Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Geriatrics 1/2019

Open Access 01.12.2019 | Research article

Role of gait speed and grip strength in predicting 10-year cognitive decline among community-dwelling older people

verfasst von: Ming-Yueh Chou, Yukiko Nishita, Takeshi Nakagawa, Chikako Tange, Makiko Tomida, Hiroshi Shimokata, Rei Otsuka, Liang-Kung Chen, Hidenori Arai

Erschienen in: BMC Geriatrics | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Background

The gait speed and handgrip strength represented the core determinants of physical frailty and sarcopenia, which were reported to be associated with cognitive impairment and decline. Different physical measures might differentially affect cognitive changes, such as higher-level cognitive change and global cognitive decline. This study examined the differential associations of gait speed and handgrip strength with 10-year cognitive changes among community-dwelling older people.

Methods

Participants aged 60 years and over living in the community were invited for study. Gait speed and handgrip strength were classified into 5 groups based on quintiles at baseline. Cognitive functions were assessed using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) every 2 years from baseline for a period of 10 years. Linear mixed effects models were used to determine the role of gait speed and handgrip strength in the prediction of 10-year cognitive changes by adjusting covariates, including age, gender, education, depressive symptoms, marital status, smoking status, instrumental activities of daily life (IADL), Charlson Comorbidity Index (CCI), and body mass index (BMI) at baseline.

Results

A total of 1096 participants were enrolled in the study. The mean age was 69.4 ± 5.8 years and 50.9% were male. The slowest gait speed group showed a significantly greater decline in the DSST scores over 10 years than the highest group (estimate = 0.28 and P = 0.003), but not in the MMSE scores (estimate = 0.05 and P = 0.078). The lowest handgrip strength group showed a significantly greater decline in the MMSE scores than the highest group (estimate = 0.06 and P = 0.039) and in the DSST scores than the highest two quintiles (estimate = 0.20 and P = 0.033 for the fourth quintile; estimate = 0.20 and P = 0.040 for the highest quintile) over 10-year follow-up.

Conclusions

A slow gait speed could predict 10-year cognitive decline using DSST, and a low handgrip strength could predict 10-year cognitive decline using MMSE in addition to DSST. Thus both physical measures are lined to cognitive decline but there may be different mechanisms between brain and physical functions.
Hinweise
Ming-Yueh Chou and Yukiko Nishita contributed equally to this work.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
BMI
Body mass index
CCI
Charlson Comorbidity Index
CESD
Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale
DSST
Digit Symbol Substitution Test
IADL
Instrumental activities of daily life
JWAIS-R-SF
The Japanese Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised Short Form
MMSE
Mini-Mental State Examination
NILS-LSA
The National Institute for Longevity Science – Longitudinal Study of Aging
SD
Standard deviation
TMIG-IC
The Instrumental Self-Maintenance subscale of the Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Gerontology Index of Competence

Background

Dementia is a global health issue and is accompanied by a loss of cognitive capacity that is severe enough to affect social participation and the capacity for independent living [1, 2]. Several studies have reported that cognitive decline may predict the development of dementia [3, 4]. Identifying the early markers of cognitive decline may help identify persons with high risks of dementia and allow for the prevention or delay of the process of developing dementia [5, 6]. Potentially modifiable risk factors in the development of cognitive decline or impairment have been well established, and among these, physical activity has been considered a factor of interest in recent studies [79]. Additionally, several studies have revealed that physical function precedes cognitive decline [1013].
The gait speed and handgrip strength represented the core determinants of physical frailty and sarcopenia, which were reported to be associated with cognitive impairment and decline [12, 14, 15]. Previous studies have also shown that physical function parameters, such as gait speed and handgrip strength, are associated with cognitive function in older people [1517]. Therefore, we explored gait speed and handgrip strength as physical function indicators in this study.
Recent review articles have shown that most studies explore cognitive decline by using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [10, 11], which is widely used as a tool to screen for global cognitive impairment. However, the MMSE assesses the basic level of cognitive functions and may be insensitive to changes in well-functioning community-dwelling adults. Therefore, we also used the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) as a more sensitive indicator of cognitive function. The DSST measures general and unspecific processing speed and is more sensitive to slight changes in higher-level cognition than the MMSE [18]. The DSST refers to how quickly one can accurately process new inputs from outside and retrieve stored information from memory [19]. Processing speed, by playing a central role in cognition, is affected by the changes experienced chronologically and can predict incident dementia [20]. Moreover, recent studies have suggested that interventions could improve cognitive functioning by increasing information processing speed among older people [21, 22].
Clouston et al., reviewed that gait speed is associated with a change of fluid cognition while handgrip strength is correlated with changes in global cognitive function [23]. Different physical measures might differentially affect cognitive changes, such as higher-level cognitive change and global cognitive decline. Therefore, the main aim of this study was to evaluate the different effects of gait speed and handgrip strength on 10-year cognitive changes by using the DSST and MMSE among community-dwelling older people.

Methods

Study design

The data for the present study was obtained as part of the National Institute for Longevity Science – Longitudinal Study of Aging (NILS-LSA) [24]. The NILS-LSA was a population-based prospective cohort study: community-dwelling adults living in the neighborhood of the institute (Obu City and Higashiura Town) and who were 40–79 years old at baseline were invited into the study based on a random sample stratified by age and gender between November 1997 and April 2000. The baseline sample included 2267 participants, and a follow up was conducted every 2 years. When participants could not attend the follow up investigation, new age-gender matched subjects aged up to 79 years old were randomly selected.
In the present study, due to a lack of data from the MMSE in the first wave, we selected a sample of participants in the second wave (April 2000 to May 2002; N = 2259). Participants with the following conditions were excluded: (1) younger than 60 years old (n = 1114) and (2) cognitive impairment (MMSE scores less than 24) (n = 37) or no cognitive information (n = 12). In total, 1096 participants were enrolled in the study [mean age 69.4 ± standard deviation (SD) 5.8 years, 50.9% male]. In addition, participants without assessment of gait speed or handgrip strength were excluded for each analysis. The study protocol was approved by the Committee of Ethics of Human Research of the National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. All methods were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

Assessment of physical function (using the 2nd wave as baseline)

In this study, the baseline measurements of physical function included gait speed and handgrip strength. The current cut-off points of gait speed and handgrip strength as the determinants of frailty and sarcopenia were suggested by the lowest quintile or quartile according to the study population, and it is also practical to implement the parameters according to the cut-off point for clinical practice rather than continuous variables [25, 26]. Therefore, we used the quintiles of physical function from the study population. Gait speed was assessed by the walking analysis system (YW-3, Yagami Co., Aichi, Japan) [27]. Participants were asked to walk at a comfortable speed on an 11-m straight walkway, including 1 m for acceleration and deceleration. Light sensors were used for the start and end points to record the time taken to walk 10 m, and habitual gait speed was measured by calculating this in meters per second. Due to the significant difference of gait speeds between genders, all participants were classified into quintiles according to gender (Q1 as the lowest quintile and Q5 as the highest quintile: males ≦1.12; 1.13–1.25; 1.26–1.37; 1.38–1.47; and > 1.48, m/s; and females ≦1.13; 1.14–1.22; 1.23–1.32; 1.33–1.42; and > 1.43, m/s). Handgrip strength was measured using a handgrip dynamometer (Takei Co., Niigata, Japan) calibrated in kg [28]. The participants stood and extended their elbows to hold a handgrip dynamometer. Two trials of both hands were tested alternately, and the maximal value was used as the measurement result. Then, all participants were classified into quintiles according to gender (Q1 as the lowest quintile and Q5 as the highest quintile: males≦30.70; 30.71–33.90; 33.91–37.40; 37.41–41.25; and > 41.25 kg; females ≦18.50; 18.51–20.90; 20.91–23.10; 23.11–25.70; and > 25.70 kg).

