Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2021

Open Access 01.12.2021 | Research article

Survival states as indicators of learning performance and biological stress in refugee children: a cross-sectional study with a comparison group

verfasst von: Andrea Hahnefeld, Thorsten Sukale, Elena Weigand, Katharina Münch, Sigrid Aberl, Lea V. Eckler, Davin Schmidt, Anna Friedmann, Paul L. Plener, Jörg M. Fegert, Volker Mall

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2021

Abstract

Background

Our goal was to accurately detect young children at risk for long-term psychiatric disturbances after potentially traumatic experiences in the course of relocation. In addition to detailed assessment of parent-rated parent and child symptomatology, we focused on disruptive behaviors in the education environment summarized as survival states, as these frequently lead to clinical referral.

Methods

We screened 52 refugee children aged 3–7 (M = 5.14 years, SD = 1.17) for symptoms of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) with the Child and Adolescent Trauma Screening (CATS) in parent rating. The parents’ mental health was assessed using the Refugee Health Screener (RHS-15). Furthermore, the child’s educators were asked to evaluate the pathological survival states of the child and we made a general assessment of the children’s symptoms with the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) rated by parents and educators. Children in the refugee sample completed a working memory learning task (Subtest Atlantis from the Kaufmann Assessment Battery for Children, KABC-II) and delivered saliva samples for testing of the cortisol level.

Results

The parental rating of their child’s PTSD symptoms was significantly related to their own mental well-being (r = .50, p < .001). Children with survival states in educator ratings exhibited weaker learning performance (F = 3.49, p < .05) and higher evening cortisol levels (U = 113, z = − 1.7, p < .05, one-tailed).

Conclusions

Survival states are promising indicators for children’s learning performance and distress level complementary to parent rating of child PTSD, which is highly intercorrelated with the parents’ own symptom load.

Trial registration

Trial registration number: DRKS00021150 on DRKS
Date of registration: 04.08.2020 retrospectively registered
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
PTSD
Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
CATS
Child and Adolescent Trauma Screening
RHS
Refugee Health Screener
SDQ
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire
KABC-II
Kaufmann Assessment Battery for Children
TST
Trauma Systems Therapy
SPZ
Social Pediatric Center
SIF
Scale of Intellectual Functioning (KABC-II)
PORTA
Providing Online Ressource and Trauma Assessment for Refugees
HPA
Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal

