Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Somnologie 4/2023

Open Access 06.06.2023 | Original studies

Onsite versus home-office: differences in sleep patterns according to workplace

verfasst von: Naomi Staller, Mirja Quante, Helen Deutsch, Christoph Randler

Erschienen in: Somnologie | Ausgabe 4/2023

Abstract

Background and objective

This study aimed to evaluate the sleep patterns of students and employees working onsite versus those working from home during the COVID-19 pandemic using actigraphy.

Methods

A total of 75 students/employees (onsite: N = 40, home-office: N = 35; age range: 19–56 years; 32% male; 42.7% students, 49.3% employees) were studied between December 2020 and January 2022 using actigraphy, a sleep diary, and an online questionnaire assessing sociodemographics and morningness–eveningness. Independent-sample t-tests, paired-sample tests, and a multivariate general linear model adjusting for age (fixed factors: sex and work environment) were applied.

Results

Overall, onsite workers had significantly earlier rise times (7:05 [SD: 1:11] versus 7:44 [1:08] hours) and midpoints of sleep (2:57 [0:58] versus 3:33 [0:58] hours) on weekdays compared to home-office workers. Sleep efficiency, sleep duration, variability of sleep timing, and social jetlag did not differ between the groups.

Discussion

Home-office workers showed a delay in sleep timing that did not affect any other sleep parameters such as sleep efficiency or nighttime sleep duration. The work environment had only marginal impact on sleep patterns and thus sleep health in this sample. Sleep timing variability did not differ between groups.
Sleep is critical for regular functioning of physical and mental health in humans [1]. It is important for maintaining long-term health through physical/cerebral functioning (sleep deprivation causes, e.g., an increased prevalence of noncommunicable diseases such as obesity [2] or cardiovascular problems [3]), and short term health by, e.g., minimizing the risk of traffic accidents related to drowsiness [4, 5].
With the onset of the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic, mental stress increased enormously due to fears about the future, about relatives, or about one’s own health. Since stress and anxiety have a strong negative impact on sleep, some research groups have reported a deterioration in sleep health (see [6] for a review). In addition, increased workloads in some professions (e.g., medical staff) have also taken their toll, leading to more sleep problems and poorer sleep health [6].
The pandemic changed the professional situation of students and employees. Remote working (hereinafter also referred to as the situation of students studying at home) as well as the closure of many business sectors (e.g., body-related services) affected everyday life, including sleep. It was shown that the shift to remote working and the associated later work start allowed participants to adjust their sleep times to their own circadian rhythm. As a result, students and employees were able to synchronize their biological and social clocks, thereby improving sleep health (e.g., [715]). In addition to the positive effects of synchronizing social and biological timing, on-demand teaching has been shown to lead to a loss of sleep timing regularity [14], which, in turn, can have negative effects on sleep and overall health (e.g., [16, 17]). In contrast, Ramírez-Contreras and colleagues [18] reported that the hybrid system of virtual and face-to-face activity was beneficial for students in terms of sleep regularity and eating habits, resulting in longer sleep duration on weekdays and reductions in sleep deprivation. Furthermore, increased screen time due to remote working may also have disrupted and delayed sleep onset due to more blue light exposure (e.g., [19, 20]) and less outdoor light exposure [21]. However, increased time spent outdoors in home-office workers has also been reported [22]. So far, there are few data on the effect of the working environment on sleep patterns during the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany.
The present study builds on the preliminary data and literature available and compares the sleep patterns of participants in two different working environments. We hypothesized later/more irregular sleep timing and midpoints of sleep, longer nighttime sleep duration, worse sleep efficiency, and less social jetlag (SJL) in home-office compared to onsite-working students/employees.

Methods

Setting

This study was approved by the Eberhard Karls University’s ethics committee (Faculty of Economics and Social Sciences: no. A.Z.: A2.5.4-157_ns).

Data collection

Data were collected from December 14, 2020, to January 18, 2022. Participants were recruited through university mailing lists and social media postings (Facebook, Instagram, WhatsApp). The study included a questionnaire that was available online on the SoSciSurvey platform (https://​www.​soscisurvey.​de/​). This hosting platform complies with European Union data protection regulations. Potential study participants were informed about the voluntary nature of participation, the possibility to stop data collection at any time without consequences, and the study compensation of 25 € in cash. Informed consent was obtained before data collection began. Exclusion criteria were shiftwork, participants who planned to travel across time zones during data collection, and participants who were not fluent in German.

Variables and instruments

Participants reported information on their demographics (such as the work environment (“working onsite,” hereafter: “onsite group”; and “working from home,” hereafter: “home-office group”), age, sex, number of children in their household, occupation), details of their sleep behavior, and a morningness–eveningness questionnaire (the Morningness–Eveningness Stability Scale improved [MESSi]).

MESSi

The MESSi consists of three subscales: morning affect (MA), eveningness (EV), and distinctness (DI). MA is designed to measure the affective component of morning orientation, while EV measures the affective component of evening orientation. In comparison to the diurnal changes measured by the MA and EV subscales, the DI subscale measures fluctuating changes in the expression of MA and EV [23].

