Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Health and Quality of Life Outcomes 1/2013

Open Access 01.12.2013 | Research

Absence of social desirability bias in the evaluation of chronic disease self-management interventions

verfasst von: Sandra Nolte, Gerald R Elsworth, Richard H Osborne

Erschienen in: Health and Quality of Life Outcomes | Ausgabe 1/2013

Abstract

Background

Bias due to social desirability has long been of concern to evaluators relying on self-report data. It is conceivable that health program evaluation is particularly susceptible to social desirability bias as individuals may be inclined to present themselves or certain health behaviors in a more positive light and/or appease the course leader. Thus, the influence of social desirability bias on self-report outcomes was explored in the present study.

Methods

Data were collected from 331 participants of group-based chronic disease self-management interventions using the highly robust eight-scale Health Education Impact Questionnaire (heiQ) and the 13-item short form Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MC-C). The majority of self-management courses were run by community-based organizations across Australia between February 2005 and December 2006 where 6 to 12 individuals have the opportunity to develop considerable rapport with course leaders and each other over about six weeks. Pre-test data were collected on the first day of courses, while post-test and social desirability scores were assessed at the end of courses. A model of partial mediation within the framework of structural equation modeling was developed with social desirability as the mediating variable between pre-test and post-test.

Results

The ‘Defensiveness’ factor of the MC-C showed clear association with heiQ pre-test data, a prerequisite for investigating mediation; however, when investigating the eight full pre-test/post-test models ‘Defensiveness’ was only associated with one heiQ scale. This effect was small, explaining 8% of the variance in the model. No other meditational effects through social desirability were observed.

Conclusions

The overall lack of association of social desirability with heiQ outcomes was surprising as it had been expected that it would explain at least some of the variance observed between pre-test and post-test. With the assumption that the MC-C captures the propensity for an individual to provide socially desirable answers, this study concludes that change scores in chronic disease self-management program evaluation are not biased by social desirability.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1477-7525-11-114) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

The authors state that there are no conflicts of interests.
Abkürzungen
heiQ
Health education impact questionnaire
MC
Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability scale
MC-C
13-item short form Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability scale
RML
Robust maximum likelihood
SEM
Structural equation modeling.

Background

There has long been a concern that considerable bias in survey research can stem from respondents providing answers that are partly determined by social influences, in particular social desirability [1]. While the influence of social desirability bias has been found to vary according to the survey method, telephone and personal interviews have been found to be particularly prone to socially desirable responding [13]. Hence, social desirability bias may be a major threat to the validity of self-report outcomes data. Although there are several elements to its conceptualization, social desirability bias can generally be described as a response style exhibited by respondents who endorse items that represent traits and/or behaviors that they think stand for a socially acceptable or endorsed position [4]. Further, it can be differentiated between two dimensions: 1) the need for social approval, i.e. creation of a positive impression of oneself to receive approval from others (impression management), and 2) self-deception or defensiveness, i.e. avoidance of disapproval by denying socially undesirable traits and/or behaviors [58]. Social desirability has been found to be related to demographic variables; it is more likely to be identified in older women [9, 10], women of lower socio-economic status [10, 11], and higher age [12, 13]. Finally, social desirability has been found to be strongly related to the positive rating of the personal qualities of self, family and friends and not of ‘people in general’, the so-called ‘better than average’ effect [14].
While social desirability bias has been a general concern in evaluations based on self-reports [3], it may play a particularly important role in chronic disease health education interventions, in particular those that are offered to groups of people with chronic conditions who were initially unknown to each other. First, it is likely that individuals would be inclined to present themselves or certain health behaviors in a more positive light. This phenomenon would generally apply to any health-related outcomes assessment. Second, in the specific context of group-based interventions, it is intended that participants and course leaders build strong rapport during the intervention that may last several weeks or months. As a result, at the end of courses, participants may be inclined to provide socially desirable answers to endorse course leaders regardless of whether they truly benefited from the intervention. That is, participants may be aware that they are indirectly evaluating the performance of both the course leader and the organization and therefore provide socially desirable responses to appease leaders rather than showing how they really felt after graduating from the self-management course. Finally, in this setting, participants often fill out questionnaires in the presence of leaders and their peers which again may trigger socially desirable responses as they may feel pressurized to endorse the leaders’ performance. Hence, social desirability bias may have a particular influence on post-test scores and thus apparent change scores.
To measure the influence of potential socially desirable responses, several scales have been developed [5, 1518]. Of these, the Marlowe-Crowne (MC) Social Desirability scale [16] is one of the most widely used indices [19]. It is commonly described as a measure of a person’s need for approval. Although the original authors defined the concept of social desirability in terms of two dimensions, i.e. need for approval and avoidance of disapproval [6, 20], they conceptualized the MC scale as a measure of a single dimension [6, 21]. However, subsequent studies found little support for this hypothesis, with results ranging from two-factor [5, 22] to multi-factor solutions [19, 21, 2326]. While such findings cast some doubt on the measurement properties of the MC scale, these studies should be treated with caution. Only two studies applied rigorous psychometric statistical techniques to investigate the properties of the MC scale [19, 21]. Moreover, the generalizability of studies is questionable as almost all samples consisted of students [19, 24, 25, 2729].
The original MC scale consists of 33 items. Therefore, for some respondents it may be a burden to complete, particularly if the scale is among a panel of scales. As a consequence, short forms have been developed, with Reynolds’ (1982) and Strahan and Gerbasi’s (1972) short forms being most frequently applied [19, 21]. Commentaries on the usefulness of the short forms vary substantially. While some suggest that all are unsatisfactory [19, 24], others show that they are improvements over the original [25, 26, 28]. However, these studies should also be treated with caution. Apart from one study [19] none applied rigorous statistical methods. Further, factor analyses on the short forms were generally aimed at confirming/rejecting the one-factor hypothesis, whereas none tested the scales for a potential two- or multi-factor solution. Of all short forms, Reynolds’ MC-C [30] has been explored extensively [31] and is one of the most frequently used short forms [3234]. It has generally been described as a reliable alternative to the full scale [30, 31, 35] with acceptable internal consistency [24, 25, 30, 31, 34].
In summary, social desirability bias has received frequent attention in the literature [20, 36]. However, in view of its potential threat to the validity of scores derived from participants of health interventions, it is surprising that this bias has rarely been explored in contexts where social desirability is likely to be an important bias. Only two out of more than 100 controlled trials of chronic disease self-management courses considered social desirability as a potential covariate [37]. The aim of this study was to explore the influence of social desirability bias on change scores derived from data collected from groups of participants taking part in chronic disease self-management courses.

