Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2013

Open Access 01.12.2013 | Research article

Temporal organization of rest defined by actigraphy data in healthy and childhood chronic fatigue syndrome children

verfasst von: Minako Kawabata, Taro Ueno, Jun Tomita, Junko Kawatani, Akemi Tomoda, Shoen Kume, Kazuhiko Kume

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2013

Abstract

Background

Accumulating evidence has shown a universality in the temporal organization of activity and rest among animals ranging from mammals to insects. Previous reports in both humans and mice showed that rest bout durations followed long-tailed (i.e., power-law) distributions, whereas activity bouts followed exponential distributions. We confirmed similar results in the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster. Conversely, another report showed that the awakening bout durations, which were defined by polysomnography in bed, followed power-law distributions, while sleeping periods, which may correspond to rest, followed exponential distributions. This apparent discrepancy has been left to be resolved.

Methods

Actigraphy data from healthy and disordered children were analyzed separately for two periods: time out of bed (UP period) and time in bed (DOWN period).

Results

When data over a period of 24 h were analyzed as a whole, rest bouts showed a power law distribution as previously reported. However, when UP and DOWN period data were analyzed separately, neither showed power law properties. Using a newly developed strict method, only 30% of individuals satisfied the power law criteria, even when the 24 h data were analyzed. The human results were in contrast to the Drosophila results, which revealed clear power-law distributions for both day time and night time rest through the use of a strict method. In addition, we analyzed the actigraphy data from patients with childhood type chronic fatigue syndrome (CCFS), and found that they showed differences from healthy controls when their UP and DOWN data were analyzed separately.

Conclusions

These results suggested that the DOWN sleep, the bout distribution of which showed exponential properties, contributes to the production of long-tail distributions in human rest periods. We propose that separate analysis of UP and DOWN period data is important for understanding the temporal organization of activity.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1471-244X-13-281) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Minako Kawabata, Taro Ueno contributed equally to this work.

Competing interests

The authors declare they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

All authors contributed to and approved the final version of the manuscript. MK, TU, JT, JK, AT, SK and KK designed the study. JK and AT acquired the data, MK, TU, JT, SK and KK conducted the data analysis, interpretation and drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Background

Most natural phenomena appear to occur stochastically and this random distribution of phenomena is thought to produce Poisson distributions. However, it has been shown recently that many phenomena, such as human behavior, form non-Poisson distributions. For example, interevent intervals of social behaviors such as e-mail communications and trade transactions follow power-law distributions [1]. Nakamura et al. showed that rest bout durations followed long-tailed (i.e., power-law) distributions, whereas activity bouts followed exponential distributions in both humans and mice [2, 3]. A similar temporal organization of rest and activity bouts has also been observed in invertebrates. In the insect Drosophila melanogaster, waiting intervals between behavioral episodes such as walking, feeding, and flight maneuvers follow the power-law distribution [48]. Using a video-recording method, we recently confirmed that the duration of the rest bout for the flies followed a power-law distribution [9], even when a strict method described by Clauset et al. [10] was employed. We recently clarified the dopaminergic circuit regulating arousal, which will bring insight how these temporal organization is installed in the brain circuit [11].
In contrast, the use of an electroencephalogram during bed time has shown that awakening bout durations follow power-law distributions, while sleeping periods follow exponential distributions [12]. This discrepancy is unexpected, since wakefulness and sleep are thought to correspond to activity and rest, respectively. We wanted to solve this apparent discrepancy, since actigraphy is more handy and versatile than polysomngraphy in routine situations and its importance in clinical diagnosis is increasing [1316]. Therefore, in order to address this issue, we analyzed human actigraphy data that was separated into time out of bed (UP) and time in bed (DOWN) periods. We also examined actigraphy data from childhood chronic fatigue syndrome (CCFS) patients. Our results revealed that separate analysis of UP and DOWN period data is crucial for understanding the temporal organization of activity, and that the rest bout during DOWN period, most of which correspond to sleep, shows exponential distributions, and this plays an important role for the production of long-tail distributions in human rest periods.

Methods

Subjects and ethical consideration

Healthy control and CCFS patient subjects were recruited as described previously [17]. Briefly, CCFS patients were recruited from patients who visited Kumamoto University Hospital between April 2007 and December 2008 because of CFS-like symptoms. 127 patients with CCFS diagnosis were assessed and 70 (37 male, 33 female, age 9 to 18 y. o.) of them were enrolled. Healthy control subjects were recruited from the regional middle school, and 34 healthy middle school teenager subjects (15 male, 18 female, 1 unknown sex, 13 to 15), underwent actigraphy examination. This study was approved by the institutional review board of Kumamoto University. Written consents were obtained from all of the subjects.

Actigraphy

Actigraphy examinations were performed using a Micro-Mini Motionlogger (Ambulatory Monitoring Inc., AMI, NY, USA) with Zero-Cross mode (ZCM) using 1 min bins, which was worn on the wrist of the non-dominant arm. In ZCM mode, the actigraph counts the number of times the accelerometer waveform crosses 0, which means the gravity velocity value changes either from minus to plus or from plus to minus, for each time period. Subjects were asked to wear it at all times over a two week period, except for when they bathed or engaged in hard physical exercise.
After collection of the data, we manually screened all the data and excluded subjects who met the following criteria: (1) when the total recorded period was less than 3 days; (2) when more than half of the data was bad; or (3) when the data was clearly abnormal, which suggested a problem with the instrument. As a result of this screening process, we used 23 healthy control subjects (10 male, 12 female, 1 unknown) and 59 CCFS subjects (30 male, 29 female).

