Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2021

Open Access 01.12.2021 | Research

Narcissistic traits in young people and how experiencing shame relates to current attachment challenges

verfasst von: Charlotte C. van Schie, Heidi L. Jarman, Samantha Reis, Brin F. S. Grenyer

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2021

Abstract

Background

Young people with pathological narcissistic traits may have more maladaptive ways of relating to themselves and others. In this study, we investigated how the experience of shame may be a mechanism by which vulnerable and grandiose pathological narcissism relates to negative and positive internalised models of the self and others, manifested as attachment styles.

Methods

Participants (N = 348) were young people who reported on pathological narcissism, the experience of shame and their model of self and others (secure, dismissive, preoccupied and fearful attachment). Mediation of the experience of shame between vulnerable and grandiose narcissism on the one hand and secure, dismissive, preoccupied and fearful attachment on the other hand, was tested using a path model.

Results

Shame mediated the relationship between vulnerable narcissism and a more negative model of others and self (i.e. less secure, more fearful and more preoccupied in attachment). Higher grandiose narcissism traits were related to a more positive model of others and self (i.e. more secure attachment) and were unrelated to the experience of shame.

Conclusions

Young people with vulnerable narcissism traits tended to report more shame, and struggled to be close to others. It may be that shame experiences highlight a discrepancy between the ideal and actual self that may contribute to a more insecure attachment style. A good working alliance and fostering self-compassion may counter some negative effects of shame in those most vulnerable, but dismissal in those most grandiose presents a clinical conundrum requiring further research.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12888-021-03249-4.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Global estimates are that personality disorder affects around 7.8% of the population [1] and poses a high burden to both the person affected and to people around them [2, 3]. Psychological treatment for borderline personality disorder shows some efficacy [4], yet there are few clinical trials of treatment for narcissistic personality disorder (NPD) [5]. Research on the clinical implications of pathological narcissism are in their infancy despite the long history of the disorder [6]. There is an urgent need to further understand where pathological narcissism may set up a cascade of self and interpersonal difficulties that may guide future therapies, particularly in youth [7, 8].
Pathological narcissism is understood to have a core of entitlement and self-importance and has vulnerable traits such as contingent self-esteem and grandiose traits such as exploitativeness [911]. Certain narcissism traits may be adaptive, even important, for going through the transition phase of adolescence and emerging adulthood [12]. There appears to be a normative increase in narcissism traits in late adolescence [13, 14]. Feelings of omnipotence may then be conducive to personal growth in the process of separating from parents and the individuation of self [12, 15]. However, it may also be particularly in this phase of life that narcissism traits become pathological and in a more extreme form may develop into NPD [1618]. Pathological narcissism may be observed as young people struggling to build genuine relationships and rigidly holding on to an extreme ideal self [16]. In normal development, the ideal self matches the young person’s talents and characteristics, is flexible and amenable to change, and may provide a sense of agency [16]. An ideal grandiose self, however, may instead act as a constant threat that one cannot measure up to [16].
These positive yet fragile selves may have their origin in childhood with overprotective and overvaluing parenting relating to more grandiose and vulnerable narcissism traits [19, 20]. This parenting environment may lead to young people being dependent on others for feedback and guidance in order to sustain a positive self-view [2124]. At the same time, people higher in narcissistic traits may be quite reactive to social interactions [25]. In seeking affirmation from others of their positive yet fragile selves [9, 2629], people high in narcissism may impair the very relationships they need by prioritising self-affirmation over being liked [6, 23]. It is important to understand how these difficulties in self and other functioning in young people with narcissism traits come about to inform treatment and prevention.
One reason that people high in narcissism traits react strongly to interpersonal interactions is that these may reveal a gap between the ideal self and the actual self. This discrepancy becomes more painful as the gap between the actual and ideal self becomes larger e.g. when the ideal self is grandiose [30]. Feelings of shame may highlight this discrepancy between the ideal and actual self [16, 30, 31]. Shame is seen as a central aspect of pathological narcissism that has been implicated in its development, functioning and treatment [26, 30, 32, 33] and is a common experience among people with NPD [34]. Shame is a self-conscious emotion that brings awareness to undesirable aspects of the self that are seen as stable over time and global across situations [3538]. This is different from other self-conscious emotions such as guilt which is tied to a specific negative behaviour and does not encompass the whole self [39]. Moreover, shame can disrupt the functioning of the self whereby a grandiose self and more negative characteristics are not well integrated resulting in a more rigid and fragile unrealistic self [30].
At the same time, shame is an interpersonal emotion that can disrupt social interactions [36, 37, 40]. Where guilt may motivate behaviour to repair relationships, shame seems to motivate withdrawal from others [39, 41]. Although, shame may also motivate mending behaviour [42, 43], it is when people have difficulty with self-regulation or are in a situation where the self is threatened, that shame may lead to withdrawal or even aggressive behaviour [36, 42, 44, 45]. For example, one study showed that children higher in narcissism responded with more aggression after their failure in performing was shamed [46]. The experience of shame may thus be a candidate mechanism by which young people with higher narcissism traits may experience disturbances in the self and difficulties in relating to others.