Background

During the recent large-scale population movement, estimations for people forcibly being displaced from their homes or escaping conflict and persecution globally rose to 68.5 million [1], with minors comprising about half of all refugees [2]. Likewise, there are increasingly more young children among asylum seekers in Germany. In 2019, children 10 years of age and younger made up 40% of the 142.509 refugees arriving in Germany [3].
The reasons and circumstances for their flight are often associated with potentially traumatic experiences, both personal and familial [4, 5], for example war, violence, separation and long routes with repeated relocation from conflict areas [6]. Children are suggested to be especially vulnerable to these cumulative psychosocial stress factors in the sense of being continuously exposed to an unpredictable environment [4, 79]. Estimates from previous studies on PTSD levels in refugee children samples who had resettled in Western countries show increased prevalence rates between 19 and 54% [4, 1012] compared to 15.9% among trauma exposed children and adolescents in the general population [13]. In our preceding study with Syrian refugees in a German camp 26% of the 0–6 -year-old children and 33% of the 7–14-year-old children fulfilled diagnostic criteria for PTSD [6]. It is particularly challenging to apply diagnostic criteria during early childhood because of rapid developmental changes and the interdependence of very young children with the primary caregiver [14]. Due to limited self-reflective and verbal abilities, symptoms of children under 6 years are often assessed by parent rating [15, 16]. A correlation between the mothers’ and the children’s symptomatology concerning PTSD [11, 1315] has been shown as well as an underestimation of PTSD symptom load in parent rating, especially concerning internalized symptoms [16, 17]. These findings underline the need of tools considering child-specific ways of expressing trauma-related stress in addition to parent rating [15]. Across different settings, the pervasiveness of the child’s symptoms may vary, therefore multimodal and observational assessment by multiple informants of the environment, the stressors and the child’s social and emotional functioning is strongly recommended [8, 14].
In previous studies, arousal symptoms were observed as the most frequent trauma-related symptoms in children 7 years and under [16], and furthermore, especially symptoms of hyperarousal are mentioned as a strong predictor of later impairments [17]. As multiple adverse experiences during childhood are associated with aggressive and violent behavior at a later point in time [7] the early evaluation of so-called “survival states “[18] as a marker for loss of behavioral control might be of help in this context.
Within the concept of survival states, the problematic manifestations of traumatic stress are understood both as a result of a child’s emotion and behavior dysregulation, as well as the inadequate capacity of their social environment to provide support. In the framework of Trauma Systems Therapy (TST) [18] there are multiple actors on the child’s needs, chiefly the parents, caregivers and educators. A critical part of TST is the so-called “survival-in-the-moment-states” or “survival states”, defined as “an individual’s experience of the present environment as threatening to his or her survival, with corresponding thoughts, emotions, behaviors, and neurochemical and neurophysiological responses” (p. 46, 18]. Following this thought, the primary developmental pathology of traumatized children is the dysregulation of emotional states when confronted by a stressor [14]. The increased sensitivity to threat cues in the environment might be a reasonable reaction to chronic variable stress caused by living in an unpredictable environment during war, flight and the relocation phase for refugee children [9]. In current descriptions of PTSD diagnostic criteria, corresponding behaviors are not adequately captured [19]. For example, a child might suddenly engage her or his fight response as a result of feeling threatened by a certain cue or wording in the kindergarten. The aggressive behavior, meant to defend herself or himself, is not tolerated and the child is punished. This could lead to repeated experiences of exclusion from social groups and educational disadvantages, hence highlighting the so-called survival states as one of the priority problems of children with a history of trauma exposure [18]. Further, growing up in an unpredictable developmental environment characterized by chronic variable stress is associated with up-regulated diurnal cortisol [9].
The concept of TST and the developmentally modified diagnostic criteria for PTSD in preschool children according to DSM-5 take into account that young children tend to express the core symptoms of PTSD (intrusions, avoidance, alterations in arousal and activity) through behavior (like irritable outbursts, phobic reactions, hyperactivity etc.) rather than being able to communicate cognitive and emotional disturbances [15, 16, 1820].
Because survival states are rated according to a standardized protocol [18] (see Method) by educators during kindergarten, pre-school or school lessons, this approach adds a complementary perspective to the parents’ view on symptom load and well-being of the potentially traumatized child. In order to answer the question whether survival states might be a useful tool to evaluate PTSD in preschool refugee children, we computed the correlation of survival states with PTSD parent rating, learning performance and biological stress evaluated by salivary cortisol.
In detail the following hypotheses were tested:
H1: The children’s symptom load is closely linked to the parents’ distress level. Children whose parents show higher levels of distress show a higher symptom load in the environment and a higher frequency of survival states in educator rating.
H2: Children with refugee background show a higher frequency of survival states in educator rating than children from an age-matched control group.
H3: Children with higher levels of post-traumatic stress indicated by survival states show a poorer learning performance and display elevated salivary cortisol levels.

Methods

Participants

The study was conducted between July 2018 and February 2020. Participants were recruited from several camps for refugees and the Social Pediatric Center (SPZ) at “kboKinderzentrum” in Munich, Germany. Data were collected directly in the camps in separate examination rooms or in the office rooms in the SPZ, respectively. The study included children with refugee background, aged 3 to 7 years. All were attended by at least one parent and were not born in Germany. Each newcomer eligible for our study was contacted by the consultant team and received information about the study. They were asked to participate and invited to the first appointment, with interpreters if necessary. About 5% declined (mostly before informed consent), leaving 72 children and their families willing to participate after informed consent. The control group was recruited in the camps and in the SPZ and consisted of children who were referred due to developmental, language and/or social-emotional problems between April 2019 and February 2020. Children with diagnoses of chronic disabling conditions were excluded.
See Table 1 for description of the sample.
Table 1
Demographic and symptom characteristics of the sample
 
Refugee Group (n = 52)
Control Group (n = 20)
Mean age in years (SD, Range)
5.14 (1.17, 3.00–7.67)
4.99 (1.41, 2.58–6.83)
Non-verbal IQ (SD, Range)
79.53 (16.33, 49–118)
86.89 (18.63, 61–133)
SDQ parent (SD, Range)
13.93 (6.83, 1–28)
11.71 (5.37, 3–19)
SDQ educators (SD, Range)
13.77 (6.90, 0–32)
15.85 (9.19, 4–40)
CATS trauma-screening
 • Events reported
32.5 (61.5%)
3 (15%)
 • Symptoms (SD, Range)
9.27 (12.17, 0–43)
0
 • Children above threshold
15 (29%)
0
 • Full DSM-V criteria for PTSD
9 (17%)
0
Survival States
28 (54%)
5 (25%)