Methods

Courses and participants

Data were collected from 331 participants of chronic disease self-management courses implemented mainly by community-based organizations across Australia between February 2005 and December 2006. As shown in Table  1, three quarters of respondents were female (74.2%), mean age 62.2 years (age range 19 to 90 years), and the majority reported to be affected by osteoarthritis (45.5%), depression (29.9%), diabetes (22.1%), and asthma (21.5%). The predominant course type (71.2%) was a generic intervention [38], while the remaining disease-specific interventions were mostly aimed at people with arthritis.
Table 1
Demographic characteristics of respondents
 
n = 331
 
n
%
Gender
 Female
244
74.2
 Male
85
25.8
Age
 Mean (standard deviation)
62.2 (13.2)
 Range
19-90
Education
 Primary education
31
9.8
 Up to year 8
100
31.7
 Year 9 to 12
82
26.0
 TAFE1
59
18.7
 University
43
13.7
Employment status
 Full-time
13
4.2
 Part-time
21
6.7
 Unemployed
28
8.9
 Home duties
42
13.4
 Retired
204
65.2
 Other
5
1.6
Birth place
 Australia
241
73.5
 Born elsewhere
87
26.5
Chronic condition (more than one could be selected)
 Asthma
69
21.5
 Cancer
17
5.3
 Coronary heart disease
42
13.1
 Depression
96
29.9
 Diabetes
71
22.1
 Fibromyalgia
37
11.5
 Osteoarthritis
146
45.5
 Osteoporosis
47
14.6
 Rheumatoid arthritis
51
15.9
 Other
140
43.8
1 TAFE Technical and further education.
Participant recruitment was undertaken at a course level where leaders were recruited through established networks and snowball recruitment as previously described [39, 40]. Pre-test data were provided at the start of courses (T1), while post-test and social desirability data were collected at the end of courses (T2), on average six weeks after pre-test. The 13-item short form MC-C was applied [30]. Questions were answered using a ‘true-false’ response scale in the same manner as in the original scale [16]. The Health Education Impact Questionnaire (heiQ), a widely used measure of impacts of self-management interventions, was used to collect patient-reported outcomes data [41, 42]. The version of the heiQ that was applied comprised 38 items, each uniquely associated with one of the following eight factors: Positive and active engagement in life, Health directed activities, Skill and technique acquisition, Constructive attitudes and approaches, Self-monitoring and insight, Health service navigation, Social integration and support, and Emotional distress. All items were measured on a 6-point Likert response scale ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”.

Statistical model

As described in the introduction, previous research on the validity of the MC scale lacked both statistical sophistication and samples including people with chronic disease [5, 7, 19, 22, 24]. Consequently, it was deemed necessary to determine the psychometric properties of the MC-C before embarking upon the analyses. This was approached in an exploratory way. Data were first analyzed in CEFA [43], a computer program for unrestricted factor analyses [44]. As the MC-C was assumed to measure one underlying construct, i.e. social desirability, multi-factor structures were analyzed with oblique rotation to allow for correlations between factors. For this GEOMIN was used [44, 45]. Due to the scaling of the MC-C, the input matrix was based on polychoric correlations and the ordinary least squares method was used for parameter estimation [43]. Once the factor structure was determined, it was again tested in LISREL version 8.72 [46], using Robust Maximum Likelihood (RML), to both confirm the model and estimate model parameters [47].
For evaluation of the model resulting from the confirmatory factor analysis, a combination of fit statistics was chosen for a comprehensive assessment of model fit, i.e. a range of qualitatively different fit statistics was applied [4850]. First, the χ2 statistic [51] was used. It is based on the comparison of the model covariance matrix with the sample covariance matrix. If a non-significant χ2 is obtained, this indicates that the two matrices do not differ significantly, i.e. it indicates that the model fits well [52]. Second, the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) was chosen, with values of < 0.05 indicating close fit and those of < 0.08 indicating acceptable fit [53]. Third, for the standardized root mean square residuals (SRMR) a value of up to 0.08 was considered acceptable. Finally, the comparative fit index (CFI) was selected, with a cut-off value of 0.95 or above [54, 55].
In a second step, a model of partial mediation was developed in the framework of structural equation modeling (SEM) again using LISREL [46]. Social desirability was included as a mediating variable between predictor (pre-test) and outcome (post-test) following Kenny and colleagues [5658]. To establish whether social desirability was a mediator between heiQ pre-test and post-test data, the following conditions had to be established [56, 57]:
1)
Mediator and predictor must correlate, i.e. the predictor must affect the mediating variable for the latter to be a mediator between predictor and outcome. This was tested by regressing mediator (MC-C) on predictor (heiQ pre-test).
 
2)
The predictor must affect the outcome. This was tested by regressing outcome (heiQ post-test) on predictor (heiQ pre-test).
 
3)
The mediator must affect the outcome, i.e. it had to be established that the regression of outcome on mediator was significant. In this model, MC-C was included as a second endogenous variable, i.e. both heiQ post-test and MC-C were regressed on heiQ pre-test.
 
4)
Once conditions (1) to (3) were met, the statistical significance of the mediational effect was tested, i.e. the statistical significance of the product of the paths from a) predictor to mediator, and b) mediator to outcome [5961].
 
5)
Finally, while steps (1) to (4) are both necessary and sufficient conditions to establish mediation, the mediational effect must be interpreted in the overall context of the model [61]. Thus, it was assessed what proportion of the total effect was being mediated.
 
An example of the model using one hypothetical heiQ scale is visualized in Figure  1 where both MC-C and heiQ post-test are regressed on heiQ pre-test, and heiQ post-test is regressed on the MC-C.
Before analyzing heiQ and MC-C data, some preparatory steps were undertaken. First, each case with more than 50% missing items was deleted. Second, due to the alternate keying of the MC-C items, it could easily be detected if participants exhibited an acquiescent response style [62]. Consequently, respondents who had provided either only ‘true’ or only ‘false’ answers were discarded. It was assumed that they had filled out the MC-C regardless of item content. Once this preparation was finalized, all remaining missing values were replaced using the EM Algorithm [63], leading to a final sample size of n = 318.