Actigraphy

Participants were asked to wear an ActiGraph (GT3X+, ActiGraph, Pensacola, FL, USA) for 7 consecutive days in order to measure sleep–wake patterns. The device is worn on the nondominant wrist similar to a wristwatch. At the end of the measurement week, participants returned their ActiGraphs and the data were downloaded using the accompanying software (ActiLife). Activity counts were collected in 1‑min epochs over a continuous period of 1 week (7 × 24 h). Any recordings that did not meet the minimum requirements of a) at least 10 h of valid signal during waking hours and b) a total of at least 4 valid days were excluded. Sleep–wake patterns were scored manually using the ActiLife software as described by Mitchell and colleagues [24]. Briefly, sleep onset and offset times were identified by diary report and observation of a sharp decrease/increase in activity. If there were discrepancies of more than 30 min between the sleep diary report and the decrease/increase in activity counts measured by the ActiGraph, sleep phases were determined visually based on the activity changes. We used the Cole–Kripke algorithm to estimate sleep and wake periods [25]. Non-wearing times were determined using the Troiano algorithm and compared with the sleep diary [26].

Sleep diary

Participants completed a sleep diary during the week of actigraphic monitoring. Participants were asked to note bedtimes, naps during the day, awakenings during the night (duration and frequency), and times when the ActiGraph was taken off (e.g., to take a shower).

Midpoint of sleep as a measure of chronotype

As a second measure of chronotype the corrected midpoint of sleep (MSFsc) as proposed by Roenneberg and colleagues was used [27]. This chronotype measure is based on clock time but adjusts for average individual sleep need [28]. It was shown to be a stable measure of chronotype during the COVID-19 pandemic in a similar German sample from Baden-Württemberg [13]. Throughout this manuscript the abbreviation “MPOS” represents “midpoint of sleep,” while in the following formula, “SDu” stands for “average sleep duration”:
$$\textit{MSFsc}=\mathrm{MPOS}_{\text{weekend}}-0.5\times (SDu_{\,\text{weekend}}-(5\times SDu_{\text{workdays}}+2\times SDu_{\text{weekend}})\div 7)$$

Social jetlag

SJL is a parameter to describe the discrepancy between a person’s social and biological clocks due to circadian preferences [29]. Weekend sleep patterns are thought to reflect the internal biological clock, whereas weekday sleep patterns reflect the social clock. SJL is calculated by subtracting the midpoint of sleep on workdays from the midpoint of sleep on days off:
$$SJL=\mathrm{MPOS}_{\text{weekend}}-M\mathrm{POS}_{\text{workdays}}$$

Sleep efficiency

Sleep efficiency is calculated by dividing the amount of time spent asleep (in minutes) by the total amount of time in bed (in minutes).

Sleep time variability

The variability in sleep times was compared between the groups. This was done by calculating the standard deviation of sleep times for each participant and using this to calculate a group mean. The result shows the average standard deviation of sleep times and is considered the variability of sleep timing in the group.

Statistical analyses

We further analyzed the actigraphy data using pyActigraphy [30], an open-source Python actigraphy analysis and visualization framework. In addition, averages of various sleep parameters were calculated for each subject using the Python data analysis library Pandas [31]. To calculate time averages after midnight, e.g., sleep onset time and midpoint of sleep, we added 24 h to times below an appropriate pivot element before calculating the mean, and then subtracted 24 h afterwards, if necessary. The software and a detailed description are available as open source at https://​github.​com/​spuetz/​actigraphy.​git. Statistical analysis of the actigraphy and questionnaire data was performed using SPSS 29.0 software (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Independent-sample t-tests and paired-sample tests were calculated to compare inter-and intra-individual differences. Furthermore, a multivariate general model was calculated with sex and work environment as fixed factors and age as a covariate to control the reliability of the t-tests. Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Shapiro–Wilk tests were used to test the normal distribution of the MSFsc variable. Levine tests indicated that equal variances could be assumed. Pearson’s chi-squared test was used for descriptive group comparisons.

