Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Somnologie 2/2021

Open Access 07.12.2020 | Original Studies

On the influence of the day of the week on objective and self-rated sleep quality of adults

verfasst von: C. Sauter, H. Dorn, H. Danker-Hopfe

Erschienen in: Somnologie | Ausgabe 2/2021

Abstract

Background and objective

The extent to which adult sleep varies depending on the day of the week has not yet been systematically investigated with electroencephalography (EEG) data. Whether such effects exist and whether they are related to age, gender, and employment status was retrospectively analyzed based on data from an experimental double-blind cross-over study in which effects of electromagnetic fields of a cell phone base station on the sleep of a general rural population had been examined.

Methods

The sleep of 397 adults (age 45.0 ± 14.2 years, range 18–81 years; 50.9% women) from ten different rural German villages was recorded for 12 nights with ambulatory devices. Self-reported sleep quality was recorded in morning and evening protocols. Friedman tests were used for statistical analysis of the comparison between the days, and the Kruskal–Wallis and Mann–Whitney U tests were used for pairwise comparisons of independent parameters between groups.

Results

For the present analysis, data from 335 participants were considered. Overall, the differences between nights were small and the quality of sleep was good. Three of the five objective and all six self-rated sleep parameters differed significantly between the days of the week. While the objective and the self-estimated total sleep time were longest on Sunday nights, the qualitatively poorest values occurred on Monday nights. People who worked fulltime had the longest sleep latencies on Sunday nights. Friday nights were rated the best.

Conclusion

The objective and self-rated sleep quality varied relatively little in a rural adult population over the course of the week, being worst on Monday nights and best on Friday nights.
It is common knowledge that most adolescents and adults have trouble falling asleep on Sunday nights and feel less refreshed on Monday mornings than on other days of the week. In a sleep health program developed and conducted for the German Armed Forces [25], several participants felt significantly impaired by these weekly recurring problems, which was also reflected in their sleep log data (unpublished data). The possible reasons are not just preoccupation with the upcoming work or school week, but also the changes in the timing of going to bed and getting up from weekends to weekdays. These differences in the sleep–wake rhythm between weekdays and weekends are attributed to the so-called “social jetlag” [28]. Social jetlag is characterized by a misalignment between the social and the biological clock, and it usually results in a phase delay of sleep–wake timing on work-free days as compared to weekdays [22, 28]. Several studies have shown that social jetlag has negative impacts on the school performance of children and adolescents [8, 9] as well as on productivity in industry, and might lead to detrimental health effects, e.g., metabolic changes and adiposity [21, 26, 27]. Whereas these negative consequences have been well studied, only few data are available on objectively derived sleep quality from adult populations across different weekdays. Most studies on social jetlag applied questionnaires and/or used retrospective evaluation for self-rated parameters [10, 15, 22]. Only a few studies applied objective data gathered by actigraphy [20, 27, 30]. To the best of our knowledge, no study has applied ambulatory polysomnography across different nights of the week in the general adult population to look for effects of different weekdays on different sleep quality measures. In the present study, objective as well as self-reported sleep data from an experimental field study on the impact of mobile phone base stations were reanalyzed with regard to effects of weekday, age, gender, and employment status in a rural adult population.

Materials and methods

Study design

For the present analysis, data were derived from a double-blind randomized cross-over study on possible effects of electromagnetic field (EMF) exposure from mobile phone base stations on sleep [5]. In ten different small German villages without any network coverage and a mean number of 286 inhabitants, an experimental mobile phone base station was set up for a study period of 2 weeks. In total, 397 residents (18–81 years, 50.9% female) participated in the study. All individuals were randomly exposed to EMF from Global System for Mobile Communications (GSM; 900 MHz and 1800 MHz) and sham for a total number of 5 nights each, while sleep was recorded at their homes.

Questionnaires prior to experimental procedures

In the runup to the study, the following questionnaires were completed by participants: Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI [3]), the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS [17]), morningness–eveningness questionnaire (MEQ [12]), self-rating depression (SDS [31]) and anxiety scale (SAS [32]), the NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI [2, 4]), and a questionnaire on attitude towards mobile communication [14]. Only results from PSQI, ESS, SDS, SAS, and MEQ are reported here, since they are closely related to sleep quality.

