Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2021

Open Access 01.12.2021 | Research article

The associations between maternal and child diet quality and child ADHD – findings from a large Norwegian pregnancy cohort study

verfasst von: Tiril Cecilie Borge, Guido Biele, Eleni Papadopoulou, Lene Frost Andersen, Felice Jacka, Merete Eggesbø, Ida Henriette Caspersen, Heidi Aase, Helle Margrete Meltzer, Anne Lise Brantsæter

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2021

Abstract

Background

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a prevalent neurodevelopmental disorder. Effective long-term treatment options are limited, which warrants increased focus on potential modifiable risk factors. The aim of this study was to investigate associations between maternal diet quality during pregnancy and child diet quality and child ADHD symptoms and ADHD diagnosis.

Methods

This study is based on the Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). We assessed maternal diet quality with the Prenatal Diet Quality Index (PDQI) and Ultra-Processed Food Index (UPFI) around mid-gestation, and child diet quality using the Diet Quality Index (CDQI) at 3 years. ADHD symptoms were assessed at child age 8 years using the Parent Rating Scale for Disruptive Behaviour Disorders. ADHD diagnoses were retrieved from the Norwegian Patient Registry.

Results

In total, 77,768 mother-child pairs were eligible for studying ADHD diagnoses and 37,787 for ADHD symptoms. Means (SD) for the PDQI, UPFI and CDQI were 83.1 (9.3), 31.8 (9.7) and 60.3 (10.6), respectively. Mean (SD) ADHD symptom score was 8.4 (7.1) and ADHD diagnosis prevalence was 2.9% (male to female ratio 2.6:1). For one SD increase in maternal diet index scores, we saw a change in mean (percent) ADHD symptom score of − 0.28 (− 3.3%) (CI: − 0.41, − 0.14 (− 4.8, − 1.6%)) for PDQI scores and 0.25 (+ 3.0%) (CI: 0.13, 0.38 (1.5, 4.5%)) for UPFI scores. A one SD increase in PDQI score was associated with a relative risk of ADHD diagnosis of 0.87 (CI: 0.79, 0.97). We found no reliable associations with either outcomes for the CDQI, and no reliable change in risk of ADHD diagnosis for the UPFI.

Conclusions

We provide evidence that overall maternal diet quality during pregnancy is associated with a small decrease in ADHD symptom score at 8 years and lower risk for ADHD diagnosis, with more robust findings for the latter outcome. Consumption of ultra-processed foods was only associated with increased ADHD symptom score of similar magnitude as for overall maternal diet quality, and we found no associations between child diet quality and either outcome. No causal inferences should be made based on these results, due to potential unmeasured confounding.
Begleitmaterial
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s12888-021-03130-4.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
ADHD
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
AME
Average marginal effect
CDQI
Child diet quality index
CI
Credible interval
FFQ
Food frequency questionnaire
MoBa
The Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study
PDQI
Prenatal diet quality index
SD
Standard deviation
UPFI
Ultra-processed food index
WHO
World Health Organization

Background

The World Health Organization (WHO) defines neurodevelopmental disorders as one of today’s greatest public health challenges [1], with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) being one of the most prevalent among children in Norway and worldwide [2, 3]. ADHD is characterized by a number of behavioural traits, including inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity [4] and it typically co-occurs with delayed language development, motor functions, impaired emotional control as well as with other psychiatric disorders [5, 6]. Furthermore, impairments related to ADHD often persist into adulthood [710], alongside poorer social functioning and an increased risk of unemployment [3, 6].
ADHD aetiology is multifactorial and complex [6], involving a genetic contribution [6, 11], as well as social- and environmental factors [12]. Because treatment options available for young children are limited [13, 14] and due to the relatively poor long-term effectiveness of medical treatment for ADHD [15], identification of early-life modifiable risk factors, such as nutritional factors, could be a key strategy in improving prevention approaches for ADHD in children [13, 16].
Findings from investigations into associations between child diet and ADHD have been equivocal. Some research on ADHD patients have found suboptimal levels of several nutrients, including zinc [17], iron [18], magnesium [19] and Omega-3 [18, 20], but dietary interventions aiming to reduce symptoms have yielded mixed results [21, 22]. A recent meta-analysis reviewed the scarce literature investigating dietary patterns and ADHD diagnosis or symptoms in children [23]. Overall, the authors found higher risk of ADHD diagnosis or symptoms for children having low scores on a healthy dietary pattern and high scores on an unhealthy dietary pattern. However, all studies were observational, where half investigated associations cross-sectionally, which severely impedes a causal interpretation of these associations.
It has been suggested that an optimal maternal diet during pregnancy might be even more crucial to child ADHD outcomes than early childhood diet [15]. Surprisingly, however, the possible impact of maternal diet quality during pregnancy on child neurodevelopmental functions related to ADHD has been largely neglected until recently. The increased interest in this area is likely due to the evidence that the in-utero environment might be vital in the development of neurodevelopmental disorders [2426]. Research on the effects of severe and prolonged maternal malnourishment on brain development in offspring highlights the important role of maternal diet [18], particularly during the prenatal and early postnatal period [6]. It remains less clear how subtle differences in maternal diet quality during pregnancy might influence the trajectory of child neurodevelopmental disorders.
We have not identified any research investigating overall maternal diet quality during pregnancy in relation to child ADHD diagnosis. Moreover, the limited amount of literature investigating prenatal and child diet in relation to neurodevelopmental problems associated with ADHD has indicated that there exists a possible association [27, 28]. However, these associations tend to be either small or from smaller studies [29]. Hence, there is a need for large population-based studies that investigate longitudinal associations between prenatal and early postnatal diet quality and ADHD.
The aim of this paper is to investigate the associations between both maternal diet quality during pregnancy and child diet quality, using diet quality indices, and maternally reported child ADHD symptoms at 8 years and child ADHD diagnosis retrieved from the Norwegian Patient Registry.

Methods

We followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement as the reporting guideline [30] (see Additional file 1).

Study population and design

The Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study (MoBa) is an ongoing prospective population-based pregnancy cohort study conducted by the Norwegian Institute of Public Health, aiming to investigate how genetic- and environmental factors affect health outcomes [31]. Pregnant women were recruited across Norway between 1999 and 2008, prior to the routine ultrasound screening around gestational week (GW) 18, with 41% of the invited women consenting to participate. The cohort includes 114,500 children, 95,200 mothers and 75,200 fathers. The participating pregnant women answered questionnaires at regular intervals prenatally (GW 22 and 30) and during the postnatal phase (6 months, 18 months, and 3, 5, 7, and 8 years after birth). The response rates for the three prenatal questionnaires were between 91 and 95% [31]. In this study we use data from the two MoBa questionnaires answered in pregnancy (baseline questionnaire and FFQ) and the questionnaires at child age 3 and 8 years. We based this study on version 11 of the quality-assured data files released for research on 18 October 2018, which provided us with an initial sample of n = 102,152 mother-child dyads. This data was linked with data from the Norwegian Medical Birth Registry (MBRN) and with the Norwegian Patient Registry (NPR), a national administrative health registry with patient information reported from hospitals and outpatient clinics in Norway. We utilised two different study designs in this paper; a prospective cohort design and a registry-based case cohort design. Figure 1 describes the process of participant inclusion for both study designs.