Results

Exploratory factor analyses of the MC-C using CEFA suggested that a one-factor solution did not fit the data well. With two eigenvalues clearly above one (3.4 and 1.9, respectively) and two further eigenvalues at 1.1, factor solutions ranging between two factors and four factors were explored. While fit statistics improved in all multi-factor solutions, models beyond two factors were not superior to the two-factor solution. Therefore, a two-factor solution – labeled SD1 ‘defensiveness’ and SD2 ‘self-presentation’ – appeared most suitable for the MC-C with a moderate correlation of the two factors (0.48). As shown in Table  2, this solution was confirmed in LISREL. While fit statistics were excellent (non-significant Satorra-Bentler chi-square [64, 65], RMSEA = 0.023 [90% CI, 0.0;0.043], CFI = 0.99, and SRMR = 0.079), some small factor loadings were obtained ranging from 0.33 to 0.76. Reliability was also relatively low, with Coefficient alpha at 0.59 for SD1 and 0.56 for SD2. As the validation of the MC-C was of exploratory nature [54], these values were deemed acceptable for the present study.
Table 2
Confirmatory factor analysis of the short form Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale MC-C (n = 318)
 
Factor loading
Error variance
Self-presentation
  
1
It is sometimes hard for me to go on with my work if I am not encouraged.
0.44
0.81
2
I sometimes feel resentful when I don’t get my way.
0.76
0.43
3
On a few occasions, I have given up doing something because I thought too little of my ability.
0.51
0.74
4
There have been times when I felt like rebelling against people in authority even though I knew they were right.
0.54
0.71
6
There have been occasions when I took advantage of someone.
0.33
0.89
8
I sometimes try to get even rather than forgive and forget.
0.48
0.77
11
There have been times when I was quite jealous of the good fortune of others.
0.51
0.74
12
I am sometimes irritated by people who ask favours of me.
0.37
0.87
Defensiveness
  
5
No matter who I’m talking to, I’m always a good listener.
0.54
0.71
7
I’m always willing to admit it when I make a mistake.
0.64
0.59
9
I am always courteous, even to people who are disagreeable.
0.70
0.51
10
I have never been irked when people expressed ideas very different from my own.
0.48
0.77
13
I have never deliberately said something that hurt someone’s feelings.
0.42
0.83
Fit statistics: χ2 SB(63) = 73.7, p = NS; RMSEA = 0.023 (90% CI, 0.0;0.043); CFI = 0.99; SRMR = 0.079. Coefficient alpha: factor 1 = 0.59; factor 2 = 0.56; Phi = 0.48.

Social desirability in heiQ data

The first step of the 5-step procedure suggested that ‘defensiveness’ correlated significantly with pre-test data across all heiQ scales. Correlations ranged from 0.24 to 0.39, equivalent to a small to medium effect [59, 66]. In contrast, none of the heiQ scales indicated an association between ‘self-presentation’ and pre-test data (Table  3). Thus, only ‘defensiveness’ was explored as a potential partial mediating variable in heiQ data, while ‘self-presentation’ could be ruled out as a mediator.
Table 3
Covariance between ‘defensiveness’, ‘self-presentation’, and heiQ pre-test data
  
HDA
PAE
ED
SMI
CAA
STA
SIS
HSN
SD1-SD2
Cov
0.254*
0.247*
0.251*
0.249*
0.248*
0.246*
0.250*
0.240*
 
(SE)
(0.060)
(0.058)
(0.059)
(0.058)
(0.058)
(0.058)
(0.059)
(0.058)
 
Corr
0.482
0.480
0.473
0.478
0.475
0.477
0.480
0.477
SD1-pre
Cov
0.208*
0.192*
0.233*
0.312*
0.360*
0.255*
0.264*
0.199*
 
(SE)
(0.071)
(0.071)
(0.069)
(0.092)
(0.084)
(0.057)
(0.079)
(0.057)
 
Corr
0.255
0.242
0.280
0.310
0.388
0.375
0.280
0.271
SD2-pre
Cov
0.089
0.015
−0.032
0.155
0.032
0.098
0.009
0.019
 
(SE)
(0.068)
(0.068)
(0.065)
(0.098)
(0.076)
(0.061)
(0.070)
(0.066)
 
Corr
0.116
0.020
−0.041
0.156
0.037
0.153
0.010
0.026
* Significant, i.e. the effect is more than twice its standard error (Bollen, 1989).
Legend
HDA Health-directed activity.
PAE Positive and active engagement in life.
ED Emotional distress.
SMI Self-monitoring and insight.
CAA Constructive attitudes and approaches.
STA Skill and technique acquisition.
SIS Social integration and support.
HSN Health service navigation.
SD1 Factor 1 ‘defensiveness’.
SD2 Factor 2 ‘self-presentation’.
Cov Covariance.
SE Standard error.
Corr Correlation.
In Step 2 it was found that all direct paths from pre-test to post-test were significant. While subscale Social integration and support showed the strongest association between the two scores, all heiQ subscales showed substantial paths from predictor to outcome (Table  4).
Table 4
Regression of heiQ post-test on heiQ pre-test datav
 
HDA
PAE
ED
SMI
CAA
STA
SIS
HSN
pre-post
Path
0.648*
0.829*
0.758*
0.593*
0.672*
0.610*
0.774*
0.743*
 
(SE)
(0.044)
(0.069)
(0.049)
(0.071)
(0.066)
(0.065)
(0.044)
(0.053)
 
stand.
0.744
0.776
0.761
0.665
0.718
0.626
0.808
0.777
* Significant, i.e. the effect is more than twice its standard error (Bollen, 1989).
Legend
For an extensive legend refer to Table  3.
Finally, Table  5 presents the associations between pre-test and post-test once SD1 was included in the models. Again, paths between pre-test and ‘defensiveness’ were significant. Once pre-test data were controlled for ‘defensiveness’, Emotional distress was the only subscale that showed a significant association between ‘defensiveness’ and heiQ post-test data.
Table 5
Regression of ‘defensiveness’ and heiQ post-test data on heiQ pre-test data, and regression of heiQ post-test data on ‘defensiveness’
 
