Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Annals of General Psychiatry 1/2016

Open Access 01.12.2016 | Primary research

The genetic and environmental structure of the character sub-scales of the temperament and character inventory in adolescence

Erschienen in: Annals of General Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2016

Abstract

Background

The character higher order scales (self-directedness, cooperativeness, and self-transcendence) in the temperament and character inventory are important general measures of health and well-being [Mens Sana Monograph 11:16–24 (2013)]. Recent research has found suggestive evidence of common environmental influence on the development of these character traits during adolescence. The present article expands earlier research by focusing on the internal consistency and the etiology of traits measured by the lower order sub-scales of the character traits in adolescence.

Methods

The twin modeling analysis of 423 monozygotic pairs and 408 same sex dizygotic pairs estimated additive genetics (A), common environmental (C), and non-shared environmental (E) influences on twin resemblance. All twins were part of the on-going longitudinal Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS).

Results

The twin modeling analysis suggested a common environmental contribution for two out of five self-directedness sub-scales (0.14 and 0.23), for three out of five cooperativeness sub-scales (0.07–0.17), and for all three self-transcendence sub-scales (0.10–0.12).

Conclusion

The genetic structure at the level of the character lower order sub-scales in adolescents shows that the proportion of the shared environmental component varies in the trait of self-directedness and in the trait of cooperativeness, while it is relatively stable across the components of self-transcendence. The presence of this unique shared environmental effect in adolescence has implications for understanding the relative importance of interventions and treatment strategies aimed at promoting overall maturation of character, mental health, and well-being during this period of the life span.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​s12991-016-0094-2) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Abkürzungen
A
additive genetics factors
C
common environmental factors
E
non-shared environmental factors
CATSS
The Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden
MZ
monozygotic
DZ
dizygotic
ICC
intraclass correlation
SD1
responsibility vs. blaming
SD2
purposefulness vs. lack of goal direction
SD3
resourcefulness vs. inertia
SD4
self-acceptance vs. self-striving
SD5
self-actualizing vs. bad habits
CO1
social acceptance vs. social intolerance
CO2
empathy vs. social disinterest
CO3
helpfulness vs. unhelpfulness
CO4
compassion vs. revengefulness
CO5
integrated conscience vs. self-serving advantage
ST1
creative self-forgetfulness vs. self-conscious experience
ST2
transpersonal identification vs. personal identification
ST3
spiritual acceptance vs. rational materialism

Background

Cloninger’s theory of personality proposes that human beings are comprised of an integrated hierarchy of biological, psychological, and social systems that allow them to adapt more or less flexibly and maturely to changes in their external and internal milieu [1]. This model consists of a temperament domain (i.e., individual differences in behavioral learning mechanisms influencing basic emotional drives) and a character domain (i.e., self-concepts about goals and values that express what people make of themselves intentionally). For the measurement of these personality domains, Cloninger has developed the temperament and character inventory [1] composed of four dimensions of temperament (novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, and persistence) and three dimensions of character (self-directedness, cooperativeness, and self-transcendence). These temperament and character dimensions serve as tools for disentangling personality profiles of healthy individuals, as well as of individuals with neuropsychiatric disorders [26]. Moreover, self-directedness, cooperativeness, and self-transcendence assessed by the Temperament and Character Inventory are important general measures of health and well-being [710]. Self-transcendence, however, is positively related to both positive and negative emotions during the adolescent years and during adulthood in cultures that discourage open emotional expression [4, 11].
Previous findings have shown that heritability influences on character are about the same across studies using different age groups. Nonetheless, there are some differences worth noting. For example, while the character scales do not show common environmental influences in research among older adults (e.g., [12]), a small common environmental influence for self-directedness and cooperativeness has been found among young adults (20–30 years of age; e.g., [13]). In addition, recent research using one of the largest population-based twin studies among adolescents, found suggestive evidence of common environmental influence for all of the character scales [14]. What is more, Gillespie and colleagues [12] showed in adults, and Garcia and colleagues [14] in adolescents, that the genetic structure of the temperament higher order scales shows no evidence of a shared or common environmental effect (C) across the scales. The exception being that in adolescents, in contrast to adults, there was a small shared environmental effect in the temperament dimension of reward dependence (i.e., the individuals' tendency to respond markedly to signals of social approval, social support, and sentimentality). The effect size is similar to that which is observed in adolescents’ character dimensions. Overall the effect size of additive genetics (A) to non-shared environmental effect (E) is slightly larger across the temperament dimensions in adolescents compared to adults (see Fig. 1a, b). In contrast, the genetic structure of the character scales in the adolescent sample shows a modest but noteworthy proportion of shared environmental influence that is not present in the adult sample studied by Gillespie and colleagues (Fig. 2a, b). In other words, there is greater consistency, between the adolescent and the adult sample, in the proportion of additive genetic effect to non-shared environmental effect with respect to temperament but not with respect to character. These results suggest a “shift” in the type of environmental influence (i.e., shared to non-shared) from adolescence to adulthood with regard to character. In this context, it is important to point out that interventions to enhance self-directedness and cooperativeness can alleviate dysfunction and suffering related to different psychiatric disorders [3]. Character traits improve with cognitive-behavioral treatments and baseline levels of character are strong predictors of clinical outcomes [1518]. If the “shift” in environmental influence exists, then interventions targeting character development may be more successful if conducted during adolescence or young adulthood.
The Temperament and Character Inventory’s character scales, as well as those scales measuring temperament, are higher order scales composed of lower order sub-scales. The higher order scales have the advantage of allowing the prediction of many outcomes (e.g., personality disorders) because they represent wide-ranging descriptions of personality (see [19]). Nevertheless, one disadvantage when personality is only investigated in terms of broad scales is that the aggregation of the lower order sub-scales in one higher order scale results in a loss of information—information that might be useful for psychological description, prediction, and explanation (see [19]). The present article expands earlier research (e.g., [14]) by focusing on the etiology of the lower order sub-scales of the character dimension of personality in adolescence. Thus, it targets information that may be useful in the study of adolescents’ mental health and well-being. For a brief description of low and high scorers in each of the lower order sub-scales for the character traits of the Temperament and Character Inventory, please see the Tables 1, 2, and 3.
Table 1
The five lower order sub-scales that compose the self-directedness (SD) scale of the Temperament and Character Inventory
High Scorers
 