Assessment of cognitive function (from 2nd to 7th waves)

Cognitive function was assessed from the second to seventh waves using the Japanese version of the MMSE (scores 0–30, higher score meaning better cognitive function) and the DSST (scores 0–93; higher scores mean better cognitive function) in interviews with a psychologist or psychology graduate students [29, 30]. The DSST was calculated from the Japanese Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised Short Form (JWAIS-R-SF), which is a popular tool for assessing intelligence [31]. The DSST measured the information processing speed. Participants were asked to write down the symbol that corresponded to a given number, as many as they could in 90 s.
Given that the number of participants who scored below the cut-off score of 23/24 on the MMSE were small across waves (n = 17—31), we used scores of the MMSE as well as the DSST as continuous variables.

Covariates (2nd wave as baseline)

Using the 2nd wave as baseline, some covariates, including age, gender (0 = male; 1 = female), education level (years), marital status (0 = not married; 1 = married), smoking status (0 = nonsmoker; 1 = smoker),and depressive symptoms (using Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CESD), 0 = non-depressive symptoms; 1 = depressive symptoms, defined as 16 or greater) [32], were collected by a self-administered questionnaire. Body mass index (BMI) was calculated as weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters. Instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs) were assessed using the Instrumental Self-Maintenance subscale of the Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Gerontology Index of Competence (TMIG-IC) [33], and comorbidity was presented as the Charlson Comorbidity Index (CCI) [34].

Statistical analysis

In this study, all continuous variables are presented as the mean ± standard deviation (SD), and categorical data are presented as numbers (percentages). Student’s t-test or one-way ANOVA was used for the comparison of continuous variables among groups, and the chi-squared test or Fisher’s exact test was used for the comparison of category variables between groups, as appropriate.
The effects of physical function on the rate of cognitive change were analyzed using the linear mixed effects models, which take into account the dependence of repeated observations by participants, an important feature of longitudinal analyses. In addition, the linear mixed effects models can handle missing data due to dropouts during the follow-up period by implying missing data using random maximum likelihood methods so a researcher can use all available data during follow up studies [35, 36].
To test the changes in MMSE and DSST in the linear or non-linear model, we performed likelihood-ratio tests to examine whether models including the time and time-squared terms fit better than those including only the time term. The results indicated that, whereas the model assuming a curvilinear linear change in DSST fit better than that assuming a linear change [χ2(4) = 102.1, P < .001], MMSE changed linearly. Therefore, we only modeled non-linear change in DSST.
The models used in the present study included fixed terms for the intercept (baseline performance for an individual with a grand-mean value of all covariates), physical parameters (gait speed Q1 or handgrip strength Q1, based on the lowest quintile as reference), time (time in years since baseline), time-squared terms (only for DSST), and a physical parameter x time interaction term. Age, gender, education years, marital status, smoking status, BMI, IADL, depressive symptoms, CCI (at baseline) and their interaction terms were included as covariates. However, we did not include interactions between independent variables (i.e., physical function and covariates) and time-squared variables in modeling DSST. If we included the latter interactions, none were significant and the model fit worsened in comparison with the model that only included interactions between independent variables and time. Random effects of the intercept (baseline performance) and slope (change over time) were calculated using an unstructured covariance matrix with residual maximum likelihood. The terms of primary interest in this study were physical parameters x time interactions, which reflect whether the levels of physical performance at baseline differed in the rate of change in cognitive functions over time. For all tests, a two-tailed p-value of < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS version 23.

Results

Baseline characteristics

Overall, 1096 participants were enrolled from the second wave, which was used as the baseline for analyses. Table 1 compares the demographic characteristics of study participants with different gait speed based on quintiles. There were 33 participants having no record for gait speed, and in total, 1063 participants with a gait speed measurement were analyzed, as shown in Table 1. The results of the analysis showed that participants with slower gait speed were older (P < 0.001), had less education (P < 0.001), showed higher depressive symptoms (P = 0.030), and had a higher CCI (P = 0.002), a lower MMSE score (P = 0.025), and a lower DSST score (P < 0.001) (Table 1). Table 2 shows the comparison of participants with different handgrip strengths. There were 15 participants without handgrip strengths measurement, and in total, the handgrip strength records of 1081 participants were analyzed, as shown in Table 2. The analysis showed that participants with lower handgrip strength were older (P < 0.001), had less education (P < 0.001), had a lower BMI (P < 0.001), had higher depressive symptoms (P = 0.009), and had a higher CCI (P < 0.001), lower MMSE score (P < 0.001), and lower DSST score (P < 0.001) (Table 2).
Table 1
Comparison of baseline demographic data according to the quintiles of gait speed
Variable
Gait Speed N = 1063
P value
Q1
230 (21.6%)
Q2
215 (20.2%)
Q3
227 (21.4%)
Q4
199 (18.7%)
Q5
192 (18.1%)
Age (years)
72.7 ± 5.4
70.4 ± 5.6
69.3 ± 5.4
67.1 ± 5.2
66.3 ± 4.9
< 0.001
Gender
     
0.684
 Male
121 (52.6%)
102 (44.4%)
122 (53.7%)
98 (49.2%)
99 (51.6%)
 
 Female
109 (47.4%)
113 (52.6%)
105 (46.3%)
101 (50.8%)
93 (48.4%)
 
Smoking status
     
0.124
 Non-smoking
180 (78.3%)
183 (85.5%)
190 (83.7%)
169 (84.9%)
167 (87.0%)
 
 Smoking
50 (21.7%)
31 (14.5%)
37 (16.3%)
30 (15.1%)
25 (13.0%)
 
Education (years)
10.4 ± 2.7
10.3 ± 2.5
10.8 ± 2.7
11.2 ± 2.4
11.5 ± 2.6
< 0.001
Marital status
     
0.060
 Single
61 (26.5%)
41 (19.2%)
44 (19.4%)
36 (18.1%)
30 (15.6%)
 
 Married
169 (73.5%)
174 (80.9%)
183 (80.6%)
163 (81.9%)
162 (84.4%)
 
BMI (kg/m2)
23.0 ± 3.3
22.7 ± 2.8
22.9 ± 3.0
22.6 ± 2.9
23.0 ± 2.7
0.616
TMIG-IC
4.8 ± 0.5
4.9 ± 0.3
4.9 ± 0.3
4.9 ± 0.3
4.9 ± 0.5
0.168
Depressive symptom
     
0.030
 Yes
34 (15.6%)
24 (11.4%)
19 (8.7%)
18 (9.4%)
12 (6.4%)
 
 No
184 (84.4%)
186 (88.6%)
200 (91.3%)
174 (90.6%)
176 (93.6%)
 