HDA
PAE
ED
SMI
CAA
STA
SIS
HSN
pre-post
Path
0.654*
0.826*
0.696*
0.575*
0.698*
0.606*
0.765*
0.757*
 
(SE)
(0.049)
(0.072)
(0.052)
(0.077)
(0.076)
(0.075)
(0.046)
(0.058)
 
stand.
0.753
0.772
0.700
0.642
0.752
0.618
0.796
0.788
pre-SD1
Path
0.172*
0.169*
0.191*
0.166*
0.229*
0.301*
0.160*
0.192*
 
(SE)
(0.057)
(0.059)
(0.056)
(0.049)
(0.052)
(0.067)
(0.047)
(0.053)
 
stand.
0.259
0.252
0.290
0.326
0.399
0.388
0.284
0.283
SD1-post
Path
−0.038
0.026
0.322*
0.128
−0.141
0.024
0.070
−0.060
(SE)
(0.088)
(0.107)
(0.099)
(0.132)
(0.123)
(0.101)
(0.092)
(0.102)
 
stand.
−0.029
0.016
0.213
0.073
−0.087
0.019
0.041
−0.043
* Significant, i.e. the effect is more than twice its standard error (Bollen, 1989).
Legend
For an extensive legend refer to Table  3.
As ‘defensiveness’ was found to be associated with Emotional distress, steps 4 and 5 were performed on this heiQ scale only. Once ‘defensiveness’ was included in the model, the path between pre-tests and post-tests decreased by 0.062, a significant effect, as it was more than twice its standard error [52], i.e. SEM = √ (0.1912 * 0.0992 + 0.3222 * 0.0562) = 0.026. The magnitude of the effect, however, was small as it contributed only 8.2% of the total variation in change scores.

Discussion

This study explored the potential mediating effect of social desirability in the measurement of outcomes of chronic disease self-management courses. For this, we used rigorous statistical techniques – including exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis of the MC-C as well as applying a comprehensive 5-step model – to explore both direct and mediating effects on key outcomes. Surprisingly, while we had expected clear evidence of bias in estimates of change through socially desirable responding, virtually no social desirability bias was found. When analyzing social desirability bias as a potential mediating variable between heiQ pre-test and post-test data, only the ‘defensiveness’ factor but not the ‘self-presentation’ factor of the MC-C showed an association with pre-test data, a prerequisite for investigating mediation. The notion of ‘defense’ and ‘self-protection’ was introduced as one critical aspect of the approval motive [6]. Subsequent research, however, suggested that subjects’ motivations to present themselves in a socially desirable way was linked more strongly to ‘defensiveness’ rather than ‘self-presentation’ [7, 67] which may explain our findings, i.e. the lack of association of pre-test data with ‘self-presentation’.
Despite the significant association of ‘defensiveness’ with all pre-tests, it exerted only little influence on heiQ post-test data once pre-test data were controlled for. Only one heiQ scale (Emotional distress) showed that ‘defensiveness’ operated as a true, albeit minor, mediator. Therefore, the influence of social desirability bias in heiQ data can largely be ruled out. This finding is contrary to our expectations. First, the specific context of group-based chronic disease self-management interventions, potential rapport among participants and between participants and course leader(s), and provision of data in the presence of course leaders are factors that may be conducive to exhibiting a socially desirable response style. Second, social desirability has been found to be related to a range of demographic variables. Among others, older women [9, 10], women of lower socio-economic status [10, 11], and older respondents [12, 13] have been found to be most prone to socially desirable responding. While we did not have socio-economic data, the remaining characteristics largely fit our sample, i.e. an additional argument for the presence of social desirability in our study.
There are several possible explanations for our findings, i.e. lack of social desirability bias in heiQ data. First, all heiQ items have been written in a way that discourage response styles [41]. That is, even people who are usually prone to socially desirable responding may have been discouraged to do so through the content and structure of heiQ items. The heiQ was developed using grounded approaches including the use of concepts and wording that were directly derived from patients. Second, the short form MC-C scale was used to explore a potential effect of social desirability bias. Although there is sufficient support in the literature that the MC-C is a valid alternative to the full MC scale, and our re-validation supported a two-factor solution with excellent fit statistics, it is possible that the analyses were hampered by a suboptimal performance of this shortened measure. Despite excellent fit indices in LISREL, low reliability and some small factor loadings may have limited the power of the analyses to detect mediational effects of social desirability.
In this study we applied a novel approach to testing the influence of social desirability bias in the context of chronic disease self-management programs. Apart from providing a detailed re-validation of the MC-C [30], with both exploratory and confirmatory analyses, a sophisticated model of partial mediation was developed that should have detected an association of social desirability if there had been any. However, it cannot be ruled out that the MC-C scale did not perform sufficiently well, while a potential co-existence of equivalent models also needs to be acknowledged [68, 69]. For example, it would have been plausible to define ‘defensiveness’ as a predictor of both pre-test and post-test or define a model of moderated mediation [57, 59], with variables such as age, gender, or education operating as moderating variables. It is possible that there was a mediating effect of socially desirable responding in older participants but not in their younger counterparts. The sample size of the dataset, however, did not allow for such modeling. Further, it is possible that social desirability moderated – rather than mediated – the effect between pre-test and post-test. However, as ‘social desirability’ was defined as a response style that was hypothesized to improve the prediction of post-test levels – i.e. the variable ‘social desirability’ was defined as part of the causal chain [56, 57] – current model definition was assumed to be most appropriate to test for socially desirable responding. In view of our specific research questions, the present model is a logical and theoretically sound approach [61]. That is, the path between pre-test and post-test was understood as the primary path in the model, and social desirability was defined as a response style that potentially partially mediated the relationship between heiQ pre-test and post-test data.

Conclusions

The analyses of this study also provided support for the measurement qualities of the heiQ. That is, data derived from this questionnaire appear robust against bias through socially desirable responding. Based on the present research, the use of the heiQ within the traditional method of assessing change (post-test minus pre-test) appears immune to potential confounding effects through social desirability. However, further research is necessary to ascertain whether this bias is present at the subject-level. To advance the field, a combination of qualitative and quantitative approaches at group-level and individual-level is needed and questionnaires other than Reynold’s short-form should be used to further explore whether social desirability bias exists in the evaluation of chronic disease self-management programs. With the assumption that Reynold’s short-form of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability scale captures the propensity for individuals to provide socially desirable answers, change scores in patient education program evaluation are not biased by social desirability.

