Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Neurology 1/2014

Open Access 01.12.2014 | Research article

Assessment of visual fixation in vegetative and minimally conscious states

verfasst von: Haibo Di, Yunzhi Nie, Xiaohua Hu, Yong Tong, Lizette Heine, Sarah Wannez, Wangshan Huang, Dan Yu, Minhui He, Aurore Thibaut, Caroline Schnakers, Steven Laureys

Erschienen in: BMC Neurology | Ausgabe 1/2014

download
DOWNLOAD
print
DRUCKEN
insite
SUCHEN

Abstract

Background

Visual fixation plays a key role in the differentiation between vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness (VS/UWS) syndrome and minimally conscious state (MCS). However, the use of different stimuli changes the frequency of visual fixation occured in patients, thereby possibly affecting the accuracy of the diagnosis. In order to establish a standardized assessment of visual fixation in patients in disorders of consciousness (DOC), we compared the frequency of visual fixation elicited by mirror,a ball and a light.

Method

Visual fixation was assessed in eighty-one post-comatose patients diagnosed with a MCS or VS/UWS. Occurrence of fixation to different stimuli was analysis used Chi-square testing.

Result

40 (49%) out of the 81 patients showed fixation to visual stimuli. Among those, significantly more patients (39, 48%) had visual fixation elicited by mirror compared to a ball (23, 28%) and mirror compared to a light (20, 25%).

Conclusion

The use of a mirror during the assessment of visual fixation showed higher positive response rate, compared to other stimuli in eliciting a visual fixating response. Therefore, fixation elicited by a mirror can be a very sensitive and accurate test to differentiate the two disorders of consciousness.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1471-2377-14-147) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Haibo Di, Yunzhi Nie contributed equally to this work.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interest.

Authors’ contributions

YN, XH, YT, WH and DY collected data and managed the patients; HD and YN performed data analyses; SL and HD designed the study; YN, LH, SL and HD wrote the paper. SW, CS, AT and MH revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. HD and YN contributed equally to the study. All authors discussed the results and commented on the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Abkürzungen
VS/UWS
Vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome
MCS
Minimally conscious state
CRS-R
Coma Recovery Scale-Revised
DOC
Disorders of consciousness.

Background

At present, there are many behavioral scales available for the evaluation of visual fixation in patients in post-comatose states. These disorders of consciousness (DOC) include VS (now also coined unresponsive wakefulness syndrome) [1], and MCS [2]. Indeed, the scales all use different stimuli to assess visual fixation: the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R) recommends using a brightly colored or illuminated object. The Sensory Modalities Assessment and Rehabilitation Technique (SMART) uses a photo of a baby. The Disorders of Consciousness Scale (DOCS) uses an object (e.g., ball). The Coma/Near Coma Scale employs flashes of light. The Wessex Head Injury Matrix (WHIM) uses a person, and the Western Neuro Sensory Stimulation Profile (WNSSP) only observes spontaneous eye contract (for references see review by Majerus and colleagues [3]).
The CRS-R is at present the most sensitive tool to differentiate between the different states of (un) consciousness [4]. However, visual behavior has been shown to still differ greatly with different stimuli [5]. Vanhaudenhuyse et al. used a mirror and other neutral stimuli to assess visual pursuit in patients with DOC, and showed that a mirror provokes visual pursuit more frequently in patients, due to the self-referential value [6]. Specifically, some patients showed visual pursuit when using a mirror, but not with other stimuli such as a colored ball or a bright light. To our knowledge, the increased effect of a mirror has not yet been investigated in visual fixation. Hence, in this study we choose the visual stimuli described in the CRS-R Administration and Scoring Guidelines: a yellow ball (as brightly colored object), a light (as illuminated object) and a mirror (as self-referential object) to induce visual fixation in patients with DOC. We will compare the ability of different stimuli in eliciting visual fixation in order to optimize the stimulation during the assessment of consciousness.