CCI
1.8 ± 2.6
1.7 ± 2.3
1.2 ± 1.8
1.4 ± 1.8
1.2 ± 1.6
0.002
MMSE
27.8 ± 1.8
28.1 ± 1.6
28.1 ± 1.6
28.1 ± 1.6
28.3 ± 1.5
0.025
DSST
37.4 ± 10.2
41.5 ± 9.7
43.2 ± 10.5
46.2 ± 11.0
48.0 ± 11.1
< 0.001
Values are presented as the mean (SD) or counts (percentages)
Gait speed in each group:
In males: Q1 as the lowest quintile and Q5 as the highest quintile; ≦1.12; 1.13–1.25; 1.26–1.37; 1.38–1.47; and > 1.48, m/s
In females: Q1 as the lowest quintile and Q5 as the highest quintile; ≦1.13; 1.14–1.22; 1.23–1.32; 1.33–1.42; and > 1.43, m/s
BMI body mass index, TMIG-IC Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Gerontology Index of Competence, CCI Charlson Comorbidity Index, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, DSST Digit Symbol Substitution Test
The following data were missing: smoking status, n = 1; TMIG-IC, n = 5; depressive symptoms, n = 36; CCI, n = 17; DSST, n = 2
Table 2
Comparison of baseline demographic data according to the quintiles of handgrip strength
Variable
Handgrip Strength N = 1081
P value
Q1
219 (20.3%)
Q2
218 (20.2%)
Q3
217 (20.1%)
Q4
217 (20.1%)
Q5
210 (19.4%)
Age (years)
72.9 ± 5.0
70.9 ± 5.5
69.3 ± 5.5
67.8 ± 5.4
65.8 ± 4.9
< 0.001
Gender
     
0.990
 Male
112 (51.1%)
111 (50.9%)
111 (51.2%)
108 (49.8%)
110 (52.4%)
 
 Female
107 (48.9%)
107 (49.1%)
106 (48.8%)
109 (50.2%)
100 (47.6%)
 
Smoking status
     
0.056
 Non-smoking
176 (80.4%)
180 (82.6%)
196 (90.3%)
179 (82.9%)
174 (82.9%)
 
 Smoking
43 (19.6%)
38 (17.4%)
21 (9.7%)
37 (17.1%)
36 (17.1%)
 
Education (years)
10.4 ± 2.6
10.3 ± 2.5
10.8 ± 2.6
11.0 ± 2.8
11.3 ± 2.6
< 0.001
Marital status
     
0.754
 Single
49 (22.4%)
46 (21.1%)
41 (18.9%)
42 (19.4%)
37 (17.6%)
 
 Married
170 (77.6%)
172 (78.9%)
176 (81.1%)
175 (80.6%)
173 (82.4%)
 
BMI (kg/m2)
22.1 ± 3.5
22.8 ± 3.1
23.0 ± 2.9
23.3 ± 2.9
23.4 ± 2.6
< 0.001
TMIG-IC
4.8 ± 0.5
4.9 ± 0.4
4.9 ± 0.4
4.9 ± 0.4
4.9 ± 0.4
0.723
Depressive symptoms
     
0.009
 Yes
35 (16.4%)
21 (10.0%)
24 (11.7%)
19 (9.0%)
12 (5.9%)
 
 No
178 (83.6%)
189 (90.0%)
181 (88.3%)
193 (91.0%)
193 (94.1%)
 
CCI
2.0 ± 2.4
1.6 ± 2.4
1.4 ± 2.0
1.2 ± 1.7
1.1 ± 1.7
< 0.001
MMSE
27.8 ± 1.8
27.9 ± 1.7
28.0 ± 1.6
28.4 ± 1.5
28.3 ± 1.5
< 0.001
DSST
38.4 ± 10.4
39.9 ± 10.4
43.1 ± 10.0
44.6 ± 11.8
48.3 ± 10.7
< 0.001
Handgrip strength in each group:
In males: Q1 as the lowest quintile and Q5 as the highest quintile: ≦30.70; 30.71–33.90; 33.91–37.40; 37.41–41.25; and > 41.25 kg
In females: Q1 as the lowest quintile and Q5 as the highest quintile: ≦18.50; 18.51–20.90; 20.91–23.10; 23.11–25.70; and > 25.70 kg
BMI body mass index, TMIG-IC Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Gerontology Index of Competence, CCI Charlson Comorbidity Index, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, DSST Digit Symbol Substitution Test
The following data were missing: smoking status, n = 1; TMIG-IC, n = 5; depressive symptoms, n = 36; CCI, n = 17; DSST, n = 2
We also examined correlations among the key variables at baseline (i.e., gait speed, handgrip strength, MMSE, and DSST) separated by gender. The intercorrelations were summarized in Appendix 1. Weak to moderate associations were observed among each other (r = .06–.36), which meant that the key variables were related with each other but were relatively independent within and across domains (i.e., physical and cognitive).

Participation in the follow up periods

The mean number of included studies from the 2nd to 7th waves was 4.05 ± 1.94. Approximately 85% of the participants participated in the study more than once. Appendix 2 summarizes the information on follow-up participation. In total, 1096 participants were included in the analysis from the 2nd wave as the baseline. Eight hundred (80.3%) participants remained in the 3rd wave, and 768 (70.1%), 663 (60.5%), 562 (51.3%) and 473 (43.2%) participants remained in the 4th to 7th waves, respectively. The participation rates were higher in the groups with faster gait speed and higher handgrip strength from wave 3 to wave 7. Compared with those who participated at least twice from wave 2, the participants who participated in only wave 2 (n = 216, 19.7%) were older (72.2 ± 5.5 years versus 68.8 ± 5.7 years, P < 0.001), more frequently female (55.1% versus 47.6%, P = 0.049), had less education (10.3 ± 2.7 years versus 10.9 ± 2.6 years, P = 0.005), showed higher depressive symptoms (17.6% versus 9.3%, P = 0.001), and had a lower MMSE (27.7 ± 1.7 versus 28.1 ± 1.6, P = 0.001) and a lower DSST (38.7 ± 10.7 versus 43.7 ± 11.1, P < 0.001).

Physical function at baseline and cognitive decline

We first performed likelihood-ratio tests to examine whether models that included the time and time-squared terms fit better than those that only included the time term. The results indicated that the models that assumed a nonlinear change in DSST and a linear change in MMSE fit well (Details are provided in the Methods section).
Next, we conducted mixed-effect models to examine whether and how physical function was associated with cognitive function over time. The results are presented in Table 3. The time term was not significant (estimate = − 0.05 and P = 0.606 in the gait speed model; estimate = − 0.04 and P = 0.710 in the handgrip strength model), but the time-squared term was significant (estimate = − 0.05 and P < 0.001 in the gait speed model; estimate = − 0.05 and P < 0.001 in the handgrip strength model) for DSST, indicating an accelerated cognitive decline over time. The time term was significant for MMSE (estimate = − 0.16 and P < 0.001 in the gait speed model; estimate = − 0.17 and P < 0.001 in the handgrip strength model), indicating a constant cognitive decline over time.
Table 3
Estimated 10-year cognitive decline in each gait speed and handgrip strength quintile by linear mixed effects models
Model terms
DSST
MMSE
Estimate (SE)
P-value
Estimate (SE)
P-value
【Gait speed】
 Intercept
40.88 (0.62)
< 0.001
27.96 (0.10)
< 0.001
 Gait speed Q1a
Reference
   
 Gait speed Q2
2.41 (0.86)
0.005
0.16 (0.14)
0.261
 Gait speed Q3
2.21 (0.85)
0.010
0.14 (0.14)
0.305
 Gait speed Q4
3.48 (0.91)
< 0.001
0.08 (0.15)
0.598
 Gait speed Q5
3.96 (0.94)
< 0.001
0.25 (0.15)
0.097
 Timeb
−0.05 (0.09)
0.606
−0.16 (0.02)
< 0.001
 Time-squaredb
−0.05 (0.01)
< 0.001
 
 Gait speed Q1 x time
Reference
   
 Gait speed Q2 x time
0.14 (0.09)
0.140
0.02 (0.03)
0.421
 Gait speed Q3 x time
0.15 (0.09)
0.101
0.05 (0.03)
0.062
 Gait speed Q4 x time
0.13 (0.09)
0.146
0.02 (0.03)
0.590
 Gait speed Q5 x time
0.28 (0.09)
0.003
0.05 (0.03)
0.078
【Handgrip Strength】
 Intercept
41.57 (0.63)
< 0.001
27.93 (0.10)
< 0.001
 Handgrip strength Q1c
Reference
   