Low Scorers
Tend to feel free to choose what they will do. They recognize that their attitudes, behaviors, and problems generally reflect their own choices. Consequently, they tend to accept responsibility for their attitudes and behavior. They are reliable and trustworthy
(SD1) responsibility vs. blaming
Tend to blame other people and external circumstances for what is happening to them. They feel that their attitudes, behavior, and choices are determined by influences outside their control or against their will. Consequently, they tend not to accept responsibility for their actions
They have a clear sense of meaning and direction in their lives. They have developed the ability to delay gratification to achieve their goals
(SD2) purposefulness vs. lack of goal direction
Tend to struggle to find direction, purpose, and meaning in their lives. They are uncertain about long-term goals, and thus feel driven to react to current circumstances and immediate needs. They may feel that their life is empty and has little or no meaning beyond the reactive impulses of the moment
Usually described as resourceful and effective. They impress other people as productive, proactive, competent, and innovative individuals who rarely lack ideas on how to solve problems or initiative in identifying opportunities to solve problems. Indeed, they tend to look at a difficult situation as a challenge or an opportunity
(SD3) resourcefulness vs. inertia
Impress others as helpless, hopeless, and ineffective. These individuals have not developed skills and confidence in solving problems and thus feel unable and incompetent when faced with obstacles. Typically, they tend to wait for others to take the lead in getting things done
Self-confident individuals who recognize and accept both their strengths and limitations. In other words, these individuals try to do the best that they can without pretending to be something they are not. Rather, they seem to accept and feel very comfortable with their actual mental and physical features, although they may try to improve these limitations by constructive training and effort
(SD4) self-acceptance vs. self-striving
Tend to manifest low self-esteem. They neither accept nor enjoy their actual mental and physical features. Rather, they often pretend to be different than they really are. That is, they tend to fantasize about unlimited wealth, importance, beauty, and perpetual youth. When confronted with evidence to the contrary, they may become severely disturbed
These individuals have developed a spectrum of goal-congruent, good habits so that they automatically act in accord with their long-term values and goals. This is achieved gradually as a consequence of self-discipline, but eventually becomes automatic. These habits usually develop through repeated practice and are typically stronger than most momentary impulses or persuasion. In other words, these individuals rarely confuse their priorities and thus feel safe and self trusting in many tempting situations
(SD5) self-actualizing vs. bad habits
These individual manifest habits that are inconsistent with and make it hard for them to accomplish worthwhile goals. Others sometimes perceive these peoples as self-defeating and weak-willed. In other words, their will power appears to be too weak to overcome many strong temptations, even if they know that they will suffer as a consequence
Table 2
The five lower order sub-scales that compose the cooperativeness (CO) scale of the Temperament and Character Inventory
High Scorers
 
