbring P, Hedman E, Sonnenstein A, Clevberger P, Bohman B, Eriksson A, Hallen A, Frykman M, Holmstrom A, et al: Guided and unguided self-help for social anxiety disorder: randomised controlled trial. The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science. 2009, 195 (5): 440-447. 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.060996.CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Furmark T, Tillfors M, Everz P, Marteinsdottir I, Gefvert O, Fredrikson M: Social phobia in the general population: prevalence and sociodemographic profile. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1999, 34 (8): 416-424. 10.1007/s001270050163.CrossRefPubMed Furmark T, Tillfors M, Everz P, Marteinsdottir I, Gefvert O, Fredrikson M: Social phobia in the general population: prevalence and sociodemographic profile. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1999, 34 (8): 416-424. 10.1007/s001270050163.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Svanborg P, Åsberg M: A new self-rating scale for depression and anxiety states based on the comprehensive psychopathological rating scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1994, 89 (1): 21-28. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1994.tb01480.x.CrossRefPubMed Svanborg P, Åsberg M: A new self-rating scale for depression and anxiety states based on the comprehensive psychopathological rating scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1994, 89 (1): 21-28. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1994.tb01480.x.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW: Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I). 1997, American Psychiatric Press, Washington, D.C. First MB, Gibbon M, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW: Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I). 1997, American Psychiatric Press, Washington, D.C.
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Mattick RP, Clarke JC: Development and validation of measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 1998, 36 (4): 455-470. 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10031-6.CrossRefPubMed Mattick RP, Clarke JC: Development and validation of measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 1998, 36 (4): 455-470. 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10031-6.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Baker SL, Heinrichs N, Kim HJ, Hofmann SG: The Liebowitz social anxiety scale as a self-report instrument: a preliminary psychometric analysis. Behav Res Ther. 2002, 40 (6): 701-715. 10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00060-2.CrossRefPubMed Baker SL, Heinrichs N, Kim HJ, Hofmann SG: The Liebowitz social anxiety scale as a self-report instrument: a preliminary psychometric analysis. Behav Res Ther. 2002, 40 (6): 701-715. 10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00060-2.CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA: An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988, 56 (6): 893-897.CrossRefPubMed Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA: An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988, 56 (6): 893-897.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Frisch MB, Cornell J, Villanueva M, Retzlaff PJ: Clinical validation of the Quality of Life Inventory. A measure of life satisfaction for use in treatment planning and outcome assessment. Psychol Assess. 1992, 4 (1): 92-101.CrossRef Frisch MB, Cornell J, Villanueva M, Retzlaff PJ: Clinical validation of the Quality of Life Inventory. A measure of life satisfaction for use in treatment planning and outcome assessment. Psychol Assess. 1992, 4 (1): 92-101.CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Wennberg P: The alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): A psychometric evaluation. 1996, Reports from the Department of Psychology, Stockholm Univ Wennberg P: The alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): A psychometric evaluation. 1996, Reports from the Department of Psychology, Stockholm Univ
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Hedman E, Ljótsson B, Rück C, Furmark T, Carlbring P, Lindefors N, Andersson G: Internet administration of self-report measures commonly used in research on social anxiety disorder: a psychometric evaluation. Comput Hum Behav. 2010, 26 (4): 736-740. 10.1016/j.chb.2010.01.010.CrossRef Hedman E, Ljótsson B, Rück C, Furmark T, Carlbring P, Lindefors N, Andersson G: Internet administration of self-report measures commonly used in research on social anxiety disorder: a psychometric evaluation. Comput Hum Behav. 2010, 26 (4): 736-740. 10.1016/j.chb.2010.01.010.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Thorndike FP, Carlbring P, Smyth FL, Magee JC, Gonder-Frederick L, Ost L-G, Ritterband LM: Web-based measurement: effect of completing single or multiple items per webpage. Comput Hum Behav. 2009, 25 (2): 393-401. 10.1016/j.chb.2008.05.006.CrossRef Thorndike FP, Carlbring P, Smyth FL, Magee JC, Gonder-Frederick L, Ost L-G, Ritterband LM: Web-based measurement: effect of completing single or multiple items per webpage. Comput Hum Behav. 2009, 25 (2): 393-401. 10.1016/j.chb.2008.05.006.CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Guy W: Clinical Global Impressions. 1976, NIMH, Rockville, 217-222. Guy W: Clinical Global Impressions. 1976, NIMH, Rockville, 217-222.
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Gueorguieva R, Krystal JH: Move over ANOVA: progress in analyzing repeated-measures data and its reflection in papers published in the archives of general psychiatry. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004, 61 (3): 310-317. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.3.310.CrossRefPubMed Gueorguieva R, Krystal JH: Move over ANOVA: progress in analyzing repeated-measures data and its reflection in papers published in the archives of general psychiatry. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004, 61 (3): 310-317. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.3.310.CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Amir N, Taylor CT, Donohue MC: Predictors of response to an attention modification program in generalized social phobia. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2011, 79 (4): 533-541.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Amir N, Taylor CT, Donohue MC: Predictors of response to an attention modification program in generalized social phobia. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2011, 79 (4): 533-541.