Ambulatory sleep recordings

Objective sleep quality was monitored at the homes of the study participants by an ambulatory device which tracks frontal electroencephalography (EEG) and electrooculography (EOG; SOMNOscreen Neuro-KombiTM by SOMNOmedics, Randersacker, Germany). The first night (Sunday) served as an adaptation night, and sleep was also recorded in the following nights until Friday. After a break on Saturday, sleep was monitored for another 6 nights (Sunday to Friday), resulting in a maximum of 12 nights per individual. In half of the nights, the experimental base station was operating in verum mode (GSM). Prior to unblinding of the data, the quality of all sleep recordings was visually controlled for artifacts resulting from wrong handling of the recording system or other technical problems. If artifacts did not allow correct automatic scoring, recordings were omitted. In a next step, sleep onset was determined according to the criteria of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine [13] in terms of the appearance of the first epoch of any sleep stage. The time of awakening (first epoch “awake” after the last stage of sleep) was manually determined and scoring of sleep in between was performed automatically with the DOMINO software from SOMNOmedics. The definitions of the five selected objective sleep parameters are shown in Table 1.
Table 1
Definitions of objective and self-rated sleep parameters
 
Objective sleep parameter
(polysomnography)
Self-rated sleep parameter
(evening/morning protocol)
Time in bed (TIB)
Time period from “lights off” to “lights on” (oTIB, min)
Time period from “lights off” to “lights on” (TIB, min)
Total sleep time (TST)
Time period from “lights off” to “lights on” minus wake (oTST, min)
Estimated total sleep time (sTST, min)
Sleep efficiency index (SEI)
(oTST/oTIB) × 100; (oSEI, %)
(sTST/sTIB) × 100; (sSEI, %)
Sleep onset latency (SOL)
Sleep onset latency stage 1: time period from “lights off” to stage 1 (oSOL, min)
Estimated time from “lights off” to falling asleep (sSOL, min)
Wake after sleep onset (WASO)
Wake after sleep onset (oWASO, min)
Estimated total time awake during night (sWASO, min)
Restfulness (VAS)
Visual analogue scale (VAS, morning): low values indicate high restfulness (0 = very restful, 100 = not restful)

Self-rated sleep parameters

Participants filled in the evening and morning protocol (sleep diary) of the German Sleep Research Society [11] parallel to the ambulatory sleep recording to assess parameters of self-rated sleep quality. The evening protocol comprises eight items and the morning protocol 11 sleep-related items. Six parameters of self-rated sleep quality corresponding to the five objective sleep parameters were considered, and their definitions are shown in Table 1.

Statistical analysis

Since EMF did not have any effect on the objectively derived and self-rated sleep quality parameters [5], data of all nights were analyzed, except for the adaption night. For each of the other days of the week, the data of the 2 weeks were averaged separately per day. Since data were not normally distributed, only non-parametric statistical analyses were applied. The Friedman test was applied to test for differences in parameters across the 6 days. Mann–Whitney U tests were used for pairwise comparisons. Chi-squared tests were used to analyze the distribution of categorized data between groups. All tests were performed with a two-tailed p < 0.05. Statistical analyses were conducted with SPSS IBM SPSS Statistics 26 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).
The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the Charité, Universitätsmedizin Berlin, and all participants gave written informed consent. The study was performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki.

Results

Characteristics of participants

From 397 residents included in the study, 21 dropped out voluntarily (10 due to issues with recording, five each due to work-related reasons and health issues, and one for unclear reasons). Data of 11 study participants had been excluded because the individuals did not live close enough (i.e. in a distance >500 m) to the mobile phone base station, finally resulting in data of morning and evening protocols of 365 individuals. For analysis of objective sleep data, data from seven individuals were omitted because a different recording system was used, and for 23 individuals the quality of recordings was not sufficient in at least 50% of all nights, resulting in a total number of datasets from 335 participants.
The characteristics of the originally included participants (n = 397) are shown in Table 2. The subgroup analyzed here (n = 335) did not differ from the voluntary dropout and excluded participants (n = 62), except for the number of individuals with elevated SDS scores, which was slightly higher among the participants not considered, while the median did not differ between groups. Fully employed participants (n = 129) were significantly younger (median: 44.0 years; interquartile range: 35.0, 51.0 years) than the not fully employed individuals (median: 47.0 years; interquartile range: 36.0, 59.0 years). Within the fully employed subsample, 75.2% were males and 24.8% were females.
Table 2
Characteristics of all participants of the study (n = 397), for the subgroup in which objective and self-rated sleep data were available (n = 335), and of those who dropped out or were excluded (n = 62)
Parameter
Total sample (n = 397)
Analysis group (n = 335)
Dropouts/excluded (n = 62)
Analysis group vs. dropouts/excluded
 