Low Scorers
They are described as tolerant and friendly. These individuals tend to accept other people as they are, even people with very different behaviors, ethics, opinions, values, or appearances
(CO1) social acceptance vs. social intolerance
They are typically impatient with and critical of other people, especially people who have different goals and values
They typically try to imagine themselves “in other people’s shoes”. In other words, these individuals are highly attenuated to and considerate of other people’s feelings. They tend to treat others with dignity and respect, and often put aside their own judgement initially so they can better understand what other people are experiencing. Empathy also involves a conscious understanding of, and respect for, the goals and values of other people
(CO2) empathy vs. social disinterest
These individuals do not seem to be very concerned about other’s feeling. Rather they seem to be unable to share in another’s emotions, suffering, or hardship, or at least are unwilling to respect (i.e., assign value to) the goals and values of other people
Tend to be helpful, supportive, encouraging, or reassuring. These individuals enjoy being in service of others. Often they share their skills and knowledge so that everyone comes out ahead. They like to work as part of a team, usually preferring this to working alone
(CO3) helpfulness vs. unhelpfulness
They are described as self-centerd, egoistic, or selfish. They tend to be inconsiderate of other people and typically look out only for themselves, even when working in a team of highly cooperative collaborators. They prefer to work alone or to be in charge of what is done
These individuals are described as compassionate, forgiving, charitable, and benevolent. They do not enjoy revenge and usually do not try to get even if they were treated badly. Rather, these individuals actively try to get over insults or unfair treatment in order to be constructive in a relationship
(CO4) compassion vs. revengefulness
Tend to enjoy getting revenge on people who hurt them. Their revengeful triumph can be either overt or disguised. The former is observed as active-aggressive behavior, such as hurting other physically, emotionally, and financially. The latter is observed as passive-aggressive behaviors, such as holding grudges, deliberate forgetfulness, stubbornness, and procrastination
They are described as honest, genuinely scrupulous, and sincere persons who treat others in a consistently fair manner. In other words, these persons have incorporated stable ethical principles and scruples in both their professional and their social and interpersonal relationships. Such ethical standards are a component of social cooperativeness, rather than related to self-Transcendence or spirituality
(CO5) integrated conscience vs. self-serving advantage
These individuals are described as opportunistic, i.e., they would do whatever they can to get away with to reach their goals without getting in immediate trouble. These individuals tend to treat other people unfairly, in a biased, self-serving manner that usually reflects their own profit. They are thus frequently described as manipulative or deceitful. In other words, they have not incorporated stable ethical principles and scruples into their social and interpersonal relationships
Table 3
The three lower order sub-scales that compose the self-transcendence (ST) scale of the Temperament and Character Inventory
High Scorers
 
Low Scorers
Tend to transcend their self-boundaries when deeply involved in a relationship or when concentrating in what they are doing, forget where they are for a while and lose awareness of the passage of time. Thus, appearing “in another world” or “absent minded”. Individuals who experience such self-forgetfulness are often described as creative and original
(ST1) creative self-forgetfulness vs. self-conscious experience
Tend to remain aware of their individuality in a relationship or when concentrating on their work. These individuals are rarely deeply moved by art or beauty. Thus, others usually perceive them as conventional, prosaic, unimaginative, or self-conscious
Tend to experience an extraordinarily strong connection to nature and the universe as a whole, including the physical environment as well as people. They often report feeling that everything seems to be a part of a living organism and are often willing to make personal sacrifices in order to make the world a better place by trying to prevent war, poverty, or injustice. They might be regarded as fuzzy-thinking idealists
(ST2) transpersonal identification vs. personal identification
Rarely experience strong connections to nature or people. They tend to be individualists who feel that they are neither directly nor indirectly responsible for what is going on with other people or the rest of the world. Such individuals view nature as an external object to be manipulated instrumentally, rather than something of which they are an integral part
Often believe in miracles, extrasensory experiences, and other spiritual phenomena such as telepathy or a “sixth sense”. They show magical thinking and are both vitalized and comforted by spiritual experiences. They might deal with suffering and even death through faith that they have, which may involve communion with their God
(ST3) spiritual acceptance vs. rational materialism
Tend to accept only materialism and objective empiricism. They are often unwilling to accept things that cannot be scientifically explained. This, in turn, is a disadvantage when they face situations over which there is no control or possibility for evaluating by rational objective means (e.g., inevitable death, suffering, or unjust punishments)

The present study

The present study expands earlier research by focusing on the internal consistency and the etiology of traits measured by the lower order sub-scales of the character traits in adolescence. The study was conducted using self-reported character measures from The Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS), which is an on-going large population-based longitudinal twin study targeting all twins born in Sweden since July 1, 1992. By January 2013, the CATSS comprised around 23,000 twins and it had a response rate of roughly 76 % (for a detailed description of the CATSS see [20]). We used data from a sample of 15-year-old twins (detailed in [14]) in order to capture a critical period of life where personality undergoes huge developmental processes related to adolescents’ ill- and well-being. We target the etiology of the character sub-scales to catch information that may be useful for psychological description, prediction, and explanation of mental health and well-being.