Ethical adherence

The study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Melbourne.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Amanda Springer, Dianne Ferguson and Luke Tellefson for data collection and management support as well as all participants who kindly took part in the study and the coordinators and course leaders who administered the questionnaires.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

The authors state that there are no conflicts of interests.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat DeMaio TJ: Social desirability and survey measurement: a review. In Surveying subjective phenomena. Volume 2. Edited by: Turner C, Martin E. New York: Russell Sage; 1984:257–282. DeMaio TJ: Social desirability and survey measurement: a review. In Surveying subjective phenomena. Volume 2. Edited by: Turner C, Martin E. New York: Russell Sage; 1984:257–282.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Holbrook AL, Green MC, Krosnick JA: Telephone versus face-to-face interviewing of national probability samples with long questionnaires: comparisons of respondent satisficing and social desirability response bias. Public Opin Quart 2003, 67: 79–125. 10.1086/346010CrossRef Holbrook AL, Green MC, Krosnick JA: Telephone versus face-to-face interviewing of national probability samples with long questionnaires: comparisons of respondent satisficing and social desirability response bias. Public Opin Quart 2003, 67: 79–125. 10.1086/346010CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwarz N, Oyserman D: Asking questions about behavior: cognition, communication and questionnaire construction. Am J Eval 2001, 22: 127–160.CrossRef Schwarz N, Oyserman D: Asking questions about behavior: cognition, communication and questionnaire construction. Am J Eval 2001, 22: 127–160.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Nunnally JC, Bernstein IH: Psychometric theory. 3rd edition. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1994. Nunnally JC, Bernstein IH: Psychometric theory. 3rd edition. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1994.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Paulhus DL: Two-component models of socially desirable responding. J Pers Soc Psychol 1984, 46: 598–609.CrossRef Paulhus DL: Two-component models of socially desirable responding. J Pers Soc Psychol 1984, 46: 598–609.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Crowne DP, Marlowe D: The approval motive: studies in evaluative dependence. New York, London, Sydney: John Wiley & Sons; 1964. Crowne DP, Marlowe D: The approval motive: studies in evaluative dependence. New York, London, Sydney: John Wiley & Sons; 1964.
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Millham J: Two components of need for approval score and their relationship to cheating following success and failure. J Res Pers 1974, 8: 378–392. 10.1016/0092-6566(74)90028-2CrossRef Millham J: Two components of need for approval score and their relationship to cheating following success and failure. J Res Pers 1974, 8: 378–392. 10.1016/0092-6566(74)90028-2CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Moorman RH, Podsakoff PM: A meta-analytic review and empirical test of the potential confounding effects of social desirability response sets in organizational behavior research. J Occup Organ Psych 1992, 65: 131–149. 10.1111/j.2044-8325.1992.tb00490.xCrossRef Moorman RH, Podsakoff PM: A meta-analytic review and empirical test of the potential confounding effects of social desirability response sets in organizational behavior research. J Occup Organ Psych 1992, 65: 131–149. 10.1111/j.2044-8325.1992.tb00490.xCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Ray JJ: Lie scales and the elderly. Pers Indiv Differ 1988, 9: 417–418. 10.1016/0191-8869(88)90106-7CrossRef Ray JJ: Lie scales and the elderly. Pers Indiv Differ 1988, 9: 417–418. 10.1016/0191-8869(88)90106-7CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Visser AP, Breemhaar B, Kleijnen JGVM: Social desirability and program evaluation in health care. Impact Assessment Bulletin 1989, 7: 99–112. 10.1080/07349165.1989.9726015CrossRef Visser AP, Breemhaar B, Kleijnen JGVM: Social desirability and program evaluation in health care. Impact Assessment Bulletin 1989, 7: 99–112. 10.1080/07349165.1989.9726015CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Kalliopuska M: Social desirability related to social class among adults. Psychol Rep 1992, 70: 808–810. 10.2466/pr0.1992.70.3.808CrossRef Kalliopuska M: Social desirability related to social class among adults. Psychol Rep 1992, 70: 808–810. 10.2466/pr0.1992.70.3.808CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Deshields T, Tait R, Gfeller J, Chibnall J: Relationship between social desirability and self-report in chronic pain patients. Clin J Pain 1995, 11: 189–193. 10.1097/00002508-199509000-00005PubMedCrossRef Deshields T, Tait R, Gfeller J, Chibnall J: Relationship between social desirability and self-report in chronic pain patients. Clin J Pain 1995, 11: 189–193. 10.1097/00002508-199509000-00005PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Komarahadi FL, Maurischat C, Harter M, Bengel J: Zusammenhänge von Depressivität und Ängstlichkeit mit sozialer Erwünschtheit bei chronischen Schmerzpatienten. Der Schmerz 2004, 18: 38–44. 10.1007/s00482-003-0282-2PubMedCrossRef Komarahadi FL, Maurischat C, Harter M, Bengel J: Zusammenhänge von Depressivität und Ängstlichkeit mit sozialer Erwünschtheit bei chronischen Schmerzpatienten. Der Schmerz 2004, 18: 38–44. 10.1007/s00482-003-0282-2PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Pedregon CA, Farleya RL, Davisa A, Wooda JM, Clark RD: Social desirability, personality questionnaires, and the “better than average” effect. Pers Indiv Differ 2012, 52: 213–217. 10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.022CrossRef Pedregon CA, Farleya RL, Davisa A, Wooda JM, Clark RD: Social desirability, personality questionnaires, and the “better than average” effect. Pers Indiv Differ 2012, 52: 213–217. 10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.022CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Edwards AL: The social desirability variable in personality assessment and research. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston; 1957. Edwards AL: The social desirability variable in personality assessment and research. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston; 1957.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Crowne DP, Marlowe D: A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. J Consult Psychol 1960, 24: 349–354.PubMedCrossRef Crowne DP, Marlowe D: A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. J Consult Psychol 1960, 24: 349–354.PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Edwards AL, Walsh JA: Response sets in standard and experimental personality scales. Am Educ Res J 1964, 1: 52–61. 10.3102/00028312001001052CrossRef Edwards AL, Walsh JA: Response sets in standard and experimental personality scales. Am Educ Res J 1964, 1: 52–61. 10.3102/00028312001001052CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Hays RD, Hayashi T, Stewart AL: A five-item measure of socially desirable response set. Educ Psychol Meas 1989, 49: 629–636. 