Median (IQR)
Median (IQR)
Median (IQR)
p‑valuea
Age (years)
45.0 (35.0, 55.5)
45.0 (35.0, 56.0)
41.0 (34.0, 55.3)
0.426
BMI kg/m2
26.2 (23.6, 29.3)
26.2 (23.6, 29.3)
25.9 (23.3, 29.8)
0.836
PSQI score
5.0 (3.0, 6.0)
5.0 (3.0, 6.0)
4.0 (3.0, 6.0)
0.816
ESS score
8.0 (5.0, 10.0)
7.0 (5.0, 10.0)
9.0 (5.0, 10.3)
0.224
SDS score
32.0 (27.0, 36.0)
32.0 (28.0, 36.0)
31.0 (26.0, 35.5)
0.633
SAS score
30.0 (26.0, 34.0)
30.0 (26.0, 34.0)
29.0 (25.0, 34.3)
0.467
 
n (%)
n (%)
n (%)
p‑valueb
Female
202 (50.9)
175 (52.2)
27 (43.5)
0.209
PSQI score >5
129 (32.5)
108 (32.2)
21 (34.4)
0.737
ESS score >10
81 (20.4)
66 (19.7)
15 (24.2)
0.420
SDS score >40
37 (9.3)
27 (8.1)
10 (16.1)
0.045
SAS score >35
76 (19.1)
62 (18.5)
14 (22.6)
0.454
MEQ—definite evening type (<31)
1 (0.3)
1 (0.3)
0 (0.0)
0.365
MEQ—moderate evening type (31–41)
17 (4.3)
13 (3.9)
4 (6.5)
MEQ—indifferent type (42–58)
178 (44.8)
146 (43.6)
32 (51.6)
MEQ—moderate morning type (59–69)
158 (39.8)
135 (40.3)
23 (37.1)
MEQ—definite morning type (>69)
43 (10.8)
40 (11.9)
3 (4.8)
Employment status
Employed (fulltime)
161 (40.6)
129 (38.5)
32 (51.6)
0.054
Part-time/not employed/training/school
236 (59.4)
206 (61.5)
30 (48.4)
BMI body mass index, ESS Epworth Sleepiness Scale, IQR interquartile range, MEQ morningness–eveningness questionnaire, PSQI Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, SAS self-rating anxiety scale, SDS self-rating depression scale
aMann–Whitney U test
bChi-squared test

Bedtimes and getting up times

To get a picture of clock times across the different nights, all 12 study days were considered. In Fig. 1a median bedtimes of all 12 nights for all participants and for the fully employed subsample separately (Fig. 1b) are displayed. Median getting up times are shown in Fig. 1c, d. Median bedtimes were earliest on Friday nights and getting up times were latest on Friday nights, i.e., Saturday mornings, in both samples. Whereas bedtimes were the same in both groups in 8 of the 12 nights (22:30), the fully employed showed earlier getting up times on all working day mornings.

Objective sleep parameter across weekdays

Three of the five objective (o) sleep parameters varied statistically significantly across the 6 days in all participants (Table 3). The median oTIB (Fig. 2a) as well as oTST (Fig. 2b) were longest on Sunday nights. Median oSOL was longest on Monday nights and shortest on Thursday and Friday nights (Fig. 2c). There were no significant differences between the nights in terms of oSEI and oWASO (Table 3). In the subsample of the fully employed participants, two parameters varied significantly across the week (Table 4). The oTIB was shortest on Monday nights and longest on Friday nights. The median oSOL was longest on Sunday nights and shortest on Thursday nights (Table 4).
Table 3
Results of objective and self-rated sleep parameters for 6 different weekdays of all participants of the analysis group
 