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Koster EH, Crombez G, Verschuere B, Van Damme S, Wiersema JR: Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety: facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. Behav Res Ther. 2006, 44 (12): 1757-1771. 10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.011.CrossRefPubMed Koster EH, Crombez G, Verschuere B, Van Damme S, Wiersema JR: Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety: facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. Behav Res Ther. 2006, 44 (12): 1757-1771. 10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.011.CrossRefPubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Matsumoto D, Ekman P: The Japanese and Caucasian facial expressions of emotion (JACFEE) and neutrals (JACNeuF). 1989, San Francisco State University, Department of Psychology, Intercultural and Emotion Research Laboratory, San Francisco Matsumoto D, Ekman P: The Japanese and Caucasian facial expressions of emotion (JACFEE) and neutrals (JACNeuF). 1989, San Francisco State University, Department of Psychology, Intercultural and Emotion Research Laboratory, San Francisco
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, Marcus DJ, Westerlund A, Casey BJ, Nelson C: The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 2009, 168 (3): 242-249. 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, Marcus DJ, Westerlund A, Casey BJ, Nelson C: The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 2009, 168 (3): 242-249. 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Schiller D, Monfils M-H, Raio CM, Johnson DC, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA: Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature. 2010, 463 (7277): 49-53. 10.1038/nature08637.CrossRefPubMed Schiller D, Monfils M-H, Raio CM, Johnson DC, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA: Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms. Nature. 2010, 463 (7277): 49-53. 10.1038/nature08637.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Milrod B, Leon AC, Busch F, Rudden M, Schwalberg M, Clarkin J, Aronson A, Singer M, Turchin W, Klass ET, et al: A randomized controlled clinical trial of psychoanalytic psychotherapy for panic disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2007, 164 (2): 265-272. 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.2.265.CrossRefPubMed Milrod B, Leon AC, Busch F, Rudden M, Schwalberg M, Clarkin J, Aronson A, Singer M, Turchin W, Klass ET, et al: A randomized controlled clinical trial of psychoanalytic psychotherapy for panic disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2007, 164 (2): 265-272. 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.2.265.CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Ost LG, Westling BE: Applied relaxation vs cognitive-behavior therapy in the treatment of panic disorder. Behav Res Ther. 1995, 33 (2): 145-158. 10.1016/0005-7967(94)E0026-F.CrossRefPubMed Ost LG, Westling BE: Applied relaxation vs cognitive-behavior therapy in the treatment of panic disorder. Behav Res Ther. 1995, 33 (2): 145-158. 10.1016/0005-7967(94)E0026-F.CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Koster EHW, Crombez G, Verschuere B, De Houwer J: Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behav Res Ther. 2004, 42 (10): 1183-1192. 10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.001.CrossRefPubMed Koster EHW, Crombez G, Verschuere B, De Houwer J: Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behav Res Ther. 2004, 42 (10): 1183-1192. 10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.001.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Klumpp H, Amir N: Examination of vigilance and disengagement of threat in social anxiety with a probe detection task. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2009, 22 (3): 283-296. 10.1080/10615800802449602.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Klumpp H, Amir N: Examination of vigilance and disengagement of threat in social anxiety with a probe detection task. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2009, 22 (3): 283-296. 10.1080/10615800802449602.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Salemink E, van den Hout M, Kindt M: Effects of positive interpretive bias modification in highly anxious individuals. J Anxiety Disord. 2009, 23 (5): 676-683. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.02.006.CrossRefPubMed Salemink E, van den Hout M, Kindt M: Effects of positive interpretive bias modification in highly anxious individuals. J Anxiety Disord. 2009, 23 (5): 676-683. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.02.006.CrossRefPubMed
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Amir N, Taylor CT: Interpretation training in individuals with generalized social anxiety disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2012, 80: 497-511.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Amir N, Taylor CT: Interpretation training in individuals with generalized social anxiety disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2012, 80: 497-511.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Heeren A, Lievens L, Philippot P: How does attention training work in social phobia: disengagement from threat or re-engagement to non-threat?. J Anxiety Disord. 2011, 25 (8): 1108-1115. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.08.001.CrossRefPubMed Heeren A, Lievens L, Philippot P: How does attention training work in social phobia: disengagement from threat or re-engagement to non-threat?. J Anxiety Disord. 2011, 25 (8): 1108-1115. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.08.001.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Heeren A, Reese HE, McNally RJ, Philippot P: Attention training toward and away from threat in social phobia: effects on subjective, behavioral, and physiological measures of anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 2012, 50 (1): 30-39. 10.1016/j.brat.2011.10.005.CrossRefPubMed Heeren A, Reese HE, McNally RJ, Philippot P: Attention training toward and away from threat in social phobia: effects on subjective, behavioral, and physiological measures of anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 2012, 50 (1): 30-39. 10.1016/j.brat.2011.10.005.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Internet-delivered attention bias modification training in individuals with social anxiety disorder - a double blind randomized controlled trial
verfasst von
Per Carlbring
Maria Apelstrand
Helena Sehlin
Nader Amir
Andreas Rousseau
Stefan G Hofmann
Gerhard Andersson
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2012
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2012
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-12-66