 Handgrip strength Q2
0.49 (0.86)
0.567
0.07 (0.14)
0.614
 Handgrip strength Q3
1.64 (0.88)
0.063
0.07 (0.14)
0.628
 Handgrip strength Q4
2.15 (0.90)
0.017
0.38 (0.14)
0.008
 Handgrip strength Q5
3.42 (0.95)
< 0.001
0.23 (0.15)
0.127
 Timeb
−0.04 (0.09)
0.710
−0.17 (0.02)
< 0.001
 Time-squaredb
− 0.05 (0.01)
< 0.001
 
 Handgrip strength Q1 x time
Reference
   
 Handgrip strength Q2 x time
0.07 (0.10)
0.439
0.04 (0.03)
0.189
 Handgrip strength Q3 x time
0.11 (0.09)
0.259
0.05 (0.03)
0.060
 Handgrip strength Q4 x time
0.20 (0.09)
0.033
0.05 (0.03)
0.082
 Handgrip strength Q5 x time
0.20 (0.10)
0.040
0.06 (0.03)
0.039
All models were adjusted by covariates (age, gender, education years, marital status, smoking status, body mass index, Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Gerontology Index of Competence, depressive symptoms and Charlson Comorbidity Index) and their interactions with time. The covariates were grand-mean centered. However, these estimates were not presented, and only the estimates of interest (intercept, time, and their interactions with physical function) are shown
In males: Q1 as the lowest quintile and Q5 as the highest quintile; ≦1.12; 1.13–1.25; 1.26–1.37; 1.38–1.47; and > 1.48, m/s
In females: Q1 as the lowest quintile and Q5 as the highest quintile; ≦1.13; 1.14–1.22; 1.23–1.32; 1.33–1.42; and > 1.43, m/s
In males: Q1 as the lowest quintile and Q5 as the highest quintile: ≦30.70; 30.71–33.90; 33.91–37.40; 37.41–41.25; and > 41.25 kg
In females: Q1 as the lowest quintile and Q5 as the highest quintile: ≦18.50; 18.51–20.90; 20.91–23.10; 23.11–25.70; and > 25.70 kg
SE standard error, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, DSST Digit Symbol Substitution Test
Among a sample of 1096 adults, those with missing values in physical and cognitive functions and covariates at baseline were excluded from the analyses. Thus, the sample sizes varied according to the models: in the gait speed models, N = 1006 for DSST, and N = 1007 for MMSE; in the grip strength models, N = 1022 for DSST, and N = 1024 for MMSE
aGait speed in each group
bYears since baseline. Time-squared was included in modeling non-linear change of DSST
cHandgrip strength in each group
We will continue to report significant associations between the intercept or linear slope with physical function to indicate whether and which physical function indicators buffered cognitive decline. In the gait speed model, the group in the lowest quintile (Q1) showed significantly lower DSST scores than the other groups (estimate = 2.41 and P = 0.005 for Q2; estimate = 2.21 and P = 0.010 for Q3; estimate = 3.48 and P < 0.001 for Q4; estimate = 3.96 and P < 0.001 for Q5), but the differences in the MMSE scores were absent. The term for the gait speed × time interaction in Q5 was significant for DSST decline (estimate = 0.28 and P = 0.003), showing that the subjects in the lowest quintile group of gait speed had a greater DSST decline over time than those in the highest quintile group.
In the handgrip strength model, those with the lowest handgrip strength (Q1) showed significantly lower DSST scores than those in Q4 and Q5 (estimate = 2.15 and P = 0.017 for Q4; estimate = 3.42 and P < 0.001 for Q5), but a significant difference was only observed for Q4 in the MMSE score (estimate = 0.38 and P = 0.008). The terms for the handgrip strength × time interaction in Q4 and Q5 were significant for DSST (estimate =0.20 and P = 0.033 for Q4; estimate = 0.20 and P = 0.040 for Q5). For MMSE, significance was found in Q5 (estimate = 0.06 and P = 0.039). The differences in cognitive decline between high and low physical function groups were shown in Fig. 1.
Regarding DSST, we failed to observe any significant independent variables (i.e., physical function and covariates) associated with the time-squared term and did not include the interactions with the time-squared term (results not shown).

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, the present longitudinal study is the first to examine differential associations of gait speed and handgrip strength with the DSST and MMSE as indicators of long-term cognitive decline among community-dwelling older people in Japan. In addition, in the present study, the participants were invited into the study based on a random sample stratified by age and gender and the follow up period was up to 10 years. Most other longitudinal studies show that physical function can predict cognitive decline by using global cognitive tests, such as MMSE, but only a few have explored executive functional decline by using the DSST at the same time in their studies of community populations [10, 11, 13]. The DSST is a specific measure of executive cognitive function, working memory, processing speed, and visuospatial attention and might be a more sensitive measure of slight cognitive changes than the MMSE when seeking to detect early cognitive decline among community-dwelling older people [18]. Moreover, the current consensus for gait speed and handgrip strength as key determinants of physical frailty and sarcopenia suggested the use of the quintile or quartile as the cut-off points, and our present study used the lowest quintile of physical function compared with the others to explore the prediction of the 10-year cognitive decline [25, 26]. Our study demonstrated that at baseline, a slow gait speed and low handgrip strength were significantly associated with lower DSST scores, but not MMSE scores except between Q1 and Q4 for handgrip strength. Moreover, the results revealed both gait speed and handgrip strength were associated with changes in cognitive function. When comparing participants with the slowest gait speed, those with the fastest gait speed (Q5) exhibited a less steep cognitive decline as measured by DSST. On the other hand, when comparing participants with the weakest grip strength, those with the strongest handgrip strength (Q5) showed a less severe cognitive decline as measured by MMSE in addition to DSST.
Our study showed that poor physical function was associated with cognitive decline, which was compatible with previous studies (15–17). When aging, health behavior and age-related biological factors generally underlie normative age-related decline in physical function, which in turn could lead to cognitive decline. Current physical activity guidelines also reported that to do multicomponent physical activity including balance training and aerobic and muscle-strengthening activities could improve cognition and reduce the risk of dementia [37]. Furthermore, poor physical condition is associated with depressive symptoms and limited social interactions and leisure activities [3840]. Subjects with depressive symptoms were reported have higher risk for developing cognitive impairment than those without [41]. Moreover, some shared pathogenic factors have been found between low muscle strength and impaired cognitive function. High oxidative stress, high inflammatory markers, insulin resistance and low sex steroid levels were reported to contribute to both muscle loss and cognitive decline and might account for the association between poor physical function and cognitive decline [4244].
Our result supports previous findings that gait speed is correlated with the DSST scores decline [4547]. Best et al reported that a slow walking speed was more strongly correlated with cognitive decline in DSST rather than in global cognition [45]. Walking and gait may rely on motor and cognitive functions, which may share similar neural and pathological mechanisms [15]. The networks in the brain that control walking involve regions that are responsible for attention, executive and visuospatial functions, as well as regions that control motor tasks. Previous research has demonstrated that the cerebellum, basal ganglia, hippocampus, and parietal and frontal cortices are related to gait and executive functions [4850]. To unravel the question of whether declines in gait speed and executive function are closely associated with aging, the time-varying nature of physical function should be further taken into consideration.
Our study found that low handgrip strength was associated with the cognitive decline with MMSE as well as DSST [12, 13, 5153]. Low handgrip strength and executive functional decline might also share a similar etiopathogenesis. Skilled hand movements and handgrip strength are associated with the primary motor area, supplementary motor area, dorsal and ventral parts of the premotor cortex area and cerebellum, as shown in neuroimaging studies, and these may also play a role in higher cognitive functions [5456]. The correlation between poor physical condition and global cognitive decline might also be explained by reduced gray and white matter volumes in multiple brain regions and white matter hyperintensities and greater activation in pre-supplementary motor, pre-dorsal motor, rostral cingulate and prefrontal cortex by a functional MRI study [57, 58]. However, in our study, we only found that low handgrip strength was correlated with global cognitive decline, but not slow gait speed. Alfaro-Acha et al., also reported that handgrip strength may be an early marker of subsequent cognitive decline [51]. We further performed a follow-up analysis to examine whether gait speed and handgrip strength had independent effects on changes in DSST. After including both physical function parameters simultaneously, the model revealed that only gait speed was associated with the decline in DSST (estimate = 0.25, P = 0.008 for Q5 in reference to Q1). The results of the follow-up analysis would indicate a unique linkage between gait speed and DSST. A recent review (Tian et al., 2017) also suggests that gait can be associated with specific brain areas important for sensorimotor function. However, these underlying mechanisms have not been unraveled yet.
Despite all the efforts that went into designing and carrying out this study, there were still several limitations. First, none the participants were diagnosed with mild cognitive impairment and dementia, and there were 37 participants with cognitive impairment (according to the definition of MMSE< 24) at baseline who were excluded for analysis. Therefore, the results are not relevant among those with mild cognitive impairment and dementia. However, all the participants were recruited by matching age and gender in the community, and therefore the results of the present study could be applied to the community-dwelling older adults without cognitive impairment. Second, the statistical models in the present study did not include potential covariates that might influence cognitive decline, such as visual and auditory functions; however, we adjusted several important factors as much as possible to minimize this confounding bias. Third, 19.7% of the participants who participated in the baseline survey or wave 2 only had more impaired physical and cognitive functions. We assumed that this attrition meant responses were missing at random. However, participants with impaired cognitive function also tended to drop out from the follow up surveys. Therefore, we might underestimate cognitive decline when using a cognitively intact sample. Although the results indicated a non-linear change in DSST, we found no significant associations with the time-squared term. Future research needs to explore potential protective factors that buffer accelerated cognitive decline.