Data analysis

Using Action W-2 software from AMI, we manually labeled the bad bins (when the subjects took off the instrument; Figure 1a, colored in purple). The software calculated sleep time using the algorithm by Cole et al. [18]. Then we determined DOWN bins, which denoted the time that the subjects were supposed to be in bed, semi-automatically using the same software and followed this with manual corrections (Figure 1a, colored in light blue). Intervals not labeled as DOWN are labeled as UP. Note that only one continuous DOWN bin interval is labeled per day, and the UP / DOWN intervals basically correspond to UP and DOWN. The raw data (activity counts by ZCM mode, indicator of good/bad, indicator of up/down) for each min bin were then exported to a comma-separated value file.
Then, using custom-made software developed using the mathematical platform R (R-Development-Core-Team), the data was processed. The process and the software are essentially the same as described previously [9]. First, all of the bad bins were removed. ‘Bad bin’ is the period when we assumed the subject removed the actigraph from their wrist, so there was no movements for an extended time. Bad bins are typically observed when the subjects take a bath. They are asked to remove actigraph when they take a bath in a traditional Japanese way, where they soak themselves in a bathtub for an extended time. Second, the threshold value for the activity counts was calculated, using either the averaging method described in Nakamura et al. [3] or the k-means clustering method used in Ueno et al. [9]; Figure 1b, see Results). Third, three classes of data set were made, namely a 24-h data set containing all of the data, a UP data set containing only the UP interval data, and a DOWN data set containing only the DOWN interval data. Fourth, each 1-min bin was classified into one of two categories, namely rest or activity according to the activity threshold value, or a rest or activity episode that was defined by a series of consecutive rest or activity bins, respectively. Thus, rest episodes and activity episodes alternated. The length of the rest or activity episode was defined as a rest or activity bout, respectively. Fifth, the resulting rest and activity bouts were statistically analyzed as described previously [9].
To quantify the distribution of bouts, we calculated the complementary cumulative probability distribution of bouts, P(x≥a), where x is the variable representing the bout, and a is the designated duration length. P(x≥a) represents the fraction of the rest or activity episode that has a length larger than a (min), respectively. Using the probability density function p(x), the cumulative probability is P x a = a p x dx . Our main interest is the property of p(x) and hence P(x). Cumulative distributions of rest and activity bouts were calculated from the data from individuals (Figure 1c, d).
To test the plausibility of the power-law hypothesis compared to alternative distributions without long tails, we performed a likelihood ratio test [10]. The likelihood ratio is defined as the likelihood of the data under the estimated power law to that under an alternative distribution estimated by the maximum likelihood method. If this value is a large positive number, the power-law assumption is considered to be plausible compared to the alternative distribution. As alternative distributions, we chose a distribution without long tails, i.e., the exponential distribution p(x) = λe − λx .
Furthermore, we fitted a power-law P(x) ∝ x − α + 1 to the cumulative distribution of rest or activity bouts using a maximum likelihood method that carefully estimates the lower bound of x, as well as a[10]. Assuming that our data are drawn from a distribution that follows a power-law, we can derive maximum likelihood estimators. The data are most likely to have been generated by the model using a parameter that maximizes this function. Next, we performed a goodness-of-fit test to determine the plausibility of the power-law hypothesis. To this end, we generated 1,000 surrogate data sets using a nonparametric bootstrap method, each being a set of artificial bouts sampled from the estimated power-law distribution. To calculate the deviation of a distribution, either empirical or artificial, from the estimated power-law distribution, we used the Kolmogorov-Smirnov (KS) statistic, which is the maximum distance between the cumulative distributions of the two distributions to be compared. The resulting p-value was defined as the fraction of surrogate realizations, such that the KS distance between the distribution generated from a surrogate data set and the estimated power law is larger than the KS distance between the empirical distribution and the estimated power law. Therefore, a large p-value implies that the power-law distribution reasonably fits the original data. Note that the notion of the p-value introduced here is different from the standard one, in which a smaller p-value in the goodness-of-fit test indicates more significance. To avoid confusion, we refer to the notion of p-value introduced here as p′-value.

Ethical considerations

This study was approved by the ethical committee of Kumamoto University. Written informed consents were obtained from the subject or their parent before the study.

Results

Threshold validation

First, we investigated whether the difference in the threshold value affected the temporal distribution profile. Nakamura et al. used the modified average value as the threshold. They removed the bins with the value of zero from the calculation of the average and confirmed that the results were stable after changing the threshold value within the range of 0.6 fold to 1.6 fold of the modified average value [2]. As shown in Figure 1b, the activity counts show a clear bimodal distribution. Since we were trying to analyze the temporal distribution of two different states, rest and activity, a clustering method was considered to be more appropriate than an average value. In the previous study, we actually used a k-means clustering method which worked well [9]. We calculated the threshold value using both methods in control and CCFS subjects. As shown in Figure 2a, the modified average method yielded larger values with larger standard deviations than the k-means clustering method. There were, however, significant correlations between the two values (correlation coefficient = 0.21 and 0.31 for control and CCFS subjects). We used k-means clustering in the following analysis since the threshold values calculated were within the range confirmed to give the similar results in the previous study described above.