Results

Descriptive group statistics are presented in Table 1. Exploratory data analysis was performed to identify outliers in the age variable, as this variable has a strong influence on other variables. Outlier limits were defined as ±3 standard deviations from the mean value. Two cases were excluded in each group. This reduced the original sample size from N = 79 to N = 75. Overall, the home-office group was significantly younger than the onsite group. Significantly more participants were students in the home-office group compared to the onsite group. We did not see any difference between groups regarding children in the household or chronotype. The sleep timing variability did not differ between the groups. In terms of intra-individual differences in the variability of sleep times, the onsite group had the lowest standard deviation of 0.17 h for bedtimes and also the highest of 2.32 h. For rise times, the minimum and maximum were 0.2 h and 2.07 h (Table 1). In the home-office group, the minimum for bedtime was 0.2 h and the maximum was 1.9 h, while the rise times were 0.25 h and 2.1 h respectively (see Table 1).
Table 1
Demographics: onsite versus home-office group
Variable
Overall (N = 75)
Onsite group (N = 40)
Home-office group (N = 35)
Age, years, mean (SD)
28.2 (7.7)
30.3 (8.7)*
25.8 (5.6)*
Sex, N (%)
Male
24 (32.0)
15 (37.5)
9 (25.7)
Female
51 (68.0)
25 (62.5)
26 (74.3)
Children in household, N (%)
Yes
5 (6.7)
4 (10.0)
1 (2.9)
No
70 (93.3)
36 (90.0)
34 (97.1)
Profession, N (%)
Student
32 (42.7)
9 (22.5)*
23 (65.7)*
Employee
37 (49.3)
25 (62.5)
12 (34.3)
Not specified
6 (8.0)
6 (15.0)
Morningness, mean (SD)
3.37 (0.79)
3.41 (0.77)
3.32 (0.82)
Eveningness, mean (SD)
3.01 (0.86)
2.97 (0.83)
3.07 (0.90)
Distinctness, mean (SD)
3.31 (0.71)
3.35 (0.71)
3.27 (0.72)
Sleep time variability—bedtimes, mean (SD), max/min [h]
0.87 (0.45), 0.17/2.32
0.92 (0.45), 0.17/2.32
0.83 (0.43), 0.2/2.07
Sleep time variability—rise times, mean (SD), max/min [h]
0.95 (0.43), 0.2/2.1
0.93 (0.45), 0.2/1.9
0.95 (0.43), 0.25/2.1
SD standard deviation
*Significant differences between onsite and home-office (p < 0.05)
Comparisons of sleep patterns are shown in Table 2. No differences were seen in total time in bed, sleep efficiency, and total sleep time on weekdays, weekends, and all days. The home-office group went to bed, reached the midpoint of sleep, and got up significantly later than the onsite group on weekdays. Interestingly, sleep parameters did not differ between groups at weekends. Also, the variability of sleep timing (standard deviations of sleep timing, Table 1) did not differ between the groups. Both groups showed a SJL of about 1 h, which was not significantly different. In addition, there was no difference in chronotype measured by the MESSi scales MA/EV/DI (Table 1) or MSFsc (Table 2) between the two groups. For both the onsite and home-office groups, the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test and the Shapiro–Wilk test indicated a normal distribution of the MSFsc.
Table 2
Comparison of onsite versus home-office groups regarding sleep patterns
Characteristic
Onsite group (N = 40)
Home-office group (N = 35)
p-value
 
Mean
SD
Std. error mean
Mean
SD
Std. error mean
 
Average total time in bed (all days), h
7.65
0.78
0.2
7.73
0.77
0.2
0.636
Average total time in bed (workdays), h
7.55
0.92
0.22
7.63
0.87
0.22
0.685
Average total time in bed (weekend), h
7.90
1.12
0.28
8.00
1.02
0.28
0.695
Average total sleep time (all days), h
6.92
0.8
0.12
6.93
0.68
0.1
0.899
Average total sleep time (workdays), h
6.83
0.83
0.12
6.82
0.77
0.12
0.980
Average total sleep time (weekend), h
7.15
1.28
0.2
7.23
0.88
0.15
0.723
Average sleep efficiency (all days), %
90.4
0.05
0.01
89.9
0.05
0.01
0.605
Average efficiency (workdays), %
90.5
0.04
0.01
89.5
0.05
0.01
0.331
Average efficiency (weekend), %
90.1
0.09
0.01
90.7
0.05
0.01
0.738
Average bedtime (all days)
23:43
0.97
0.15
00:16
1.08
0.18
0.023*
Average bedtime (workdays)
23:28
0.98
0.15
00:07
1.05
0.17
0.008*
Average bedtime (weekend)
00:21
1.3
0.2
00:45
1.33
0.22
0.197
Average midpoint of sleep all days
3:14
0.92
0.13
3:48
1.0
0.17
0.015*
Average midpoint of sleep workdays
2:57
0.97
0.15
3:33
0.97
0.15
0.009*
Average midpoint of sleep weekend
3:59
1.17
0.18
4:25
1.18
0.2
0.122
Average corrected midpoint of sleep
3:51
1.27
0.2
4:17
1.28
0.22
0.158
Average rise time (all days)
7:25
1.0
0.15
8:01
1.05
0.17
0.014*
Average rise time (workdays)
7:05
1.18
0.18
7:44
1.13
0.18
0.019*
Average rise time (weekend)
8:17
1.12
0.17
8:47
1.2
0.2
0.068
Social Jetlag, h
1.03
1.02
0.15
0.87
0.72
0.12
0.403
SD standard deviation
*Significant differences between onsite and home-office (p < 0.05)
As age differed significantly between the onsite and home-office groups (30.3 versus 25.8 years, p = 0.01), a multivariate general linear model adjusting for age was applied (fixed factors: sex and work environment) (Table 3).
Table 3
Results of the general linear model controlling for age as a covariate
Dependent variable
 