Theories suggest that shame is a core affect of vulnerable narcissism [47]. Research supports a relation between vulnerable narcissism and the experience of shame [29, 4850]. Shame may be experienced when one receives negative feedback that feels undeserved [50]. Receiving positive feedback may also relate to experiencing shame when it does not match with the individuals own perception [50, 51]. Interestingly, this may support the notion that it is not so much the valence of feedback (i.e. negative or positive) that elicits shame but whether a discrepancy between and ideal and actual self is experienced. Facets of vulnerable narcissism that have previously been related to shame are relying on others for feedback (i.e., contingent self-esteem) and devaluing others when they do not provide recognition, whereas entitlement rage may be related to experiencing less shame [49]. It has been proposed that grandiose narcissism may be related to the denial of shame [47] with some studies finding a negative relationship between shame and grandiose narcissism [35, 52] and higher levels of implicit shame in people with NPD compared to people with BPD [34]. However, other studies have not found an association with shame [48] or have found increased experiences of shame specifically in relation to grandiose fantasies [29, 49] i.e., feeling shame for having grandiose needs and ambitions [47]. Thus, research on the association between grandiose narcissism and shame is far from conclusive.
In the context of narcissism research, attachment theory can provide a useful framework to conceptualise problems relating to the self and interpersonal functioning. An examination of current attachment style can indicate how someone tends to see themselves and relate to others, and how this may influence functioning in later in life [53]. Different combinations of negative and positive models of self and of others generate four adult attachment styles i.e., secure, preoccupied, fearful and dismissive attachment, that can be dimensionally rated as more or less applicable to the individual [5355]. Individuals manifesting a predominantly secure attachment style tend to see themselves as worthy of love and others as capable of fulfilling their needs in relationships [53]. Within the therapeutic context, attachment security has been shown to facilitate a positive treatment outcome [56]. The preoccupied attachment style is comprised of a negative model of self and a positive model of others, such that individuals manifesting a predominantly preoccupied style see themselves as unworthy of love, and are dependent on valued others to compensate for this lack of self-worth [53]. The fearful avoidant attachment style also features a negative model of self, but alongside a negative model of others [53]. It is thought that this distinct negative self-negative other profile creates conflict for the individual – whilst they desire relationships to compensate for chronically low self-worth, their belief that others cannot be trusted, leads to avoidance of relationships out of fear of being rejected. Fearful attachment, like the preoccupied style, has also been linked to poorer outcome from psychotherapy (e.g. [57]). Finally, the dismissive attachment style features a positive self model combined with a negative model of others. People with this style typically view others as untrustworthy or unable to meet their needs.
It has been observed that people with NPD hold both positive and negative models of themselves and tend to distrust others [9, 26, 27, 58]. A negative model of self may be defended against in unhelpful ways (e.g. fantasy, avoidance, aggression [33]) resulting in disturbed relatedness to others. Research indicates that vulnerable narcissism is related to a preoccupation with relationships for fear of being rejected [27, 5963]. Grandiose narcissism has been linked to more secure and more dismissive attachment [64, 65]. However, here again, findings regarding grandiose narcissism and attachment have been less consistent with some studies finding no relation to attachment (e.g. [59, 62]). Inconsistencies in findings for grandiose narcissism with respect to shame as well as attachment may result from differences in how grandiose narcissism is operationalised e.g., as adaptive and agentic or maladaptive and antagonistic [16, 66]. Findings may also differ depending on whether the association between shame and grandiose narcissism is considered alongside vulnerable narcissism as overlap between grandiose and vulnerable narcissism has been observed both in clinical practice and in research [6, 67, 68]. Therefore, investigating vulnerable and grandiose narcissism simultaneously using multivariate models can account for their shared variance and allows one to discern the unique associations of each [69].
Furthermore, it should be noted that the way people relate to self and others may be in part traced back to early interactions with attachment figs [53, 70, 71].. These early ways of relating to self and others become internalised models of attachment that remain observable in adolescence and adulthood [53, 70, 71]. Similarly, narcissism traits and shame experiences may have their origin in early life [30, 72]. It is likely that a complex interplay between narcissism traits, shame experiences and attachment styles exists throughout life. However, from a therapeutic perspective, we seek to investigate the mechanism by which young people presenting with pathological narcissistic traits may experience intrapersonal and interpersonal difficulties. Therefore, this study seeks to examine whether shame is a mediator by which pathological grandiose and vulnerable narcissism relates to more negative models of self and others (expressed as current attachment style). By studying young people (i.e., late adolescence and emerging adulthood), we aim to capture a period within which pathological narcissism traits may be particularly relevant, given the high incidence of emerging mental health problems and opportunities for early intervention, coinciding with the formation of adult attachment styles and associated personality vulnerabilities [7, 12, 7375].
We hypothesize that people higher in pathological narcissism, particularly vulnerable narcissism, will have greater experiences of shame and thereby have a negative model of self and others (preoccupied and/or fearful attachment). To further shed light on the associations with grandiose narcissism, particularly in young people, we will test the same associations and mediation for grandiose narcissism as for vulnerable narcissism. However, as previous findings on grandiose narcissism are mixed, the expectations of which associations should arise are less clear. As described above, we distinguish between grandiose and vulnerable pathological narcissism, and simultaneously assess their associations in a multivariate model to account for their shared variance. Finally, previous research indicate that certain facets of narcissism are specifically related to shame (e.g., contingent self-esteem, devaluing, grandiose fantasies). We will therefore explore in additional analyses how the different facets of narcissism (irrespective of grandiose or vulnerable narcissism) relate to shame and attachment experiences.