Exposure definition

Maternal diet quality during pregnancy

The MoBa food frequency questionnaire (FFQ) was administered around gestational week 22 and provides comprehensive information about maternal dietary habits and intake of foods, beverages and dietary supplements since beginning of pregnancy [32]. The MoBa FFQ was specifically developed for the MoBa [33] and has been validated using biomarkers in urine and blood samples in addition to a 4-day weighed food diary [3336]. We converted intake frequencies to intake in grams per day for 255 foods and beverages assuming standard portion sizes and using FoodCalc [37], and the Norwegian food composition database [38].
We evaluated maternal diet quality during pregnancy by applying two dietary indices to the FFQ-derived data. The first index, called prenatal diet quality index (PDQI) (possible range of scores = 0–110), captures adherence to the Norwegian food-based dietary guidelines. We have previously used this index to investigate associations with child developmental outcomes [29]. The second index, called the ultra-processed food index (UPFI) (possible range = 0–100%), captures the relative contribution of ultra-processed foods to total energy intake. The UPFI was used to measure the opposite aspects of diet quality to those captured by the PDQI.
The PDQI is largely based on the well-known Healthy Eating Index [39]. We calculated the PDQI score using the method put forward by von Ruesten et al. [40] with modifications to correspond to the current Norwegian food-based dietary guidelines [41]. We list an overview of the components in Table 1. Detailed description of the calculation has been published in a previous article [29] and is included in Additional file 2.
Table 1
Overview of the PDQI components, corresponding recommended intake and maximum component scores
PDQI components
Recommended intake
Maximum component score*
Fresh fruits & berries
Minimum 250 g/day
10
Vegetables
Minimum 250 g/day
10
Whole grain
Ca. 70 g/day for women
10
Total Fish
300–450 g/week
5
Fatty fish
Minimum 200 (to max. 450) grams/week
5
Red meat
Maximum 500 g/week
10
Dairy
3 servings (1 serving = 20 g cheese or 2 dl milk or 1 pot yoghurt (125 g)
10
Saturated fat
Maximum 10% of total energy
10
Trans fat
Maximum 1% of total energy
10
Salt
Maximum 6 g salt/day (= 2.4 g sodium/day)
10
Added sugar
Maximum 10% of total energy intake
10
Dietary diversity score
Measure of diversity of foods within 4 major food groups eaten daily (grains, vegetables, fruits, dairy)
10
PDQI: Prenatal Diet Quality Index.
*Maximum total score = 110
The second maternal dietary index, the UPFI, was created by calculating the relative contribution of energy from ultra-processed foods to total energy intake. Following the NOVA classification [42], foods and beverages are classified into four distinct categories according to the level of processing, ranging from minimally processed (group 1, e.g. fresh fruits and vegetables, raw nuts, legumes, whole meats with no additives or preservatives), to ultra-processed (group 4, e.g. savoury and sweet snacks, breads containing emulsifiers, reconstituted meat products, products with artificial sweeteners). We only included foods from group 4, as our aim was to investigate ultra-processed foods and their contribution to the overall energy intake, and we already include a measure of overall diet quality represented by the PDQI. For a full list of the food and beverage groups included in the UPFI, see Additional file 3. We calculated and summed the energy intake from the MoBa FFQ items in group 4 and calculated its relative contribution to total energy intake to create the UPFI score, with possible range of 0–100, corresponding to percent contribution.

Child diet at 3 years

Child intake of 36 food and beverage items was assessed by parent-reported food intake questions included in the MoBa questionnaire at 3 years. We evaluated overall child diet quality by defining the Diet Quality Index (DQI), as developed by Huybrechts et al. for use in pre-school children [43], which also has been used in a multi-country study of more than 7000 European pre-schoolers [44]. We refer to it as child diet quality index (CDQI) throughout the paper to make a clear distinction from the PDQI. In common with the PDQI, the CDQI assesses adherence to dietary recommendations. As basis for the calculations, we used information from the food intake questions included in the MoBa questionnaire at 3 years, where mothers report the child’s average intake frequency of 36 selected foods and beverages covering 6 main food groups (FG’s) (dairy, fruits, vegetables, meat, fish, grains) in addition to sweets/snacks. Since there was no assessment of water intake in the MoBa questionnaire at 3 years, we used reported water intake from the MoBa questionnaire at 18 months as proxy for water intake at 3 years. Intake frequencies were converted to intakes in grams per day by using standardized portion sizes [45, 46].
The CDQI consists of the following components: diet diversity, diet quality and diet equilibrium, where the total CDQI score is the average score across the three components, with higher scores equalling better diet quality. The original DQI also includes a meal pattern component, but there were no questions on child meal patterns in the MoBa questionnaires. Below is a brief description of each component. For more detailed information on the calculation method, see Additional file 4 and Huybrechts et al. [43].
The diet diversity component (possible range of 0–100%), reflects the intake variation in the foods by giving one point if the child had consumed at least one daily serving from each main FG.
The diet quality component (possible range of − 100 - 100%) is used to categorize food intake, within each FG, according to their quality, into preferred foods (e.g. High fibre bread in the bread and cereal group) factored by 1, moderation foods (e.g. low-fibre bread in the bread and cereal group) factored by 0 and the “low nutrient, energy dense” foods (e.g. sweet bread/buns in the bread and cereal group); factored by − 1. After categorization based on intake in grams per day, the foods were summed and divided by the total food intake.
The diet equilibrium consists of adequacy and moderation (possible range of 0–100%). The diet equilibrium component introduces the concept of dietary balance to the CDQI, taking into account both “moderation” and “adequacy”: intake moderation (percentage of intake exceeding the upper level of the recommendation), with particular focus on limiting the intake of low nutrient and energy dense foods, subtracted from intake adequacy of foods in the preferred group (percentage, reported intake of food/minimum recommended intake), divided by total number of FGs.

Outcome definitions

ADHD symptom score - MoBa

Child ADHD symptoms were assessed in the MoBa eight-year questionnaire, using items from the Parent Rating Scale for Disruptive Behaviour Disorders [47]. Mothers reported on child ADHD symptoms on a four-point Likert scale (never/rarely, sometimes, often, or very often, scored 0–3) covering two domains: inattention (nine items) and hyperactivity/impulsivity (nine items). We calculated ADHD symptom sum scores, with higher scores indicating more and stronger symptoms.

ADHD diagnoses - NPR

We obtained information about children’s ADHD diagnoses from the Norwegian Patient Registry (NPR). The NPR originated in 1997, and person-identifiable records are available from 2008 onwards [48]. The NPR classifies an ADHD diagnosis according to the International Classification of Disease 10th revision (ICD-10) as hyperkinetic disorder [49]. Diagnostic information from the NPR was retrieved and linked to the MoBa data file, comprising all MoBa children registered at least once with an ICD-10 code of hyperkinetic disorder (coded as F90.0, F90.1, F90.8, or F90.9), corresponding to an ADHD diagnosis, between 2008 and 2017. Hyperkinetic disorder requires the combination of persistent inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms before the age of six and impairment in two or more settings.