F
p-value
Estimated marginal means
Mean
Std. error
Average total time in bed (all days), h
Onsite
0.006
0.938
7.65
0.13
Home-office
7.63
0.13
Average total time in bed (workdays), h
Onsite
0.068
0.795
7.57
0.15
Home-office
7.52
0.17
Average total time in bed (weekend), h
Onsite
0.147
0.702
7.83
0.18
Home-office
7.93
0.2
Average total sleep time (all days), h
Onsite
0.258
0.613
6.88
0.12
Home-office
6.80
0.13
Average total sleep time (workdays), h
Onsite
0.575
0.451
6.82
0.13
Home-office
6.67
0.15
Average total sleep time (weekend), h
Onsite
0.042
0.837
7.05
0.18
Home-office
7.12
0.20
Average efficiency (all days), %
Onsite
0.511
0.477
0.90
0.008
Home-office
0.89
0.008
Average efficiency (workdays), %
Onsite
1.183
0.280
0.90
0.007
Home-office
0.89
0.008
Average efficiency (weekend), %
Onsite
0.006
0.940
0.90
0.012
Home-office
0.90
0.013
Average bedtime (all days)
Onsite
5.034
0.028*
23:51
00:10
Home-office
00:23
00:11
Average bedtime (workdays)
Onsite
7.225
0.009*
23:37
00:10
Home-office
00:15
00:11
Average bedtime (weekend)
Onsite
1.615
0.208
00:26
00:13
Home-office
00:50
00:14
Average midpoint of sleep all days
Onsite
4.522
0.037*
03:21
00:09
Home-office
03:50
00:10
Average midpoint of sleep workdays
Onsite
5.022
0.028*
03:04
00:09
Home-office
03:35
00:10
Average midpoint of sleep weekend
Onsite
2.103
0.151
04:01
00:11
Home-office
04:27
00:13
Average corrected midpoint of sleep
Onsite
1.362
0.247
03:55
00:13
Home-office
04:17
00:14
Average rise time (all days)
Onsite
4.338
0.041*
07:32
00:10
Home-office
08:03
00:11
Average rise time (workdays)
Onsite
3.528
0.064
07:14
00:11
Home-office
07:45
00:12
Average rise time (weekend)
Onsite
3.173
0.079
08:18
00:12
Home-office
08:49
00:13
Social jetlag, h
Onsite
0.179
0.673
0.95
0.15
Home-office
0.85
0.17
Means of squares are given as a decimal number
*Significant differences between onsite and home-office
In comparison with the independent-sample test (Table 2), the general linear model confirmed the significant findings. The only exception was the variable “average rise time workdays,” which was no longer significant in the general linear model.
Sleep parameters on weekdays were also compared with weekend days to investigate intra-individual differences within each group using paired-sample t-tests (Table 4). Intra-individual differences were found in both the onsite and home-office groups. The sleep parameters of the onsite group differed significantly between weekdays and weekend days with regard to sleep efficiency, bedtime, total sleep time, rise time, and midpoint of sleep. The home-office group differed in the same sleep parameters except for total sleep time.
Table 4
Intra-individual differences between sleep parameters on weekdays versus weekends
Characteristic
Onsite group (N = 40)
Home-office group (N = 35)
Mean
SD
p-value
Mean
SD
p-value
Average total time in bed
−0.33
1.30
0.208
−0.35
1.10
0.058
Average total sleep time
−0.32
1.28
0.037*
−0.40
0.97
0.063
Average efficiency
0.004
0.07
< 0.001*
−0.01
0.04
< 0.001*
Average bedtime
−0.87
1.03
< 0.001*
−0.63
0.75
< 0.001*
Average midpoint of sleep
−1.03
1.02
< 0.001*
−0.87
0.72
< 0.001*
Average rise time
−1.20
1.30
0.026*
−1.05
1.05
< 0.001*
SD standard deviation
*Significant differences between weekdays and weekend days (p < 0.05)
Onsite workers had a higher sleep efficiency on weekdays compared to weekends, while home-office workers had a higher sleep efficiency on weekends compared to weekdays. Average bedtime and midpoint of sleep were significantly later at weekends compared to weekdays for both groups.