Materials and methods

Participants and procedure

Participants (N = 348, 78% women) in this study were young people (M = 19.27 years (SD = 1.61, Range = 17–25)), see Table 1. We targeted persons in late adolescence (16–18 years) and emerging adulthood (18–25 years) to recruit our sample and used the definition of young people for this age period [7, 17, 73, 76]. A snowball method of recruitment was used, where notices for the study were provided to young people who had finished high school and were contemplating study at university. Those participating were further encouraged to let others know about the study. Most participants were born in Australia (N = 310, 90%) and came from a family where parents were not divorced, separated or widowed (N = 267, 77%). Participants reported an average level but broad range of trait self-esteem (M = 30.33, SD = 5.83, Range = 10–40), comparable to those found in other general population samples [77]. Some participants reported having been diagnosed in their life with a mental health condition (N = 38, 11%) with depression and anxiety as the most commonly reported diagnoses.
Table 1
Demographic information on the sample (N = 348)
Demographic
N (%)/M (SD)
Gender
 - Female
270 (77.6%)
 - Male
78 (22.4%)
Age
M = 19.27 (SD = 1.61)
Education
 - Completed high school
337 (96.8%)
 - Completed Vocational college or training
11 (3.2%)
Marital status participant
 - Never married
325 (93.4%)
 - Married
2 (0.6%)
 - Widowed
1 (0.3%)
 - Divorced or separated
20 (5.7%)
 - Living together
0 (0%)
Family situation parents
- Separated
12 (3.4%)
 - Divorced
57 (16.4%)
 - Widowed
12 (3.4%)
 - Not separated, divorced or widowed
267 (76.7%)
Lifetime diagnosis
38 (10.9%)
Trait self-esteem
M = 30.33 (SD = 5.83)
The study received ethical approval from the Institutional Review Board of the University of Wollongong, Australia (HE10/370). Participants provided informed consent prior to participating. Participants volunteered their time (approx. 30 min) to participate in the study. Participants completed an online assessment module via a secure website. The data were checked for flat responses resulting in the exclusion of one participant resulting in the sample described above (N = 348). Part of this sample has been described in a separate paper [19].