Temporal organization of rest and activity of control and CCFS subjects

First, we analyzed and compared the 24 h data from healthy control subjects with CCFS subjects. For both groups, rest durations showed a gradual downslope curve with a slight upward flexion around 10 min, followed by a rapid downward flexion around 100 min (Figure 3a, e). The average curves for the two groups were similar in shape (Figure 3i) and activity durations showed a smooth, continuously decreasing curve for both groups (Figure 3d, h). The shape of the average graphs looked similar, but the CCFS group decreased more rapidly than the control group (Figure 3l).
We then re-analyzed the rest data after separation into UP and DOWN periods. The rest durations during the UP period showed a rapid linear decrease for both groups (Figure 3b, f). The average slopes for the two groups were similar, though the control group revealed a slightly steeper decrease (Figure 3j). The rest durations during the DOWN period showed a smooth, continuously decreasing curve for both groups (Figure 3c, g) and the average curves for the two groups almost completely overlapped (Figure 3k). The shapes for the average curves for rest are remarkably distinct for the 24 h, UP and DOWN periods.
Since almost all the activity episodes during the DOWN periods are short (less than 5 min), there were not enough data of activity during DOWN period for statistical analysis for regression, and the probability distribution of activity during UP period is almost indistinguishable from that during 24H period. In addition, since the main purpose of this study is to examine the power-law properties of rest period distributions, we did not include separately analyzed data of the activity.

Quantitative analysis of rest duration

Next, we quantitatively analyzed the distribution pattern for rest duration. All the results are summarized in Tables 1 and 2. Tables 1 and 2 show the individual data for control and CCFS subjects, respectively. First, we compared the distribution of each subject with power law and exponential distributions using a likelihood ratio test. When the likelihood ratio (LR) was a positive value with a p value less than 0.05, the distribution was significantly closer to the power law than to the exponential. When the LR was a negative value with a p value less than 0.05, it was significantly closer to the exponential than to the power law. Otherwise, there was no significant deviation from either distribution, and thus the distribution favors to neither of them. When the p value is less than 0.05, it is written in red. As summarized in Figure 4a, all cases from both control and CCFS subject groups showed distributions closer to the power law distribution than to the exponential distribution for the 24 h data. However, when the UP period data were analyzed separately, only about 35% of control and 56% of CCFS subjects showed significant similarity to the power law over the exponential distribution. The rest (65% of control and 44% of CCFS) favored neither to the power law nor to the exponential distribution. Moreover, no DOWN data showed any similarity to the power law in either subject group. On the contrary, more than 60% of control and 10% of CCFS DOWN data were similar to the exponential distribution, while the rest (40% of control and 90% of CCFS) favored neither to the power law nor to the exponential distribution (Figure 4b). In addition, we analyzed the 24 h activity data for both groups, but none was judged to be close to the power law (data not shown).
Table 1
Statistical tests for the significance of the power-law property in distributions of rest bouts according to the method described by Clauset et al. (control)
No.
Basic parameters
Power law vs. exponential
Power law test
24h
UP
DOWN
n
<x>
Total
sd
LR
p
LR
p
LR
p
xmax
xmin
α
ntail
p’
1
570
12.3
6998
34.8
12.4
0.00
1.5
0.13
-6.8
0.00
416
1
1.66
570
0.14
2
652
10.9
7111
48.0
9.9
0.00
1.9
0.06
-3.1
0.00
468
1
1.82
652
0.01
3
340
18.9
6424
62.8
12.4
0.00
1.6
0.12
-0.8
0.40
439
1
1.67
340
0.10
4
627
12.6
7870
53.5
11.7
0.00
0.6
0.52
-3.4
0.00
660
2
1.87
342
0.00
5
636
10.5
6657
36.0
14.3
0.00
1.0
0.31
-2.8
0.01
342
1
1.82
636
0.00
6
864
9.3
8005
34.2
11.4
0.00
1.3
0.20
-5.1
0.00
395
2
1.90
486
0.01
7
782
8.7
6825
35.7
9.1
0.00
1.0
0.31
-2.5
0.01
420
1
1.80
782
0.00
8
719
10.4
7469
40.7
9.5
0.00
1.2
0.23
-2.1
0.03
446
2
1.85
407
0.23
9
1035
7.8
8083
25.6
12.2
0.00
0.9
0.35
-10.6
0.00
250
2
1.88
539
0.03
10
687
10.8
7420
47.1
9.7
0.00
2.5
0.01
-2.6
0.01
444
1
1.80
687
0.02
11
561
10.8
6050
34.6
13.7
0.00
1.6
0.11
-1.5
0.15
406
1
1.79
561
0.00
12
990
8.9
8828
40.4
8.1
0.00
1.0
0.34
-5.3
0.00
688
2
1.91
556
0.02
13
759
9.0
6804
20.0
8.3
0.00
2.2
0.03
-9.8
0.00
185
2
1.73
449
0.00
14
680
11.4
7738
38.5
10.8
0.00
2.2
0.02
-4.4
0.00
461
1
1.70
680
0.14
15
223
12.3
2750
58.2
6.8
0.00
3.2
0.00
1.6
0.12
475
1
1.90
223
0.21
16
489
7.3
3568
30.3
5.3
0.00
1.2
0.23
-1.0
0.33
424
2
1.98
282
0.01
17
246
12.1
2967
29.5
10.4
0.00
3.5
0.00
-1.8
0.08
184
1
1.67
246
0.03
18
415
10.6
4393
30.6
10.6
0.00
2.3
0.02
-6.8
0.00
311
1
1.71
415
0.21
19
409
10.8
4416
53.1
8.3
0.00
1.4
0.15
-2.2
0.03
464
1
1.93
409
0.04
20
381
6.6
2505
34.3
6.3
0.00
0.8
0.42
1.0
0.33
396
2
2.19
174
0.03
21
503
10.7
5359
37.2
8.3
0.00
3.2
0.00
-1.1
0.27
332
2
1.80
281
0.11
22
290
13.5
3928
56.1
9.5
0.00
1.8
0.07
-1.4
0.18
501
1
1.85
290
0.01
23
629
12.5
7861
52.3
10.4
0.00
1.4
0.16
-2.3
0.02
501
2
1.87
377
0.20
mean
586
10.8
6088
 