Conclusions

A slow gait speed could predict 10-year cognitive decline using DSST, and a low handgrip strength could predict 10-year cognitive decline using MMSE in addition to DSST. Thus both physical measures are lined to cognitive decline but there may be different mechanism between brain and physical functions.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.
The study protocol was approved by the Committee of Ethics of Human Research of the National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. All methods were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that Liang-Kung Chen is a member of the editorial board as Associated Editor of BMC Geriatrics.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Anhänge

Appendix 1

Table 4
Intercorrelations among the key variables at baseline separated by gender
 
1
2
3
4
1. Gait speed
.37*
.06
.31*
2. Handgrip strength
.33*
.15*
.34*
3. MMSE
.06
.15*
.36*
4. DSST
.31*
.34*
.36*
The results for male are below and those for females are above the diagonals
MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, DSST Digit Symbol Substitution Test
*P < .001

Appendix 2

Table 5
Comparison for the characteristics of those participating only once in 2nd wave and others
 
Maintain in 3rd wave n = 880 (80.3%)
Only participation in 2nd wave n = 216 (19.7%)
P value
Age (years)
68.8 ± 5.7
72.2 ± 5.5
< 0.001
Gender
  
0.049
 Male
461 (52.4%)
97 (44.9%)
 
 Female
419 (47.6%)
119 (55.1%)
 
Smoking status
  
0.712
 No smoking
735 (83.6%)
182 (84.7%)
 
 Smoking
144 (16.4%)
33 (15.3%)
 
Education
10.9 ± 2.6
10.3 ± 2.7
0.005
Marital status
 Single
174 (19.8%)
48 (22.2%)
.422
 Married
706 (80.2%)
168 (77.8%)
 
BMI (kg/m2)
22.8 ± 2.9
23.2 ± 3.6
0.110
TMIG-IC
4.9 ± 0.4
4.8 ± 0.6
0.211
Depressive symptoms
 
0.001
 Yes
79 (9.3%)
36 (17.6%)
 
 No
774 (90.7%)
169 (82.4%)
 