Covariates

We assessed a number of covariates for inclusion in the analyses, based on previous knowledge and directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) [50]. For maternal diet quality during pregnancy, the final covariate set comprised: maternal pre-pregnancy BMI in kg/m2; maternal education (< 12 years, Upper secondary (12 years), Bachelor (15 years), Master + (17+ years)); smoking during pregnancy (yes/no); alcohol intake during pregnancy (yes/no); reported symptom score of maternal prenatal depression (0–3) and ADHD (0–4); maternal age (in years); child sex (boy/girl); parity (0–3); child diet quality score at 3 years (CDQI) and child season of birth (in quarters) (see Additional file 5 for DAG). We adjusted the maternal diet models for child diet as this removes bias from common unobserved causes of maternal and child diet.
For child diet quality, the final covariate set comprised: maternal pre-pregnancy BMI in kg/m2; maternal education (< 12 years, Upper secondary (12 years), Bachelor (15 years), Master + (17+ years)); maternal symptoms of ADHD (0–4); maternal age (in years); maternal diet quality score during pregnancy (PDQI); child sex (boy/girl); parity (0–3); reported child sleep problems at 3 years (yes/no); and child season of birth (in quarters) (see Additional file 6 for DAG). The maternal variables included, except for ADHD symptoms, were assessed during pregnancy. Lastly, we included birth quarter as a covariate for all exposure variables to increase the precision of our estimates, as children born in October–December are 45% more likely to have an ADHD diagnosis compared to children born in January–March [51].

Statistical analyses

Reliability and validity of the PDQI in the MoBa sample have been reported previously [29]. We evaluate construct validity for the UPFI, and CDQI construct validity and reliability. For construct validity we calculate differences between groups known to have different diet quality and report the differences as Hedge’s g. Hedge’s g is calculated by dividing the mean differences of two groups by their pooled standard deviation and is expressed in units of the pooled standard deviation. E.g. a Hedge’s g of 0.5 indicate that there is a difference in means between groups of 0.5 pooled standard deviation. For reliability we report the Omega total [52], which is an index of internal consistency, i.e. how well the items included in a scale (e.g. the diet diversity, quality and equilibrium components in the CDQI) measure one underlying construct, i.e. the CDQI. The Omega is estimated using a factor analysis with oblique rotation, followed by a Schmid-Leiman transformation for general factor extraction [52], using the Omega function in the Psych R package [53].
We evaluated the strength of associations between the diet quality indices and outcomes by estimating Bayesian regression models, using a beta-binomial likelihood for ADHD symptoms and logistic regression for ADHD diagnosis. For the analyses we utilised the R statistical software, version 3.4.3 [54] and the brms package, version 2.1.0 [55]. To avoid losing variation in our data, all exposure and outcome variables except for ADHD diagnosis were kept in its continuous form, in line with suggested approaches [56].
For all exposure variables, we used child sex and maternal ADHD symptoms as stratification variables to explore possible differences in associations between strata. MoBa assesses maternal ADHD symptoms in the 3 year questionnaire with the Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS), which consists of six questions relating to symptoms of adult ADHD based on the DSM-IV diagnosis criteria [57]. Stratifying on maternal ADHD symptoms also adjusts for maternal ADHD as a confounder that influences maternal diet and child ADHD.
For child ADHD symptoms as outcome, we report the results as average marginal effect (AME), i.e. the average change in the outcome score for a one standard deviation (1SD) change in the exposure. We report both change in mean score (absolute AME) and corresponding percent change (relative AME) with corresponding credible intervals (CI’s) (CI’s can be considered as synonymous with confidence intervals in traditional statistical approaches). For ADHD diagnosis, we report the results as relative risk (RR) with corresponding CI’s. For each exposure, we fit two separate models; one crude and one adjusted including all covariates.
Based on prior predictive simulations, we set the prior for population level (fixed) effects to a normal distribution with a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 2, and the prior for the standard deviation of random effects to a half-normal distribution with a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 2 [58]. Note that priors will not have substantial influence on the final estimates, due to the large sample size of MoBa [59, 60].
Participation and loss to follow-up in MoBa is not random but dependent on maternal education, age and child parity, which can lead to biased estimates [61]. Therefore, we use inverse probability weighting (IPW) based on these three variables to control for bias due to self-selection into the study and cohort attrition [61, 62]. We calculated weights from simple participation probabilities, i.e. the number of mothers in a population subgroup in the study sample divided by the number of mothers in the same subgroup in the target population (mothers who gave birth in Norway during MoBa’s inclusion period). We obtained population data from Statistics Norway, which provided maternal age, parity and education for the Norwegian pregnant population for the MoBa recruitment period.
For all models we used complete cases for the variables included in each respective model, see Table 2 for details. We believe using complete case analyses is justified due to the uncertainties relating to the pattern of missingness in our data and since we use IPW in all analyses.
Table 2
Complete cases for variables included in each model for all exposures and outcomes
 
ADHD symptoms
ADHD diagnosis (cases in parenthesis)
 
Crude model
Adjusted model
Crude model
Adjusted model
PDQI/ UPFI
n = 31,152
n = 19,403
n = 46,976 (n = 1412)
n = 27,769 (n = 812)
CDQI
n = 22,699
n = 22,290
n = 32,687 (n = 956)
n = 32,034 (n = 936)

Results

Reliability and construct validity

UPFI and PDQI

We investigated whether the UPFI was able to differentiate between groups known to have different diet quality: smokers vs. non-smokers, lower (< 12 years education) vs higher (master degree) education, younger mothers (<=21 years) vs older mothers (> = 30 years) and people presenting with and without depressive symptoms. We calculated standardized mean differences between groups, reported as Hedges’ g, with a medium effect size seen for low vs. high education (g = 0.59), low vs. high age (g = 0.48) and smoking vs. no smoking (g = 0.53) and small effect size for with vs. without depressive symptoms (g = 0.24). In addition, the UPFI score correlated with energy adjusted intakes of key nutrients in the FFQ in the expected direction (fibre (r = − 0.50), sugar (r = 0.59), protein (r = − 0.55) saturated fat (r = 0.30)), as well as with the PDQI (r = − 0.53). In a recent paper [29], we described reliability and construct validity for the PDQI.

CDQI

Reliability analysis revealed an Omega total of 0.69, which is satisfactory. We also explored how well the CDQI was able to differentiate between groups related to the mother (age, education and diet quality during pregnancy) and the child (screen time and physical activity) known to have different diet quality. We calculated standardized mean difference between groups, reported as Hedges g. We saw a large effect size for maternal diet quality during pregnancy (g = − 0.93 (<=20th percentile vs. > = 80th percentile)) and screen time at 3 years (g = − 0.79 (> = 3 h/day vs. < 1 h/day)), medium effect size for maternal education (g = − 0.64 (< 12 years education vs. master degree)) and child outdoor time at 3 years (g = − 0.55 (< 1 h/day vs. > 3 h/day) and small effect size for maternal age (g = − 0.35 (<=21 years vs. > = 30 years)). Also, the CDQI showed correlations with the maternal dietary indices in the expected direction (PDQI: r = 0.32, UPFI: r = − 0.24).