Procedure

Informed written consent was given by the parents, and children provided verbal consent. The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of the Technical University of Munich.
All parents were asked for general information on age, sex, language and cultural background. The parents in the refugee group were asked for the duration of flight and time since arrival in Germany.
While children completed the Scale of Intellectual Functioning (SIF) of the Kaufmann-Assessment-Battery for Children (KABC-II) [21] and the learning task, parents filled out the online questionnaires mentioned below in their native language (“PORTA”) [22]. If parents had difficulties with the written format, the questionnaires were administered as interviews with interpreters, which was the case in the majority of participants.
In the refugee group, saliva samples were collected at home by parents after a practice demonstration in the office and were analyzed using the Demeditec Cortisol free in Saliva ELISA kit at Synlab [23]. The children were advised to spit in small labeled containers in the morning immediately after waking, in the afternoon (between 3 and 4 pm) and in the evening, right before going to bed. Parents were asked to collect samples on two consecutive days. All children with at least three saliva samples composing one daily profile (morning, afternoon and evening) were included in the analysis.

Assessment

Measures

Refugee health screener (RHS-15, parent self-rating)
We assessed parental well-being with the “Refugee Health Screener” (RHS-15) [24], by screening for emotional distress as a common marker across psychiatric diagnoses in many ethnic groups. The RHS is an empirically developed standard instrument and we followed the authors’ recommendation that “a score of ≥12 or a distress thermometer score of > 5 is considered a positive case” (p. 6) [24]. We calculated a sum score by multiplying the symptom score with the thermometer score.
Child and adolescent trauma screening (CATS, child screening in parent-rating)
All children were screened for potentially traumatic experiences and symptoms of PTSD with the “Child and Adolescent Trauma Screening” (CATS) [25], a short open accessible screening instrument directly based on the DSM-5 criteria for PTSD with satisfactory psychometric properties, constructed with emphasis on sensitivity. We used the recommended cut-off ≥16 (total symptom score) as an indicator of a clinically relevant level of symptoms in preschool children.
Strengths and difficulties questionnaire” (SDQ, child assessment in parent- and educator-rating)
The “Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire” (SDQ) [26] is a fully evaluated and widely used brief behavioral screening questionnaire for the age group 3–17 years with 25 items and forms for parents and educators. The SDQ is reported to be able to distinguish between community and clinical samples [27].
TST assessment form (assessment of child’s survival states in educator rating)
We assessed survival states in the environment via educator ratings with the German version of the “TST Assessment Form “[18], translated by Andrea Hahnefeld (2018), asking for “shifts in awareness, affect, and action, when a threat is perceived in the present environment” (p. 399) [18] and also enquiring if these episodes include potentially dangerous behaviors and/or result in any problem with the child’s functioning [18].
Kaufmann-assessment-battery for children (KABC-II)
Using the culture-fair, reliable and neuropsychologically developed instrument “Kaufmann-Assessment-Battery for Children “(KABC-II) [21] we assessed the children’s cognitive abilities on a language-free scale (Scale of Intellectual Functioning, SIF). The KABC SIF has good psychometric properties concerning construct validity (.59–.72 with other non-verbal test batteries). Only small score differences between ethnic groups are marked [21].
The memory-task (subtest Atlantis) to assess the children’s short-term learning performance shows low intercorrelations with the general cognitive scale (.28) and is reported to be reliable (.97) and independent from the parents’ migration status [21].
With low correlations with tests for short-term selective attention (−.06 SIF, −.02 Atlantis) both tasks show good discriminant validity [21].
Psychometric testing and statistical analyses
We conducted nonparametric correlations (Spearman rank correlation, one-tailed), cross-tabs (refugee vs. control group/survival states yes vs. no) with χ2-Test, one-way ANOVA with factors CATS (parent rating) and survival states (educator rating), respectively, and non-parametric Mann-Whitney-U-Test for comparing the salivary cortisol levels in groups with and without survival states in the refugee group. All calculations were done with IBM SPSS Statistics Version 25.

Results

Participants

Of initially 72 eligible children with refugee background, complete data could be obtained for 52 children (58% boys, 42% girls) from 44 families, which was mostly due to short-term and unpredictable transfers of the families and personnel turnover in the camps or infrequent participation of the children in playgroups. For 37 refugee children valid cortisol samples could be collected. Participants with and without saliva samples did not differ in age, flight duration, symptom load child and parent, IQ or learning performance. The mean duration of time since arrival in Germany was 19 months (SD 17.72, range 0.5–60 months) after an average flight duration of 24 months (SD 34.70, range 0–140 months). Families were from 14 nations, mostly of Nigerian (14), Afghan (13), Syrian (7) origin in the refugee group, and from 13 nations and mostly of Nigerian (4), German (4) and Turkish (2) origin in the control group. The control group consisted of 20 children born and raised in Germany (45% boys, 55% girls) from 19 families. The groups did not differ in age (t = −.46 (70), p > .05), IQ (t = 1.60 (66), p > .05) or general symptom load in the SDQ (parent- und educator-rating, t = − 1.22 (66), p > .05 and t = 1.04 (70), p > .05). See Table 1 for description of the sample.