Methods

Ethical statement

The present analyses included twins who provided data at the CATSS-9/12, CATSS-15, and DOGSS studies. All data collections have separate ethical approvals from the Karolinska Institute ethical review board (DNR: 02-289, 2010/597-31/1, 2010/1356/31/1, 03-672, and 2009/739-31/5). The participants, both parent and children/adolescents are protected by informed consent process. They were informed of what is being collected and were repeatedly given the option to withdraw their consent and discontinue their participation.

Sample and procedure

In the present study we used data from the CATSS, earlier described in Garcia et al. [14], whose parents were interviewed by telephone using the Autism—Tics, ADHD ,and other Comorbidities inventory [21] when the twins were 9 or 12 years of age. At the age of 15, the twins completed a battery of questionnaires that were sent by mail (overall response rate 48 %), including the short version (125 items) of the Temperament and Character Inventory. Moreover, twins who screened positive for any neuropsychiatric disorder and controls were part of a detailed clinical interview that included the longer Temperament and Character Inventory version (238 items).1 Previously, Garcia and colleagues [14] developed a valid and reliable item-extraction procedure to generate the short version from the larger version of temperament and character inventory. This allowed us to conduct the correlation, reliability, and the twin modeling analysis using the whole twin sample based on the short version of the Temperament and Character Inventory. Only twins who had a maximum of 5 % missed items and have answered the control questions correctly were included in the final analyses (a common procedure regarding the Temperament and Character Inventory, [22]).
For the correlation and reliability analysis, addressing the internal consistency of the lower order sub-scales (Additional file 1: Tables S1–S3), we used a total of 2714 twins (878 monozygotic, 885 same sex dizygotic, 638 different sex dizygotic, and 313 of unknown zygosity). The twin modeling analysis addressing the etiology of traits measured by the lower order sub-scales of the character traits required only twins with known zygosity. In essence the model compares traits in monozygotic twins, who are genetically identical, with traits in dizygotic twins, who on average share 50 % of their segregating alleles. The difference in genetic relatedness can then be used to disentangle the genetic and environmental contribution to a trait, in this case the lower order character traits. Hence, for this specific analysis we were only able to use 423 monozygotic pairs and 408 same sex dizygotic pairs.

Measures

Zygosity

Zygosity was determined on the basis of 48 single nucleotide polymorphisms [20]. For twins without available DNA, zygosity was determined using a validated algorithm based on five questions on twin similarity derived from 571 pairs of twins with known zygosity. Only twins with more than 95 % probability of being correctly classified, compared to DNA testing, were assigned zygosity by this method. In other words, the twins with less than 95 % probability of being correctly classified were assigned as unknown zygosity [23].

Temperament and Character Inventory

The temperament and character inventory measures the seven scales, and its sub-scales, of the psychobiological model of personality (binary answer: true = 1, false = 0). The five sub-scales of the self-directedness scales are: responsibility vs. blaming (SD1, e.g., “I often feel that I am the victim of circumstances”, reverse coded), purposefulness vs. lack of goal direction (SD2, e.g., “My behavior is strongly guided by certain goals that I have set for my life”), resourcefulness vs. inertia (SD3, e.g., “I usually look at a difficult situation as a challenge or opportunity”), self-acceptance vs. self-striving (SD4, e.g., “I often wish I was stronger than everyone else”, reverse coded), and self-actualization (former congruent second nature) vs. bad habits (SD5, e.g., “Many of my habits make it hard for me to accomplish worthwhile goals”, reverse coded).
The five sub-scales of the cooperativeness scales are: social acceptance vs. social intolerance (CO1, e.g., “I can usually accept other people as they are, even when they are very different from me”), empathy vs. social disinterest (CO2, e.g., “I often consider another person’s feelings as much as my own”), helpfulness vs. unhelpfulness (CO3, e.g., “I like to share what I have learned with other people”), compassion vs. revengefulness (CO4, e.g., “I hate to see anyone suffer”), and integrated conscience vs. self-serving advantage (CO5, e.g., “I cannot have any peace of mind if I treat other people unfairly, even if they are unfair to me”).
The three sub-scales of the self-transcendence scales are: creative self-forgetfulness vs. self-conscious experience (ST1, e.g., “I often become so fascinated with what I’m doing that I get lost in the moment—like I’m detached from time and place”), transpersonal identification vs. personal identification (ST2, e.g., “I sometimes feel so connected to nature that everything seems to be part of one living organism”), and spiritual acceptance vs. rational materialism (ST3, e.g., “I seem to have a “sixth sense” that sometimes allows me to know what is going to happen”).

Statistical treatment

All data were considered to be normally distributed after graphical exploration (histograms), thus all statistical tests were conducted using parametric methods in SPSS version 19. Cronbach ‘s alphas and Pearson’s correlations coefficients for the character lower order sub-scales are reported in Additional file 1: Table S1–S3.
The etiology of the character lower order sub-scales was investigated using twin methodology. The genetic and environmental contributions are portioned into three variance components: additive genetic factors (A), common environmental factors that make the twins similar (C), and unique environmental factors that make the twins dissimilar (E). In the first step, intraclass correlation (ICC) coefficients, for the character sub-scales, were calculated separately for monozygotic twins and same sex dizygotic twins. As a second step, we performed univariate genetic analyses, using a model-fitting approach with structural equation-modeling techniques conducted in Mx [24].