Main results

We saw a strong relationship between ADHD symptoms in the MoBa 8-year questionnaire and ADHD diagnosis, as the median ADHD symptom score of children with a registered diagnosis was + 2.8 SD higher than the score of those without a registered diagnosis.
The mean (max) PDQI, UPFI and CDQI scores were 83.1 (107.1), 31.8 (89.5) and 60.3 (86.5), respectively. The mothers were on average 30.2 years of age at delivery, with 67% having at least a bachelor’s degree. The mean (range) age when the children were first registered with an ADHD diagnosis in NPR was 8 (2.6–13.8) years, with 80% being 6 years old or older. For additional details on descriptive statistics, see Table 3.
Table 3
Descriptive Statistics for the Exposures, Outcomes and Covariates*
Variables
n
Missing
Mean (SD)
Median (IQR)
Range
n
%
Exposures
 PDQI score
77,768
0
0%
83.1 (9.3)
83.9 (12.7)
38.7–107.1
 UPFI score
77,768
0
0%
31.8 (9.7)
31.2 (12.7)
0–89.5
 CDQI score
32,937
44,831
58%
60.3 (10.6)
61.5 (13.9)
2.8–86.5
Outcomes
 ADHD symptom score
37,796
39,972
51%
8.4 (7.1)
7 (7)
0–54
 ADHD diagnosis, NPR
 
0
0%
   
  Yes
2546
     
  No
75,222
     
Covariates
 Maternal age
77,768
0
0%
30.2 (4.5)
30 (6)
15–47
 Pre-pregnant BMI
75,804
1964
3%
24.0 (4.3)
23.1 (4.8)
12.5–57.8
 Maternal ADHD symptom score, mean
46,976
30,792
40%
1.1 (0.57)
1 (0.83)
0–4
 Maternal Hopkins symptom score, mean
76,321
1447
2%
0.25 (0.39)
0 (0.4)
0–3
 Maternal completed education
 
0
0%
   
   < 12 years
5474
     
  Upper secondary
21,234
     
  Bachelor
32,497
     
  Master
18,563
     
 Prenatal smoking
 
1099
1%
   
  Yes
5816
     
  No
70,853
     
 Prenatal energy intake (MJ)
77,768
0
0%
9.7 (2.6)
9.4 (3.2)
4.5–20
 Parity
 
0
0%
0.8 (0.8)
1 (1)
0–3
  0
35,140
     
  1
27,922
     
  2
11,634
     
  3
3072
     
 Child sex
 
0
0%
   
  Girls
38,037
     
  Boys
39,731
     
 Child sleep problems at 3 y
 
29,594
38%
   
  Yes
2648
     
  No
45,526
     
*For categorical and dichotomous variables, only n is given
For a one standard deviation (1SD) increase in mean diet quality score, we obtained the following results for ADHD symptom score in the adjusted models: a change in mean ADHD symptom score of − 0.28 (CI: −0.41, −0.14) for the PDQI and + 0.25 (CI: 0.13, 0.38) for the UPFI, corresponding to a relative change in mean ADHD symptom score of −3.3% (CI: −4.8, −1.6%) and + 3% (CI: 1.5, 4.5%), respectively (Fig. 2 and Additional file 7). These changes are equivalent to a 1SD change in the PDQI and UPFI scores being associated with a − 0.04 SD and + 0.035 SD change of the ADHD symptom score, respectively. We found no change in ADHD symptom score in the adjusted model for CDQI (− 0.7% (CI: − 2.2, 0.7%)).
For ADHD diagnosis, we found a relative risk in the adjusted models of 0.87 (CI: 0.79, 0.97) for the PDQI. To compare, for a 1SD increase in ADHD symptom score, the relative risk of ADHD diagnosis is 3.3 (CI: 3.0, 3.7). We found no reliable change in risk for the UPFI (1.07 (CI: 0.99, 1.18)) or CDQI (0.99 (CI: 0.90, 1.08) (Fig. 3 and Additional file 8). Analyses stratified on child sex and maternal ADHD symptoms revealed no differences in associations for either ADHD symptom scores (see Additional file 9) or ADHD diagnosis (see Additional file 10) between strata.
We estimated changes in prevalence of ADHD diagnosis for incremental changes in the PDQI score from the adjusted model (Fig. 4). To include PDQI scores covering the observed range of scores in the study population (38.7–107.1), we calculated change in prevalence of ADHD diagnosis for -5SD (PDQI score of 37) to +3SD (PDQI score of 111) from the mean PDQI score. In the effective study sample, the number of children with an ADHD diagnosis was 812, corresponding to a prevalence of 2.9%, represented by the triangle on the dashed line in Fig. 4. For example, the estimated prevalence of child ADHD in a population of mothers with a mean PDQI score 3SD’s below the study population mean is 4.4%, i.e. a 52% increase in prevalence.

Discussion

In this study, we investigated the associations between maternal and child diet quality and maternally reported ADHD symptom score at 8 years and child ADHD diagnosis. We observed inverse associations between maternal diet quality during pregnancy (measured by the PDQI) and both child ADHD symptom scores and child ADHD diagnosis, and a positive association between the contribution of ultra-processed food to total maternal energy intake in pregnancy (measured by the UPFI) and child ADHD symptom scores. We found no reliable association between UPFI and child ADHD diagnosis, or between child diet quality (measured by the CDQI at 3 years) and either outcome.
The difference in results for maternal diet quality during pregnancy and child diet quality at 3 years might be due to difference in measurement detail, since the maternal FFQ is more extensive than the questions about food intake in the child questionnaire (255 vs. 36 food items), which is likely to influence the specificity and accuracy of the instrument and subsequent dietary index scores calculated from those instruments. Nevertheless, we acknowledge that the child dietary questions include the main foods and beverages consumed at three years of age.
If the associations between maternal diet quality during pregnancy and child diet quality and ADHD symptoms and diagnosis are due to causal effects from maternal diet quality on child development, these results would support the idea of prenatal programming. During critical stages of the prenatal period, the foetus may be particularly sensitive to environmental influences like maternal nutrition, and the subsequent insults based on these influences may sustain long after birth and into adulthood [26]. A proposed mechanism behind prenatal programming is epigenetic modifications [25, 6365] via e.g. immune activation [24, 66, 67]. Interestingly, one study has found an association between prenatal high fat/high sugar diet and ADHD in youth with early onset conduct problems indirectly via epigenetic modifications of the insulin-like growth factor gene [68]. Still, studies that use robust causal identification strategies to ensure that observed associations between diet and ADHD are due to causal effects of diet are an important next step.
We found no associations between child diet quality at 3 years and either outcome, which adds to the so far undecided evidence base investigating child diet and ADHD. A recent meta-analysis found healthy dietary patterns to be associated with low ADHD symptoms in children (3–11 years old) and adolescents (12–16 years old) and unhealthy dietary patterns a risk factor for ADHD, but the authors emphasize that due to limitations in study design across most included studies, these associations do not constitute evidence for causal effects [23]. Also, the authors do not explain how they dealt with dependencies in the effect estimates, nor if they investigated for sources of heterogeneity other than publication bias, which indicates that further caution should be made in making firm inferences from their findings. Conversely, Mian et al. [69] found ADHD symptoms at 6 years to be predictive of child diet quality assessed at 8 years, but not vice versa, with ADHD symptoms at 10 years, suggesting that postnatal diet quality does not influence ADHD symptoms, but rather that some ADHD symptoms, e.g. low levels of impulse control, may result in a poorer diet.
We found associations of similar magnitude for both maternal diet quality indices in relation to child ADHD symptom score, however the associations were small. This is in line with the small association estimates found in a previous review and meta-analysis investigating maternal diet quality and child neurodevelopmental outcomes. More recently, Mesirow and colleagues [70] studied aspects of maternal diet quality in relation to child behavioural issues in children with either low conduct problems or early-onset persistent conduct problems. They found that, specifically for children presenting with early-onset conduct problems, mothers had a poorer diet quality (lower fish intake, higher processed food consumption) compared to mothers with children having low conduct problems. Additionally, maternal processed food consumption was associated with higher childhood hyperactivity (4–10 years) independent of conduct problem trajectories, but the effect estimate was small.
Isaac and Oates [71] propose that large effect estimates cannot be expected in generally healthy populations with adequate diet quality, such as the MoBa sample which consists of, on average, well educated mothers in a rich, industrialized country, when investigating outcomes related to child developmental functions. Isaac and Oates further argue that even though results might indicate an association, there might be no clinical significance as the absolute effect estimates might be minute [71]. This increases our confidence in proposing that associations seen between the maternal diet quality indices and child ADHD symptoms in generally healthy populations with, on average, adequate diet quality are so small that they have no clinical relevance. However, small effects have the potential to influence outcomes at the population level, particularly in the situation where the exposure, in this case diet, affects the entire population. Still, more research is needed to understand the association between maternal diet and child ADHD symptoms and diagnoses in populations with a higher prevalence of inadequate maternal diet quality during pregnancy.
For child ADHD diagnosis, we only found associations with the PDQI (indicates adherence to the Norwegian food-based dietary guidelines) and not for the UPFI (indicates consumption of ultra-processed foods). It is likely that the ADHD diagnosis outcome (retrieved from patient registry) is less afflicted by bias than the ADHD symptom score outcome (based on parent reported symptoms), which might be one reason for the difference in results for the two outcomes. If we assume that the results for ADHD diagnosis are more accurate due to less presence of bias, compared to the associations seen for the ADHD symptom score outcome, it can be an indication of overall maternal high diet quality during pregnancy being more important for ADHD development than the proportion of intake of ultra-processed foods, which are mostly of low nutritional value. This is in line with recent findings investigating maternal dietary patterns and birth outcomes, which found stronger associations with preterm birth for the healthy pattern compared to the unhealthy, and the opposite trend for birth weight [72], but only for data driven patterns, and not a-priori defined indices that are more generalizable. Looking at studies with outcomes that are related to ADHD, Jacka et al. [73] found the opposite trend, wherein the unhealthy dietary pattern showed a stronger association with symptoms of child behavioural difficulties compared to the healthy dietary pattern. Conversely, Steenweg-de Graaff et al. [74] found similar strengths of associations for low adherence to a healthy dietary pattern and high adherence to an unhealthy dietary pattern in relation to child externalizing difficulties.
In sum, the evidence so far relating to the comparison of association strengths between healthy and unhealthy diets is equivocal. Perhaps the most plausible reason for this is due to heterogeneous methodological approaches related to the dietary definitions and statistical analyses chosen, which should serve as an encouragement to develop standardized methodological approaches within the nutritional epidemiological field.