Discussion

In summary, it was found that onsite workers had significantly earlier sleep timing and midpoints of sleep on weekdays compared to home-office workers, whereas there were no clinically meaningful differences regarding sleep efficiency, sleep duration, and variability in sleep timing. Importantly, also SJL did not differ between the groups. The results of the group comparison showed that the onsite group was significantly older than the home-office group. The group means differed by 4.5 years. This age difference is particularly important when considering the present results. Chronotype, as an individual difference trait, is subject to changes in expression over the course of a person’s life [32]. While young children tend to be morning types, this tendency changes strongly during adolescence [32]. Adolescents show a distinct delay in chronotype, peaking at the age of 20 years [27]. With the end of adolescence, the daytime preference shifts again and begins to advance. Roenneberg and colleagues [27] showed a continuous advancement of chronotype from the age of 20 until about 65 years. There is also a steeper regression line between the ages of 20 and 45 years than between 45 and 65 years. Most adults do not belong to an extreme chronotype and are therefore described as neither type [32]. In the sample of Randler and colleagues [32], a distribution of 21.1% evening, 71.7% neither, and 7.2% morning type was reported for the 26-year-old subjects. Among the 30-year-old subjects, 11.9% were classified as evening type, 74.8% as neither, and 13.3% as morning type. This means that the 30-year-old subjects were 9.2% less likely to be classified as evening type than the 26-year-old subjects. As the average age of the present sample groups differed by about 4.5 years (mean 25.8 years in home-office, mean 30.3 years in onsite), it could be assumed that also the chronotypes differ significantly from each other. Participants in the home-office group may therefore be more evening oriented, reflecting a higher proportion of evening types in this group than in the onsite group.
In this study two different measures of chronotype were used (MSFsc: clock time-based measure, MESSi: continuous morningness-eveningness measure). The analysis of the MSFsc and the MESSi showed no significant differences between the groups. Therefore, the expression of chronotype did not differ between the groups.
Regarding the MSFsc variable, Roenneberg and colleagues [33] described a comparable shape and width of the distribution in all age groups, with a varying mean value. Graphical comparison of the group histograms (see Supplement 1) with the expected values ([33], N ≥ 55,000) showed that the distribution in the home-office group had a similar shape. In the onsite group, two equally high peaks existed, one at 3:00 a.m. and one at 4:30 a.m., showing the shape of the distribution to be different from the reference. In the sample of Roenneberg and colleagues [33], the 26-year-old subjects showed peaks at around 4:45 a.m. compared to 4:17 a.m. for the home-office group. For the 30-year-old participants, Roenneberg and colleagues [33] reported an average MSFsc of approximately 4:30 a.m., compared to 3:51 a.m. for the onsite group. Taken together, these two discrepancies led us to consider the MSFsc of the onsite group to be unrepresentative.
In summary, a) the expected distribution of chronotype based on the age groups of the samples and b) the finding that the distribution of chronotype in the onsite group cannot be considered representative suggest that the chronotypes of the groups can generally be expected to differ. The younger group would normally have a higher prevalence of evening types/more pronounced eveningness, although this was not the case in the present samples.
Increased eveningness is associated with a higher risk of SJL. SJL has repeatedly been shown to be particularly detrimental to public health (e.g., cardiovascular outcomes, psychiatric disorders, obesity [34]). However, softer factors such as productivity at work are also significantly affected by SJL. To test whether the measured SJL in the home-office group was within the expected range, the results were compared to an earlier sample of the authors’ research group [13]. In that study, only people who worked in home-office during the COVID-19 pandemic had been surveyed. The mean age of the sample was reported to be ~ 28.6 years (SD: ~ 10.5 years), which was not different compared to the sample of the present study (p = 0.11; Supplement 2). With regard to SJL, the means of the present home-office group (0.80, SD: 0.73 h) and the previous sample (0.82; SD: 0.70 h) were also not significantly different (p = 0.87; Supplement 2). Furthermore, the chronotype within the two groups measured by MSFsc did not differ (p = 0.21; Supplement 2). It can therefore be assumed that the SJL in the present sample is representative for this age group working from home. In another sample of the same age who were not working from home, the SJL was almost 1 h higher (1.78 h) compared to the present sample with 0.8 h [35]. It could be postulated that the implementation of home-office might potentially be beneficial for young people to reduce SJL. The onsite group had an average SJL of 1.03 h, which was also compared to other samples in the current literature. As no age-matching sample could be found in the literature, the present results were compared to slightly younger and older samples. Both showed a meaningful but equal age difference compared to the present sample. Interestingly, the SJL (1.4 ± 1.0, [36]; 1.33 ± 0.88 h, [37]) of the two studies did not significantly differ (p = 0.42, t = 0.78, df = 727, Standard Error of Difference (SED) = 0.09). Therefore, these results were used as representative reference values for SJL in the current sample. Even the value closer to the current study [37] differed significantly from the SJL reported herein (p = 0.04, t = 2.07, df = 637, SED = 0.15; Supplement 2). This led to the conclusion that the SJL of the onsite group cannot be considered representative, as it was significantly below the expected range.
Although the SJL of the present two study groups were not significantly different, there was a tendency towards lower SJL in the home-office group. This tendency was reinforced by an overrepresentation of low SJL in the onsite group, while the home-office group showed more similar values to age-comparable samples. Therefore, the current results suggest that working from home might help to reduce SJL.
The present study differs from the authors’ previous studies which examined only home-office participants and not two samples in different working environments. As spatial flexibility, in addition to temporal flexibility [14], may play a role in the increasing irregularity of students’ sleep schedules, a possible relationship was investigated here.
The sleep timing of the two groups on workdays differed significantly. The home-office group went to bed later and also got up later. However, both groups had similar fluctuations regarding sleep timing variability and no difference in overall nightly sleep duration. Thus, home-office had no negative influence on sleep variability and duration in this sample. There was also no influence of spatial flexibility/working environment on the loss of sleep duration/regularity observed. Instead, data support the hypothesis that the ability to work with unrestricted temporal flexibility is the main influencing factor of the increased sleep timing regularity in students, as discussed in one of the authors’ previous studies [14] by excluding another possible influencing factor.
Sleep efficiency did not differ between the two groups. However, a difference within the groups could be detected. In the onsite group, sleep efficiency was higher on weekdays than at weekends. The opposite was true for the home-office group, where sleep efficiency was higher at weekends. The reason for this could be explained by the way the sleep efficiency variable is calculated. On weekdays, participants in the onsite group could have either gotten up immediately after waking up (e.g., because of the increased pressure to get up on time in order to commute to work compared to the weekend) or gone straight to sleep after going to bed (e.g., because of the increased exhaustion from onsite work with probably fewer breaks compared to the weekend). This would explain the high ratio of total sleep time to time in bed, which corresponds to a higher sleep efficiency. At the weekend, on the other hand, they may have stayed in bed even longer although they were already awake, or it took longer for them to fall asleep, resulting in lower sleep efficiency. In contrast, the opposite was true for the home-office group, with a higher sleep efficiency at weekends compared to weekdays. Although statistically significant, this effect was small and not clinically meaningful.

Conclusion

In summary, it was found that home-office workers had a delay in sleep timing that did not have a clinically meaningful effect on any other sleep parameter such as sleep efficiency or nightly sleep duration. Compared to values in the current literature, SJL seemed to be reduced in the home-office group. Overall, the work environment had only marginal impact on sleep patterns and thus sleep health in this sample. Sleep timing variability did not differ between groups.