Test results

H1: Parents’ own distress level and parent rating of the child vs. children’s symptom load in the environment in educator rating.
Potentially traumatic experiences in parent rating were reported for 32 of the children with refugee background (61,5%) and for 3 children in the control group (15%). No symptoms were reported by the parent rated CATS in the control group, whereas in the refugee group, 15 children (29%) were scored above threshold (see Table 1).
There were significant intercorrelations within all the variables that were assessed in parent rating. For Spearman rank correlations RHS, CATS, SDQ parent and educator rating see Table 2, for correlation CATS-RHS see Fig. 1.
Table 2
Correlation coefficients (Spearman rank correlations, one-tailed) for CATS (parent rating of the child), RHS (parent self-rating), SDQ (parent and educator rating of the child) and survival states (educator rating) in the refugee group
 
CATS
RHS
SDQ-parent
SDQ-educator
CATS
1
.50b
.47b
.15
RHS
 
1
.64b
.13
SDQ-parent
  
1
.44b
Survival States
.25a
−.03
−.03
.35b
ap < .05
bP < .001
With correlations of RHS and SDQ educator of r = .13 (p = .18) and survival states of r = −.03 (p = .41), the hypothesis that children of highly distressed parents displayed more symptoms in the education environment could not be confirmed.
Concerning the general symptom load (SDQ), parent and educator ratings showed a significant correlation (r = .44, p < .001), but there was no correlation between CATS in parent rating and general symptom-load in educator rating (r = .15, p = .14).
H2: Survival states.
Survival states in the education environment were reported for 28 children (54%) in the refugee group and for 5 children (25%) in the control group. χ2-Test yielded significant results (χ2 = 4,84 (1), p < .05), survival states in the education environment were reported significantly more often for refugee children than for control children.
The survival states assessed in educator rating correlated with the CATS-scores above threshold in parent rating (r = .25, p < .05) and showed a significant correlation with the general symptom score (SDQ) in educator rating (r = .35, p < .001), see Table 2.
H3: Short-term learning performance and salivary cortisol levels.
In relation to refugee group short-term learning performance, a one-factor ANOVA was performed with IQ and subtest Atlantis as variables. This yielded a significant main effect for subtest Atlantis (F = 3.49 (50,1), p < .05, one-tailed). Children exhibiting survival states showed lower short-term learning performance (see Fig. 2). There was no significant main effect for IQ (F = .46 (47,1), p = .25, one-tailed).
By computing ANOVA with group-factor CATS traumatic vs. non-traumatic scoring there were no significant main effects for learning performance or IQ (F = 1.25 (50,1), p = .13; F = 1.22 (47,1), p = .14, one-tailed).
In the refugee group, children with survival states displayed significantly higher evening salivary cortisol levels than children without survival states (Mann-Whitney-Ue = 113, z = − 1.7, p < .05, one-tailed) with a tendency for more variant and higher levels of total diurnal cortisol (Mann-Whitney-Udiurnal = 118, z = − 1.5, p = .08, one-tailed) and significantly higher levels summed up for afternoon and evening cortisol (Mann-Whitney-Uae = 112, z = − 1.7, p < .05, one-tailed), see Fig. 3.
No significant effects could be observed for CATS scores as group variable concerning salivary cortisol level for any time of the day (Mann-Whitney-Ue = 128.5, z = −.70, p = .25, one-tailed, for evening salivary cortisol, Mann-Whitney-Udiurnal = 132, z = −.54, p = .28, one-tailed, for diurnal salivary cortisol).
Whereas most of the children without survival states had a normal diurnal slope with elevated levels of cortisol in the morning and declining values during daytime, reaching the nadir at prebedtime in the evening with values close to zero, a number of children in the refugee group with survival states showed elevated levels of cortisol at prebedtime measurement.
As expected, children with survival states showed a weaker short-term learning performance and displayed higher cortisol levels with significant differences in the evening (prebedtime) values than children without survival states, see Figs. 2 and 3.
Children with higher evening and diurnal cortisol levels had longer duration of flight from home country (re = .45, rdiurnal = .56, p < .001) and shorter time since arrival in Germany (re = −.39, rdiurnal = −.42, p < .05). We also found a longer duration of flight phase for children rated positive in CATS by their parents (r = .29, p < .05).