Measures

Pathological narcissism inventory

Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism traits were measured using the Pathological Narcissism Inventory [PNI; 29]. The PNI contains 52-items that are rated on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from not at all like me (0) to very much like me (5). Psychometric qualities of this instrument have been established [10, 29]. Grandiose narcissism was calculated as the mean score of the items of three subscales (Grandiose Fantasy (α = 0.86), Exploitativeness (α = 0.76) and Self-Sacrificing Self-Enhancement (α = 0.77)). Vulnerable narcissism was calculated as the mean score of the items of four subscales (Contingent Self-Esteem (α = 0.92), Hiding the Self (α = 0.81), Devaluing (α = 0.85) and Entitlement Rage (α = 0.85)) [10]. Grandiose (α = 0.87) and vulnerable (α = 0.94) narcissm showed good internal consistency in this study. For means, standard deviations and correlations, see Table 2.
Table 2
Means, standard deviations, and correlations with confidence intervals
Variable
M
SD
1
2
3
4
5
6
1. Grandiose narcissism
2.77
0.76
      
2. Vulnerable narcissism
2.22
0.82
.65**
     
   
[.59, .71]
     
3. Secure attachment
55.57
25.29
−.09
−.36**
    
   
[−.19, .02]
[−.44, −.26]
    
4. Dismissive-avoidant attachment
53.22
25.42
.09
.06
−.03
   
   
[−.01, .20]
[−.05, .16]
[−.13, .08]
   
5. Preoccupied attachment
37.39
26.16
.25**
.34**
−.06
−.12*
  
   
[.15, .35]
[.24, .43]
[−.17, .04]
[−.23, −.02]
  
6. Fearful-avoidant attachment
49.14
30.48
.17**
.37**
−.48**
.16**
.23**
 
   
[.06, .27]
[.27, .46]
[−.56, −.40]
[.06, .26]
[.13, .33]
 
7. Experience of shame
2.23
0.60
.30**
.50**
−.32**
.02
.29**
.34**
   
[.20, .40]
[.41, .58]
[−.41, −.21]
[−.09, .13]
[.19, .39]
[.24, .43]
Note. M and SD are used to represent mean and standard deviation, respectively. Values in square brackets indicate the 95% confidence interval
* p-values are denoted as follows: * indicates p < .05. ** indicates p < .01

Relationship questionnaire

The Relationship Questionnaire (RQ) [53] asked participants to rate four paragraphs that describe secure, fearful, preoccupied and dismissive attachment style on a scale of not at all like me (0) to very much like me (100). The RQ was treated dimensionally producing a score for each of the four styles per participant. The four attachment styles represent different combinations of positive and negative model of self and others. A negative model of self is present in fearful and preoccupied attachment. A negative model of other is present in dismissive and fearful attachment. Secure attachment is characterized as a positive model of self and other. The RQ is widely used and has been validated against other attachment measures and differentially predicts treatment outcomes (e.g. [57]).

Experience of shame scale

The Experience of Shame Scale (ESS) assessed shame in three domains, i.e. experiencing shame about one’s character, behaviour and body [78]. Each of the domains is measured on a feeling, cognitive (concern for others opinion) and behavioural (avoidance) component. The ESS contains 25 items rating experiences in the past year on a 4-point scale ranging from not at all (1) to very much (4). The total score showed good internal consistency (α = 0.95).

Statistical analysis

We conducted a mediation analysis to test whether the experience of shame mediates the relation between grandiose and vulnerable narcissism on the one hand and the four attachment styles on the other hand. We used the Lavaan package (version 0.6–3) in R (3.6.0) [79] to define a path analysis model where the direct effect of vulnerable and grandiose narcissism on the four attachment styles was simultaneously assessed, as well as the mediating (indirect) effect via the experience of shame. In this multivariate model, grandiose and vulnerable narcissism were allowed to correlate to take their shared variance into account [69]. The total effect was defined as the sum of the direct and indirect effect of vulnerable and grandiose narcissism on attachment styles. For inference on the parameter estimates we used the maximum likelihood estimation with bootstrapped standard errors (5000 bootstraps) [80]. There were missing data for 38 participants on the experience of shame scale, therefore the case-wise maximum likelihood estimation was used [79, 81]. All variables were standardized before analysis. Simulation studies have shown that a minimum sample size of N = 300 is sufficient to estimate small, medium and large indirect effects in a path analysis model with a power level of > 0.80 [82].
We performed an exploratory analysis where we repeated the mediation analysis as described above but replaced grandiose and vulnerable narcissism with the facet level subscales of narcissism traits. All seven PNI subscales were entered simultaneously as predictors.

Results

Pathological narcissism and attachment

Vulnerable narcissism is related to less positive and more negative models of self, i.e., a less secure (b = − 0.43, SE = 0.07, p < .001), more preoccupied (b = 0.26, SE = 0.08, p = .002) and more fearful attachment (b = 0.33, SE = 0.07, p < .001). Grandiose narcissism was related to more secure attachment (b = 0.24, SE = 0.06, p < .001). There was no relation between grandiose narcissism and dismissive attachment (b = 0.13, SE = 0.07, p = .069). For all parameter estimates, see Supplementary Table 1.