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2012

BMC Psychiatry 1/2012 Zur Ausgabe

„Übersichtlicher Wegweiser“: Lauterbachs umstrittener Klinik-Atlas ist online

17.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Sie sei „ethisch geboten“, meint Gesundheitsminister Karl Lauterbach: mehr Transparenz über die Qualität von Klinikbehandlungen. Um sie abzubilden, lässt er gegen den Widerstand vieler Länder einen virtuellen Klinik-Atlas freischalten.

ADHS-Medikation erhöht das kardiovaskuläre Risiko

16.05.2024 Herzinsuffizienz Nachrichten

Erwachsene, die Medikamente gegen das Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-Hyperaktivitätssyndrom einnehmen, laufen offenbar erhöhte Gefahr, an Herzschwäche zu erkranken oder einen Schlaganfall zu erleiden. Es scheint eine Dosis-Wirkungs-Beziehung zu bestehen.

Klinikreform soll zehntausende Menschenleben retten

15.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Gesundheitsminister Lauterbach hat die vom Bundeskabinett beschlossene Klinikreform verteidigt. Kritik an den Plänen kommt vom Marburger Bund. Und in den Ländern wird über den Gang zum Vermittlungsausschuss spekuliert.

Typ-2-Diabetes und Depression folgen oft aufeinander

14.05.2024 Typ-2-Diabetes Nachrichten

Menschen mit Typ-2-Diabetes sind überdurchschnittlich gefährdet, in den nächsten Jahren auch noch eine Depression zu entwickeln – und umgekehrt. Besonders ausgeprägt ist die Wechselbeziehung laut GKV-Daten bei jüngeren Erwachsenen.