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study evaluating survival states rated by educators as expressions of increased sensitivity to threat cues in the environment in preschool refugee children. Primarily, we confirmed that the refugee children’s symptom load is closely linked to the parents’ distress level, when both is assessed in parent rating. But contrary to our expectations, children with highly distressed parents did not show a higher symptom load or a higher frequency of survival states in the environment assessed by educator rating.
Further, our results yielded that survival states arise more often in refugee children than in control children born and raised in Germany. As a main result concerning survival states within the refugee group, we found poorer learning performance and elevated salivary cortisol levels for those children with survival states in the education environment. There was no corresponding effect for learning performance and salivary cortisol for refugee children rated positive for PTSD in parent rating.
We conclude from this that the assessment of survival states in the environment adds important information to identify the risk for long-term psychiatric disturbances after potentially traumatic experiences in the course of relocation for several reasons: a) documenting highly disruptive behavior expressing elevated arousal in young children, b) focusing on the child in a group environment and c) offering a complementary perspective to the parents’ view on trauma-related child symptomatology by educator rating.
Relating to a) and b) and concerning the role of impairment, adequately regulated social behavior in a group environment can be seen as one of the core competencies during rapid development in childhood. Especially when adapting to a new environment, behavioral regulation is a prerequisite to fulfill further developmental tasks [28]. Language acquisition happens more effectively in social interactions, and the majority of formal education usually takes place in a group environment [29].
However, due to their previous experiences in the face of adversity, some refugee children do still show a behavior which facilitated survival in the face of potential danger but which is highly dysfunctional in the new context of a secure education environment in a non-malevolent ambiance [18, 30]. For example, immediate strong reactions to small triggers signaling danger and self-defending reactions can be life-saving during flight but showing disruptive behavior after being triggered for no obvious reason in the classroom is highly undesirable.
In this context, to offer a complementary perspective to parent rating of the child’s trauma-related symptomatology (c) is particularly important, since solely applying the diagnostic category of PTSD in parent rating is reported to underestimate the children’s trauma-specific symptom load and consecutive impairment [16, 17], especially in the long term [3035]. In line with that, we found higher rates for survival states in educator rating than CATS-scores above threshold in parent rating in both groups (54% vs. 29% in the refugee group, 25% vs. 0% in the control group). It is remarkable that the children’s general symptom load was not underestimated by their parents as we were able to demonstrate a significant correlation between parent and educator SDQ-rating. This points towards specific reasons for the mismatch concerning their child’s trauma-related psychopathology such as the parents’ own trauma experience and their efforts to deal with its sequela in daily life (for example by reinstituting familiar routines). This might reduce their capacity and judgement of the child’s trauma-related problem load, an assumption which is further supported by our finding that 38.5% of the parents in our highly vulnerable sample of the refugee group did not indicate any potentially traumatic experiences for their children after flight and relocation. Perhaps parents were reluctant in reporting these experiences for their children in a questionnaire format. Communicating their child’s traumatic experiences could as well be difficult owing to the parents’ own PTSD symptoms (e.g. avoidance, concentration problems or strong emotional arousal) [8]. Correspondingly, in a prospective follow-up of preschool children by Scheeringa et al. [32] two to three times more children were considered functionally impaired by their caregivers than were diagnosed with PTSD in parent rating, with growing discrepancy between these two measures in the course of 2 years. The authors found that overall PTSD symptoms (evaluated by parent interviews) did not decrease over time, even with community treatment. They report significantly more children to be functionally impaired one (49%) and 2 years (74%) after first assessment for PTSD (23%). The number of positive ratings for survival states in our study rather equals the rate of children being functionally impaired 1 year after first assessment for PTSD in the study by Scheeringa et al. [32]. Because we and others [8, 16, 33] provide evidence that trauma-specific aspects of their child’s mental health may be difficult to be perceived or communicated by parents, educator rating of survival states might be an important supplement in PTSD diagnosis. Considering this, we correlated survival states with other markers that are connected with high levels of stress and impairment.