Sun
Mon
Tue
Wed
Thu
Fri
 
Median
[IQR]
Median
[IQR]
Median
[IQR]
Median
[IQR]
Median
[IQR]
Median
[IQR]
p‑valuea
Objective sleep parameters
Time in bed
(oTIB, min)
447.6
[389.4; 492.6]
432.4
[391.6; 476.6]
438.1
[392.0; 480.2]
432.0
[392.4; 473.7]
432.9
[389.8; 477.5]
439.9
[396.4; 476.7]
<0.0001
Total sleep time
(oTST, min)
414.5
[360.0; 458.5]
400.5
[359.3; 441.0]
402.0
[360.3; 437.3]
402.3
[359.5; 437.0]
399.0
[361.0; 443.0]
404.3
[363.8; 441.8]
0.0151
Sleep efficiency index
(oSEI, %)
93.0
[90.3; 95.2]
93.0
[90.0; 94.8]
93.3
[90.7; 95.0]
93.5
[90.6; 95.1]
93.3
[90.5; 95.1]
93.3
[90.3; 95.1]
0.1786
Sleep onset latency
(oSOL, min)
9.8
[4.8; 18.7]
10.4
[6.4; 16.2]
9.9
[6.4; 15.6]
9.9
[6.3; 15.3]
8.7
[5.4; 16.5]
8.7
[5.3; 14.3]
<0.0001
Wake after sleep onset
(oWASO, min)
23.7
[14.2; 36.3]
22.4
[14.9; 36.4]
23.7
[15.1; 33.8]
21.3
[14.4; 33.4]
21.4
[13.9; 32.8]
22.0
[14.9; 35.2]
0.2692
Self-rated sleep parameters
Time in bed
(sTIB, min)
445.0
[389.5; 502.0]
433.5
[391.3; 479.0]
436.5
[388.0; 482.5]
435.0
[390.0; 475.0]
434.0
[389.3; 477.5]
446.0
[397.5; 490.0]
<0.0001
Total sleep time
(sTST, min)
410.0
[360.0; 450.0]
390.0
[360.0; 435.0]
390.0
[360.0; 435.0]
405.0
[360.0; 445.0]
405.0
[360.0; 437.5]
405.0
[360.0; 450.0]
<0.0001
Sleep efficiency index
(sSEI, %)
92.6
[83.5; 97.5]
92.5
[86.3; 97.1]
92.9
[86.4; 96.8]
93.4
[87.9; 97.8]
93.3
[88.5; 97.3]
93.8
[88.1; 98.0]
0.0110
Sleep onset latency
(sSOL, min)
10.0
[5.0; 22.5]
12.5
[7.5; 25.0]
12.5
[7.5; 22.5]
10.0
[5.0; 21.3]
10.0
[5.0; 20.0]
10.0
[5.0; 17.5]
<0.0001
Wake after sleep onset
(sWASO, min)
10.0
[5.0; 30.0]
12.5
[7.5; 30.0]
12.5
[6.0; 22.5]
10.5
[6.4; 22.5]
10.3
[5.4; 20.0]
10.0
[5.0; 22.8]
0.0157
Restfulness
(VAS)
33.0
[22.0; 52.0]
37.5
[26.3; 51.3]
35.0
[24.0; 47.0]
31.0
[21.0 45.5]
33.0
[22.3; 45.5]
31.0
[21.3; 43.5]
<0.0001
IQR interquartile range, Sun Sunday, Mon Monday, Tue Tuesday, Wed Wednesday, Thu Thursday, Fri Friday
aFriedman test
Table 4
Results of objective and self-rated sleep parameters for 6 different weekdays of fully employed participants of the analysis group
 
Sun
Mon
Tue
Wed
Thu
Fri
 
Median
[IQR]
Median
[IQR]
Median
[IQR]
Median
[IQR]
Median
[IQR]
Median
[IQR]
p‑valuea
Objective sleep parameters
Time in bed
(oTIB, min)
422.9
[374.4; 458.6]
414.5
[369.6; 460.3]
429.4
[368.6; 462.5]
416.0
[369.6; 460.3]
421.8
[377.6; 460.4]
428.2
[386.3; 474.0]
0.0304
Total sleep time
(oTST, min)
387.0
[349.6; 422.5]
383.3
[345.8; 421.4]
391.3
[347.3; 430.3]
384.1
[345.5; 427.8]
389.1
[353.1; 427.5]
393.0
[356.8; 431.4]
0.1062
Sleep efficiency index
(oSEI, %)
93.4
[90.8; 95.7]
93.5
[90.9; 95.3]
94.0
[91.9; 95.5]
94.0
[92.0; 95.7]
94.0
[91.7; 95.2]
94.2
[91.4; 95.2]
0.1186
Sleep onset latency
(oSOL, min)
9.0
[4.7; 16.6]
8.4
[6.2; 14.0]
8.1
[5.2; 13.1]
8.4
[5.8; 11.8]
7.4
[5.0; 11.8]
7.6
[5.0; 11.3]
0.0097
Wake after sleep onset
(oWASO, min)
20.8
[12.0; 33.0]
19.0
[13.6; 29.3]
18.6
[12.3; 28.5]
18.1
[12.1; 24.7]
17.7
[11.3; 28.3]
18.4
[13.1; 28.2]
0.0894
Self-rated sleep parameters
Time in bed
(sTIB, min)
420.0
[374.0; 465.0]
413.5
[372.0; 462.0]
415.0
[370.0; 454.5]
419.0
[380.0; 463.8]
419.5
[372.5; 464.3]
428.5
[391.8; 483.5]
0.0197
Total sleep time
(sTST, min)
390.0
[348.8; 420.0]
382.5
[345.0; 420.0]
387.5
[345.4; 427.5]
390.0
[345.0; 427.5]
390.0
[338.8; 420.0]
400.0
[360.0; 450.0]
0.0145
Sleep efficiency index
(sSEI, %)
93.3
[87.3; 97.9]
93.3
[87.5; 97.4]
93.7
[87.4; 97.3]
94.8
[89.4; 98.0]
94.1
[90.3; 97.2]
93.9
[89.2; 97.9]
0.1702
Sleep onset latency
(sSOL, min)
10.0
[5.0; 20.0]
10.0
[5.0; 20.0]
10.0
[5.0; 15.0]
7.5
[5.0; 13.8]
7.5
[5.0; 12.5]
7.5
[4.0; 12.5]
<0.0001
Wake after sleep onset
(sWASO, min)
10.0
[5.0; 30.0]
12.5
[7.5; 22.5]
10.0
[5.7; 17.5]
10.0
[5.9; 25.0]
10.0
[5.0; 20.0]
8.8
[5.0; 22.0]
0.1070
Restfulness
(VAS)
34.0
[24.0; 51.8]
39.8
[26.5; 51.5]
37.2
[23.6; 49.1]
34.5
[21.5 46.9]
32.0
[22.0; 46.0]
31.0
[22.1; 42.0]
0.0001
IQR interquartile range, Sun Sunday, Mon Monday, Tue Tuesday, Wed Wednesday, Thu Thursday, Fri Friday
aFriedman test