Method

Eighty-one patients recovering from coma were recruited from Wujing Hospital of Hangzhou City, Hangzhou, China, and were free of sedative drugs. Each patient was assessed in a sitting position. If patients exhibited sustained eyelid closure and/or stopped following commands for a period of at least one minute, a standardized arousal facilitation protocol (i.e., deep pressure stimulations from the facial muscles to the toes) was employed in order to prolong the time the patients maintained aroused, and this protocol was re-administered if patients showed sustained eye closure again or behavioral responsiveness ceased despite sustained eye opening [7]. Visual fixation was evaluated through the standardized methodology as described in the CRS-R [7]. In brief, a visual stimulus (i.e., a mirror (round, diameter = 15 cm), a ball (yellow, diameter = 6 cm) and a continuous burning light (power = 1.2w)) was presented by the experimenter in front of the patient’s face (15–20 cm) and then rapidly moved above and below the horizontal midline, as well as to the right and left of the vertical midline. Thus the stimulus moved once in each direction (4 trials). The order of presentation was randomized using a “random number” procedure in Excel. Visual fixation is defined as a movement of the eyes from the initial fixation point with a re-fixation on the new target location for more than 2 seconds. At least 2 episodes of fixation are required for the scoring of visual fixation. Differences between fixation as assessed by mirror, ball and light were measured using Chi-square test. Results were considered significant at p < 0.01. Eye movements were observed before administration of stimuli to avoid scoring of spontaneous movements. For example, for subjects with roving eye movements the stimuli were presented in a manner unrelated to pre-existing spontaneous eye movements. When any doubt existed, the movement was not scored. Patients’ visual reaction was visually judged by one experienced assessor who was blinded to diagnosis (e.g., did not do CRS-R assessment), and was unaware of the hypothesis of this study, and the same examiner conducted the trials in all patients. Clinical diagnosis was made according to the Aspen work group criteria for disorders of consciousness [2] and based on the CRS-R assessments [7] performed by two trained and experienced neuropsychologists. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Hangzhou Normal University and Wujing Hospital which complies with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). Informed consents were obtained by the patient’s legal surrogates.