Strengths and limitations

The strengths of this study include a large sample size, validation of dietary indices used and use of IPW to account for selection bias. We use robust statistical methods that to our knowledge have not been previously used within the nutritional epidemiology field. Also, investigations of associations between prenatal diet quality and child ADHD diagnosis has not previously been investigated.
One limitation of this study is that self-reported information, particularly in relation to aspects of health, introduces many challenges [7577], and collecting data with FFQ’s has generated much criticism [78]. However, the maternal FFQ utilized in this study has been extensively validated and was explicitly developed for the target population [34]. Moreover, using a composite measure of overall diet quality is more robust than looking at e.g. estimations of single nutrients with regards to misreporting, and it is a recommended method for investigating diet-disease relationships [79].
Another limitation is that the MoBa is a selected group of participants and not representative of the whole population of Norwegian mothers and children. On average, MoBa mothers are older and more educated than the general pregnant population in Norway [80], and both these factors are related to better prenatal diet quality and lower levels of child difficulties. However, as we used IPW for maternal age, education and child parity in our analyses, the results might be generalized with some caution, to populations similar to MoBa’s source population.
There are some limitations related to the information on ADHD diagnosis from the NPR. The youngest children in MoBa were born in 2009 and we have information on ADHD diagnosis in MoBa up to 2017 in our data file, hence the youngest children are 8 years old in the NPR sample. As children, especially girls, might receive an ADHD diagnosis at an age older than 8, there might be some false negatives in the sample, which has been found in the comparative pregnancy cohort in Denmark [81]. In addition, a recent study investigating the practice of diagnosing ADHD in children in Norway found that only about half of the diagnoses were properly documented in the medical records, with inadequate differential diagnostic assessment being the main reason for unsatisfactory documentation for the ADHD diagnosis [82].

Conclusion

In this paper we found that higher overall maternal diet quality and lower consumption of ultra-processed foods in pregnancy were associated with lower child ADHD symptom scores, but the association estimates were small. For child ADHD diagnosis, lower risk was found only for higher overall maternal diet quality measure. We found no associations of child diet quality at 3 years with either outcome.
We emphasize that no inferences regarding causation should be made based on these results, as unmeasured confounding could contribute to the observed associations. Also, as this is the first study investigating maternal and child diet quality in relation to both child ADHD symptom score and ADHD diagnosis, it is too early to draw firm conclusions about the associations we found. Instead we encourage more research conducted on this topic, with robust methodological approaches related to study design, variable definitions and statistical analyses, which will allow for better identification of possible causal relationships.

Acknowledgements

The Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study is supported by the Norwegian Ministry of Health and Care Services and the Ministry of Education and Research. We are grateful to all the participating families in Norway who take part in this on-going cohort study.

Declarations

The establishment and data collection in MoBa was previously based on a license from the Norwegian Data Protection Agency and approval from The Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics, and it is now based on regulations related to the Norwegian Health Registry Act. The current study was approved by The Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics in South-Eastern Norway (ref: 2015/2393/REK). All MoBa mothers initially signed an informed consent form to be able to participate in the study, and they can withdraw from the study at any time. MoBa participants are informed about new projects and project updates through the MoBa newsletter and MoBa homepage.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

FJ has written two books on the topic of diet and health for commercial publication. Other authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Anhänge