10.1177/001316448904900315CrossRef Hays RD, Hayashi T, Stewart AL: A five-item measure of socially desirable response set. Educ Psychol Meas 1989, 49: 629–636. 10.1177/001316448904900315CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Barger S: The Marlowe-Crowne affair: short forms, psychometric structure, and social desirability. J Pers Assess 2002, 79: 286–305. 10.1207/S15327752JPA7902_11PubMedCrossRef Barger S: The Marlowe-Crowne affair: short forms, psychometric structure, and social desirability. J Pers Assess 2002, 79: 286–305. 10.1207/S15327752JPA7902_11PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Paulhus DL: Measurement and control of response bias. In Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. Edited by: Robinson J, Shaver P, Wrightsman L. New York: Academic Press; 1991:17–59.CrossRef Paulhus DL: Measurement and control of response bias. In Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. Edited by: Robinson J, Shaver P, Wrightsman L. New York: Academic Press; 1991:17–59.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Leite WL, Beretvas SN: Validation of scores on the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale and the Balanced Inventory of desirable responding. Educ Psychol Meas 2005, 65: 140–154. 10.1177/0013164404267285CrossRef Leite WL, Beretvas SN: Validation of scores on the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale and the Balanced Inventory of desirable responding. Educ Psychol Meas 2005, 65: 140–154. 10.1177/0013164404267285CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Ramanaiah NV, Schill T, Leung LS: A test of the hypothesis about the two-dimensional nature of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. J Res Pers 1977, 11: 251–259. 10.1016/0092-6566(77)90022-8CrossRef Ramanaiah NV, Schill T, Leung LS: A test of the hypothesis about the two-dimensional nature of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. J Res Pers 1977, 11: 251–259. 10.1016/0092-6566(77)90022-8CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Crino MD, Svoboda M, Rubenfeld S, White MC: Data on the Marlowe-Crowne and Edwards Social Desirability scales. Psychol Rep 1983, 53: 963–968. 10.2466/pr0.1983.53.3.963CrossRef Crino MD, Svoboda M, Rubenfeld S, White MC: Data on the Marlowe-Crowne and Edwards Social Desirability scales. Psychol Rep 1983, 53: 963–968. 10.2466/pr0.1983.53.3.963CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Ballard R: Short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Psychol Rep 1992, 71: 1155–1160.PubMedCrossRef Ballard R: Short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Psychol Rep 1992, 71: 1155–1160.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Loo R, Thorpe K: Confirmatory factor analyses of the full and short versions of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. J Soc Psychol 2000, 140: 628–635. 10.1080/00224540009600503PubMedCrossRef Loo R, Thorpe K: Confirmatory factor analyses of the full and short versions of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. J Soc Psychol 2000, 140: 628–635. 10.1080/00224540009600503PubMedCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Loo R, Loewen P: Confirmatory factor analyses of scores from full and short versions of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability scale. J Appl Soc Psychol 2004, 34: 2343–2352. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb01980.xCrossRef Loo R, Loewen P: Confirmatory factor analyses of scores from full and short versions of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability scale. J Appl Soc Psychol 2004, 34: 2343–2352. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb01980.xCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Ballard R, Crino M, Rubenfeld S: Social desirability response bias and the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Psychol Rep 1988, 63: 227–237. 10.2466/pr0.1988.63.1.227CrossRef Ballard R, Crino M, Rubenfeld S: Social desirability response bias and the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Psychol Rep 1988, 63: 227–237. 10.2466/pr0.1988.63.1.227CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Fischer DG, Fick C: Measuring social desirability: short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Educ Psychol Meas 1993, 53: 417–424. 10.1177/0013164493053002011CrossRef Fischer DG, Fick C: Measuring social desirability: short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Educ Psychol Meas 1993, 53: 417–424. 10.1177/0013164493053002011CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Fraboni M, Cooper D: Further validation of three short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne scale of Social Desirability. Psychol Rep 1989, 65: 595–600. 10.2466/pr0.1989.65.2.595CrossRef Fraboni M, Cooper D: Further validation of three short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne scale of Social Desirability. Psychol Rep 1989, 65: 595–600. 10.2466/pr0.1989.65.2.595CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Reynolds WM: Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. J Clin Psychol 1982, 38: 119–125. 10.1002/1097-4679(198201)38:1<119::AID-JCLP2270380118>3.0.CO;2-ICrossRef Reynolds WM: Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. J Clin Psychol 1982, 38: 119–125. 10.1002/1097-4679(198201)38:1<119::AID-JCLP2270380118>3.0.CO;2-ICrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Zook A, Sipps GJ: Cross-validation of a short form of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. J Clin Psychol 1985, 41: 236–238. 10.1002/1097-4679(198503)41:2<236::AID-JCLP2270410217>3.0.CO;2-HCrossRef Zook A, Sipps GJ: Cross-validation of a short form of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. J Clin Psychol 1985, 41: 236–238. 10.1002/1097-4679(198503)41:2<236::AID-JCLP2270410217>3.0.CO;2-HCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Frasure-Smith N, Lespérance F, Juneau M, Talajic M, Bourassa MG: Gender, depression, and one-year prognosis after myocardial infarction. Psychosom Med 1999, 61: 26–37.PubMedCrossRef Frasure-Smith N, Lespérance F, Juneau M, Talajic M, Bourassa MG: Gender, depression, and one-year prognosis after myocardial infarction. Psychosom Med 1999, 61: 26–37.PubMedCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Leake R, Friend R, Wadhwa N: Improving adjustment to chronic illness through strategic self-presentation: an experimental study on a renal dialysis unit. Health Psychol 1999, 18: 54–62.PubMedCrossRef Leake R, Friend R, Wadhwa N: Improving adjustment to chronic illness through strategic self-presentation: an experimental study on a renal dialysis unit. Health Psychol 1999, 18: 54–62.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Andrews P, Meyer R: Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale and Short Form C: forensic norms. J Clin Psychol 2003, 59: 483–492. 10.1002/jclp.10136PubMedCrossRef Andrews P, Meyer R: Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale and Short Form C: forensic norms. J Clin Psychol 2003, 59: 483–492. 