Results

The correlation and reliability analysis addressing the internal consistency of the lower order sub-scales is presented in Additional file 1: Table S1–S3. The twin modeling analysis addressing the etiology of traits suggested a common environmental contribution for the following self-directedness sub-scales: purposefulness vs. lack of goal direction (0.14) and self-actualizing (former congruent second nature) vs. bad habits (0.23); for three of the cooperativeness sub-scales: empathy vs. social disinterest (0.10), helpfulness vs. unhelpfulness (0.07), and compassion vs. revengefulness (0.17); and for all three self-transcendence sub-scales: creative self-forgetfulness vs. self-conscious experience, transpersonal identification vs. personal identification, and spiritual acceptance vs. rational materialism (between 0.10 and .12). All sub-scales in the self-directedness scale were under a large unique environmental influence that ranged from 0.49 to 0.70 (Table 4). This pattern could be discerned in all sub-scales in the cooperativeness (Table 5) and self-transcendence (Table 6) scales as well. There was a general trend suggesting that the genetic component had a larger influence than the common environmental component, in all sub-scales of the three character dimensions. The confidence intervals were, however, overlapping in all cases.
Table 4
Intraclass correlations (ICC) according to zygosity and estimates of genetic and environmental effects for the five lower order sub-scales that compose the self-directedness scale of the Temperament and Character Inventory [95 % confidence interval]
 
MZ
DZ
A
C
E
(n = 423 pairs)
(n = 408 pairs)
Additive genetics
Common environment
Unique environment
Self-directedness
0.52
0.36
.29
.22
.49
[.44, .58]
[.27, .44]
[.09, .50]
[.04, .38]
[.43, .56]
(SD1) responsibility vs. blaming
0.45
0.21
0.42
0.01
0.57
[.37, .52]
[.12, .31]
[.19, .50]
[.00, .21]
[.50, .64]
(SD2) purposefulness vs. lack of goal direction
0.30
0.22
0.16
0.14
0.70
[.21, .38]
[.12, .31]
[.00, .38]
[.00, .31]
[.62, .79]
(SD3) resourcefulness vs. inertia
0.40
0.23
0.32
0.07
0.61
[.32, .48]
[.13, .32]
[.08, .46]
[.00, .27]
[.54, .69]
(SD4) self-acceptance vs. self-striving
0.48
0.21
0.47
0.00
0.53
[.41, .55]
[.11, .30]
[.30, .53]
[.00, .14]
[.47, .60]
(SD5) self-actualizing vs. bad habits
0.33
0.29
0.11
0.23
0.66
[.23, .41]
[.20, .38]
[.00, .35]
[.03, .36]
[.58, .75]
MZ monozygotic, DZ dizygotic
Table 5
Intraclass correlations (ICC) according to zygosity and estimates of genetic and environmental effects for the five lower order sub-scales that compose the cooperativeness scale of the Temperament and Character Inventory [95 % confidence interval]
 
MZ
DZ
A
C
E
 
(n = 423 pairs)
(n = 408 pairs)
Additive genetics
Common environment
Unique environment
Cooperativeness
0.59
0.40
.38
.21
.41
[.52, .65]
[.32, .48]
[.19, .57]
[.04, .37]
[.36, .47]
(CO1) social acceptance vs. social Intolerance
0.40
0.22
0.42
0.00
0.58
[.32, .48]
[.12, .31]
[.20, .49]
[.00, .18]
[.51, .66]
(CO2) empathy vs. social disinterest
0.41
0.25
0.30
0.10
0.60
[.31, .47]
[.16, .34]
[.06, .47]
[.00, .29]
[.53, .68]
(CO3) helpfulness vs. unhelpfulness
0.35
0.20
0.27
0.07
0.66
[.27, .43]
[.11, .29]
[.03, .42]
[.00, .27]
[.58, .74]
(CO4) compassion vs. revengefulness
0.51
0.36
0.36
0.17
0.47
[.44, .58]
[.27, .44]
[.16, .56]
[.00, .33]
[.41, .55]
(CO5) integrated conscience vs. self-serving advantage
0.34
0.16
0.33
0.00
0.67
[.25, .42]
[.06, .25]
[.11, .41]
[.00, .18]
[.59, .75]
MZ monozygotic, DZ dizygotic
Table 6
Intraclass correlations (ICC) according to zygosity and estimates of genetic and environmental effects for the three lower order sub-scales that compose the self-transcendence scale of the Temperament and Character Inventory [95 % confidence interval]
 