It is well known that early adversity and its impact on later functioning is associated with alterations in the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA) axis [9, 30, 3639] with initial upregulation of the stress response system (hypercortisolism) and later downregulation [40] in a way that early life stress programs the developing brain to react differently to future stressors [37, 41, 42]. In our study, we found refugee children with survival states to display higher levels of salivary cortisol in the evening than those children who did not show that kind of episodes in the education environment. As illustrated in Fig. 3, cortisol was high near wake-up for both groups and decreased over the day. The group without survival states showed a normal diurnal rhythm with declines of pre-bedtime values of cortisol close to zero. For children with survival states there is a marked inability to bring cortisol concentrations to low levels in the evening, which might reflect a fundamental dysregulation of the neuroendocrine system, as reported in previous studies [36, 38, 43]. In their review, Kuhlman et al. [9] conclude that children growing up in conditions of unpredictable events show increased neural sensitivity to threat which is in turn related to more frequent activation of the physiological stress system and elevated diurnal cortisol. Comparably, we found higher cortisol levels in children with experiences of war/flight/relocation not only to be correlated with survival states but also with longer flight durations and shorter time of residence in Germany. As the measurement point for salivary cortisol in our sample was relatively close to the traumatic experiences or even during ongoing stress, we may have captured the initial up-regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) system [40].
Furthermore, there is a line of evidence linking higher levels of daytime cortisol in combination with adversity and high arousal symptoms to poorer cognitive development in the long term [38, 44, 45]. Raffington et al. [46] performed a study concerning the link between cortisol secretion, reduced hippocampal volume and memory performance in children from high stress environments. They found lower family income to be associated with smaller hippocampal volume, flatter cortisol awakening reaction and hyporeactivity in cortisol stress reaction. This, in turn, was related to lower memory function [46]. An early impairment of learning performance in children with high levels of salivary cortisol might be setting the stage for poorer cognitive outcome in the long-term. This is especially important as the majority of the children in our sample were examined during the sensitive phase of hippocampus development at age 3–5 years [36, 45] and those with disturbing survival states displayed a lower short-term learning performance.
Although the one-point survey-design of the study does not allow causal inferences with our data, we established the link between survival states as behavioral disturbances due to high arousal, elevated cortisol levels in the evening and learning difficulties.
While using widespread standardized questionnaires, we noticed that parents had trouble with the forms and were sometimes restrained to answer or disclose too much. Other studies report similar issues, suggesting the validity of psycho-diagnostic measures developed in western populations might be restrictive for refugees from non-western countries [10]. As it is common practice for other psychiatric disorders during childhood (for example, autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder are characterized by certain patterns of behavior across multiple settings) [47], we added a complementary perspective to parent view with supplemental assessment of survival states by educators in a different context. Even more so, shifting the focus from the individual or families to a more public health approach as it is done in global mental health might be useful in our system as well when being confronted with patients from different cultural backgrounds [48]. A suggestion for further research might be to consider the effects of corresponding therapeutic treatment according to trauma systems therapy (TST) [18] for children with high biological stress and associated learning difficulties: Concluding from our results, treatment of dysregulated young (refugee) children should not only focus on child and family, but also operate through trauma-sensitive child-care in the social and education environment.
The unique combination of the multi-rater- and multi-method-perspective to link behavioral and questionnaire data with learning performance and the objectively observable underlying neurobiological marker of salivary cortisol can be seen as a strength of this study, measuring not only the occurrence of events but also their impact.
For organizational reasons and because of characteristics of the refugee sample, we could not control for socioeconomic status. Voluntariness and the particular challenge for the parents to collect saliva samples add further potential bias of the sample, reducing the ability to extrapolate the findings, although we did not find any differences in the variables assessed between children with and without saliva samples.
The heterogeneity of the sample with refugees from 14 different countries limits the generalization for certain ethnic groups, but at the same time yields a good representation of the general refugee population in Germany. Chiefly underlying deficits in behavior regulation, salivary cortisol levels and learning behavior might be universal and not limited to certain ethnic subgroups, as in Scheeringa and Zeanah’s study [32] differences by ethnic background and gender were reported largely non-significant.
Additionally, the control group was also composed of families with a multinational background. As a clinical sample, the control group is from a help-seeking cohort and not representing the general population, which further highlights the relevance of our results.