10.1002/jclp.10136PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Robinette RL: The relationship between the Marlowe-Crowne Form C and the validity scales of the MMPI. J Clin Psychol 1991, 47: 396–399. 10.1002/1097-4679(199105)47:3<396::AID-JCLP2270470311>3.0.CO;2-KPubMedCrossRef Robinette RL: The relationship between the Marlowe-Crowne Form C and the validity scales of the MMPI. J Clin Psychol 1991, 47: 396–399. 10.1002/1097-4679(199105)47:3<396::AID-JCLP2270470311>3.0.CO;2-KPubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Loevinger J: Theory and techniques of assessment. Annu Rev Psychol 1959, 10: 287–316. 10.1146/annurev.ps.10.020159.001443CrossRef Loevinger J: Theory and techniques of assessment. Annu Rev Psychol 1959, 10: 287–316. 10.1146/annurev.ps.10.020159.001443CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Nolte S: Approaches to the measurement of outcomes of chronic disease self-management interventions using a self-report inventory. Global Studies, Social Science & Planning: RMIT University; 2008. Nolte S: Approaches to the measurement of outcomes of chronic disease self-management interventions using a self-report inventory. Global Studies, Social Science & Planning: RMIT University; 2008.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Lorig KR, González VM, Laurent DD: The chronic disease self-management program: leaders manual. Palo Alto: Stanford University; 1999. Lorig KR, González VM, Laurent DD: The chronic disease self-management program: leaders manual. Palo Alto: Stanford University; 1999.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Osborne RH, Hawkins M, Sprangers MAG: Change of perspective: a measurable and desired outcome of chronic disease self-management intervention programs that violates the premise of preintervention/postintervention assessment. Arthrit Care Res 2006, 55: 458–465. 10.1002/art.21982CrossRef Osborne RH, Hawkins M, Sprangers MAG: Change of perspective: a measurable and desired outcome of chronic disease self-management intervention programs that violates the premise of preintervention/postintervention assessment. Arthrit Care Res 2006, 55: 458–465. 10.1002/art.21982CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Nolte S, Elsworth GR, Sinclair AJ, Osborne RH: The extent and breadth of benefits from participating in chronic disease self-management courses: a national patient-reported outcomes survey. Patient Educ Couns 2007, 65: 351–360. 10.1016/j.pec.2006.08.016PubMedCrossRef Nolte S, Elsworth GR, Sinclair AJ, Osborne RH: The extent and breadth of benefits from participating in chronic disease self-management courses: a national patient-reported outcomes survey. Patient Educ Couns 2007, 65: 351–360. 10.1016/j.pec.2006.08.016PubMedCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Osborne RH, Elsworth GR, Whitfield K: The health education impact questionnaire (heiQ): an outcomes and evaluation measure for patient education and self-management interventions for people with chronic conditions. Patient Educ Couns 2007, 66: 192–201. 10.1016/j.pec.2006.12.002PubMedCrossRef Osborne RH, Elsworth GR, Whitfield K: The health education impact questionnaire (heiQ): an outcomes and evaluation measure for patient education and self-management interventions for people with chronic conditions. Patient Educ Couns 2007, 66: 192–201. 10.1016/j.pec.2006.12.002PubMedCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Osborne RH, Batterham R, Livingston J: The evaluation of chronic disease self-management support across settings: the international experience of the health education impact questionnaire monitoring system. Nurs Clin N Am 2011, 46: 255–270. 10.1016/j.cnur.2011.05.010CrossRef Osborne RH, Batterham R, Livingston J: The evaluation of chronic disease self-management support across settings: the international experience of the health education impact questionnaire monitoring system. Nurs Clin N Am 2011, 46: 255–270. 10.1016/j.cnur.2011.05.010CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat McDonald RP: Semiconfirmatory factor analysis: the example of anxiety and depression. Struct Equ Modeling 2005, 12: 163–172. 10.1207/s15328007sem1201_9CrossRef McDonald RP: Semiconfirmatory factor analysis: the example of anxiety and depression. Struct Equ Modeling 2005, 12: 163–172. 10.1207/s15328007sem1201_9CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Browne MW: An overview of analytic rotation in exploratory factor analysis. Multivar Behav Res 2001, 36: 111–150. 10.1207/S15327906MBR3601_05CrossRef Browne MW: An overview of analytic rotation in exploratory factor analysis. Multivar Behav Res 2001, 36: 111–150. 10.1207/S15327906MBR3601_05CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Jöreskog KG, Sörbom D: LISREL 8: User’s reference guide. 2nd edn. Lincolnwood: IL: Scientific Software International; 1996–2001. Jöreskog KG, Sörbom D: LISREL 8: User’s reference guide. 2nd edn. Lincolnwood: IL: Scientific Software International; 1996–2001.
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Bollen K, Long J: Introduction. In Testing structural equation models. Edited by: Bollen K, Long J. Newbury Park, London, New Delhi: Sage Publications; 1993. Bollen K, Long J: Introduction. In Testing structural equation models. Edited by: Bollen K, Long J. Newbury Park, London, New Delhi: Sage Publications; 1993.
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Tanaka J: Multifaceted conceptions of fit in structural equation models. In Testing structural equation models. Edited by: Bollen K, Long J. Newbury Park, London, New Delhi: Sage Publications; 1993. Tanaka J: Multifaceted conceptions of fit in structural equation models. In Testing structural equation models. Edited by: Bollen K, Long J. Newbury Park, London, New Delhi: Sage Publications; 1993.
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Marsh HW, Balla JR, Hau K: An evaluation of incremental fit indices: a clarification of mathematical and empirical properties. In Advanced structural equation modeling: issues and techniques. Edited by: Marcoulides GA, Schumacker RE. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1996:315–353. Marsh HW, Balla JR, Hau K: An evaluation of incremental fit indices: a clarification of mathematical and empirical properties. In Advanced structural equation modeling: issues and techniques. Edited by: Marcoulides GA, Schumacker RE. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1996:315–353.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Gerbing D, Anderson J: Monte Carlo evaluations of goodness-of-fit indices for structural equation models. In Testing structural equation models. Edited by: Bollen K, Long J. Newbury Park, London, New Delhi: Sage Publications; 1993. Gerbing D, Anderson J: Monte Carlo evaluations of goodness-of-fit indices for structural equation models. In Testing structural equation models. Edited by: Bollen K, Long J. Newbury Park, London, New Delhi: Sage Publications; 1993.