Result

Of the 81 patients (69 males; mean age 45 (SD17) years), 38 (47%) were diagnosed as in a VS/UWS and 43 (53%) in a MCS. Mean time between injury and assessment was 9 months (SD15). Etiology was traumatic in 59 (73%) and non-traumatic in 22 (27%) patients (Table 1). 40 (49%) out of the 81 patients showed visual fixation, which were all diagnosed as in a MCS. Of the patients who showed visual fixation, 39 (48%, 28 traumatic) showed fixation to a mirror, 23 (28%, 15 traumatic) showed fixation to a ball, and 20 (25%, 15 traumatic) showed fixation to light. Except one, no patient showed fixation to the ball or the light without the mirror. The global value of the observed Chi-2 statistic between the mirror, ball and light was 27.22, and p < 0.001. When doing the local comparisons between the three stimuli, the mirror elicited significantly more visual fixation compared to the two other stimuli (p < 0.001), while the difference between the frequency of visual fixation assessed by a ball and a light was not significant (Figure 1). The occurrence of visual fixation had no significant relationship with etiology or time since insult (p > 0.05). The overall behavioral responsiveness as assessed by the CRS-R total score tended to be higher when patients fixated on all three stimuli (n = 17) compared to no fixation (n = 41) (Table 1). In fact, patients showing a response to the mirror, the ball and the light had a mean CRS-R total score 9.6 whereas patients showing no fixation to any of the stimuli had a mean score 4.8, and patients having fixation to two (n = 8) or one (n = 15) stimuli showed intermediate mean CRS-R total scores of respectively 9.3 and 8.3. Correlation analysis showed that the rank correlation coefficient between the number of stimuli fixated by patients and the CRS-R total score was 0.743, p < 0.001. Multiple regression analysis did not show an effect of sex or age and the p values of the partial regression coefficient were 0.174 for sex and 0.553 for age.
Table 1
Clinical data of patients in VS/UWS and MCS
Patient
Gender
Aetiology
Time since injury**
CRS-R total score
Visual subscale score
Stimulation elicited positive visual fixation
MCS1
male
trauma
2
6
0
none
MCS2
male
trauma
6
6
1
none
MCS3
male
Non-trauma
12
8
1
none
MCS4*
male
trauma
3
5
3
mirror
MCS5*
male
trauma
4
5
3
mirror
MCS6*
female
trauma
1
6
3
mirror
MCS7*
male
Non-trauma
2
7
3
mirror
MCS8*
female
trauma
5
7
3
mirror
MCS9*
male
trauma
5
7
3
mirror
MCS10*
male
trauma
4
7
3
mirror
MCS11*
female
trauma
9
8
3
mirror
MCS12*
male
Non-trauma
119
8
3
mirror
MCS13*
male
trauma
33
9
3
mirror
MCS14*
male
trauma
3
9
3
mirror
MCS15*
male
trauma
7
10
4
mirror
MCS16*
male
trauma
4
13
3
mirror
MCS17*
male
trauma
7
16
4
mirror
MCS18
male
trauma
5
7
2
ball
MCS19
male
trauma
8
6
3
Mirror and ball
MCS20
male
trauma
9
7
3
Mirror and ball
MCS21
male
trauma
5
7
3
Mirror and ball
MCS22
male
trauma
27
8
3
Mirror and ball
MCS23
female
trauma
4
10
2
Mirror and ball
MCS24
male
Non-trauma
1
10
3
Mirror and light
MCS25
male
trauma
8
11
3
Mirror and light
MCS26
male
trauma
10
15
5
Mirror and light
MCS27
male
trauma
2
6
3
All three
MCS28
male
Non-trauma
2
6
4
All three
MCS29
male
trauma
3
7
3
All three
MCS30
female
trauma
37
8
3
All three
MCS31
male
Non-trauma
11
8
3
All three
MCS32
male
Non-trauma
2
8
2
All three
MCS33
male
trauma
12
8
3
All three
MCS34
female
Non-trauma
4
9
3
All three
MCS35
male
trauma
47
9
3
All three
MCS36
male
Non-trauma
31
9
3
All three
MCS37
female
trauma
12
10
3
All three
MCS38
female
Non-trauma
3
10
3
All three
MCS39
male
trauma
3
10
4
All three
MCS40
male
trauma
4
12
3
All three
MCS41
male
Non-trauma
12
13
4
All three
MCS42
male
trauma
6
15
4
All three
MCS43
male
Non-trauma
30
16
4
All three
VS1
male
trauma
5
1
0
none
VS2
female
Non-trauma
5
2
0
none
VS3
male
Non-trauma
11
2
0
none
VS4
male
trauma
3
2
0
none
VS5
male
trauma
9
2
0
none
VS6
male
trauma
1
3
0
none
VS7
male
Non-trauma
1
3
0
none
VS8
male
Non-trauma
5
3
0
none
VS9
female
trauma
1
3
0
none
VS10
male
trauma
6
3
0
none
VS11
male
trauma
7
3
1
none
VS12
male
trauma
3
4
1
none
VS13
male
trauma
8
4
0
none
VS14
male
trauma
4
4
1
none
VS15
male
trauma
8
4
0
none
VS16
male
trauma
6
4
1
none
VS17
male
trauma
3
4
1
none
VS18
male
Non-trauma
10
5
0
none
VS19
male
trauma
14
5
0
none
VS20
male
Non-trauma
3
5
0
none
VS21
male
trauma
5
5
0
none
VS22
male
trauma
9
5
0
none
VS23
male
trauma
8
5
1
none
VS24
male
trauma
4
6
0
none
VS25
male
trauma
3
6
0
none
VS26
male
Non-trauma
5
6
1
none
VS27
male
trauma
17
6
0
none
VS28
male
Non-trauma
5
6
1
none
VS29
male
Non-trauma
9
6
0
none
VS30
male
trauma
3
6
1
none
VS31
female
trauma
3
6
0
none
VS32
male
trauma
4
6
0
none
VS33
male
trauma
5
6
0
none
VS34
male
trauma
2
7
1
none
VS35
male
Non-trauma
2
7
0
none
VS36
male
trauma
12
7
1
none
VS37
female
trauma
5
7
1
none
VS38
male
trauma
5
7
1
none
*MCS patients who only showed visual fixation to a mirror but not to a ball and a light.