Limitations and strengths

Strengths of the study were the objective data collection via actigraphy and the assessment of different sleep variables covering sleep quantity, quality, and circadian patterns. The results of the study were limited by the fact that the sample was relatively small and there was a significant age difference between the groups. In addition, students were overrepresented in the home-office group. Moreover, although the data were collected during the same seasons, which reduces seasonal effects, they were collected over a long period of time, this may have influenced the data in terms of the evolution of the pandemic (different severity of lockdown regimes, psychological effects due to different familiarity and understanding).

Declarations

Conflict of interest

N. Staller, M. Quante, H. Deutsch, and C. Randler declare that they have no competing interests.
For this article no studies with animals were performed by any of the authors. All studies mentioned were in accordance with the ethical standards indicated in each case.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

Somnologie

Print-Titel

  • Aktuelles, gesichertes Fachwissen zu Ätiologie, Pathophysiologie, Differentialdiagnostik und Therapie von Schlafstörungen

• Multidisziplinärer Ansatz

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Dent – Das Online-Abo der Zahnmedizin

Online-Abonnement

Mit e.Dent erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen zahnmedizinischen Fortbildungen und unseren zahnmedizinischen und ausgesuchten medizinischen Zeitschriften.

Weitere Produktempfehlungen anzeigen
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Ramar K, Malhotra RK, Carden KA et al (2021) Sleep is essential to health: an American Academy of Sleep Medicine position statement. J Clin Sleep Med 17(10):2115–2119CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ramar K, Malhotra RK, Carden KA et al (2021) Sleep is essential to health: an American Academy of Sleep Medicine position statement. J Clin Sleep Med 17(10):2115–2119CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Sun W, Huang Y, Wang Z, Yu Y, Lau A, Ali G, Shan G (2015) Sleep duration associated with body mass index among Chinese adults. Sleep Med 16(5):612–616CrossRefPubMed Sun W, Huang Y, Wang Z, Yu Y, Lau A, Ali G, Shan G (2015) Sleep duration associated with body mass index among Chinese adults. Sleep Med 16(5):612–616CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Sabanayagam C, Shankar A (2010) Sleep duration and cardiovascular disease: results from the National Health Interview Survey. Sleep 33(8):1037–1042CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Sabanayagam C, Shankar A (2010) Sleep duration and cardiovascular disease: results from the National Health Interview Survey. Sleep 33(8):1037–1042CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Hillman DR, Lack LC (2013) Public health implications of sleep loss: the community burden. Med J Aust 199:S7–S10CrossRefPubMed Hillman DR, Lack LC (2013) Public health implications of sleep loss: the community burden. Med J Aust 199:S7–S10CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Philip P, Chaufton C, Orriols L, Lagarde E, Amoros E, Laumon B, Sagaspe P (2014) Complaints of poor sleep and risk of traffic accidents: a population-based case-control study. Plos One 9(12):e114102CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Philip P, Chaufton C, Orriols L, Lagarde E, Amoros E, Laumon B, Sagaspe P (2014) Complaints of poor sleep and risk of traffic accidents: a population-based case-control study. Plos One 9(12):e114102CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Lin YN, Liu ZR, Li SQ, Li CX, Zhang L, Li N, Li QY (2021) Burden of sleep disturbance during COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review. NSS 13:933CrossRef Lin YN, Liu ZR, Li SQ, Li CX, Zhang L, Li N, Li QY (2021) Burden of sleep disturbance during COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review. NSS 13:933CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Conroy DA, Hadler NL, Cho E, Moreira A, MacKenzie C, Swanson LM, Goldstein CA (2021) The effects of COVID-19 stay-at-home order on sleep, health, and working patterns: a survey study of US health care workers. J Clin Sleep Med 17(2):185–191CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Conroy DA, Hadler NL, Cho E, Moreira A, MacKenzie C, Swanson LM, Goldstein CA (2021) The effects of COVID-19 stay-at-home order on sleep, health, and working patterns: a survey study of US health care workers. J Clin Sleep Med 17(2):185–191CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Hallman DM, Januario LB, Mathiassen SE, Heiden M, Svensson S, Bergström G (2021) Working from home during the COVID-19 outbreak in Sweden: effects on 24‑h time-use in office workers. Bmc Public Health 21(1):1–10CrossRef Hallman DM, Januario LB, Mathiassen SE, Heiden M, Svensson S, Bergström G (2021) Working from home during the COVID-19 outbreak in Sweden: effects on 24‑h time-use in office workers. Bmc Public Health 21(1):1–10CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Korman M, Tkachev V, Reis C, Komada Y, Kitamura S, Gubin D, Roenneberg T (2020) COVID-19-mandated social restrictions unveil the impact of social time pressure on sleep and body clock. Sci Rep 10(1):1–10CrossRef Korman M, Tkachev V, Reis C, Komada Y, Kitamura S, Gubin D, Roenneberg T (2020) COVID-19-mandated social restrictions unveil the impact of social time pressure on sleep and body clock. Sci Rep 10(1):1–10CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Leone MJ, Sigman M, Golombek DA (2020) Effects of lockdown on human sleep and chronotype during the COVID-19 pandemic. Curr Biol 30(16):R930–R931CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Leone MJ, Sigman M, Golombek DA (2020) Effects of lockdown on human sleep and chronotype during the COVID-19 pandemic. Curr Biol 30(16):R930–R931CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Raman S, Coogan AN (2022) Effects of societal-level COVID-19 mitigation measures on the timing and quality of sleep in Ireland. Sleep Med 91:179–184CrossRefPubMed Raman S, Coogan AN (2022) Effects of societal-level COVID-19 mitigation measures on the timing and quality of sleep in Ireland. Sleep Med 91:179–184CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Vollmer C, Jankowski KS (2022) COVID-19-lockdown delayed sleep timing in Tyrolean teachers. Chronobiol Int 39(12):1601–1610CrossRefPubMed Vollmer C, Jankowski KS (2022) COVID-19-lockdown delayed sleep timing in Tyrolean teachers. Chronobiol Int 39(12):1601–1610CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Huang T, Mariani S, Redline S (2020) Sleep irregularity and risk of cardiovascular events: the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. J Am Coll Cardiol 75(9):991–999CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Huang T, Mariani S, Redline S (2020) Sleep irregularity and risk of cardiovascular events: the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. J Am Coll Cardiol 75(9):991–999CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Ramírez-Contreras C, Zerón-Rugerio MF, Izquierdo-Pulido M (2022) Life before and after COVID-19: the ‘new normal’benefits the regularity of daily sleep and eating routines among college students. Nutrients 14(2):351CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ramírez-Contreras C, Zerón-Rugerio MF, Izquierdo-Pulido M (2022) Life before and after COVID-19: the ‘new normal’benefits the regularity of daily sleep and eating routines among