Supplementary Information

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat WHO, Neurological disorders : public health challenges. 2006, World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland. WHO, Neurological disorders : public health challenges. 2006, World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Report - Mental Health in Norway [in Norwegian], A. Reneflot, et al., Editors. 2018: Oslo, Norway. Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Report - Mental Health in Norway [in Norwegian], A. Reneflot, et al., Editors. 2018: Oslo, Norway.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Biederman J, Faraone SV. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet. 2005;366(9481):237–48.PubMedCrossRef Biederman J, Faraone SV. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet. 2005;366(9481):237–48.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Thapar A, et al. Rutter's child and adolescent psychiatry. 6th ed. Chichester, West Sussex, United Kingdom: Wiley Blackwell; 2015.CrossRef Thapar A, et al. Rutter's child and adolescent psychiatry. 6th ed. Chichester, West Sussex, United Kingdom: Wiley Blackwell; 2015.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Thapar A, et al. What causes attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? Arch Dis Child. 2012;97(3):260–5.PubMedCrossRef Thapar A, et al. What causes attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? Arch Dis Child. 2012;97(3):260–5.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Rutter M, Kim-Cohen J, Maughan B. Continuities and discontinuities in psychopathology between childhood and adult life. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(3–4):276–95.PubMedCrossRef Rutter M, Kim-Cohen J, Maughan B. Continuities and discontinuities in psychopathology between childhood and adult life. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(3–4):276–95.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Reef J, et al. Developmental trajectories of child to adolescent externalizing behavior and adult DSM-IV disorder: results of a 24-year longitudinal study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2011;46(12):1233–41.PubMedCrossRef Reef J, et al. Developmental trajectories of child to adolescent externalizing behavior and adult DSM-IV disorder: results of a 24-year longitudinal study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2011;46(12):1233–41.PubMedCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Elbro C, Dalby M, Maarbjerg S. Language-learning impairments: a 30-year follow-up of language-impaired children with and without psychiatric, neurological and cognitive difficulties. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2011;46(4):437–48.PubMedCrossRef Elbro C, Dalby M, Maarbjerg S. Language-learning impairments: a 30-year follow-up of language-impaired children with and without psychiatric, neurological and cognitive difficulties. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2011;46(4):437–48.PubMedCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Snowling MJ, et al. Psychosocial outcomes at 15 years of children with a preschool history of speech-language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(8):759–65.PubMedCrossRef Snowling MJ, et al. Psychosocial outcomes at 15 years of children with a preschool history of speech-language impairment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(8):759–65.PubMedCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Faraone SV, Larsson H. Genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;24(4):562–75.PubMedCrossRef Faraone SV, Larsson H. Genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;24(4):562–75.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Palladino VS, et al. Genetic risk factors and gene-environment interactions in adult and childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr Genet. 2019;29(3):63–78.PubMedCrossRef Palladino VS, et al. Genetic risk factors and gene-environment interactions in adult and childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr Genet. 2019;29(3):63–78.PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Gaynes BN, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: identifying high priority future research needs. J Psychiatr Pract. 2014;20(2):104–17.PubMedCrossRef Gaynes BN, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: identifying high priority future research needs. J Psychiatr Pract. 2014;20(2):104–17.PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Savill NC, et al. The efficacy of atomoxetine for the treatment of children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a comprehensive review of over a decade of clinical research. CNS Drugs. 2015;29(2):131–51.PubMedCrossRef Savill NC, et al. The efficacy of atomoxetine for the treatment of children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a comprehensive review of over a decade of clinical research. CNS Drugs. 2015;29(2):131–51.PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Curtis LT, Patel K. Nutritional and environmental approaches to preventing and treating autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a review. J Altern Complement Med. 2008;14(1):79–85.PubMedCrossRef Curtis LT, Patel K. Nutritional and environmental approaches to preventing and treating autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a review. J Altern Complement Med. 2008;14(1):79–85.PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Arnold LE, DiSilvestro RA. Zinc in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2005;15(4):619–27.PubMedCrossRef Arnold LE, DiSilvestro RA. Zinc in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2005;15(4):619–27.PubMedCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Sinn N. Nutritional and dietary influences on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Nutr Rev. 2008;66(10):558–68.PubMedCrossRef Sinn N. Nutritional and dietary influences on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Nutr Rev. 2008;66(10):558–68.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Glanzman M. ADHD and nutritional supplements. Current Attention Disorders Reports. 2009;1(2):75–81.CrossRef Glanzman M. ADHD and nutritional supplements. Current Attention Disorders Reports. 2009;1(2):75–81.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Field SS. Interaction of genes and nutritional factors in the etiology of autism and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorders: a case control study. Med Hypotheses. 2014;82(6):654–61.PubMedCrossRef Field SS. Interaction of genes and nutritional factors in the etiology of autism and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorders: a case control study. Med Hypotheses. 2014;82(6):654–61.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Pelsser LM, et al. Diet and ADHD, reviewing the evidence: a systematic review of meta-analyses of double-blind placebo-controlled trials evaluating the efficacy of diet interventions on the behavior of children with ADHD. PLoS One. 2017;12(1):e0169277.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Pelsser LM, et al. Diet and ADHD, reviewing the evidence: a systematic review of meta-analyses of double-blind placebo-controlled trials evaluating the efficacy of diet interventions on the behavior of children with ADHD. PLoS One. 2017;12(1):e0169277.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Stevens LJ, et al. Dietary sensitivities and ADHD symptoms: thirty-five years of research. Clin Pediatr (Phila). 2011;50(4):279–93.CrossRef Stevens LJ, et al. Dietary sensitivities and ADHD symptoms: thirty-five years of research. Clin Pediatr (Phila). 2011;50(4):279–93.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Del-Ponte B, et al. Dietary patterns and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2019;252:160–73.PubMedCrossRef Del-Ponte B, et al. Dietary patterns and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2019;252:160–73.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Spencer SJ, Meyer U. Perinatal programming by inflammation. Brain Behav Immun. 2017;63:1–7.PubMedCrossRef Spencer SJ, Meyer U. Perinatal programming by inflammation. Brain Behav Immun. 2017;63:1–7.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Heindel JJ, Vandenberg LN. Developmental origins of health and disease: a paradigm for understanding disease cause and prevention. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2015;27(2):248–53.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Heindel JJ, Vandenberg LN. Developmental origins of health and disease: a paradigm for understanding disease cause and prevention. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2015;27(2):248–53.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat O'Neil A, et al. Relationship between diet and mental health in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Am J Public Health. 2014;104(10):e31–42.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef O'Neil A, et al. Relationship between diet and mental health in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Am J Public Health. 2014;104(10):e31–42.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Borge TC, et al. The importance of maternal diet quality during pregnancy on cognitive and behavioural outcomes in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2017:7(9). Borge TC, et al. The importance of maternal diet quality during pregnancy on cognitive and behavioural outcomes in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2017:7(9).