Pathological narcissism and shame

Vulnerable narcissism was related to greater experiences of shame (b = 0.57, SE = 0.07, p < .001). Grandiose narcissism was not related to the experience of shame (b = − 0.07, SE = 0.07, p = .332).

Shame as mediator for vulnerable narcissism

The experience of shame was related to less positive and more negative models of self, i.e. a less secure (b = − 0.15, SE = 0.06, p = .020), more preoccupied (b = 0.15, SE = 0.06, p = .016) and more fearful attachment (b = 0.19, SE = 0.06, p = .001), see Fig. 1 and Supplementary Table 1.
Shame mediated the relation between vulnerable narcissism and more negative models of self, i.e. fearful attachment (negative model of self and others) (b = 0.110, SE = 0.04, p = .003), preoccupied attachment (b = 0.09, SE = 0.04, p = .024), and (less) secure attachment (b = − 0.09, SE = 0.04, p = .022), see Fig. 1 and Supplementary Table 1.

Grandiose narcissism

Shame did not mediate the relation between grandiose narcissism and any form of attachment, see Supplementary Table 1.

Exploratory analysis of mediation at facet level

The exploratory analysis revealed that mainly contingent self-esteem was related to insecure attachment and shame, see Supplementary Table 2. Shame mediated between contingent self-esteem and fearful attachment (b = 0.11, SE = 0.04, p = .008).