Self-rated sleep parameters across weekdays

All six endpoints from the self-rated (s) evening- and morning protocols differed statistically significantly between the days, with the longest sTIB (Fig. 3a) and highest sSEI (Fig. 3c) on Friday nights, and the longest sTST (Fig. 3b) on Sunday nights (Table 3). All self-reported parameters were rated worst on Monday nights (and also on Tuesday nights in sTST, sSOL, and sWASO; see Fig. 3a–f) by all participants and by the subsample of the fully employed only (Table 4). In addition, in the fully employed (Table 4) sSOL was of equal length on Sundays and Tuesdays, and sSEI was of equal magnitude on Sundays and Mondays.

Effect of gender

The durations of oTIB and oTST were longer in female participants than in males on all nights and differed statistically significantly on Sunday and Friday nights (Fig. 4a, b); oTIB also differed statistically significantly on Wednesday nights. Objective SOL was significantly longer in females on all nights, except for Monday nights. None of the other objective sleep parameters differed between women and men.
In the self-rated sleep variables, only time in bed and sleep onset latencies indicated significant differences between female and male participants (Fig. 5a, b). The results were similar to those of the objective parameters and the direction was the same, with longer estimated sSOL in females in nearly all nights, except for Thursday nights, and significant differences with regard to sTIB on Sunday and Friday nights and also on Wednesday nights.

Effect of employment status

The differences in objective sleep parameters between participants who were fully employed and those who were not are shown in the Fig. 6a–e. While oTIB (Fig. 6a) and oTST (Fig. 6b) in the fully employed were significantly shorter, in particular at the beginning of the week, oSEI was always significantly higher (Fig. 6c), except for Sunday nights. Objective WASO (Fig. 6e) was significantly shorter on all nights in the fully employed. On weekdays, oSOL (Fig. 6d) was significantly shorter on all nights in the fully employed subsample, except for Thursday and Sunday nights.
Self-rated sleep parameters show a similar picture to the objective ones in sTIB (Fig. 7a) and sTST (Fig. 7b), but just one significant difference in sSEI on Thursdays (Fig. 7c) and sWASO on Tuesdays (Fig. 7e). The sSOL (Fig. 7d) was rated significantly shorter on all nights in fully employed participants in contrast to on 4 nights in oSOL (Fig. 7d). The fully employed participants rated sleep of Wednesday nights as significantly less restful than the other participants (Fig. 7f). There were no significant differences in all other nights with regard to the degree of restfulness of sleep.