CCI
 MMSE
28.1 ± 1.6
27.7 ± 1.7
0.001
 DSST
43.7 ± 11.1
38.7 ± 10.7
< 0.001
BMI body mass index, TMIG-IC Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Gerontology Index of Competence, CCI Charlson Comorbidity Index, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, DSST Digit Symbol Substitution Test
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Prince M, Wimo A, Guerchet M, Ali G, Wu Y, Prina M. World Alzheimer Report 2015. In: The global impact of dementia. Alzheimer’s disease international. London: Alzheimer’s Disease International (ADI). p. 2015. Prince M, Wimo A, Guerchet M, Ali G, Wu Y, Prina M. World Alzheimer Report 2015. In: The global impact of dementia. Alzheimer’s disease international. London: Alzheimer’s Disease International (ADI). p. 2015.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Prince M, Bryce R, Albanese E, Wimo A, Ribeiro W, Ferri CP. The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9(1):63–75 e62.PubMedCrossRef Prince M, Bryce R, Albanese E, Wimo A, Ribeiro W, Ferri CP. The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9(1):63–75 e62.PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Li C, Dowling NM, Chappell R. Quantile regression with a change-point model for longitudinal data: an application to the study of cognitive changes in preclinical alzheimer's disease. Biometrics. 2015;71(3):625–35.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Li C, Dowling NM, Chappell R. Quantile regression with a change-point model for longitudinal data: an application to the study of cognitive changes in preclinical alzheimer's disease. Biometrics. 2015;71(3):625–35.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Howieson DB. Cognitive decline in Presymptomatic Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2016;73(4):384–5.PubMedCrossRef Howieson DB. Cognitive decline in Presymptomatic Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2016;73(4):384–5.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Brasure M, Desai P, Davila H, Nelson VA, Calvert C, Jutkowitz E, Butler M, Fink HA, Ratner E, Hemmy LS, et al. Physical activity interventions in preventing cognitive decline and Alzheimer-type dementia: a systematic review. Ann Intern Med. 2018;168(1):30–8.PubMedCrossRef Brasure M, Desai P, Davila H, Nelson VA, Calvert C, Jutkowitz E, Butler M, Fink HA, Ratner E, Hemmy LS, et al. Physical activity interventions in preventing cognitive decline and Alzheimer-type dementia: a systematic review. Ann Intern Med. 2018;168(1):30–8.PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat de la Torre JC. Alzheimer's disease is incurable but preventable. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;20(3):861–70.PubMedCrossRef de la Torre JC. Alzheimer's disease is incurable but preventable. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;20(3):861–70.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, Costafreda SG, Huntley J, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, Burns A, Cohen-Mansfield J, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017;390(10113):2673–734.PubMedCrossRef Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, Costafreda SG, Huntley J, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, Burns A, Cohen-Mansfield J, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017;390(10113):2673–734.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolinsky FD, Bentler SE, Hockenberry J, Jones MP, Weigel PA, Kaskie B, Wallace RB. A prospective cohort study of long-term cognitive changes in older Medicare beneficiaries. BMC Public Health. 2011;11(1):710.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wolinsky FD, Bentler SE, Hockenberry J, Jones MP, Weigel PA, Kaskie B, Wallace RB. A prospective cohort study of long-term cognitive changes in older Medicare beneficiaries. BMC Public Health. 2011;11(1):710.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin KL, Blizzard L, Wood AG, Srikanth V, Thomson R, Sanders LM, Callisaya ML. Cognitive function, gait, and gait variability in older people: a population-based study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68(6):726–32.PubMedCrossRef Martin KL, Blizzard L, Wood AG, Srikanth V, Thomson R, Sanders LM, Callisaya ML. Cognitive function, gait, and gait variability in older people: a population-based study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68(6):726–32.PubMedCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Verghese J, Wang C, Lipton RB, Holtzer R, Xue X. Quantitative gait dysfunction and risk of cognitive decline and dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78(9):929–35.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Verghese J, Wang C, Lipton RB, Holtzer R, Xue X. Quantitative gait dysfunction and risk of cognitive decline and dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78(9):929–35.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Inzitari M, Newman AB, Yaffe K, Boudreau R, de Rekeneire N, Shorr R, Harris TB, Rosano C. Gait speed predicts decline in attention and psychomotor speed in older adults: the health aging and body composition study. Neuroepidemiology. 2007;29(3–4):156–62.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Inzitari M, Newman AB, Yaffe K, Boudreau R, de Rekeneire N, Shorr R, Harris TB, Rosano C. Gait speed predicts decline in attention and psychomotor speed in older adults: the health aging and body composition study. Neuroepidemiology. 2007;29(3–4):156–62.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Hsu CL, Liang CK, Liao MC, Chou MY, Lin YT. Slow gait speed as a predictor of 1-year cognitive decline in a veterans' retirement community in southern Taiwan. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2017;17:14–9.PubMedCrossRef Hsu CL, Liang CK, Liao MC, Chou MY, Lin YT. Slow gait speed as a predictor of 1-year cognitive decline in a veterans' retirement community in southern Taiwan. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2017;17:14–9.PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Sternäng O, Reynolds CA, Finkel D, Ernsth-Bravell M, Pedersen NL, Dahl Aslan AK. Grip strength and cognitive abilities: associations in old age. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015;71(5):841–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Sternäng O, Reynolds CA, Finkel D, Ernsth-Bravell M, Pedersen NL, Dahl Aslan AK. Grip strength and cognitive abilities: associations in old age. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015;71(5):841–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Chang K-V, Hsu T-H, Wu W-T, Huang K-C, Han D-S. Association between sarcopenia and cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2016;17(12):1164 e1167–15.CrossRef Chang K-V, Hsu T-H, Wu W-T, Huang K-C, Han D-S. Association between sarcopenia and cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2016;17(12):1164 e1167–15.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Buracchio T, Dodge HH, Howieson D, Wasserman D, Kaye J. The trajectory of gait speed preceding mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2010;67(8):980–6.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Buracchio T, Dodge HH, Howieson D, Wasserman D, Kaye J. The trajectory of gait speed preceding mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2010;67(8):980–6.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Demnitz N, Esser P, Dawes H, Valkanova V, Johansen-Berg H, Ebmeier KP, Sexton C. A systematic review and meta-analysis of cross-sectional studies examining the relationship between mobility and cognition in healthy older adults. Gait Posture. 2016;50:164–74.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Demnitz N, Esser P, Dawes H, Valkanova V, Johansen-Berg H, Ebmeier KP, Sexton C. A systematic review and meta-analysis of cross-sectional studies examining the relationship between mobility and cognition in healthy older adults. Gait Posture. 2016;50:164–74.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Proust-Lima C, Amieva H, Dartigues J-F, Jacqmin-Gadda H. Sensitivity of four psychometric tests to measure cognitive changes in brain aging-population–based studies. Am J Epidemiol. 2006;165(3):344–50.PubMedCrossRef Proust-Lima C, Amieva H, Dartigues J-F, Jacqmin-Gadda H. Sensitivity of four psychometric tests to measure cognitive changes in brain aging-population–based studies. Am J Epidemiol. 2006;165(3):344–50.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Iwasa H, Kai I, Yoshida Y, Suzuki T, Kim H, Yoshida H. Information processing speed and 8-year mortality among community-dwelling elderly Japanese. J Epidemiol. 2014;24(1):52–9.PubMedCrossRef Iwasa H, Kai I, Yoshida Y, Suzuki T, Kim H, Yoshida H. Information processing speed and 8-year mortality among community-dwelling elderly Japanese. J Epidemiol. 2014;24(1):52–9.PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Rapp MA, Reischies FM. Attention and executive control predict Alzheimer disease in late life: results from the Berlin aging study (BASE). Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;13(2):134–41.PubMedCrossRef Rapp MA, Reischies FM. Attention and executive control predict Alzheimer disease in late life: results from the Berlin aging study (BASE). Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;13(2):134–41.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Iwasa H, Gondo Y, Yoshida Y, Kwon J, Inagaki H, Kawaai C, Masui Y, Kim H, Yoshida H, Suzuki T. Cognitive performance as a predictor of functional decline among the non-disabled elderly dwelling in a Japanese community: a 4-year population-based prospective cohort study. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2008;47(1):139–49.PubMedCrossRef Iwasa H, Gondo Y, Yoshida Y, Kwon J, Inagaki H, Kawaai C, Masui Y, Kim H, Yoshida H, Suzuki T. Cognitive performance as a predictor of functional decline among the non-disabled elderly dwelling in a Japanese community: a 4-year population-based prospective cohort study. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2008;47(1):139–49.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Edwards JD, Wadley VG, Vance DE, Wood K, Roenker DL, Ball KK. The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday performance. Aging Ment Health. 2005;9(3):262–71.PubMedCrossRef Edwards JD, Wadley VG, Vance DE, Wood K, Roenker DL, Ball KK. The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday performance. Aging Ment Health. 2005;9(3):262–71.PubMedCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Clouston SA, Brewster P, Kuh D, Richards M, Cooper R, Hardy R, Rubin MS, Hofer SM. The dynamic relationship between physical function and cognition in longitudinal aging cohorts. Epidemiol Rev. 2013;35:33–50.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Clouston SA, Brewster P, Kuh D, Richards M, Cooper R, Hardy R, Rubin MS, Hofer SM. The dynamic relationship between physical function and cognition in longitudinal aging cohorts. Epidemiol Rev. 2013;35:33–50.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Shimokata H, Ando F, Niino N. A new comprehensive study on aging--the National Institute for longevity sciences, longitudinal study of aging (NILS-LSA). J Epidemiol. 2000;10(1 Suppl):S1–9.PubMedCrossRef Shimokata H, Ando F, Niino N. A new comprehensive study on aging--the National Institute for longevity sciences, longitudinal study of aging (NILS-LSA). J Epidemiol. 2000;10(1 Suppl):S1–9.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Cruz-Jentoft AJ, Baeyens JP, Bauer JM, Boirie Y, Cederholm T, Landi F, Martin FC, Michel J-P, Rolland Y, Schneider SM, Cruz-Gentoft AJ, et al. Sarcopenia: European consensus on definition and diagnosis Report of the European working group on sarcopenia in older People. Age Ageing. 2010;39(4):412–23.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Cruz-Jentoft AJ, Baeyens JP, Bauer JM, Boirie Y, Cederholm T, Landi F, Martin FC, Michel J-P, Rolland Y, Schneider SM, Cruz-Gentoft AJ, et al. Sarcopenia: European consensus on definition and diagnosis Report of the European working group on sarcopenia in older People. Age Ageing. 2010;39(4):412–23.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen LK, Liu LK, Woo J, Assantachai P, Auyeung TW, Bahyah KS, Chou MY, Chen LY, Hsu PS, Krairit O, et al. Sarcopenia in Asia: consensus report of the Asian working Group for Sarcopenia. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2014;15(2):95–101.PubMedCrossRef Chen LK, Liu LK, Woo J, Assantachai P, Auyeung TW, Bahyah KS, Chou MY, Chen LY, Hsu PS, Krairit O, et al. Sarcopenia in Asia: consensus report of the Asian working Group for Sarcopenia. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2014;15(2):95–101.PubMedCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Nakamoto M, Otsuka R, Yuki A, Nishita Y, Tange C, Tomida M, Kato Y, Ando F, Shimokata H, Suzuki T. Higher gait speed and smaller sway area decrease the risk for decline in higher-level functional capacity among middle-aged and elderly women. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2015;61(3):429–36.PubMedCrossRef Nakamoto M, Otsuka R, Yuki A, Nishita Y, Tange C, Tomida M, Kato Y, Ando F, Shimokata H, Suzuki T. Higher gait speed and smaller sway area decrease the risk for decline in higher-level functional capacity among middle-aged and elderly women. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2015;61(3):429–36.PubMedCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Kozakai R, Ando F, Kim HY, Yuki A, Otsuka R, Shimokata H. Sex-differences in age-related grip strength decline: a 10-year longitudinal study of community-living middle-aged and older Japanese. Jpn J Phys Fitness Sports Med. 2016;5(1):87–94.CrossRef Kozakai R, Ando F, Kim HY, Yuki A, Otsuka R, Shimokata H. Sex-differences in age-related grip strength decline: a 10-year longitudinal study of community-living middle-aged and older Japanese. Jpn J Phys Fitness Sports Med. 2016;5(1):87–94.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–98.PubMedCrossRef Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–98.PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Mori E. Usefulness of a Japanese version of the mini-mental state test in neurological patients. Jpn J Neuropsychol. 1985;1:82–90. Mori E. Usefulness of a Japanese version of the mini-mental state test in neurological patients. Jpn J Neuropsychol. 1985;1:82–90.