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Borge TC, et al. Estimating strength of associations between prenatal diet quality and child developmental outcomes – results from a large prospective pregnancy cohort. Am J Epidemiol. 2019;188(11):1902–12.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Borge TC, et al. Estimating strength of associations between prenatal diet quality and child developmental outcomes – results from a large prospective pregnancy cohort. Am J Epidemiol. 2019;188(11):1902–12.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat von Elm E, et al. The strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Int J Surg. 2014;12(12):1495–9.CrossRef von Elm E, et al. The strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Int J Surg. 2014;12(12):1495–9.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Magnus P, et al. Cohort profile update: The Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). Int J Epidemiol. 2016;45(2):382–8.PubMedCrossRef Magnus P, et al. Cohort profile update: The Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). Int J Epidemiol. 2016;45(2):382–8.PubMedCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Meltzer HM, et al. Methodological challenges when monitoring the diet of pregnant women in a large study: experiences from the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). Matern Child Nutr. 2008;4(1):14–27.PubMedCrossRef Meltzer HM, et al. Methodological challenges when monitoring the diet of pregnant women in a large study: experiences from the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). Matern Child Nutr. 2008;4(1):14–27.PubMedCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Brantsaeter AL, et al. Evaluation of urinary iodine excretion as a biomarker for intake of milk and dairy products in pregnant women in the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). Eur J Clin Nutr. 2009;63(3):347–54.PubMedCrossRef Brantsaeter AL, et al. Evaluation of urinary iodine excretion as a biomarker for intake of milk and dairy products in pregnant women in the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). Eur J Clin Nutr. 2009;63(3):347–54.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Brantsaeter AL, et al. Validity of a new food frequency questionnaire for pregnant women in the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). Matern Child Nutr. 2008;4(1):28–43.PubMedCrossRef Brantsaeter AL, et al. Validity of a new food frequency questionnaire for pregnant women in the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). Matern Child Nutr. 2008;4(1):28–43.PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Brantsaeter AL, et al. Urine flavonoids and plasma carotenoids in the validation of fruit, vegetable and tea intake during pregnancy in the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). Public Health Nutr. 2007;10(8):838–47.PubMedCrossRef Brantsaeter AL, et al. Urine flavonoids and plasma carotenoids in the validation of fruit, vegetable and tea intake during pregnancy in the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). Public Health Nutr. 2007;10(8):838–47.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Brantsaeter AL, et al. Exploration of biomarkers for total fish intake in pregnant Norwegian women. Public Health Nutr. 2010;13(1):54–62.PubMedCrossRef Brantsaeter AL, et al. Exploration of biomarkers for total fish intake in pregnant Norwegian women. Public Health Nutr. 2010;13(1):54–62.PubMedCrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Lauritsen, J., Foodcalc v. 1.3 diet, cancer and health project, Danish Cancer Society, 1998. Lauritsen, J., Foodcalc v. 1.3 diet, cancer and health project, Danish Cancer Society, 1998.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Rimestad AH, et al., The Norwegian Food Composition Table [in Norwegian]. 2005, Norwegian Food safety authority; Norwegian Directorate of Health; University of Oslo, department of nutrition; Gyldendal Undervisning: Oslo. Rimestad AH, et al., The Norwegian Food Composition Table [in Norwegian]. 2005, Norwegian Food safety authority; Norwegian Directorate of Health; University of Oslo, department of nutrition; Gyldendal Undervisning: Oslo.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Kennedy ET, et al. The healthy eating index: design and applications. J Am Diet Assoc. 1995;95(10):1103–8.PubMedCrossRef Kennedy ET, et al. The healthy eating index: design and applications. J Am Diet Assoc. 1995;95(10):1103–8.PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat von Ruesten A, et al. Adherence of pregnant women to Nordic dietary guidelines in relation to postpartum weight retention: results from the Norwegian mother and child cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:75.CrossRef von Ruesten A, et al. Adherence of pregnant women to Nordic dietary guidelines in relation to postpartum weight retention: results from the Norwegian mother and child cohort study. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:75.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Norwegian Directorate of Health, Norwegian guidelines on diet, nutrition and physical activity [in Norwegian]. 2014: Oslo, Norway. Norwegian Directorate of Health, Norwegian guidelines on diet, nutrition and physical activity [in Norwegian]. 2014: Oslo, Norway.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Monteiro CA, et al. The UN decade of nutrition, the NOVA food classification and the trouble with ultra-processing. Public Health Nutr. 2018;21(1):5–17.PubMedCrossRef Monteiro CA, et al. The UN decade of nutrition, the NOVA food classification and the trouble with ultra-processing. Public Health Nutr. 2018;21(1):5–17.PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Huybrechts I, et al. Reproducibility and validity of a diet quality index for children assessed using a FFQ. Br J Nutr. 2010;104(1):135–44.PubMedCrossRef Huybrechts I, et al. Reproducibility and validity of a diet quality index for children assessed using a FFQ. Br J Nutr. 2010;104(1):135–44.PubMedCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Pinket AS, et al. Diet quality in European pre-schoolers: evaluation based on diet quality indices and association with gender, socio-economic status and overweight, the ToyBox-study. Public Health Nutr. 2016;19(13):2441–50.PubMedCrossRef Pinket AS, et al. Diet quality in European pre-schoolers: evaluation based on diet quality indices and association with gender, socio-economic status and overweight, the ToyBox-study. Public Health Nutr. 2016;19(13):2441–50.PubMedCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat WHO, Food and nutrition policy for schools. A tool for the development of school nutrition programmes in the European Region, P.f.N.a.F. Security, Editor. 2006, World Health Organization: Copenhagen. WHO, Food and nutrition policy for schools. A tool for the development of school nutrition programmes in the European Region, P.f.N.a.F. Security, Editor. 2006, World Health Organization: Copenhagen.
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Norwegian Directorate of Health. Weights, measures and portion sizes for foods. 2015. Norwegian Directorate of Health. Weights, measures and portion sizes for foods. 2015.
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Silva RR, et al. A rating scale for disruptive behavior disorders, based on the DSM-IV item pool. Psychiatr Q. 2005;76(4):327–39.PubMedCrossRef Silva RR, et al. A rating scale for disruptive behavior disorders, based on the DSM-IV item pool. Psychiatr Q. 2005;76(4):327–39.PubMedCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat World Health Organization, ICD-10 : psykiske lidelser og atferdsforstyrrelser : kliniske beskrivelser og diagnostiske retningslinjer [In Norwegian]. The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders 1999, Oslo: Universitetsforl. World Health Organization, ICD-10 : psykiske lidelser og atferdsforstyrrelser : kliniske beskrivelser og diagnostiske retningslinjer [In Norwegian]. The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders 1999, Oslo: Universitetsforl.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Karlstad O, et al. ADHD treatment and diagnosis in relation to children's birth month: Nationwide cohort study from Norway. Scand J Public Health. 2017;45(4):343–9.PubMedCrossRef Karlstad O, et al. ADHD treatment and diagnosis in relation to children's birth month: Nationwide cohort study from Norway. Scand J Public Health. 2017;45(4):343–9.PubMedCrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Revelle W, Zinbarg RE. Coefficients alpha, Beta, omega, and the glb: comments on Sijtsma. Psychometrika. 2008;74(1):145.CrossRef Revelle W, Zinbarg RE. Coefficients alpha, Beta, omega, and the glb: comments on Sijtsma. Psychometrika. 2008;74(1):145.CrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat R Core Team, R: A language and environment for statistical computing. 2017, R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria. R Core Team, R: A language and environment for statistical computing. 2017, R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria.