Discussion

The present study provides data on ambulatorily recorded and self-rating-derived sleep parameters of a large sample of the rural German population. Bedtimes and getting up times followed a similar pattern in the two study weeks, with earlier bedtimes on Friday nights and clearly later getting up times on Saturday mornings. These findings were independent of whether individuals were fully employed or not. Sleep recordings in this rural population of a broad age range indicated short median sleep latencies ≤10 min. The median sleep duration of all participants was about 7 h in all nights. Median sleep efficiency indices varied between 93.0 and 93.5%, which is quite high when compared to age- and sex-specific reference values [6]. Whereas the sleep latencies were shorter in the present sample than in the reference study, which is based on data of laboratory nights with polysomnography [6], a higher sleep efficiency in the present sample might at least in part result from shorter TIB as compared to the given 8 h TIB in the reference study.
In general, differences in sleep parameters between weekdays in the present sample were quite small. This finding is in agreement with the American time use survey [1], in which 47,731 respondents older than 14 years answered questions on sleep time and waking activities. The differences in average sleep time were larger between weekdays and weekend days than between weekdays (Monday to Friday).
In a study on the contribution of various types of activities to recovery, 28 women and 18 men of different working populations (with a mean age of 34.93 years) completed a diary for 7 days and nights [24]. Sleep duration, sleep quality, and feelings of being refreshed increased at the weekend, which is consistent with our findings of the highest self-rated sleep duration and level of restfulness on Saturday mornings in the fully employed sample. In the latter cited study [24], Sunday nights were rated worst, which was not the case for most of the sleep parameters in our study. Interestingly, in our study, Monday nights appeared to be the least refreshing nights and ranked last in all of the self-rated outcome parameters.
In the fully employed subsample, the objectively measured time to fall asleep was longest on Sunday nights. This would corroborate findings of social jetlag, since later getting up times on Saturday mornings and strict bedtimes on all nights before working days may induce prolonged times to fall asleep on Sunday nights. Furthermore, in the fully employed individuals, sleep latencies decreased significantly across the weekdays, possibly due at least in some part to a higher sleep pressure as a consequence of accumulated sleep debt during the week [16, 23]. A high sleep pressure might additionally have led to the observed earlier bedtimes on Friday nights and to later getting up times on Saturday mornings. The observed longer time in bed and total sleep time on Sunday nights in the total group might also reflect such an accumulated sleep debt [7] with subsequent recovery sleep, a phenomenon which is observed in many adolescents and adults [1, 23]. Similar to the present findings, in the American time use survey, sleep duration was also longest on Sundays nights [1].
In the present study, the fully employed individuals had significantly shorter TST during most of the weekdays compared to the not fully employed. These results could be attributed to the higher prevalence of male participants and the younger age in the fully employed sample, in which the oldest was 60 years old compared to 20.4% in the not fully employed sample who were 60 years or older. In the American time use study, participants aged ≥65 years slept longer during weekdays, whereas those <35 years showed longer weekend sleep times, especially on Sundays [1].
Female study participants in our study had longer TIB and TST as well as longer sleep latencies on all days except Mondays. These findings match with the results of other studies that have shown that on the one hand, women have longer sleep durations and sleep latencies [5, 19], and on the other hand, are more prone to insomnia disorders [29].
A limitation of the present study was that Saturday nights were missing, and only half of the available Sunday nights could be used, since the first nights were always adaption nights and were therefore excluded from further analyses.
A strength of the study is the large population and the objective data of several nights recorded in the participants’ familiar surroundings. In addition, observational studies are usually prone to the Hawthorne effect, i.e., individuals who participate in a study are informed about the topic and aim of the study, which might bias their behavior and self-reporting [18]. This effect was not applicable to the present study since the original objective was to evaluate the possible impact of electromagnetic fields of a base station on sleep. Therefore, a retrospective analysis of data with regard to the effects of day of the week on sleep is not affected by this kind of bias.

Conclusion

The present study shows that in a rural adult population, social jetlag and negative consequences on objective and self-rated sleep quality are small. Nevertheless, accumulated sleep debt across the week may lead to more pronounced negative effects on sleep and wakefulness than possible effects of social jetlag.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank all participants and local organizers of the study. The study (M8837) was funded by the German Mobile Telecommunication Research Program (DMF).

Compliance with ethical guidelines

Conflict of interest

C. Sauter, H. Dorn, and H. Danker-Hopfe declare that they have no competing interests.
All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

Somnologie

Print-Titel

  • Aktuelles, gesichertes Fachwissen zu Ätiologie, Pathophysiologie, Differentialdiagnostik und Therapie von Schlafstörungen

• Multidisziplinärer Ansatz

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Dent – Das Online-Abo der Zahnmedizin

Online-Abonnement

Mit e.Dent erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen zahnmedizinischen Fortbildungen und unseren zahnmedizinischen und ausgesuchten medizinischen Zeitschriften.