Conclusions

In our refugee sample, survival states in the education environment were reported more often than trauma-screenings above threshold in parent rating. The assessment of survival states proved to be feasible and valid, as we found significant correlations with lower learning performance and elevated evening salivary cortisol levels, which in turn are known to be associated with adversity and higher levels of distress. As for other psychiatric disorders during childhood, the educator perspective should be considered as an additional measure for the assessment of PTSD symptoms in preschool refugee children.

Acknowledgements

Statistical expert of the study was Dr. rer. Nat. Victoria Kehl, Chair of Medical Informatics, Statistics and Epidemiology, TUM University Hospital, Technical University of Munich, Germany.

Role of the funding body

The funding body did not participate in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data or in writing the manuscript.

Declarations

The research was approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of the Technical University Munich (221–18 s, 19.06.18).
All participating parents provided written consent and all participating children provided verbal consent.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

PP was involved in clinical trials with Lundbeck and Servier. He served as a speaker for Shire. He obtained funding from the German Federal Ministry of education and research (BMBF), the German Federal Institute for Drugs and Medical Devices (BfArM), the state foundation of Baden-Wuerttemberg and the VW foundation.
JF served as speaker for several German foundations, Infectopharm, UNICEF and several German ministries. He is involved as a consultant in several German Federal Ministries. He obtained funding from several European and German Foundations (e.g. BMG, BMBF, BMFSFJ, DFG, G-BA Innovationsfonds).
For all the other authors, there are no conflicts of interest to be stated.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Fegert JM, Diehl C, Leyendecker B, et al. Psychosocial problems in traumatized refugee families: overview of risks and some recommendations for support services. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2018;12:1–8. Fegert JM, Diehl C, Leyendecker B, et al. Psychosocial problems in traumatized refugee families: overview of risks and some recommendations for support services. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2018;12:1–8.
7.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Fegert JM, Sukale T, Brown RC. Mental health service provision for child and adolescent refugees: European perspectives. In: Hodes M, Shur-Fen S, de Vries PJ, editors. Understanding uniqueness and diversity in child and adolescent mental health. Elsevier: Academic Press; 2018. p. 195–222. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-815310-9.00008-3. Fegert JM, Sukale T, Brown RC. Mental health service provision for child and adolescent refugees: European perspectives. In: Hodes M, Shur-Fen S, de Vries PJ, editors. Understanding uniqueness and diversity in child and adolescent mental health. Elsevier: Academic Press; 2018. p. 195–222. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​B978-0-12-815310-9.​00008-3.
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruf M, Schauer M, Elbert T. Prävalenz von traumatischen Stresserfahrungen und seelischen Erkrankungen bei in Deutschland lebenden Kindern von Asylbewerbern. ZeitschriftfürKlinischePsychologie und Psychotherapie. 2010;39(3):151–60. Ruf M, Schauer M, Elbert T. Prävalenz von traumatischen Stresserfahrungen und seelischen Erkrankungen bei in Deutschland lebenden Kindern von Asylbewerbern. ZeitschriftfürKlinischePsychologie und Psychotherapie. 2010;39(3):151–60.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Blackmore R, Gray KM, Boyle JA, Fazel M, Ranasinha S, Fitzgerald G, et al. Systematic review and meta-analysis: the prevalence of mental illness in child and adolescent refugees and asylum seekers. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020;59(6):705–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.11.011 Epub 2019 Nov 26. Blackmore R, Gray KM, Boyle JA, Fazel M, Ranasinha S, Fitzgerald G, et al. Systematic review and meta-analysis: the prevalence of mental illness in child and adolescent refugees and asylum seekers. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020;59(6):705–14. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​jaac.​2019.​11.​011 Epub 2019 Nov 26.
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Egger HL, Emde RN. Developmentally sensitive diagnostic criteria for mental health disorders in early childhood: the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders-IV, the research diagnostic criteria-preschool age, and the diagnostic classification of mental health and developmental disorders of infancy and early childhood-revised. Am Psychol. 2011;66(2):95–106. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021026. Egger HL, Emde RN. Developmentally sensitive diagnostic criteria for mental health disorders in early childhood: the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders-IV, the research diagnostic criteria-preschool age, and the diagnostic classification of mental health and developmental disorders of infancy and early childhood-revised. Am Psychol. 2011;66(2):95–106. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​a0021026.