Discussion

People with more pathological narcissistic traits may have more maladaptive ways of relating to themselves and others. In this study, we found support for the hypothesis that the experience of shame acts a mechanism by which pathological narcissism traits (specifically vulnerable narcissism) relate to more negative models of self and more difficulty relating to others (i.e. less secure, more preoccupied and more fearful self-reported attachment).
Young people higher in vulnerable narcissism traits reported a need for being close to others but also a difficulty trusting others. Exploratory analyses indicated that three facets of vulnerable narcissism, namely contingent self-esteem, hiding the self and devaluing self and others, were mainly related to self and interpersonal difficulties. Previous studies in adults have indicated that vulnerable narcissism relates to more maladaptive interpersonal styles [5962, 64]. Studies in adults and adolescents with NPD indicate that these interpersonal styles may not so much consist of avoidance but more of anxiety and preoccupation with relationships while also being dismissive of others [63, 65, 71]. Studies in adolescents show that vulnerable narcissism is associated with higher pro-active aggression and social stress [7] and that peers perceive adolescents higher in narcissism as more antagonistic [83]. It may be that young people express the need to be close to others, while perceiving others as not willing or able to reciprocate, in an antagonistic manner.
We further found that vulnerable narcissism, particularly contingent self-esteem (i.e., being dependent on others for self-esteem), is related to more experiences of shame. Although increases in shame experiences are in part normative in adolescence and emerging adulthood [84], it appears that young people with narcissism traits experience shame to a greater degree. In line with our finding, a recent study in (emerging) adults indicated that vulnerable narcissism, particularly contingent self-esteem, was related to experiences of shame on a day-to-day basis [49]. Moreover, our finding is in line with the theory that people higher in narcissism have developed a weaker sense of self that relies more heavily on how other people view them (‘objective self’) at the cost of relying less on inner experiences (‘subjective self’) [25, 30, 85]. However, a weak sense of self (i.e. a less integrated self) may relate to people being more prone to experiencing shame [86], as we have observed in this study. Shame highlights undesirable, stable and global attributes of the self [37, 38]. Since shame involves evaluating the self as a whole more negatively, rather than focussing on specific aspects of the self (e.g., feeling guilty about a certain behaviour) [38, 87], the experience of shame can highlight a discrepancy between the ideal and actual self [84]. This process may contribute to developing a more negative model of self by inhibiting the integration and strengthening of the self [33, 88].
Indeed, our findings indicate that shame mediated the relation between vulnerable narcissism traits and preoccupied, fearful and less secure attachment (i.e., difficulty relating to self and others). In other words, more negative models of self can be partially explained by experiencing shame. Moreover, shame may give rise to more interpersonal difficulties such as clinging to others but also disengaging from others [30, 36, 8991]. Research indicates that for adolescents, shame may be related to less pro-social behaviours and is even causally related to greater hostility [92, 93]. More aggression has also been observed in children with narcissistic traits who were shamed [46]. Although, further research should further clarify the direction of effects, it could be that young people who rely more on others for their self-worth may be more prone to experiencing shame. Young people responding to shame in a hostile and antagonistic manner may further impede self and interpersonal functioning.
Young people higher in grandiose narcissism self-reported a more positive model of self and others (i.e. more secure), consistent with previous research in adults [61, 63, 64]. Although previous research has observed a relation between grandiose narcissism and dismissive attachment, in this study we did not observe this relation [64, 65]. Moreover, there was no relation between grandiose narcissism and the experience of shame. As we analysed grandiose narcissism simultaneously with vulnerable narcissism in a multivariate model, it could be that we did not observe a unique relationship between grandiose narcissism and more maladaptive attachment or shame. Different configurations of the narcissism facets that compose grandiose and vulnerable narcissism have been proposed [67]. However, our exploratory analysis on the facet level indicates that facets typically associated with grandiose narcissism did not relate to shame or attachment, except for self-sacrificing self-enhancement which related to more secure and less fearful attachment.
It could be that more maladaptive grandiose traits were not captured as our sample of young people was relatively healthy or because certain narcissism traits, such as the idea of omnipotence, could be adaptive in adolescence and emerging adulthood [15]. Other studies in adolescence, however, have related grandiose narcissism traits to psychopathology [94, 95]. It is thus unclear whether grandiose narcissism being unrelated to shame and insecure attachment in this study is an adaptive form of narcissism [66, 96] or whether the relation between grandiose narcissism and shame and attachment experiences might be challenging to capture with self-report measures [31]. Research using measures of implicit self-esteem and implicit shame have found a relation with grandiose narcissism [34, 97, 98]. Moreover, informant reports of narcissism traits which partially overlap with self-reports, also highlight a dismissive yet affirmation needing stance towards others as a feature of grandiose narcissism [99101]. Why those with higher grandiosity may potentially dismiss emotions such as shame and attachment anxieties requires further research, but it may be due to a self-presentation of a “false” positive self as described in the clinical literature [102].
In sum, for vulnerable narcissism, the experience of shame relates to evaluating the self more negatively and struggling between wanting to be close to others but also not trusting others. It should be also considered that both grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic traits can be present within the same person [6, 68]. Research in adolescents has shown that NPD can be distinguished in three subtypes: predominantly grandiose/malignant, fragile and high-functioning/exhibitionistic [95, 103]. However, feelings of grandiosity and vulnerability may still fluctuate on a state basis within the individual [104]. Moreover, particularly when grandiose narcissism is higher (more pathological) grandiose and vulnerable narcissism may co-occur blending in their antagonistic core [105, 106], posing challenges for the therapeutic alliance and treatment outcomes [3, 5, 107, 108]. Fostering self-compassion in treatment, including with young people, may counter the disruptive effects of shame by allowing one’s needs and sense of self, including negative aspects of the self, to be experienced [26, 109]. The challenge here is that feelings of shame may relate to less self-compassion or even fear thereof in adolescents with narcissistic traits [110, 111] and that therapists may show less warm responses to adolescents presenting with pathological narcissistic traits [103]. In clinical practice, it is important to be aware of emotional reactions to clients with narcissistic traits to be able to foster self-compassion [103].

Strengths and limitations

A limitation was that we only sampled the young participants once through a cross-sectional survey, and therefore cannot draw conclusions on the direction of effects proposed in this mediation analyses. Thus, it may also be possible that shame plays a role in the onset of more vulnerable narcissistic traits [30] or that the relationship might co-occur. This work may contribute to longitudinal and experimental approaches that are needed in this area [8]. Strengths of this study included the large sample allowing confidence in the tests for mediation and estimating the effects of vulnerable and grandiose narcissism traits simultaneously allowing for a more ecological valid model to be obtained. Moreover, the sample consisted of young people who were at a vulnerable age for developing personality disorders such as NPD [7, 17, 73]. However, it was a relatively healthy sample from the general population and replication in a clinical sample is warranted, particularly to shed more light on the associations involving grandiose narcissism.