Weitere Produktempfehlungen anzeigen
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Basner M, Fomberstein KM, Razavi FM et al (2007) American time use survey: sleep time and its relationship to waking activities. Sleep 30:1085–1095CrossRef Basner M, Fomberstein KM, Razavi FM et al (2007) American time use survey: sleep time and its relationship to waking activities. Sleep 30:1085–1095CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Borkenau P, Ostendorf F (1993) NEO-Fünf-Faktoren-Inventar (NEO-FFI) nach Costa und McCrae (Handanweisung). Hogrefe, Göttingen Borkenau P, Ostendorf F (1993) NEO-Fünf-Faktoren-Inventar (NEO-FFI) nach Costa und McCrae (Handanweisung). Hogrefe, Göttingen
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH et al (1989) The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 28:193–213CrossRef Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH et al (1989) The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 28:193–213CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Costa PT, Mccrae RR (1992) Revised NEO personality inventory and NEO five-factor inventory (Professional Manual). Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa Costa PT, Mccrae RR (1992) Revised NEO personality inventory and NEO five-factor inventory (Professional Manual). Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Danker-Hopfe H, Dorn H, Bornkessel C et al (2010) Do mobile phone base stations affect sleep of residents? Results from an experimental double-blind sham-controlled field study. Am J Hum Biol 22:613–618CrossRef Danker-Hopfe H, Dorn H, Bornkessel C et al (2010) Do mobile phone base stations affect sleep of residents? Results from an experimental double-blind sham-controlled field study. Am J Hum Biol 22:613–618CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Danker-Hopfe H, Richter S, Eggert T (2020) Referenzwerte zur Schlafarchitektur für Erwachsene. Somnologie 24:39–64CrossRef Danker-Hopfe H, Richter S, Eggert T (2020) Referenzwerte zur Schlafarchitektur für Erwachsene. Somnologie 24:39–64CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Dement WC (2005) Sleep extension: getting as much extra sleep as possible. Clin Sports Med 24:251–268CrossRef Dement WC (2005) Sleep extension: getting as much extra sleep as possible. Clin Sports Med 24:251–268CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Diaz-Morales JF, Escribano C (2015) Social jetlag, academic achievement and cognitive performance: Understanding gender/sex differences. Chronobiol Int 32:822–831CrossRef Diaz-Morales JF, Escribano C (2015) Social jetlag, academic achievement and cognitive performance: Understanding gender/sex differences. Chronobiol Int 32:822–831CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Haraszti RA, Ella K, Gyongyosi N et al (2014) Social jetlag negatively correlates with academic performance in undergraduates. Chronobiol Int 31:603–612CrossRef Haraszti RA, Ella K, Gyongyosi N et al (2014) Social jetlag negatively correlates with academic performance in undergraduates. Chronobiol Int 31:603–612CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Haun VC, Oppenauer V (2019) The role of job demands and negative work reflection in employees’ trajectory of sleep quality over the workweek. J Occup Health Psychol 24:675–688CrossRef Haun VC, Oppenauer V (2019) The role of job demands and negative work reflection in employees’ trajectory of sleep quality over the workweek. J Occup Health Psychol 24:675–688CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Hoffmann RM, Müller T, Hajak G et al (1997) Abend-Morgenprotokolle in Schlafforschung und Schlafmedizin – Ein Standardinstrument für den deutschsprachigen Raum. Somnologie 1:103–109CrossRef Hoffmann RM, Müller T, Hajak G et al (1997) Abend-Morgenprotokolle in Schlafforschung und Schlafmedizin – Ein Standardinstrument für den deutschsprachigen Raum. Somnologie 1:103–109CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Horne JA, Östberg O (1976) A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 4:97–110PubMed Horne JA, Östberg O (1976) A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 4:97–110PubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Iber C, Ancoli-Israel S, Chesson A et al (2007) The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events: rules, terminology and technical specifications. American Academy of Sleep Medicine, Westchester Iber C, Ancoli-Israel S, Chesson A et al (2007) The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events: rules, terminology and technical specifications. American Academy of Sleep Medicine, Westchester
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Infas (2006) Ermittlung der Befürchtungen und Ängste der breiten Öffentlichkeit hinsichtlich möglicher Gefahren der hochfrequenten elektromagnetischen Felder des Mobilfunks – jährliche Umfragen (Abschlussbericht der Befragung 2006) Infas (2006) Ermittlung der Befürchtungen und Ängste der breiten Öffentlichkeit hinsichtlich möglicher Gefahren der hochfrequenten elektromagnetischen Felder des Mobilfunks – jährliche Umfragen (Abschlussbericht der Befragung 2006)
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Jankowski KS (2017) Actual versus preferred sleep times as a proxy of biological time for social jet lag. Chronobiol Int 34:1175–1176CrossRef Jankowski KS (2017) Actual versus preferred sleep times as a proxy of biological time for social jet lag. Chronobiol Int 34:1175–1176CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Jankowski KS (2017) Social jet lag: sleep-corrected formula. Chronobiol Int 34:531–535CrossRef Jankowski KS (2017) Social jet lag: sleep-corrected formula. Chronobiol Int 34:531–535CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Johns MW (1991) A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep 14:540–545CrossRef Johns MW (1991) A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep 14:540–545CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Mccambridge J, Witton J, Elbourne DR (2014) Systematic review of the Hawthorne effect: new concepts are needed to study research