MZ
DZ
A
C
E
(n = 423 pairs)
(n = 408 pairs)
Additive genetics
Common environment
Unique environment
Self-transcendence
0.51
0.31
.40
.11
.49
[.43, .58]
[.22, .39]
[.19, .56]
[.00, .28]
[.43, .56]
(ST1) creative self-forgetfulness vs. self-conscious experience
0.42
0.27
0.32
0.11
0.57
[.34, .50]
[.17, .36]
[.09, .49]
[.00, .29]
[.50, .65]
(ST2) transpersonal Identification vs. personal identification
0.41
0.26
0.31
0.10
0.59
[.33, .48]
[.17, .35]
[.08, .49]
[.00, .29]
[.51, .67]
(ST3) spiritual acceptance vs. rational materialism
0.46
0.27
0.33
0.12
0.55
[.38, .53]
[.18, .36]
[.11, .51]
[.00, .30]
[.49, .63]
MZ monozygotic, DZ dizygotic

Discussion

In the introduction section we have detailed the differences between adolescents and adults in the genetic structure of temperament and character dimensions of personality. These differences suggest a “shift” in the type of environmental influence (i.e., shared to non-shared) from adolescence to adulthood with regard to character. Our study looks in greater depth at these variations, in particular the evidence for a shared environmental effect on the lower order sub-scales of the character traits during adolescence.
In their study among older adults, Gillespie and colleagues [12] expected shared environmental effects to account for a significant proportion in character variance because character traits were earlier hypothesized by Cloninger [25] to be partly due to socio-cultural learning. Nevertheless, these researchers found that additive genetic effects alone provided the most parsimonious explanation for the source of familial aggregation in each character higher order scale. Based on their univariate analysis, genetic effects explained 27–44 % of the variance in the three character higher order scales. Despite limitations of power in their study, the rejection of an ACE model in favor of AE was consistent with other studies in adult populations [26, 27].
In contrast to this evidence from adults, we provide evidence to support the role of shared environmental effects (C) on character variability in adolescence. Our findings support an ACE model and are therefore more consistent with earlier theoretical expectations [25] and recent empirical findings about the important influence of parental rearing and cultural norms on character development [28, 29]. The importance of both the underlying biological and social determinants during this critical phase of personality development are therefore likely to be critical in character maturation. It may be that this common environmental effect supported by our study in adolescents operates primarily in early development or that the methodology is concealing the effect in adults.
The genetic structure at the level of the character lower order sub-scales in adolescents shows that the proportion of the shared environmental component varies among sub-scales of self-directedness and cooperativeness, while it is relatively stable across the trait of self-transcendence (see Figs. 3a, b, 4). We also note that SD4 (self-acceptance vs. self-striving), CO1 (social acceptance vs. social intolerance), and CO5 (integrated conscience vs. self-serving advantage) have no evidence of shared environmental effects. This is also what is observed among adult populations. On the other hand, in adolescents a shared environmental effect is clearly present in all of the lower order sub-scales of self-transcendence and some of the other lower order sub-scales of cooperativeness and self-directedness (see Fig. 5). In particular the common environment influence is substantial for adolescents to develop purposefulness (i.e., SD2), self-actualization (i.e., SD5), and compassion (i.e., CO4). Therefore, it is important to consider how these traits might be related to the processes of socio-cultural learning. We know that character develops in directions that correspond to socially sanctioned norms [28, 29], but we know little about the details of the psychobiological mechanisms by which such socio-cultural learning occurs. However, we also know that individual differences in character traits, measured by the temperament and character inventory, are correlated with variability in the structure and function of particular networks in the human cerebral cortex [3032]. The processes of purposefulness and self-actualization requires regulating and cultivating particular lifestyle habits consistent with personally chosen goals and values, which requires personal discipline but also may be strongly reinforced or extinguished by cultural effects. Similarly, the development of compassion, forgiving others and not holding grudges, and the development of a purpose have strong cultural connections [33, 34].