Conclusion

People with vulnerable narcissism traits tended to report more shame, and struggled to be close to others. It may be that shame experiences highlight a discrepancy between the ideal and actual self that may contribute to a more insecure attachment style. Those with more grandiose narcissism traits we think were more likely to dismiss negative emotions or attachments. Clinically, a good working alliance and fostering self-compassion may counter some negative effects of shame, strengthening a person’s internal security and positive attachments with others.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Declarations

The study received ethical approval from the Institutional Review Board (HE10/370) from the University of Wollongong, Australia. Participants provided informed consent prior to participating. The study was conducted in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki [112].
Consent for publication is not applicable as no identity revealing data is present in the study.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Day NJS, Bourke M, Townsend ML, Grenyer BFS. Pathological narcissism: a study of burden on partners and family. J Personal Disord. 2019;33:1–15.CrossRef Day NJS, Bourke M, Townsend ML, Grenyer BFS. Pathological narcissism: a study of burden on partners and family. J Personal Disord. 2019;33:1–15.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Grenyer BFS: Historical overview of pathological narcissism. In: Understanding and treating pathological narcissism. edn. Edited by Ogrodniczuk JS. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2013: 15–26. Grenyer BFS: Historical overview of pathological narcissism. In: Understanding and treating pathological narcissism. edn. Edited by Ogrodniczuk JS. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2013: 15–26.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Hill PL, Lapsley DK: Adaptive and maladaptive narcissism in adolescent development. In: Narcissism and Machiavellianism in Youth: Implications for the Development of Adaptive and Maladaptive Behavior. edn. Edited by Barry CT, Kerig PK, Stellwagen KK, Barry TD: American Psychological Association; 2011: 89–105. Hill PL, Lapsley DK: Adaptive and maladaptive narcissism in adolescent development. In: Narcissism and Machiavellianism in Youth: Implications for the Development of Adaptive and Maladaptive Behavior. edn. Edited by Barry CT, Kerig PK, Stellwagen KK, Barry TD: American Psychological Association; 2011: 89–105.
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Aalsma MC, Lapsley DK, Flannery DJ. Personal fables, , And Adolescent Adjustment narcissism. Psychol Sch. 2006;43(4):481–91.CrossRef Aalsma MC, Lapsley DK, Flannery DJ. Personal fables, , And Adolescent Adjustment narcissism. Psychol Sch. 2006;43(4):481–91.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Horton RS: Parenting as a Cause of Narcissism: Empirical Support for Psychodynamic and Social Learning Theories. In: The Handbook Of Narcissism And Narcissistic Personality Disorder Theoretical Approaches, Empirical Findings, And Treatments. edn. Edited by Campbell DW, Miller JD. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 2011: 181–190. Horton RS: Parenting as a Cause of Narcissism: Empirical Support for Psychodynamic and Social Learning Theories. In: The Handbook Of Narcissism And Narcissistic Personality Disorder Theoretical Approaches, Empirical Findings, And Treatments. edn. Edited by Campbell DW, Miller JD. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 2011: 181–190.
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Rothstein A. The theory of narcissism: an object-relations perspective. In: Essential Papers on Narcissism. Edn. Edited by Morrison AP. New York, NY: New York University; 1986. Rothstein A. The theory of narcissism: an object-relations perspective. In: Essential Papers on Narcissism. Edn. Edited by Morrison AP. New York, NY: New York University; 1986.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat McWilliams N. Psychoanalytic diagnosis. Understanding personality structure in the clinical process., 2nd edn. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2011. McWilliams N. Psychoanalytic diagnosis. Understanding personality structure in the clinical process., 2nd edn. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2011.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Broucek FJ. Shame and its relationship to early narcissistic developments. Int J Psychoanal. 1982;63(Pt 3):369–78.PubMed Broucek FJ. Shame and its relationship to early narcissistic developments. Int J Psychoanal. 1982;63(Pt 3):369–78.PubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Bosson JK, Prewitt-Freilino JL: Overvalued and ashamed: considering the roles of self-esteem and self-conscious emotions in covert narcissism. In: The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research. 2 edn. Edited by Tracy JL, Robins RW, Tangney JP. New York: Guilford; 2007. Bosson JK, Prewitt-Freilino JL: Overvalued and ashamed: considering the roles of self-esteem and self-conscious emotions in covert narcissism. In: The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research. 2 edn. Edited by Tracy JL, Robins RW, Tangney JP. New York: Guilford; 2007.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Schoenleber M, Berenbaum H. Shame regulation in personality pathology. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011. Schoenleber M, Berenbaum H. Shame regulation in personality pathology. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Kemeny ME, Gruenewald TL, Dickerson SS. Shame as the emotional response to threat to the social self: implications for behavior, physiology, and health. Psychol Inq. 2004;15(2):153–60. Kemeny ME, Gruenewald TL, Dickerson SS. Shame as the emotional response to threat to the social self: implications for behavior, physiology, and health. Psychol Inq. 2004;15(2):153–60.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Shen L: The evolution of shame and guilt. PLoS One 2018, 13(7). Shen L: The evolution of shame and guilt. PLoS One 2018, 13(7).