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Bürkner, P.-C., brms: An R Package for Bayesian Multilevel Models Using Stan. J Stat Softw, 2017. 80(1): p. 28. Bürkner, P.-C., brms: An R Package for Bayesian Multilevel Models Using Stan. J Stat Softw, 2017. 80(1): p. 28.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Altman DG, Royston P. The cost of dichotomising continuous variables. BMJ (Clinical research ed). 2006;332(7549):1080.CrossRef Altman DG, Royston P. The cost of dichotomising continuous variables. BMJ (Clinical research ed). 2006;332(7549):1080.CrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Kessler RC, et al. The World Health Organization Adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med. 2005;35(2):245–56.PubMedCrossRef Kessler RC, et al. The World Health Organization Adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med. 2005;35(2):245–56.PubMedCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Gelman A, Simpson D, Betancourt M. The prior can often only be understood in the context of the likelihood. Entropy. 2017;19(10):555.CrossRef Gelman A, Simpson D, Betancourt M. The prior can often only be understood in the context of the likelihood. Entropy. 2017;19(10):555.CrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat van de Schoot R, et al. A gentle introduction to Bayesian analysis: applications to developmental research. Child Dev. 2014;85(3):842–60.PubMedCrossRef van de Schoot R, et al. A gentle introduction to Bayesian analysis: applications to developmental research. Child Dev. 2014;85(3):842–60.PubMedCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat van de Schoot R, Depaoli S. Bayesian analyses: where to start and what to report. Eur Health Psychol. 2014;16(2):75–84. van de Schoot R, Depaoli S. Bayesian analyses: where to start and what to report. Eur Health Psychol. 2014;16(2):75–84.
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Biele G, et al. Bias from self selection and loss to follow-up in prospective cohort studies. Eur J Epidemiol. 2019. Biele G, et al. Bias from self selection and loss to follow-up in prospective cohort studies. Eur J Epidemiol. 2019.
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Nohr EA, Liew Z. How to investigate and adjust for selection bias in cohort studies. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand. 2018;97(4):407–16.PubMedCrossRef Nohr EA, Liew Z. How to investigate and adjust for selection bias in cohort studies. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand. 2018;97(4):407–16.PubMedCrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Moody L, Chen H, Pan YX. Early-life nutritional programming of cognition-The fundamental role of epigenetic mechanisms in mediating the relation between early-life environment and learning and memory process. Adv Nutr. 2017;8(2):337–50.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Moody L, Chen H, Pan YX. Early-life nutritional programming of cognition-The fundamental role of epigenetic mechanisms in mediating the relation between early-life environment and learning and memory process. Adv Nutr. 2017;8(2):337–50.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Banik A, et al. Maternal Factors that Induce Epigenetic Changes Contribute to Neurological Disorders in Offspring. Genes (Basel). 2017:8(6). Banik A, et al. Maternal Factors that Induce Epigenetic Changes Contribute to Neurological Disorders in Offspring. Genes (Basel). 2017:8(6).
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Gartstein MA, Skinner MK. Prenatal influences on temperament development: The role of environmental epigenetics. Dev Psychopathol. 2018;30(4):1269–303.PubMedCrossRef Gartstein MA, Skinner MK. Prenatal influences on temperament development: The role of environmental epigenetics. Dev Psychopathol. 2018;30(4):1269–303.PubMedCrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Bilbo SD, Schwarz JM. Early-life programming of later-life brain and behavior: a critical role for the immune system. Front Behav Neurosci. 2009;3:14.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bilbo SD, Schwarz JM. Early-life programming of later-life brain and behavior: a critical role for the immune system. Front Behav Neurosci. 2009;3:14.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Rijlaarsdam J, et al. Prenatal unhealthy diet, insulin-like growth factor 2 gene (IGF2) methylation, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in youth with early-onset conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58(1):19–27.PubMedCrossRef Rijlaarsdam J, et al. Prenatal unhealthy diet, insulin-like growth factor 2 gene (IGF2) methylation, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in youth with early-onset conduct problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58(1):19–27.PubMedCrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Mian A, et al. Children's attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms predict lower diet quality but not vice versa: results from bidirectional analyses in a population-based cohort. J Nutr. 2019;149(4):642–8.PubMedCrossRef Mian A, et al. Children's attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms predict lower diet quality but not vice versa: results from bidirectional analyses in a population-based cohort. J Nutr. 2019;149(4):642–8.PubMedCrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Mesirow MS, et al. Associations between prenatal and early childhood fish and processed Food intake, conduct problems, and co-occurring difficulties. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2017;45(5):1039–49.PubMedCrossRef Mesirow MS, et al. Associations between prenatal and early childhood fish and processed Food intake, conduct problems, and co-occurring difficulties. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2017;45(5):1039–49.PubMedCrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Isaacs E, Oates J. Nutrition and cognition: assessing cognitive abilities in children and young people. Eur J Nutr. 2008;47(Suppl 3):4–24.PubMedCrossRef Isaacs E, Oates J. Nutrition and cognition: assessing cognitive abilities in children and young people. Eur J Nutr. 2008;47(Suppl 3):4–24.PubMedCrossRef
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacka FN, et al. Maternal and early postnatal nutrition and mental health of offspring by age 5 years: a prospective cohort study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(10):1038–47.PubMedCrossRef Jacka FN, et al. Maternal and early postnatal nutrition and mental health of offspring by age 5 years: a prospective cohort study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(10):1038–47.PubMedCrossRef
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Steenweg-de Graaff J, et al. Maternal dietary patterns during pregnancy and child internalising and externalising problems. The Generation R Study. Clinical Nutrition. 2014;33(1):115–21.PubMedCrossRef Steenweg-de Graaff J, et al. Maternal dietary patterns during pregnancy and child internalising and externalising problems. The Generation R Study. Clinical Nutrition. 2014;33(1):115–21.PubMedCrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Hebert JR, et al. Social desirability bias in dietary self-report may compromise the validity of dietary intake measures. Int J Epidemiol. 1995;24(2):389–98.PubMedCrossRef Hebert JR, et al. Social desirability bias in dietary self-report may compromise the validity of dietary intake measures. Int J Epidemiol. 1995;24(2):389–98.PubMedCrossRef
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Kipnis V, et al. Bias in dietary-report instruments and its implications for nutritional epidemiology. Public Health Nutr. 2002;5(6a):915–23.PubMedCrossRef Kipnis V, et al. Bias in dietary-report instruments and its implications for nutritional epidemiology. Public Health Nutr. 2002;5(6a):915–23.PubMedCrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Abel MH, et al. Suboptimal maternal iodine intake is associated with impaired child neurodevelopment at 3 years of age in the Norwegian mother and child cohort study. J Nutr. 2017. Abel MH, et al. Suboptimal maternal iodine intake is associated with impaired child neurodevelopment at 3 years of age in the Norwegian mother and child cohort study. J Nutr. 2017.
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Archer E, Marlow ML, Lavie CJ. Controversy and debate: memory based methods paper 1: The fatal flaws of Food frequency questionnaires and other memory-based dietary assessment methods. J Clin Epidemiol. In press. Archer E, Marlow ML, Lavie CJ. Controversy and debate: memory based methods paper 1: The fatal flaws of Food frequency questionnaires and other memory-based dietary assessment methods. J Clin Epidemiol. In press.
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Ioannidis JPA. Implausible results in human nutrition research. Br Med J. 2013;347. Ioannidis JPA. Implausible results in human nutrition research. Br Med J. 2013;347.
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Nilsen RM, et al. Self-selection and bias in a large prospective pregnancy cohort in Norway. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2009;23(6):597–608.PubMedCrossRef Nilsen RM, et al. Self-selection and bias in a large prospective pregnancy cohort in Norway. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2009;23(6):597–608.PubMedCrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Madsen KB, et al. Characteristics of undiagnosed children with parent-reported ADHD behaviour. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2018;27(2):149–58.CrossRef Madsen KB, et al. Characteristics of undiagnosed children with parent-reported ADHD behaviour. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2018;27(2):149–58.CrossRef
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Suren P, et al. Diagnosis of hyperkinetic disorder among children in Norway. Tidsskr Nor Laegeforen. 2018:138(20). Suren P, et al. Diagnosis of hyperkinetic disorder among children in Norway. Tidsskr Nor Laegeforen. 2018:138(20).
Metadaten
Titel
The associations between maternal and child diet quality and child ADHD – findings from a large Norwegian pregnancy cohort study
verfasst von
Tiril Cecilie Borge
Guido Biele
Eleni Papadopoulou
Lene Frost Andersen
Felice Jacka
Merete Eggesbø
Ida Henriette Caspersen
Heidi Aase
Helle Margrete Meltzer
Anne Lise Brantsæter
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2021
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2021
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-021-03130-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2021

BMC Psychiatry 1/2021 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).