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Misawa G, Kobayashi S, Fujita K, Maekawa H, Dairoku H. Japanese wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised short forms. Tokyo: Nihon Bunka Kagakusha; 1993. Misawa G, Kobayashi S, Fujita K, Maekawa H, Dairoku H. Japanese wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised short forms. Tokyo: Nihon Bunka Kagakusha; 1993.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1(3):385–401.CrossRef Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1(3):385–401.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Koyano W, Shibata H, Nakazato K, Haga H, Suyama Y. Measurement of competence: reliability and validity of the TMIG index of competence. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 1991;13(2):103–16.PubMedCrossRef Koyano W, Shibata H, Nakazato K, Haga H, Suyama Y. Measurement of competence: reliability and validity of the TMIG index of competence. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 1991;13(2):103–16.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Chaudhry S, Jin L, Meltzer D. Use of a self-report-generated Charlson Comorbidity Index for predicting mortality. Med care. 2005;43(6):607-15.PubMedCrossRef Chaudhry S, Jin L, Meltzer D. Use of a self-report-generated Charlson Comorbidity Index for predicting mortality. Med care. 2005;43(6):607-15.PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Laird NM, Ware JH. Random-effects models for longitudinal data. Biometrics. 1982;38(4):963–74.PubMedCrossRef Laird NM, Ware JH. Random-effects models for longitudinal data. Biometrics. 1982;38(4):963–74.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Morrell CH, Brant LJ, Ferrucci L. Model choice can obscure results in longitudinal studies. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64(2):215–22.PubMedCrossRef Morrell CH, Brant LJ, Ferrucci L. Model choice can obscure results in longitudinal studies. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64(2):215–22.PubMedCrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Piercy KL, Troiano RP, Ballard RM, Carlson SA, Fulton JE, Galuska DA, George SM, Olson RD. The physical activity guidelines for Americans. Jama. 2018;320(19):2020–8.PubMedCrossRef Piercy KL, Troiano RP, Ballard RM, Carlson SA, Fulton JE, Galuska DA, George SM, Olson RD. The physical activity guidelines for Americans. Jama. 2018;320(19):2020–8.PubMedCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Singh-Manoux A, Richards M, Marmot M. Leisure activities and cognitive function in middle age: evidence from the Whitehall II study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57(11):907–13.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Singh-Manoux A, Richards M, Marmot M. Leisure activities and cognitive function in middle age: evidence from the Whitehall II study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57(11):907–13.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat McDermott LM, Ebmeier KP. A meta-analysis of depression severity and cognitive function. J Affect Disord. 2009;119(1–3):1–8.PubMedCrossRef McDermott LM, Ebmeier KP. A meta-analysis of depression severity and cognitive function. J Affect Disord. 2009;119(1–3):1–8.PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Hsu YH, Liang CK, Chou MY, Liao MC, Lin YT, Chen LK, Lo YK. Association of cognitive impairment, depressive symptoms and sarcopenia among healthy older men in the veterans retirement community in southern T aiwan: a cross-sectional study. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2014;14:102–8.PubMedCrossRef Hsu YH, Liang CK, Chou MY, Liao MC, Lin YT, Chen LK, Lo YK. Association of cognitive impairment, depressive symptoms and sarcopenia among healthy older men in the veterans retirement community in southern T aiwan: a cross-sectional study. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2014;14:102–8.PubMedCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Gao Y, Huang C, Zhao K, Ma L, Qiu X, Zhang L, Xiu Y, Chen L, Lu W, Huang C. Retracted: depression as a risk factor for dementia and mild cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013;28(5):441–9.PubMedCrossRef Gao Y, Huang C, Zhao K, Ma L, Qiu X, Zhang L, Xiu Y, Chen L, Lu W, Huang C. Retracted: depression as a risk factor for dementia and mild cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013;28(5):441–9.PubMedCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Weaver JD, Huang MH, Albert M, Harris T, Rowe JW, Seeman TE. Interleukin-6 and risk of cognitive decline: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Neurology. 2002;59(3):371–8.PubMedCrossRef Weaver JD, Huang MH, Albert M, Harris T, Rowe JW, Seeman TE. Interleukin-6 and risk of cognitive decline: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Neurology. 2002;59(3):371–8.PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Cesari M, Penninx BW, Pahor M, Lauretani F, Corsi AM, Rhys Williams G, Guralnik JM, Ferrucci L. Inflammatory markers and physical performance in older persons: the InCHIANTI study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59(3):242–8.PubMedCrossRef Cesari M, Penninx BW, Pahor M, Lauretani F, Corsi AM, Rhys Williams G, Guralnik JM, Ferrucci L. Inflammatory markers and physical performance in older persons: the InCHIANTI study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59(3):242–8.PubMedCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Hogervorst E, Bandelow S, Combrinck M, Smith A. Low free testosterone is an independent risk factor for Alzheimer's disease. Exp Gerontol. 2004;39(11–12):1633–9.PubMedCrossRef Hogervorst E, Bandelow S, Combrinck M, Smith A. Low free testosterone is an independent risk factor for Alzheimer's disease. Exp Gerontol. 2004;39(11–12):1633–9.PubMedCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Best JR, Liu-Ambrose T, Boudreau RM, Ayonayon HN, Satterfield S, Simonsick EM, Studenski S, Yaffe K, Newman AB, Rosano C. An evaluation of the longitudinal, bidirectional associations between gait speed and cognition in older women and men. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2016;71(12):1616–23.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Best JR, Liu-Ambrose T, Boudreau RM, Ayonayon HN, Satterfield S, Simonsick EM, Studenski S, Yaffe K, Newman AB, Rosano C. An evaluation of the longitudinal, bidirectional associations between gait speed and cognition in older women and men. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2016;71(12):1616–23.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Gale CR, Allerhand M, Sayer AA, Cooper C, Deary IJ. The dynamic relationship between cognitive function and walking speed: the English longitudinal study of ageing. Age. 2014;36(4):9682.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Gale CR, Allerhand M, Sayer AA, Cooper C, Deary IJ. The dynamic relationship between cognitive function and walking speed: the English longitudinal study of ageing. Age. 2014;36(4):9682.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith EE, O’Donnell M, Dagenais G, Lear SA, Wielgosz A, Sharma M, Poirier P, Stotts G, Black SE, Strother S, et al. Early cerebral small vessel disease and brain volume, cognition, and gait. Ann Neurol. 2015;77(2):251–61.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Smith EE, O’Donnell M, Dagenais G, Lear SA, Wielgosz A, Sharma M, Poirier P, Stotts G, Black SE, Strother S, et al. Early cerebral small vessel disease and brain volume, cognition, and gait. Ann Neurol. 2015;77(2):251–61.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Holtzer R, Epstein N, Mahoney JR, Izzetoglu M, Blumen HM. Neuroimaging of mobility in aging: a targeted review. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69(11):1375–88.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Holtzer R, Epstein N, Mahoney JR, Izzetoglu M, Blumen HM. Neuroimaging of mobility in aging: a targeted review. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69(11):1375–88.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Baillieux H, De Smet HJ, Paquier PF, De Deyn PP, Mariën P. Cerebellar neurocognition: insights into the bottom of the brain. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2008;110(8):763–73.PubMedCrossRef Baillieux H, De Smet HJ, Paquier PF, De Deyn PP, Mariën P. Cerebellar neurocognition: insights into the bottom of the brain. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2008;110(8):763–73.PubMedCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Belghali M, Chastan N, Cignetti F, Davenne D, Decker LM. Loss of gait control assessed by cognitive-motor dual-tasks: pros and cons in detecting people at risk of developing Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. GeroScience. 2017;39(3):305–29.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Belghali M, Chastan N, Cignetti F, Davenne D, Decker LM. Loss of gait control assessed by cognitive-motor dual-tasks: pros and cons in detecting people at risk of developing Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. GeroScience. 2017;39(3):305–29.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Alfaro-Acha A, Al Snih S, Raji MA, Kuo YF, Markides KS, Ottenbacher KJ. Handgrip strength and cognitive decline in older Mexican Americans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006;61(8):859–65.PubMedCrossRef Alfaro-Acha A, Al Snih S, Raji MA, Kuo YF, Markides KS, Ottenbacher KJ. Handgrip strength and cognitive decline in older Mexican Americans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006;61(8):859–65.PubMedCrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Auyeung TW, Lee J, Kwok T, Woo J. Physical frailty predicts future cognitive decline—a four-year prospective study in 2737 cognitively normal older adults. J Nutr Health Aging. 2011;15(8):690–4.PubMedCrossRef Auyeung TW, Lee J, Kwok T, Woo J. Physical frailty predicts future cognitive decline—a four-year prospective study in 2737 cognitively normal older adults. J Nutr Health Aging. 2011;15(8):690–4.PubMedCrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Taekema DG, Gussekloo J, Maier AB, Westendorp RG, de Craen AJ. Handgrip strength as a predictor of functional, psychological and social health. A prospective population-based study among the oldest old. Age Ageing. 2010;39(3):331–7.PubMedCrossRef Taekema DG, Gussekloo J, Maier AB, Westendorp RG, de Craen AJ. Handgrip strength as a predictor of functional, psychological and social health. A prospective population-based study among the oldest old. Age Ageing. 2010;39(3):331–7.PubMedCrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Ward NS, Newton JM, Swayne OB, Lee L, Frackowiak RS, Thompson AJ, Greenwood RJ, Rothwell JC. The relationship between brain activity and peak grip force is modulated by corticospinal system integrity after subcortical stroke. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25(6):1865–73.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ward NS, Newton JM, Swayne OB, Lee L, Frackowiak RS, Thompson AJ, Greenwood RJ, Rothwell JC. The relationship between brain activity and peak grip force is modulated by corticospinal system integrity after subcortical stroke. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25(6):1865–73.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Ismail S, Mohamad M, Syazarina S, Nafisah W. Hand grips strength effect on motor function in human brain using fMRI: a pilot study. In: Journal of Physics: Conference Series; 2014. p. 012005. Ismail S, Mohamad M, Syazarina S, Nafisah W. Hand grips strength effect on motor function in human brain using fMRI: a pilot study. In: Journal of Physics: Conference Series; 2014. p. 012005.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Olivier E, Davare M, Andres M, Fadiga L. Precision grasping in humans: from motor control to cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17(6):644–8.PubMedCrossRef Olivier E, Davare M, Andres M, Fadiga L. Precision grasping in humans: from motor control to cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17(6):644–8.PubMedCrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Callisaya ML, Beare R, Phan TG, Blizzard L, Thrift AG, Chen J, Srikanth VK. Brain structural change and gait decline: a longitudinal population-based study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2013;61(7):1074–9.PubMedCrossRef Callisaya ML, Beare R, Phan TG, Blizzard L, Thrift AG, Chen J, Srikanth VK. Brain structural change and gait decline: a longitudinal population-based study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2013;61(7):1074–9.PubMedCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Heuninckx S, Wenderoth N, Debaere F, Peeters R, Swinnen SP. Neural basis of aging: the penetration of cognition into action control. J Neurosci. 2005;25(29):6787–96.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Heuninckx S, Wenderoth N, Debaere F, Peeters R, Swinnen SP. Neural basis of aging: the penetration of cognition into action control. J Neurosci. 2005;25(29):6787–96.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Role of gait speed and grip strength in predicting 10-year cognitive decline among community-dwelling older people
verfasst von
Ming-Yueh Chou
Yukiko Nishita
Takeshi Nakagawa
Chikako Tange
Makiko Tomida
Hiroshi Shimokata
Rei Otsuka
Liang-Kung Chen
Hidenori Arai
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2019
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Geriatrics / Ausgabe 1/2019
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2318
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-019-1199-7