**Time since injury in months.
Fourteen patients (MCS 4 to 17 labeled with “*” in Table 1) showed visual pursuit but no fixation when assessed with only a bright colored or illuminated object as stated in the CRS-R operation. However, they showed visual fixation when a mirror was presented.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to determine whether the assessment of visual fixation in patients recovering from coma is influenced by the choice of visual stimuli. We used a mirror, a colored ball and a light to assess visual fixation and compared the occurrences elicited by different stimuli. Our results showed that indeed the frequency of visual fixation in patients with DOC is related to the visual stimulus used. MCS patients tended to fixate significantly more on their own reflection (48%) as compared to fixation on a brightly colored (28%) and illuminated object (25%). As mentioned by Vanhaudenhuyse et al. [6], auto referential stimuli capture our attention and give rise to a sense of self-awareness in everyday social interactions. This is reflected in the cocktail party phenomenon, which refers to the fact that one’s own name can easily catch his/her attention in a cacophony of conversations and background noise [8]. Similarly, one’s own name induced a larger response in patients with DOC as compared with neutral auditory stimuli [9]. Seeing one’s own face also has similarly strong attention grabbing properties [10]. Functional imaging has previously shown activation of anterior and posterior midline structures (i.e., mesiofrontal and precuneal cortices) during presentation of one’s own face in healthy volunteers [11]. Interestingly, these areas are amidst the most metabolically impaired in patients in a vegetative state [12], possibly explaining why they cannot visual follow or fixate.
The occurrence of visual fixation seems to be related to the patient’s overall behavioral profile. VS/UWS patients showed no visual fixation, the more stimuli the patient showed fixation to the higher the CRS-R score was (i.e., fixation to 0, 1, 2 and 3 stimuli, obtained a score of 4.8, 8.3, 9.3 and 9.6, respectively). Due to clinical limitations we could not use an eye-tracker, to more objectively measure fixation. However, our results overlap with a study that used an eye tracking machine to assess visual tracking behavior and was able to differentiate MCS from VS/UWS [13]. In our group, only three out of 43 patients in MCS failed to show visual fixation. Neurological assessment showed that one of these 3 patients failed to eye blink to threat, indicating impaired brainstem reflexes. The remaining two patients had intact brainstem reflexes and reproducible but inconsistent command following, which could be explained by a visual impairment [14].
Our results showed that except one, all of the patients who responded to the ball and light also responded to the mirror. The one patient that showed visual fixation to the ball but not to the mirror or light. In this case, presentation of the mirror was the last stimulus and hence the fluctuating levels of arousal, generally observed in MCS, might account for the fixation on a ball in the absence of fixation on the mirror. One could argue that the order of presentation could have impacted the level of response of our patients. However, the stimuli were presented in a randomized order and not in a fixed order; suggesting that the high rate of response observed using the mirror cannot be explained by the order of presentation.
According to current guidelines for behavioral assessment, visual fixation differentiates unresponsive from minimally responsive [2]. The assessment of visual fixation according to these guidelines (e.g., use an object), failed to show fixating behavior in many of our subjects, while higher cognitive functions existed. This is inconsistent with the structuring principle of CRS-R. However, this phenomenon disappears when a mirror is used for the assessment of visual fixation. Therefore, using a mirror for visual fixation is sensitive and accurate in the differentiation between MCS and VS/UWS. These results are in line with earlier work emphasizing the use of a mirror in the evaluation of visual following [6]. Thus, we advise to use a mirror for the assessment of visual fixation, especially when visual following is observed using other stimuli. The clinical implications of our findings are important. More than 35% of the MCS patients with visual fixation only fixate on a mirror (and not on other objects). Hence, these patients would be misdiagnosed as being unresponsive when other sensory modalities fail to elicit a behavioral response. Our findings emphasize the importance of using a mirror when evaluating visual fixation in post-comatose states.
Although the use of a mirror is a strong and sensitive stimulus to elicit visual responses due to its self-referential value, we have not tested all possible stimuli as advised in other existing scales. As the CRS-R is currently considered as the gold standard in the assessment of disorders of consciousness [4], we chose the visual stimuli described in the guidelines (i.e., a colored ball or a bright light). We here advise to use a mirror in the assessment of visual fixation; however we cannot make any conclusions about the sensitivity and accuracy of other stimuli, like those mentioned in other existing scales. Future research should therefore focus on including more visual stimuli. Besides, in our study, we did not have the opportunity to collect follow-up data. Nevertheless, this could be done in the future. Indeed, as considering visual fixation as a sign of consciousness has been previously debated [15], future studies should investigate if patients showing visual fixation (detected using a mirror) present a more frequently full recovery of consciousness as compared to patients who do not show such behavior.
One could argue that using an eye-recorder could standardize the way in which the data are collected. However, the use of such method is difficult in our population. Patients recovering from coma often have brainstem lesions or ocular trauma that may affect eye-movements and complicate the use of an automated recording (often validated in healthy volunteers).