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Hejdenburg J, Andrews B. The relationship between shame and different types of anger: a theory-based investigation. Pers Individ Dif. 2011;50:1278–82.CrossRef Hejdenburg J, Andrews B. The relationship between shame and different types of anger: a theory-based investigation. Pers Individ Dif. 2011;50:1278–82.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Tracey JL, Robins RW. "Death of a (narcissistic) salesman:" An Integrative Model of Fragile Self-Esteem. Psychol Inq. 2003;1:57–62. Tracey JL, Robins RW. "Death of a (narcissistic) salesman:" An Integrative Model of Fragile Self-Esteem. Psychol Inq. 2003;1:57–62.
49.
Zurück zum Zitat DiSarno M, Zimmerman J, Madeddu F, Casini E, Di Pierro R. Shame behind the corner? A daily diary investigation of pathological narcissism. J Res Pers. 2020;85. DiSarno M, Zimmerman J, Madeddu F, Casini E, Di Pierro R. Shame behind the corner? A daily diary investigation of pathological narcissism. J Res Pers. 2020;85.
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Freis SD, Brown AA, Carroll PJ, Arkin RM. Shame, rage, And Unsuccessful Motivated Reasoning In Vulnerable Narcissism. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2015;34(10):877–95.CrossRef Freis SD, Brown AA, Carroll PJ, Arkin RM. Shame, rage, And Unsuccessful Motivated Reasoning In Vulnerable Narcissism. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2015;34(10):877–95.CrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Fossati A, Feeney J, Pincus A, Borroni S, Maffei C. The structure of pathological narcissism and its relationships with adult attachment styles: a study of Italian nonclinical and clinical adult participants. Psychoanal Psychol. 2015;32(3):403–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037175.CrossRef Fossati A, Feeney J, Pincus A, Borroni S, Maffei C. The structure of pathological narcissism and its relationships with adult attachment styles: a study of Italian nonclinical and clinical adult participants. Psychoanal Psychol. 2015;32(3):403–31. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​a0037175.CrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Weiss M, Fradkin I, Huppert JD. Modelling pathological narcissism using the brief PNI in terms of structure and convergent and divergent validity: a new perspective. Assessment. 2020:1–11. Weiss M, Fradkin I, Huppert JD. Modelling pathological narcissism using the brief PNI in terms of structure and convergent and divergent validity: a new perspective. Assessment. 2020:1–11.
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosseel Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48(2):1–36.CrossRef Rosseel Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48(2):1–36.CrossRef
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Grafeman SJ, Barry CT, Marcus DK, Leachman LL. Interpersonal perceptions of narcissism in an at-risk adolescent sample: a social relations analysis. J Res Adolesc. 2013;25(1):92–100.CrossRef Grafeman SJ, Barry CT, Marcus DK, Leachman LL. Interpersonal perceptions of narcissism in an at-risk adolescent sample: a social relations analysis. J Res Adolesc. 2013;25(1):92–100.CrossRef
102.
Zurück zum Zitat Winnicott DW. Ego distortion in terms of true and false self. In: The Maturational Processes and the Facilitating Environment: studies in the theory of emotional development. Edn. London: The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psychoanalysis; 1965. Winnicott DW. Ego distortion in terms of true and false self. In: The Maturational Processes and the Facilitating Environment: studies in the theory of emotional development. Edn. London: The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psychoanalysis; 1965.
108.
Zurück zum Zitat WHJ M, et al. Ann Am Psychother Assoc. 2005;8:2. WHJ M, et al. Ann Am Psychother Assoc. 2005;8:2.
112.
Zurück zum Zitat World Medical Association. World medical association declaration of Helsinki ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association. 2013;310(20):2191–4.CrossRef World Medical Association. World medical association declaration of Helsinki ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association. 2013;310(20):2191–4.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Narcissistic traits in young people and how experiencing shame relates to current attachment challenges
verfasst von
Charlotte C. van Schie
Heidi L. Jarman
Samantha Reis
Brin F. S. Grenyer
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2021
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2021
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-021-03249-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2021

BMC Psychiatry 1/2021 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).