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

BMC Geriatrics 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Innere Medizin

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Notfall-TEP der Hüfte ist auch bei 90-Jährigen machbar

26.04.2024 Hüft-TEP Nachrichten

Ob bei einer Notfalloperation nach Schenkelhalsfraktur eine Hemiarthroplastik oder eine totale Endoprothese (TEP) eingebaut wird, sollte nicht allein vom Alter der Patientinnen und Patienten abhängen. Auch über 90-Jährige können von der TEP profitieren.

Niedriger diastolischer Blutdruck erhöht Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Komplikationen

25.04.2024 Hypotonie Nachrichten

Wenn unter einer medikamentösen Hochdrucktherapie der diastolische Blutdruck in den Keller geht, steigt das Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Ereignisse: Darauf deutet eine Sekundäranalyse der SPRINT-Studie hin.

Bei schweren Reaktionen auf Insektenstiche empfiehlt sich eine spezifische Immuntherapie

Insektenstiche sind bei Erwachsenen die häufigsten Auslöser einer Anaphylaxie. Einen wirksamen Schutz vor schweren anaphylaktischen Reaktionen bietet die allergenspezifische Immuntherapie. Jedoch kommt sie noch viel zu selten zum Einsatz.

Therapiestart mit Blutdrucksenkern erhöht Frakturrisiko

25.04.2024 Hypertonie Nachrichten

Beginnen ältere Männer im Pflegeheim eine Antihypertensiva-Therapie, dann ist die Frakturrate in den folgenden 30 Tagen mehr als verdoppelt. Besonders häufig stürzen Demenzkranke und Männer, die erstmals Blutdrucksenker nehmen. Dafür spricht eine Analyse unter US-Veteranen.

Update Innere Medizin

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.