10.0
0.05>
1.7
0.05>
-3.3
0.05>
418
1.4
1.83
451
0.1<
s.d.
221
2.5
1933
 
2.3
23
0.8
8
3.0
15
121
0.5
0.1
169
7
n, number of all of the rest bouts; <x>, average length of the rest bout (min); total, total length of rest bout (min); sd, standard deviation of the length of the rest bout (min); LR, log likelihood ratio of the power-law distribution to exponential distribution; p, p-value, positive values of the LR with p<0.05 indicate that the power-law distribution is statistically favored over the alternative distribution. p-values less than 0.05 are indicated in italic. In the power law test, we calculated the p′-value for the best power-law fit for the empirical data set. p′-values larger than 0.1 are indicated in italic. xmax, maximum value of fitted portion; xmin, minimum value of fitted portion; a, exponent of the fitted power law function; ntail, number of bouts with x ≥ xmin.
Table 2
Statistical tests for the significance of the power-law property in distributions of rest bouts according to the method described by Clauset et al. (CCFS)
No.
Basic parameters
Power Law vs. exponential
Power law test
24h
UP
DOWN
n
<x>
Total
sd
LR
p
LR
p
LR
p
xmax
xmin
α
ntail
p’
1
2286
7.7
17608
21.8
8.7
0.00
2.0
0.04
0.7
0.50
301
4
1.91
758
0.02
2
783
7.9
6218
28.7
12.1
0.00
0.8
0.41
-0.2
0.83
344
1
1.87
783
0.00
3
449
21.4
9595
56.2
17.4
0.00
2.0
0.04
-0.8
0.41
426
1
1.61
449
0.00
4
366
12.3
4514
37.1
8.0
0.00
1.4
0.15
0.6
0.56
401
1
1.64
366
0.42
5
642
8.5
5466
40.4
6.6
0.00
1.8
0.07
-1.2
0.23
549
1
1.81
642
0.00
6
509
13.1
6668
53.2
10.8
0.00
1.6
0.11
-1.0
0.34
512
1
1.78
509
0.01
7
322
17.5
5646
62.7
6.6
0.00
1.4
0.16
-0.3
0.78
592
2
1.69
215
0.26
8
965
13.0
12566
43.5
12.3
0.00
3.3
0.00
-1.5
0.13
541
2
1.73
555
0.05
9
1264
10.8
13604
55.1
12.5
0.00
3.0
0.00
-1.5
0.14
683
1
1.88
1264
0.00
10
1239
9.3
11475
44.5
13.9
0.00
3.2
0.00
-2.0
0.05
501
2
2.02
631
0.00
11
507
11.2
5678
55.8
5.7
0.00
-0.3
0.73
-0.3
0.73
558
2
1.85
298
0.00
12
577
13.5
7806
46.7
15.1
0.00
1.6
0.12
-1.7
0.09
371
1
1.76
577
0.02
13
1625
8.9
14494
31.2
16.3
0.00
0.1
0.95
-0.6
0.52
356
1
1.79
1625
0.00
14
1168
11.8
13771
40.5
7.6
0.00
0.2
0.81
-0.7
0.51
573
7
2.13
383
0.02
15
1050
12.0
12567
49.0
15.4
0.00
1.4
0.15
-1.8
0.07
469
1
1.81
1050
0.00
16
1529
10.6
16184
36.0
9.8
0.00
4.0
0.00
-1.3
0.19
537
3
1.83
696
0.28
17
1226
9.9
12123
51.7
11.3
0.00
1.1
0.27
-1.5
0.14
592
2
2.00
656
0.00
18
491
9.6
4723
42.9
7.6
0.00
2.6
0.01
0.4
0.72
470
1
1.83
491
0.00
19
842
10.5
8850
39.0
10.1
0.00
1.8
0.07
-0.9
0.38
488
2
1.82
483
0.73
20
1787
8.5
15131
36.1
14.9
0.00
4.3
0.00
-1.4
0.15
479
1
1.87
1787
0.00
21
171
21.9
3741
72.4
9.2
0.00
1.0
0.30
0.2
0.85
579
1
1.66
171
0.00
22
572
12.7
7241
40.6
13.0
0.00
2.4
0.02
-0.2
0.88
375
1
1.70
572
0.02
23
555
10.6
5870
36.3
10.6
0.00
1.7
0.09
0.5
0.61
468
1
1.74
555
0.01
24
2244
8.1
18070
33.3
12.3
0.00
4.4
0.00
-2.3
0.02
457
2
1.92
1262
0.00
25
1029
7.5
7738
27.1
9.1
0.00
1.2
0.24
-0.7
0.47
351
2
1.92
575
0.38
26
1073
11.8
12610
28.8
17.9
0.00
1.4
0.17
-1.7
0.09
233
1
1.63
1073
0.00
27
550
10.8
5964
43.5
10.4
0.00
4.1
8.00
-0.6
0.58
454
2
1.88
294
0.01
28
706
9.2
6495
24.4
10.0
0.00
1.7
0.08
-1.3
0.20
227
2
1.80
438
0.10
29
545
13.8
7527
32.3
12.3
0.00
5.7
0.00
0.2
0.82
384
1
1.59
545
0.00
30
1050
9.3
9756
33.4
8.5
0.00
4.7
0.00
-0.6
0.56
383
3
1.90
463
0.37
31
1278
8.9
11314
19.1
3.2
0.00
1.3
0.21
-1.2
0.22
207
7
1.99
348
0.00
32
903
10.7
9647
39.3
12.4
0.00
2.1
0.04
-1.6
0.11
470
1
1.75
903
0.03
33
1994
7.7
15294
35.8
11.0
0.00
5.0
0.00
-2.5
0.01
504
2
1.97
1069
0.00
34
1472
15.7
23068
55.8
5.6
0.00
2.7
0.01
-1.0
0.31
1007
7
1.83
454
0.01
35
1119
10.8
12141
58.5
11.3
0.00
3.1
0.00
-1.3
0.18
866
2
2.00
610
0.00
36
543
15.4
8387
58.5
12.8
0.00
2.8
0.01
-1.6
0.11
501
1
1.73
543
0.00
37
594
11.8
7038
46.9
9.4
0.00
2.1
0.04
0.2
0.82
568
1
1.73
594
0.29
38
1077
10.8
11591
38.0
14.3
0.00
2.7
0.01
-1.2
0.22
422
1
1.75
1077
0.07
39
1812
10.0
18073
32.8
17.6
0.00
2.0
0.05
-2.1
0.04
345
2
1.84
1051
0.00
40
1690
10.0
16965
27.9
13.9
0.00
3.1
0.00
-0.5
0.61
372
2
1.75
1009
0.07
41
756
9.2
6991
33.3
8.4
0.00
2.6
0.01
-0.9
0.35
434
3
1.94
324
0.07
42
1646
10.8
17749
35.0
14.4
0.00
1.8
0.07
-1.2
0.24
483
2
1.78
1004
0.09
43
1542
12.0
18481
52.4
13.0
0.00
1.4
0.17
-0.8
0.43
809
2
1.82
867
0.00
44
1850
8.2
15235
27.3
12.2
0.00
2.2
0.03
0.2
0.81
444
3
1.91
739
0.00
45
1542
12.0
18481
52.4
13.0
0.00
1.4
0.17
-0.8
0.43
809
2
1.82
867
0.00
46
1096
8.0
8746
31.3
13.5
0.00
1.5
0.14
-1.9
0.06
364
2
1.98
568
0.00
47
1316
11.7
15350
49.4
17.5
0.00
2.4
0.02
-1.2
0.21
567
2
1.92
727
0.00
48
425
9.8
4153
30.7
7.9
0.00
3.8
0.00
-0.4
0.70
281
1
1.70
425
0.18
49
1159
9.3
10780
26.3
6.8
0.00
2.8
0.01
-0.8
0.41
267
5
1.90
356
0.20
50
785
11.7
9212
39.8
12.1
0.00
4.8
0.00
-0.2
0.81
439
1
1.70
785
0.18
51
580
10.8
6272
58.0
6.8
0.00
4.5
0.00
-0.9
0.39
704
2
1.94
328
0.02
52
2239
9.4
20994
52.5
10.4
0.00
0.9
0.36
-1.6
0.12
720
3
2.06
945
0.00
53
562
16.7
9385
56.3
16.9
0.00
1.2
0.22
-0.5
0.63
520
1
1.74
562
0.00
54
916
16.1
14710
50.6
24.4
0.00
8.6
0.00
-2.3
0.02
410
1
1.73
916
0.00
55
798
13.0
10413
37.6
14.2
0.00
5.4
0.00
0.2
0.85
502
1
1.64
798
0.09
56
1272
8.9
11302
38.0
12.2
0.00
2.2
0.03
-2.2
0.03
396
1
1.83
1272
0.00
57
612
9.8
5996
39.4
5.6
0.00
0.8
0.45
0.9
0.39
435
4
2.05
250
0.01
mean
1047
11.3
11008
 