A smaller effect size for common environmental influence and social learning is also seen in the sub-scales of self-transcendence (ST1–ST3). This might at first glance seem paradoxical since self-transcendence is often associated with the religious cultural environment [1]. However, while there is clearly an overlap with religious experience and religiosity, self-transcendence is measuring a phenomenon quite distinct from notions of religion, an observation supported by the neurophysiological data [35]. That being said, Magen’s [36] research suggests that adolescents address simple forms of self-transcendence—usually not referring to a macrocosmic unity. Perhaps because adolescents' pursuit of positive emotions tend to be egocentric and directed by their own desires, which in turn is contradictory to the willingness to become dedicated to the well-being of others or pro-social causes that transcend the self [37]. Nonetheless, Magen [36] points out that some adolescents can express transcendent feelings (e.g., mystical identification with a crowd on a strike in the streets) and that even adolescents’ homelier joys uncover “those universals that lead from and go beyond personal experience” (pp. 167). Finally, a slightly smaller size effect size is seen in CO2 (empathy vs. social disinterest), SD3 (resourcefulness vs. inertia), and CO3 (helpfulness vs. unhelpfulness), while SD1 (responsibility vs. blaming) has a very small effect size.
The presence of a shared environmental effect in all character traits in this adolescent sample is suggestive of the greater significance of socio-cultural learning at this critical developmental stage in the human life cycle. Something unique may be taking place not just biologically and psychologically, but also at a socio-cultural level during this phase. The power of cultural reinforcement and the impact of shared narratives may be at its greatest during the adolescent phase of development, compared to adults [3841]. It may be that in children there is a greater shared environmental effect that is tailing off in adolescence or that the peak period of a shared environmental effect is occurring in adolescence and that the effect sizes might therefore vary for each lower order sub-scale. For instance, Erikson’s stage of identity vs. role confusion [42], which occurs during adolescence, underscores the interaction between the internal drives of identity and socio-cultural awareness of place and identity in the community or environment. To some extent every adolescent must reconcile the identity which she ascertains from the family culture and wider social culture that she happens to be born into with her identity; which is a result from her growing awareness of her individual differences, whether they be relatively common (e.g., being sporty, being tall, being intellectual) or more profound (e.g., being physically different, sexual orientation or, indeed, being a twin).
We are, indeed, beginning to understand how the current models of genetic effects and genetic architecture might be inadequate because they have neglected the cultural inheritance [43, 44] and the complex adaptive processes that are crucial in personality development (e.g., [45]). Personality maturity is itself a complex dynamic system [3, 46]. It is therefore likely that the power of the shared environmental effects across the lifespan is underestimated when complex dynamical patterns of development are neglected.
The evidence we provide for the presence of this unique shared environmental effect in adolescence implies that socio-cultural effects may have implications for understanding the relative importance of interventions and treatment strategies aimed at promoting overall maturation of character. The development of a mature character has been found to correlate with health, happiness, and well-being in the adult human. (e.g., [25]). One of the ways this maturity influences well-being is by the increased ability to temper the emotions. The adolescent is exercising her character’s influence over her temperament in new and important ways, developing her relationship with herself, her fellow humans, and the complex and awe-inspiring universe in which she finds herself. In addition to her self-narrative, the narratives that she is exposed to through the cultural milieu in which she moves will significantly influence this maturation [36, 37, 3941, 47].
Narratives of unity and connectedness foster the sense of her place in the universe [7]. A culture that fosters tolerance, empathy, and compassion fosters love and cooperativeness. Narratives of responsibility and purpose in life might strengthen her self-exploration in hope and increase her self-directedness. We might consider that in the development of brain connectedness and personality structure, we know the infant draws greatly on the material plasticity of her brain; the adolescent draws in addition the flexibility of her response to and learning from the socio-cultural environment. In later life, as adults mature in character, they may become more self-aware thus increasing the importance of their individual experiences and their expressions of individual virtue in action [3].