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Gadeberg AK, Montgomery E, Frederiksen HW, Norredam M. Assessing trauma and mental health in refugee children and youth: a systematic review of validated screening and measurement tools. Eur J Pub Health. 2017;27(3):439–46. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckx034. Gadeberg AK, Montgomery E, Frederiksen HW, Norredam M. Assessing trauma and mental health in refugee children and youth: a systematic review of validated screening and measurement tools. Eur J Pub Health. 2017;27(3):439–46. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1093/​eurpub/​ckx034.
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen JA, Scheeringa MS. Post-traumatic stress disorder diagnosis in children: challenges and promises. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2009;11(1):91–9. Cohen JA, Scheeringa MS. Post-traumatic stress disorder diagnosis in children: challenges and promises. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2009;11(1):91–9.
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Saxe GN, Ellis BH, Brown AD. Trauma systems therapy for children and teens. 2nd ed. New York: Guildford Press; 2016. Saxe GN, Ellis BH, Brown AD. Trauma systems therapy for children and teens. 2nd ed. New York: Guildford Press; 2016.
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldbeck L, Jensen TK. The diagnostic spectrum of trauma-related disorders in children and adolescents. In: Landolt MA, Cloitre M, Schnyder U, editors. Evidence-based treatments for trauma related disorders in children and adolescents. Cham: Springer; 2017. p. 3–28. Goldbeck L, Jensen TK. The diagnostic spectrum of trauma-related disorders in children and adolescents. In: Landolt MA, Cloitre M, Schnyder U, editors. Evidence-based treatments for trauma related disorders in children and adolescents. Cham: Springer; 2017. p. 3–28.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaufmann AS, Kaufmann NL, Kaufmann A. Kaufmann-Assessment-Battery for Children (KABC-II). In: Melchers P, Melchers M, editors. DeutschsprachigeFassung. Frankfurt/Main: Pearson; 2015. Kaufmann AS, Kaufmann NL, Kaufmann A. Kaufmann-Assessment-Battery for Children (KABC-II). In: Melchers P, Melchers M, editors. DeutschsprachigeFassung. Frankfurt/Main: Pearson; 2015.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Demeditec Diagnostics GmbH. Product information. User’s Manual. Cortisol free in Saliva ELISA. Enzyme immunoassay for the quantitative in vitro diagnostic measurement of active free cortisol in saliva Version 15–01/19. Updated 190108. Kiel Germany. 2019. Demeditec Diagnostics GmbH. Product information. User’s Manual. Cortisol free in Saliva ELISA. Enzyme immunoassay for the quantitative in vitro diagnostic measurement of active free cortisol in saliva Version 15–01/19. Updated 190108. Kiel Germany. 2019.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Klasen H, Woerner W, Rothenberger A, et al. Die deutsche Fassung des StrengthsandDifficultiesQuestionnaire (SDQ-Deu) – Übersicht und Bewertung erster Validierungs- und Normierungsbefunde. PraxKinderpsycholKinderpsychiat. 2003;52:491–502. Klasen H, Woerner W, Rothenberger A, et al. Die deutsche Fassung des StrengthsandDifficultiesQuestionnaire (SDQ-Deu) – Übersicht und Bewertung erster Validierungs- und Normierungsbefunde. PraxKinderpsycholKinderpsychiat. 2003;52:491–502.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Korte M. Wie Kinder heute lernen. 2nded. München: Random House; 2011. Korte M. Wie Kinder heute lernen. 2nded. München: Random House; 2011.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Spitzer M. Lernen: Gehirnforschung und Schule des Lebens. Spektrum: Heidelberg, Berlin; 2002. Spitzer M. Lernen: Gehirnforschung und Schule des Lebens. Spektrum: Heidelberg, Berlin; 2002.
33.
34.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Plener PL, Ignatius A, Huber-Lang M, Fegert JM. The consequences of child abuse, maltreatment and neglect on mental and somatic health in adulthood. Nervenheilkunde. 2017;36:161–7. Plener PL, Ignatius A, Huber-Lang M, Fegert JM. The consequences of child abuse, maltreatment and neglect on mental and somatic health in adulthood. Nervenheilkunde. 2017;36:161–7.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Roth G, Strüber N. Wie das Gehirn die Seele macht. 4th ed. Klett-Cotta: Stuttgart; 2015. Roth G, Strüber N. Wie das Gehirn die Seele macht. 4th ed. Klett-Cotta: Stuttgart; 2015.
44.
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Hebebrand J, Anagnostopoulos D, Eliez S, Linse H, Pejovic-Milovancevic M, Klasen H. A first assessment of the needs of young refugees arriving in Europe: what mental health professionals need to know. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;25(1):1–6. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-015-0807-0. Hebebrand J, Anagnostopoulos D, Eliez S, Linse H, Pejovic-Milovancevic M, Klasen H. A first assessment of the needs of young refugees arriving in Europe: what mental health professionals need to know. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;25(1):1–6. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s00787-015-0807-0.
Metadaten
Titel
Survival states as indicators of learning performance and biological stress in refugee children: a cross-sectional study with a comparison group
verfasst von
Andrea Hahnefeld
Thorsten Sukale
Elena Weigand
Katharina Münch
Sigrid Aberl
Lea V. Eckler
Davin Schmidt
Anna Friedmann
Paul L. Plener
Jörg M. Fegert
Volker Mall
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2021
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2021
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-021-03233-y

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2021

BMC Psychiatry 1/2021 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).