11.5
0.05>
2.5
0.05>
-0.9
0.05>
482
2.0
1.83
694
0.1<
s.d.
530
3.1
4761
 
3.9
57
1.6
31
0.8
6
156
1.5
0.1
341
11
n, number of all of the rest bouts; <x>, average length of the rest bout (min); total, total length of rest bout (min); sd, standard deviation of the length of the rest bout (min); LR, log likelihood ratio of the power-law distribution to exponential distribution; p, p-value, positive values of the LR with p<0.05 indicate that the power-law distribution is statistically favored over the alternative distribution. p-values less than 0.05 are indicated in italic. In the power law test, we calculated the p′-value for the best power-law fit for the empirical data set. p′-values larger than 0.1 are indicated in italic. xmax, maximum value of fitted portion; xmin, minimum value of fitted portion; a, exponent of the fitted power law function; ntail, number of bouts with x ≥ xmin.
Thus, we submitted the 24 h rest data to a strict power law test developed by Clauset et al. [10]. As described in the materials and methods, we fitted the individual data to a power law distribution and generated 1,000 surrogate data sets, each of which was a set of artificial bouts sampled from the estimated power-law distribution with noise. Then we compared these surrogate data with the actual data and calculated a p′-value for the best power-law fit, which was used for the judgment. Note that we deemed the distribution to be significantly similar to the power law when this p′-value was larger than 0.1. As shown in Table 1a and b, the power law distribution that was fitted to the actual data spans from 1 to 2 min (xmin) to about 400 min (xmax) and the α in Figure 4c, approximately 30% of control and 20% of CCFS subjects showed a p′ value larger than 0.1. These proportions were significantly smaller when compared with the Drosophila study, where all the individuals showed significant similarity to the power law distributions [9].