college students. Nutrients 14(2):351CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Saxena R, Parmar N, Prabhleen K, Allen T (2021) Effect of screen-time on sleep pattern and dietary habits among college-going students in COVID-19 pandemic. Indian J Community Health 33:1CrossRef Saxena R, Parmar N, Prabhleen K, Allen T (2021) Effect of screen-time on sleep pattern and dietary habits among college-going students in COVID-19 pandemic. Indian J Community Health 33:1CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Korman M, Tkachev V, Reis C, Komada Y, Kitamura S, Gubin D, Roenneberg T (2022) Outdoor daylight exposure and longer sleep promote wellbeing under COVID-19 mandated restrictions. J Sleep Res 31(2):e13471CrossRefPubMed Korman M, Tkachev V, Reis C, Komada Y, Kitamura S, Gubin D, Roenneberg T (2022) Outdoor daylight exposure and longer sleep promote wellbeing under COVID-19 mandated restrictions. J Sleep Res 31(2):e13471CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Randler C, Tryjanowski P, Jokimäki J, Kaisanlahti-Jokimäki ML, Staller N (2020) SARS-CoV2 (COVID-19) Pandemic lockdown influences nature-based recreational activity: The case of birders. IJERPH 17(19):7310CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Randler C, Tryjanowski P, Jokimäki J, Kaisanlahti-Jokimäki ML, Staller N (2020) SARS-CoV2 (COVID-19) Pandemic lockdown influences nature-based recreational activity: The case of birders. IJERPH 17(19):7310CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Randler C, Díaz-Morales JF, Rahafar A, Vollmer C (2016) Morningness–eveningness and amplitude–development and validation of an improved composite scale to measure circadian preference and stability (MESSi). Chronobiol Int 33(7):832–848CrossRefPubMed Randler C, Díaz-Morales JF, Rahafar A, Vollmer C (2016) Morningness–eveningness and amplitude–development and validation of an improved composite scale to measure circadian preference and stability (MESSi). Chronobiol Int 33(7):832–848CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Mitchell JA, Quante M, Godbole S, James P, Hipp JA, Marinac CR, Kerr J (2017) Variation in actigraphy-estimated rest-activity patterns by demographic factors. Chronobiol Int 34(8):1042–1056CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Mitchell JA, Quante M, Godbole S, James P, Hipp JA, Marinac CR, Kerr J (2017) Variation in actigraphy-estimated rest-activity patterns by demographic factors. Chronobiol Int 34(8):1042–1056CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Cole RJ, Kripke DF, Gruen W, Mullaney DJ, Gillin JC (1992) Automatic sleep/wake identification from wrist activity. Sleep 15(5):461–469CrossRefPubMed Cole RJ, Kripke DF, Gruen W, Mullaney DJ, Gillin JC (1992) Automatic sleep/wake identification from wrist activity. Sleep 15(5):461–469CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Troiano RP, Berrigan D, Dodd KW, Masse LC, Tilert T, McDowell M (2008) Physical activity in the United States measured by accelerometer. Med Sci Sports Exerc 40(1):181CrossRefPubMed Troiano RP, Berrigan D, Dodd KW, Masse LC, Tilert T, McDowell M (2008) Physical activity in the United States measured by accelerometer. Med Sci Sports Exerc 40(1):181CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Pramstaller PP, Ricken J, Havel M, Guth A, Merrow M (2004) A marker for the end of adolescence. Curr Biol 14(24):R1038–R1039CrossRefPubMed Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Pramstaller PP, Ricken J, Havel M, Guth A, Merrow M (2004) A marker for the end of adolescence. Curr Biol 14(24):R1038–R1039CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Zavada A, Gordijn MC, Beersma DG, Daan S, Roenneberg T (2005) Comparison of the Munich chronotype questionnaire with the Horne-Östberg’s morningness-eveningness score. Chronobiol Int 22(2):267–278CrossRefPubMed Zavada A, Gordijn MC, Beersma DG, Daan S, Roenneberg T (2005) Comparison of the Munich chronotype questionnaire with the Horne-Östberg’s morningness-eveningness score. Chronobiol Int 22(2):267–278CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, Roenneberg T (2006) Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol Int 23(1–2):497–509CrossRefPubMed Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, Roenneberg T (2006) Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol Int 23(1–2):497–509CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat McKinney W (2012) Python for data analysis: Data wrangling with Pandas, NumPy, and IPython. O’Reilly Media, Inc McKinney W (2012) Python for data analysis: Data wrangling with Pandas, NumPy, and IPython. O’Reilly Media, Inc
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Randler C, Faßl C, Kalb N (2017) From Lark to Owl: developmental changes in morningness-eveningness from new-borns to early adulthood. Sci Rep 7(1):1–8CrossRef Randler C, Faßl C, Kalb N (2017) From Lark to Owl: developmental changes in morningness-eveningness from new-borns to early adulthood. Sci Rep 7(1):1–8CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Juda M, Kantermann T, Allebrandt K, Gordijn M, Merrow M (2007) Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Med Rev 11(6):429–438CrossRefPubMed Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Juda M, Kantermann T, Allebrandt K, Gordijn M, Merrow M (2007) Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Med Rev 11(6):429–438CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Caliandro R, Streng AA, van Kerkhof LW, van der Horst GT, Chaves I (2021) Social jetlag and related risks for human health: A timely review. Nutrients 13(12):4543CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Caliandro R, Streng AA, van Kerkhof LW, van der Horst GT, Chaves I (2021) Social jetlag and related risks for human health: A timely review. Nutrients 13(12):4543CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Randler C, Vollmer C, Kalb N, Itzek-Greulich H (2019) Breakpoints of time in bed, midpoint of sleep, and social jetlag from infancy to early adulthood. Sleep Med 57:80–86CrossRefPubMed Randler C, Vollmer C, Kalb N, Itzek-Greulich H (2019) Breakpoints of time in bed, midpoint of sleep, and social jetlag from infancy to early adulthood. Sleep Med 57:80–86CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Choi SJ, Joo EY, Hong SB (2016) Sleep–wake pattern, chronotype and seizures in patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy Res 120:19–24CrossRefPubMed Choi SJ, Joo EY, Hong SB (2016) Sleep–wake pattern, chronotype and seizures in patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy Res 120:19–24CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Hayes JF, Schumacher LM, Lanoye A, LaRose JG, Tate DF, Espeland MA, Wing RR (2022) Persistent, high levels of social jetlag predict poor weight outcomes in a weight gain prevention study for young adults. J Behav Med 45(5):794–803CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hayes JF, Schumacher LM, Lanoye A, LaRose JG, Tate DF, Espeland MA, Wing RR (2022) Persistent, high levels of social jetlag predict poor weight outcomes in a weight gain prevention study for young adults. J Behav Med 45(5):794–803CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Onsite versus home-office: differences in sleep patterns according to workplace
verfasst von
Naomi Staller
Mirja Quante
Helen Deutsch
Christoph Randler
Publikationsdatum
06.06.2023
Verlag
Springer Medizin
Erschienen in
Somnologie / Ausgabe 4/2023
Print ISSN: 1432-9123
Elektronische ISSN: 1439-054X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11818-023-00408-5