Conclusions

This study emphasizes the use of a mirror during the assessment of visual fixation, as shown by the higher positive response rate of the mirror, compared to other stimuli in eliciting a visual fixating response, adding to previous studies the importance of using auto-referential stimuli in patients with disorders of consciousness (i.e., the use of a mirror in the assessment of visual tracking and one’s own name in the assessment of localization to sound [6, 9]).

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (KG08027,KG13007), the Science and Technology Department of Zhejiang Province (KZ09037), the Hangzhou Normal University (JTAS2011-01-016), the Belgian Funds for Scientific Research (FNRS), Fonds Léon Frédericq, and the University of Liège. The authors thank Cunlai Xu, Jian Gao, Kehong Liu, Kun Li,Yu Zhang, Hongyan Song and Yan Dong for their assistance in patients’ assessment, and Didier Ledoux for his statistical advices. LH and SW are doctoral researchers and SL is research director at FNRS.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interest.

Authors’ contributions

YN, XH, YT, WH and DY collected data and managed the patients; HD and YN performed data analyses; SL and HD designed the study; YN, LH, SL and HD wrote the paper. SW, CS, AT and MH revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. HD and YN contributed equally to the study. All authors discussed the results and commented on the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, Leon-Carrion J, Sannita WG, Sazbon L, Schmutzhard E, von Wild KR, Zeman A, Dolce G: Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med. 2010, 8: 68-10.1186/1741-7015-8-68.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, Leon-Carrion J, Sannita WG, Sazbon L, Schmutzhard E, von Wild KR, Zeman A, Dolce G: Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med. 2010, 8: 68-10.1186/1741-7015-8-68.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz DI, Kelly JP, Rosenberg JH, Whyte J, Zafonte RD, Zasler ND: The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 2002, 58 (3): 349-353. 10.1212/WNL.58.3.349.CrossRefPubMed Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz DI, Kelly JP, Rosenberg JH, Whyte J, Zafonte RD, Zasler ND: The minimally conscious state: definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 2002, 58 (3): 349-353. 10.1212/WNL.58.3.349.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Majerus S, Gill-Thwaites H, Andrews K, Laureys S: Behavioral evaluation of consciousness in severe brain damage. Prog Brain Res. 2005, 150: 397-413.CrossRefPubMed Majerus S, Gill-Thwaites H, Andrews K, Laureys S: Behavioral evaluation of consciousness in severe brain damage. Prog Brain Res. 2005, 150: 397-413.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Seel RT, Sherer M, Whyte J, Katz DI, Giacino JT, Rosenbaum AM, Hammond FM, Kalmar K, Pape TL, Zafonte R, Biester RC, Kaelin D, Kean J, Zasler N: Assessment scales for disorders of consciousness: evidence-based recommendations for clinical practice and research. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2010, 91 (12): 1795-1813. 10.1016/j.apmr.2010.07.218.CrossRefPubMed Seel RT, Sherer M, Whyte J, Katz DI, Giacino JT, Rosenbaum AM, Hammond FM, Kalmar K, Pape TL, Zafonte R, Biester RC, Kaelin D, Kean J, Zasler N: Assessment scales for disorders of consciousness: evidence-based recommendations for clinical practice and research. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2010, 91 (12): 1795-1813. 10.1016/j.apmr.2010.07.218.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Trojano L, Moretta P, Loreto V, Santoro L, Estraneo A: Affective saliency modifies visual tracking behavior in disorders of consciousness: a quantitative analysis. J Neurol. 2013, 260 (1): 306-308. 10.1007/s00415-012-6717-x.CrossRefPubMed Trojano L, Moretta P, Loreto V, Santoro L, Estraneo A: Affective saliency modifies visual tracking behavior in disorders of consciousness: a quantitative analysis. J Neurol. 2013, 260 (1): 306-308. 10.1007/s00415-012-6717-x.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers C, Bredart S, Laureys S: Assessment of visual pursuit in post-comatose states: use a mirror. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008, 79 (2): 223-10.1136/jnnp.2007.121624.CrossRefPubMed Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers C, Bredart S, Laureys S: Assessment of visual pursuit in post-comatose states: use a mirror. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008, 79 (2): 223-10.1136/jnnp.2007.121624.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J: The JFK Coma Recovery Scale-Revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2004, 85 (12): 2020-2029. 