Discussion

We analyzed the temporal organization of rest and activity bouts using actigraphy data from both healthy youngsters and individuals with CCFS. We applied a k-means clustering method to calculate the threshold values and compared them with those calculated by the average method used by Nakamura et al. As shown in Figure 1a, the k-means clustering method gave smaller values than the average method, but with significant correlations. Nakamura et al. reported that changing the threshold value within the range of 0.6 to 1.6 fold did not affect the results [3]. However, when we analyzed our data using the threshold value, which corresponded to 1.6 fold of the value calculated by the average method, most of the bins were judged as ‘rest’ even during daytime (UP period), thus, we could not obtain satisfactory results (data not shown). Since we did not directly compare the actual data, we do not know why this occurred. However, we speculate that differences in the sensitivity or properties of the actigraphy could be the reason. The threshold values calculated by the k-means clustering method were smaller, and stable among the subjects as shown in Figure 2a. Therefore, we decided that the k-means clustering method was more appropriate for our data so we chose to use it in this study. We anticipated no essential differences in the results, depending on the calculation method used for the threshold.
The distribution of rest over 24 h showed long-tailed distributions and when the power law and exponential distributions were compared, all the individual data significantly favored the power law (Figure 4a, Tables 1, 2). These results are consistent with the previous analysis from Nakamura et al. [3]. However, the distribution curve showed a flexion (Figures 1c and 3a,e,i) which was not evident when we analyzed the fruit fly data. Since we expected longer rest to be derived from nighttime sleep, we re-analyzed the data after separation into UP and DOWN periods. The results showed that the rest bout during UP periods had a weak similarity to the power law distribution, but the rest bout during DOWN periods did not (Figure 4a, Table 1). Even for the rest bout of 24 h, which favored the power law over the exponential distribution, only 30% passed the strict power law judgment proposed by Clauset et al. [10]. One of the most famous examples of power law properties in animal behavior was Levy flight described by Viswanathan [19]. However, the amount of data presented in that paper was limited, therefore the power law properties reported have been questioned [20]. In addition, recently, Petrovskii et al. reported that the summations of individual data, which alone show exponential and non-power law distributions, can reveal power law properties [21]. Thus, we should pay more attention to the interpretation of power law properties for 24 h rest bouts.
The rest bout during DOWN periods, which corresponds to nighttime sleep, tended to show a similarity to an exponential distribution in healthy control subjects (Figure 4c). This is consistent with the fact that human sleep consists of repeating sleep units. It is also consistent with a previous report by Lo et al., which used an electroencephalogram to demonstrate a sleep bout that showed an exponential distribution [12].
As we described previously, in fruit fly, rest period showed a clear power law distribution even when the daytime and nighttime data were analyzed separately [9]. The difference in the temporal distribution of rest between human and fruit fly may reflect the qualitative difference of rest and sleep between them.
From the clinical point of view, actigraphy has been broadly used as a versatile tool to monitor activity and sleep rhythm in normal and patients with psychiatric and other disorders [1316]. The temporal distribution of activity and rest were described to show difference between normal and patients with disorders. Recently, Sano et al described enhanced persistence of rest and active periods in schizophrenia patients [22].
Analysis of CCFS children did not disclose difference from normal subject when analyzed as a whole day data. But, the rest bout during DOWN periods in CCFS subjects showed a reduced tendency (60% in the control, 10% in CCFS) to favor the exponential distribution (Figure 4c). Since the average plots for these two groups almost overlap (Figure 3i-l), it is interesting to find such a large difference in the individual analysis. This may be due to the instability of sleep in CCFS children and could be used as pathological marker for this condition. However, more studies are necessary to find the exact cause and clinical value.
In conclusion, although power law properties in animal behavior have attracted attention since they are apparently universal, we should pay more careful attention when interpreting power law properties of the data, since apparent power law property can be derived from the summation of two non-power law distributions.

Conclusion

We analyzed the temporal organization of rest and activity bouts using actigraphy data from both healthy youngsters and individuals with CCFS, after separating them into UP and DOWN periods. The rest durations of either UP or DOWN period did not show a power law distribution. We propose that separate analysis of UP and DOWN period data is important for understanding the temporal organization of activity.