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Bollen KA: Structural equations with latent variables. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1989.CrossRef Bollen KA: Structural equations with latent variables. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1989.CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Browne M, Cudeck R: Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In Testing structural equation models. Edited by: Bollen K, Long J. Newbury Park, London, New Delhi: Sage Publications; 1993. Browne M, Cudeck R: Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In Testing structural equation models. Edited by: Bollen K, Long J. Newbury Park, London, New Delhi: Sage Publications; 1993.
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Hair JF, Black WC, Rabin BJ, Anderson RE, Tatham RL: Multivariate data analysis. 6th edition. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, Inc.; 2006. Hair JF, Black WC, Rabin BJ, Anderson RE, Tatham RL: Multivariate data analysis. 6th edition. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, Inc.; 2006.
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Hu L, Bentler P: Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling 1999, 6: 1–55. 10.1080/10705519909540118CrossRef Hu L, Bentler P: Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling 1999, 6: 1–55. 10.1080/10705519909540118CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Judd CM, Kenny DA: Process analysis: estimating mediation in treatment evaluations. Evaluation Rev 1981, 5: 602–619. 10.1177/0193841X8100500502CrossRef Judd CM, Kenny DA: Process analysis: estimating mediation in treatment evaluations. Evaluation Rev 1981, 5: 602–619. 10.1177/0193841X8100500502CrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Baron R, Kenny D: The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986, 51: 1173–1182.PubMedCrossRef Baron R, Kenny D: The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 1986, 51: 1173–1182.PubMedCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Kenny DA, Kashy DA, Bolger N: Data analysis in social psychology. In The handbook of social psychology. Volume 1. 4th edition. Edited by: Gilbert D, Fiske S, Lindzey G. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1998:233–265. Kenny DA, Kashy DA, Bolger N: Data analysis in social psychology. In The handbook of social psychology. Volume 1. 4th edition. Edited by: Gilbert D, Fiske S, Lindzey G. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1998:233–265.
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Shrout PE, Bolger N: Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol Methods 2002, 7: 422–445.PubMedCrossRef Shrout PE, Bolger N: Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol Methods 2002, 7: 422–445.PubMedCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Sobel ME: Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equations models. In Sociological methodology. Edited by: Leinhart S. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1982:290–312. Sobel ME: Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equations models. In Sociological methodology. Edited by: Leinhart S. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1982:290–312.
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Little TD, Card NA, Bovaird JA, Preacher KJ, Crandall CS: Structural equation modeling of mediation and moderation with contextual factors. In Modeling contextual effects in longitudinal studies. Edited by: Little TD, Bovaird JA, Card NA. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2007:207–230. Little TD, Card NA, Bovaird JA, Preacher KJ, Crandall CS: Structural equation modeling of mediation and moderation with contextual factors. In Modeling contextual effects in longitudinal studies. Edited by: Little TD, Bovaird JA, Card NA. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2007:207–230.
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Cronbach LJ: Response sets and test validity. Educ Psychol Meas 1946, 6: 475–494. Cronbach LJ: Response sets and test validity. Educ Psychol Meas 1946, 6: 475–494.
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Dempster AP, Laird NM, Rubin DB: Maximum likelihood from incomplete data via the EM algorithm. J R Stat Soc Series B (Methodological) 1977, 39: 1–38. Dempster AP, Laird NM, Rubin DB: Maximum likelihood from incomplete data via the EM algorithm. J R Stat Soc Series B (Methodological) 1977, 39: 1–38.
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Satorra A, Bentler PM: Scaling corrections for chi-square statistics in covariance structure analysis. Proceedings of the Business and Economic Statistics Section of the American Statistical Association 1988, 1988: 308–313. Satorra A, Bentler PM: Scaling corrections for chi-square statistics in covariance structure analysis. Proceedings of the Business and Economic Statistics Section of the American Statistical Association 1988, 1988: 308–313.
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Satorra A, Bentler PM: Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis. In Latent variable analysis: applications for developmental research. Edited by: von Eye A, Clogg C. CA: Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks; 1994:399–419. Satorra A, Bentler PM: Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis. In Latent variable analysis: applications for developmental research. Edited by: von Eye A, Clogg C. CA: Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks; 1994:399–419.
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen J: Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. 2nd edition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988. Cohen J: Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. 2nd edition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Millham J, Kellogg RW: Need for social approval: impression management or self-deception? J Res Pers 1980, 14: 445–457. 10.1016/0092-6566(80)90003-3CrossRef Millham J, Kellogg RW: Need for social approval: impression management or self-deception? J Res Pers 1980, 14: 445–457. 10.1016/0092-6566(80)90003-3CrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat MacCallum RC, Wegener DT, Uchino BN, Fabrigar LR: The problem of equivalent models in applications of covariance structure analysis. Psychol Bull 1993, 114: 185–199.PubMedCrossRef MacCallum RC, Wegener DT, Uchino BN, Fabrigar LR: The problem of equivalent models in applications of covariance structure analysis. Psychol Bull 1993, 114: 185–199.PubMedCrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Frazier PA, Tix AP, Barron KE: Testing moderator and mediator effects in counseling psychology research. J Couns Psychol 2004, 51: 115–134.CrossRef Frazier PA, Tix AP, Barron KE: Testing moderator and mediator effects in counseling psychology research. J Couns Psychol 2004, 51: 115–134.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Absence of social desirability bias in the evaluation of chronic disease self-management interventions
verfasst von
Sandra Nolte
Gerald R Elsworth
Richard H Osborne
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2013
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Health and Quality of Life Outcomes / Ausgabe 1/2013
Elektronische ISSN: 1477-7525
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1477-7525-11-114

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2013

Health and Quality of Life Outcomes 1/2013 Zur Ausgabe