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.02.033.CrossRefPubMed Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J: The JFK Coma Recovery Scale-Revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2004, 85 (12): 2020-2029. 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.02.033.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat McDermott JH: The cocktail party problem. Curr Biol. 2009, 19 (22): R1024-R1027. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.09.005.CrossRefPubMed McDermott JH: The cocktail party problem. Curr Biol. 2009, 19 (22): R1024-R1027. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.09.005.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Cheng L, Gosseries O, Ying L, Hu X, Yu D, Gao H, He M, Schnakers C, Laureys S, Di H: Assessment of localisation to auditory stimulation in post-comatose states: use the patient’s own name. BMC Neurol. 2013, 13: 27-10.1186/1471-2377-13-27.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cheng L, Gosseries O, Ying L, Hu X, Yu D, Gao H, He M, Schnakers C, Laureys S, Di H: Assessment of localisation to auditory stimulation in post-comatose states: use the patient’s own name. BMC Neurol. 2013, 13: 27-10.1186/1471-2377-13-27.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Bredart S, Delchambre M, Laureys S: One’s own face is hard to ignore. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2006, 59 (1): 46-52. 10.1080/17470210500343678.CrossRef Bredart S, Delchambre M, Laureys S: One’s own face is hard to ignore. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2006, 59 (1): 46-52. 10.1080/17470210500343678.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Laureys S, Perrin F, Bredart S: Self-consciousness in non-communicative patients. Conscious Cogn. 2007, 16 (3): 722-741. 10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.004. discussion 742–725CrossRefPubMed Laureys S, Perrin F, Bredart S: Self-consciousness in non-communicative patients. Conscious Cogn. 2007, 16 (3): 722-741. 10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.004. discussion 742–725CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Laureys S, Owen AM, Schiff ND: Brain function in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders. Lancet Neurol. 2004, 3 (9): 537-546. 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00852-X.CrossRefPubMed Laureys S, Owen AM, Schiff ND: Brain function in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders. Lancet Neurol. 2004, 3 (9): 537-546. 10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00852-X.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Trojano L, Moretta P, Loreto V, Cozzolino A, Santoro L, Estraneo A: Quantitative assessment of visual behavior in disorders of consciousness. J Neurol. 2012, 259 (9): 1888-1895. 10.1007/s00415-012-6435-4.CrossRefPubMed Trojano L, Moretta P, Loreto V, Cozzolino A, Santoro L, Estraneo A: Quantitative assessment of visual behavior in disorders of consciousness. J Neurol. 2012, 259 (9): 1888-1895. 10.1007/s00415-012-6435-4.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Andrews K, Murphy L, Munday R, Littlewood C: Misdiagnosis of the vegetative state: retrospective study in a rehabilitation unit. BMJ. 1996, 313 (7048): 13-16. 10.1136/bmj.313.7048.13.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Andrews K, Murphy L, Munday R, Littlewood C: Misdiagnosis of the vegetative state: retrospective study in a rehabilitation unit. BMJ. 1996, 313 (7048): 13-16. 10.1136/bmj.313.7048.13.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Bruno MA, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers C, Boly M, Gosseries O, Demertzi A, Majerus S, Moonen G, Hustinx R, Laureys S: Visual fixation in the vegetative state: an observational case series PET study. BMC Neurol. 2010, 10: 35-10.1186/1471-2377-10-35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bruno MA, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers C, Boly M, Gosseries O, Demertzi A, Majerus S, Moonen G, Hustinx R, Laureys S: Visual fixation in the vegetative state: an observational case series PET study. BMC Neurol. 2010, 10: 35-10.1186/1471-2377-10-35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Assessment of visual fixation in vegetative and minimally conscious states
verfasst von
Haibo Di
Yunzhi Nie
Xiaohua Hu
Yong Tong
Lizette Heine
Sarah Wannez
Wangshan Huang
Dan Yu
Minhui He
Aurore Thibaut
Caroline Schnakers
Steven Laureys
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2014
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Neurology / Ausgabe 1/2014
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2377
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2377-14-147

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2014

BMC Neurology 1/2014 Zur Ausgabe