Authors’ information

TU, JK, AT and KK are medical doctors, and TU and KK are physicians and JK and AT are pediatricians. MK and SK are pharmacists. KK is a certified physician of sleep medicine accredited by the Japanese Society of Sleep Research (JSSR). As of 2012, there are 413 accredited in Japan.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Dr. Aaron Clauset for the statistical analysis code and Naoki Masuda for the advice on the initial phase of this project. This work was supported by the Grants-in-Aid (22300132) from the Japanese Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) to KK; a Global COE grant from MEXT to SK; RISTEX/Japan Science and Technology Agency “Brain Science and Education” to JK and AT. TU is a global COE junior research associate and JSPS fellow. SK is a member of the Global COE Program (Cell Fate Regulation Research and Education Unit), MEXT, Japan.
Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

The authors declare they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

All authors contributed to and approved the final version of the manuscript. MK, TU, JT, JK, AT, SK and KK designed the study. JK and AT acquired the data, MK, TU, JT, SK and KK conducted the data analysis, interpretation and drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Barabási A-L: The origin of bursts and heavy tails in human dynamics. Nature. 2005, 435: 207-211. 10.1038/nature03459.CrossRefPubMed Barabási A-L: The origin of bursts and heavy tails in human dynamics. Nature. 2005, 435: 207-211. 10.1038/nature03459.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Nakamura T, Kiyono K, Yoshiuchi K, Nakahara R, Struzik Z, Yamamoto Y: Universal Scaling Law in Human Behavioral Organization. Phys Rev Lett. 2007, 99: 1-4. Nakamura T, Kiyono K, Yoshiuchi K, Nakahara R, Struzik Z, Yamamoto Y: Universal Scaling Law in Human Behavioral Organization. Phys Rev Lett. 2007, 99: 1-4.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Nakamura T, Takumi T, Takano A, Aoyagi N, Yoshiuchi K, Struzik ZR, Yamamoto Y: Of mice and men–universality and breakdown of behavioral organization. PLoS One. 2008, 3: e2050-10.1371/journal.pone.0002050.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Nakamura T, Takumi T, Takano A, Aoyagi N, Yoshiuchi K, Struzik ZR, Yamamoto Y: Of mice and men–universality and breakdown of behavioral organization. PLoS One. 2008, 3: e2050-10.1371/journal.pone.0002050.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Shimada I, Minesaki Y, Hara H: Temporal fractal in the feeding behavior of Drosophila melanogaster. J Ethol. 1995, 13: 153-158. 10.1007/BF02350106.CrossRef Shimada I, Minesaki Y, Hara H: Temporal fractal in the feeding behavior of Drosophila melanogaster. J Ethol. 1995, 13: 153-158. 10.1007/BF02350106.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Cole B: Fractal time in animal behaviour: the movement activity of Drosophila. Anim Behav. 1995, 50: 1317-1324. 10.1016/0003-3472(95)80047-6.CrossRef Cole B: Fractal time in animal behaviour: the movement activity of Drosophila. Anim Behav. 1995, 50: 1317-1324. 10.1016/0003-3472(95)80047-6.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin J-R, Ernst R, Heisenberg M: Temporal pattern of locomotor activity in Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A. 1999, 184: 73-84. 10.1007/s003590050307.CrossRefPubMed Martin J-R, Ernst R, Heisenberg M: Temporal pattern of locomotor activity in Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A. 1999, 184: 73-84. 10.1007/s003590050307.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Reynolds AM, Frye MA: Free-flight odor tracking in Drosophila is consistent with an optimal intermittent scale-free search. PLoS One. 2007, 2: e354-10.1371/journal.pone.0000354.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Reynolds AM, Frye MA: Free-flight odor tracking in Drosophila is consistent with an optimal intermittent scale-free search. PLoS One. 2007, 2: e354-10.1371/journal.pone.0000354.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Ueno T, Masuda N, Kume S, Kume K: Dopamine modulates the rest period length without perturbation of its power law distribution in Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS One. 2012, 7: e32007-10.1371/journal.pone.0032007.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ueno T, Masuda N, Kume S, Kume K: Dopamine modulates the rest period length without perturbation of its power law distribution in Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS One. 2012, 7: e32007-10.1371/journal.pone.0032007.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Clauset A, Shalizi CR, Newman MEJ: Power-law distributions in empirical data. SIAM Rev. 2009, 51: 43-CrossRef Clauset A, Shalizi CR, Newman MEJ: Power-law distributions in empirical data. SIAM Rev. 2009, 51: 43-CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Ueno T, Tomita J, Tanimoto H, Endo K, Ito K, Kume S, Kume K: Identification of a dopamine pathway that regulates sleep and arousal in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci. 2012, 15: 1516-1523. 10.1038/nn.3238.CrossRefPubMed Ueno T, Tomita J, Tanimoto H, Endo K, Ito K, Kume S, Kume K: Identification of a dopamine pathway that regulates sleep and arousal in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci. 2012, 15: 1516-1523. 10.1038/nn.3238.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Lo C-C, Amaral LAN, Havlin S, Ivanov PC, Penzel T, Peter J-H, Stanley HE: Dynamics of sleep-wake transitions during sleep. Europhysics Letters (EPL). 2002, 57: 625-631. 10.1209/epl/i2002-00508-7.CrossRef Lo C-C, Amaral LAN, Havlin S, Ivanov PC, Penzel T, Peter J-H, Stanley HE: Dynamics of sleep-wake transitions during sleep. Europhysics Letters (EPL). 2002, 57: 625-631. 10.1209/epl/i2002-00508-7.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Teicher MH: Actigraphy and motion analysis: new tools for psychiatry. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 1995, 3: 18-35. 10.3109/10673229509017161.CrossRefPubMed Teicher MH: Actigraphy and motion analysis: new tools for psychiatry. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 1995, 3: 18-35. 10.3109/10673229509017161.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Warren JM, Ekelund U, Besson H, Mezzani A, Geladas N, Vanhees L: Assessment of physical activity - a review of methodologies with reference to epidemiological research: a report of the exercise physiology section of the European Association of Cardiovascular Prevention and Rehabilitation. Eur J Cardiovasc Prev Rehabil. 2010, 17: 127-139. 10.1097/HJR.0b013e32832ed875.CrossRefPubMed Warren JM, Ekelund U, Besson H, Mezzani A, Geladas N, Vanhees L: Assessment of physical activity - a review of methodologies with reference to epidemiological research: a report of the exercise physiology section of the European Association of Cardiovascular Prevention and Rehabilitation. Eur J Cardiovasc Prev Rehabil. 2010, 17: 127-139. 10.1097/HJR.0b013e32832ed875.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Lubans DR, Okely AD, Morgan PJ, Cotton W, Puglisi L, Miller J: Description and evaluation of a social cognitive model of physical activity behaviour tailored for adolescent girls. Health Educ Res. 2012, 27: 115-128. 10.1093/her/cyr039.CrossRefPubMed Lubans DR, Okely AD, Morgan PJ, Cotton W, Puglisi L, Miller J: Description and evaluation of a social cognitive model of physical activity behaviour tailored for adolescent girls. Health Educ Res. 2012, 27: 115-128. 10.1093/her/cyr039.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Hasler BP, Dahl RE, Holm SM, Jakubcak JL, Ryan ND, Silk JS, Phillips ML, Forbes EE: Weekend-weekday advances in sleep timing are associated with altered reward-related brain function in healthy adolescents. Biol Psychol. 2012, 91: 334-341. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.08.008.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hasler BP, Dahl RE, Holm SM, Jakubcak JL, Ryan ND, Silk JS, Phillips ML, Forbes EE: Weekend-weekday advances in sleep timing are associated with altered reward-related brain function in healthy adolescents. Biol Psychol. 2012, 91: 334-341. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.08.008.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Kawatani J, Mizuno K, Shiraishi S, Takao M, Joudoi T, Fukuda S, Watanabe Y, Tomoda A: Cognitive dysfunction and mental fatigue in childhood chronic fatigue syndrome - A 6-month follow-up study. Brain Dev. 2011, 33: 832-841. 10.1016/j.braindev.2010.12.009.CrossRefPubMed Kawatani J, Mizuno K, Shiraishi S, Takao M, Joudoi T, Fukuda S, Watanabe Y, Tomoda A: Cognitive dysfunction and mental fatigue in childhood chronic fatigue syndrome - A 6-month follow-up study. Brain Dev. 2011, 33: 832-841. 10.1016/j.braindev.2010.12.009.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Cole RJ, Kripke DF, Gruen W, Mullaney DJ, Gillin JC: Automatic sleep/wake identification from wrist activity. Sleep. 1992, 15: 461-469.PubMed Cole RJ, Kripke DF, Gruen W, Mullaney DJ, Gillin JC: Automatic sleep/wake identification from wrist activity. Sleep. 1992, 15: 461-469.PubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Viswanathan GM, Buldyrev SV, Havlin S, Da Luz MG, Raposo EP, Stanley HE: Optimizing the success of random searches. Nature. 1999, 401: 911-914. 10.1038/44831.CrossRefPubMed Viswanathan GM, Buldyrev SV, Havlin S, Da Luz MG, Raposo EP, Stanley HE: Optimizing the success of random searches. Nature. 1999, 401: 911-914. 10.1038/44831.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Edwards AM, Phillips RA, Watkins NW, Freeman MP, Murphy EJ, Afanasyev V, Buldyrev SV, Da Luz MGE, Raposo EP, Stanley HE, Viswanathan GM: Revisiting Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses, bumblebees and deer. Nature. 2007, 449: 1044-1048. 10.1038/nature06199.CrossRefPubMed Edwards AM, Phillips RA, Watkins NW, Freeman MP, Murphy EJ, Afanasyev V, Buldyrev SV, Da Luz MGE, Raposo EP, Stanley HE, Viswanathan GM: Revisiting Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses, bumblebees and deer. Nature. 2007, 449: 1044-1048. 10.1038/nature06199.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Petrovskii S, Mashanova A, Jansen VAA: Variation in individual walking behavior creates the impression of a Levy flight. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011, 108: 8704-8707. 10.1073/pnas.1015208108.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Petrovskii S, Mashanova A, Jansen VAA: Variation in individual walking behavior creates the impression of a Levy flight. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011, 108: 8704-8707. 10.1073/pnas.1015208108.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Sano W, Nakamura T, Yoshiuchi K, Kitajima T, Tsuchiya A, Esaki Y, Yamamoto Y, Iwata N: Enhanced persistency of resting and active periods of locomotor activity in schizophrenia. PLoS One. 2012, 7: e43539-10.1371/journal.pone.0043539.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Sano W, Nakamura T, Yoshiuchi K, Kitajima T, Tsuchiya A, Esaki Y, Yamamoto Y, Iwata N: Enhanced persistency of resting and active periods of locomotor activity in schizophrenia. PLoS One. 2012, 7: e43539-10.1371/journal.pone.0043539.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Temporal organization of rest defined by actigraphy data in healthy and childhood chronic fatigue syndrome children
verfasst von
Minako Kawabata
Taro Ueno
Jun Tomita
Junko Kawatani
Akemi Tomoda
Shoen Kume
Kazuhiko Kume
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2013
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2013
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-13-281

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2013

BMC Psychiatry 1/2013 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).