participation effects. J Clin Epidemiol 67:267–277CrossRef Mccambridge J, Witton J, Elbourne DR (2014) Systematic review of the Hawthorne effect: new concepts are needed to study research participation effects. J Clin Epidemiol 67:267–277CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C et al (2004) Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep 27:1255–1273CrossRef Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C et al (2004) Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep 27:1255–1273CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Paine SJ, Gander PH (2016) Differences in circadian phase and weekday/weekend sleep patterns in a sample of middle-aged morning types and evening types. Chronobiol Int 33:1009–1017CrossRef Paine SJ, Gander PH (2016) Differences in circadian phase and weekday/weekend sleep patterns in a sample of middle-aged morning types and evening types. Chronobiol Int 33:1009–1017CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Roenneberg T, Allebrandt KV, Merrow M et al (2012) Social jetlag and obesity. Curr Biol 22:939–943CrossRef Roenneberg T, Allebrandt KV, Merrow M et al (2012) Social jetlag and obesity. Curr Biol 22:939–943CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Juda M et al (2007) Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Med Rev 11:429–438CrossRef Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Juda M et al (2007) Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Med Rev 11:429–438CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M (2003) Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms 18:80–90CrossRef Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M (2003) Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms 18:80–90CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Rook JW, Zijlstra FRH (2006) The contribution of various types of activities to recovery. Eur J Work Organ Psychol 15:218–240CrossRef Rook JW, Zijlstra FRH (2006) The contribution of various types of activities to recovery. Eur J Work Organ Psychol 15:218–240CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Sauter C, Kowalski JT, Stein M et al (2019) Effects of a workplace-based sleep health program on sleep in members of the German armed forces. J Clin Sleep Med 15:417–429CrossRef Sauter C, Kowalski JT, Stein M et al (2019) Effects of a workplace-based sleep health program on sleep in members of the German armed forces. J Clin Sleep Med 15:417–429CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Sletten TL, Magee M, Murray JM et al (2018) Efficacy of melatonin with behavioural sleep-wake scheduling for delayed sleep-wake phase disorder: A double-blind, randomised clinical trial. PLoS Med 15:e1002587CrossRef Sletten TL, Magee M, Murray JM et al (2018) Efficacy of melatonin with behavioural sleep-wake scheduling for delayed sleep-wake phase disorder: A double-blind, randomised clinical trial. PLoS Med 15:e1002587CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M et al (2006) Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol Int 23:497–509CrossRef Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M et al (2006) Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol Int 23:497–509CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang B, Wing YK (2006) Sex differences in insomnia: a meta-analysis. Sleep 29:85–93CrossRef Zhang B, Wing YK (2006) Sex differences in insomnia: a meta-analysis. Sleep 29:85–93CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang Z, Cajochen C, Khatami R (2019) Social jetlag and chronotypes in the Chinese population: analysis of data recorded by wearable devices. J Med Internet Res 21:e13482CrossRef Zhang Z, Cajochen C, Khatami R (2019) Social jetlag and chronotypes in the Chinese population: analysis of data recorded by wearable devices. J Med Internet Res 21:e13482CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Zung WW (1971) A rating instrument for anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics 12:371–379CrossRef Zung WW (1971) A rating instrument for anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics 12:371–379CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Zung WW (1965) A self-rating depression scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry 12:63–70CrossRef Zung WW (1965) A self-rating depression scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry 12:63–70CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
On the influence of the day of the week on objective and self-rated sleep quality of adults
verfasst von
C. Sauter
H. Dorn
H. Danker-Hopfe
Publikationsdatum
07.12.2020
Verlag
Springer Medizin
Erschienen in
Somnologie / Ausgabe 2/2021
Print ISSN: 1432-9123
Elektronische ISSN: 1439-054X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11818-020-00288-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2021

Somnologie 2/2021 Zur Ausgabe

Mitteilungen der DGSM

Mitteilungen der DGSM

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Niedriger diastolischer Blutdruck erhöht Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Komplikationen

25.04.2024 Hypotonie Nachrichten

Wenn unter einer medikamentösen Hochdrucktherapie der diastolische Blutdruck in den Keller geht, steigt das Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Ereignisse: Darauf deutet eine Sekundäranalyse der SPRINT-Studie hin.

Frühe Alzheimertherapie lohnt sich

25.04.2024 AAN-Jahrestagung 2024 Nachrichten

Ist die Tau-Last noch gering, scheint der Vorteil von Lecanemab besonders groß zu sein. Und beginnen Erkrankte verzögert mit der Behandlung, erreichen sie nicht mehr die kognitive Leistung wie bei einem früheren Start. Darauf deuten neue Analysen der Phase-3-Studie Clarity AD.

Viel Bewegung in der Parkinsonforschung

25.04.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Neue arznei- und zellbasierte Ansätze, Frühdiagnose mit Bewegungssensoren, Rückenmarkstimulation gegen Gehblockaden – in der Parkinsonforschung tut sich einiges. Auf dem Deutschen Parkinsonkongress ging es auch viel um technische Innovationen.

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.