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2023

Somnologie 4/2023 Zur Ausgabe

Mitteilungen der DGSM

Mitteilungen der DGSM

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Schützt Olivenöl vor dem Tod durch Demenz?

10.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Konsumieren Menschen täglich 7 Gramm Olivenöl, ist ihr Risiko, an einer Demenz zu sterben, um mehr als ein Viertel reduziert – und dies weitgehend unabhängig von ihrer sonstigen Ernährung. Dafür sprechen Auswertungen zweier großer US-Studien.

Bluttest erkennt Parkinson schon zehn Jahre vor der Diagnose

10.05.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Ein Bluttest kann abnorm aggregiertes Alpha-Synuclein bei einigen Menschen schon zehn Jahre vor Beginn der motorischen Parkinsonsymptome nachweisen. Mit einem solchen Test lassen sich möglicherweise Prodromalstadien erfassen und die Betroffenen früher behandeln.

Darf man die Behandlung eines Neonazis ablehnen?

08.05.2024 Gesellschaft Nachrichten

In einer Leseranfrage in der Zeitschrift Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology möchte ein anonymer Dermatologe bzw. eine anonyme Dermatologin wissen, ob er oder sie einen Patienten behandeln muss, der eine rassistische Tätowierung trägt.

Wartezeit nicht kürzer, aber Arbeit flexibler

Psychotherapie Medizin aktuell

Fünf Jahren nach der Neugestaltung der Psychotherapie-Richtlinie wurden jetzt die Effekte der vorgenommenen Änderungen ausgewertet. Das Hauptziel der Novellierung war eine kürzere Wartezeit auf Therapieplätze. Dieses Ziel wurde nicht erreicht, es gab jedoch positive Auswirkungen auf andere Bereiche.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.