Limitations

It is possible that our findings regarding the genetic structure of Cloninger’s model of personality differ from those of earlier research because of differences in measurement. Most research has been done using the longer version of the Temperament and Character Inventory, but similar results to those obtained with the long version have been found using shorter versions (e.g., Gillespie and colleagues used a 35-item version for measuring the character dimensions). Nonetheless, the short version character scores that were extracted from the clinical sample are highly correlated to their respective long version character scores (see [14]), thus, suggesting that it will produce comparable results when the genetic structure of the model is investigated. In addition, of the 13 character sub-scales only one (SD5) has a 95 % CI for common environmental effects (C) that does not include zero. The estimates of common environmental effects (C) are quite small: 3 are zero, 5 more are .10 or less, and 3 of the remaining 5 are under .15. Such a relatively small shared environmental contributions would seem to provide very little guidance for intervention, except, perhaps, to suggest that the environmental variables currently differing among families in Sweden don’t have much effect on the character traits measured by the sub-scales, so something quite different should be tried if one aspires to change them much. Indeed, well-being interventions recently developed (e.g., well-being coaching; http://​www.​anthropedia.​org/​learn-more/​) require the development of self-awareness and personality of the whole human being (i.e., body, mind, and psyche or soul2).

Conclusion and final remarks

In thinking about the influence of socio-cultural learning we must consider character development at the lower order sub-scale level: each of the lower sub-scales demonstrates the possibility for an outlook of unity [e.g., being able to show integrity (SD5), to be forgiving (CO4), and creative (ST3)] and an outlook of separation (e.g., undisciplined, revengeful, and judgmental) [52]. The cultural learning environment of the adolescent can support this process of discriminating the two. In the development of self-actualization (SD5) and compassion (CO3) for example, we can see that an outlook of unity might be reinforced by socio-cultural learning experiences. An outlook of unity reinforces the awareness of how our actions have consequences not only for ourself, but also for others and the universe as a whole. With such insight, people become motivated to exercise discipline in changing their daily habits in order to live in accord with their most deeply held values and understanding of their place in the world [3, 7].
On the other hand a narrative of separation will reinforce the almost magical notion that we exist in separation to any consequences, or that consequences themselves do not exist. The balance between our outlooks of separation and unity, therefore, has important and far-reaching implications for happiness, well-being, and mental health. Self-defeating behaviors, often witnessed in adolescence, might continue into adulthood despite evidence of the negative consequences, because the connectedness is not directly understood. New approaches to counteract bullying in schools implicitly acknowledge the importance of this learning. In an outlook of separation, the consequences of the behaviors are rarely appreciated and hardly seem relevant. Approaches based on restorative justice [47], for instance, aim to create a socio-cultural experience for the adolescent allowing them to connect consequences and people affected by her behaviors, thus providing opportunities of learning that integrate values with behavior (see [53]).
“Harry, I owe you an explanation,ʹ said Dumbledore. `An explanation of an old manʹs mistakes. For I see now that what I have done, and not done, with regard to you, bears all the hallmarks of the failings of age. Youth cannot know how age thinks and feels. But old men are guilty if they forget what it was to be young … and I seem to have forgotten, lately …ʹ”
Harry Potter and the Order of the Phoenix by J. K. Rowling.

Authors’ contributions

NL and DG wrote the paper, prepared figures and/or tables, reviewed drafts of the paper. SL SB and DG analyzed the data, prepared figures and/or tables, reviewed drafts of the paper. CRC, TN, NK, and HA reviewed drafts of the paper. DG and HA conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

The CATSS is supported by the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research, the Swedish Research Council, Systembolaget, the National Board of Forensic Medicine, Swedish prison and Probation Services, and Bank of Sweden Tercentenary Foundation. The development of this article was supported by AFA Insurance (Dnr. 130345), Stiftelsen Kempe-Carlgrenska Fonden, and the Swedish Research Council (Dnr. 2015-01229).  The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests. Danilo Garcia is the director of the Blekinge Center of Competence, which is the research and development unit of the Blekinge County Council and the municipalities in Blekinge, Sweden. Researchers, experts, and practitioners work at the Blekinge Center of Competence for higher quality of life among the habitants of Blekinge by innovating health care through person-centered methods.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Fußnoten
1
“The exact algorithm to select candidates for the questionnaire study was a DSM A-TAC score for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder ≥8, autism spectrum disorders ≥4.5, conduct ≥1, opposition ≥2, compulsions ≥1, Tics ≥1, eating problems ≥1 and an endorsement of dysfunction and/or suffering related to the symptoms (a problem score of ≥1), or had a parentally reported clinical diagnosis of one or more of these conditions, in total corresponding to 7 % of the children in 13 % of the twin pairs, and a random sample of control twin pairs (1 in 20 interviews). Since November 2008, with access to new validation information, the questionnaires have also been sent to pairs in which one or both twins scored ≥8 in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, ≥4.5 in autism spectrum disorders, ≥1.5 in eating problems, ≥3 in oppositional/conduct ≥2 in Tics, ≥1 in compulsions, ≥1 in motor control, or ≥3 in Learning using the DSM score regardless of whether they indicated dysfunction or suffering related to the problems or characteristics” [20].
 
2
The Greek word psyche found in psychology and psychiatry stands for "life, soul, or spirit,", which is distinct from soma, which refers to the "body" [3]; see also [4851].
 
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR. A Psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:975–89.CrossRefPubMed Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR. A Psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:975–89.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Anckarsäter H, Ståhlberg O, Larson T, Håkansson C, Jutblad SB, Niklasson L, Råstam M. The impact of ADHD and autism spectrum disorders on temperament, character, and personality development. [Comparative Study Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:1239–44. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.7.1239.CrossRefPubMed Anckarsäter H, Ståhlberg O, Larson T, Håkansson C, Jutblad SB, Niklasson L, Råstam M. The impact of ADHD and autism spectrum disorders on temperament, character, and personality development. [Comparative Study Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:1239–44. doi:10.​1176/​appi.​ajp.​163.​7.​1239.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR. Feeling good: The science of well-being. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. Cloninger CR. Feeling good: The science of well-being. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Svrakic DM, Whitehead C, Przybeck T, Cloninger R. Differential diagnosis of personality disorders by the seven-factor model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:991–9.CrossRefPubMed Svrakic DM, Whitehead C, Przybeck T, Cloninger R. Differential diagnosis of personality disorders by the seven-factor model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:991–9.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Söderström H, Råstam M, Gillberg C. Temperament and character in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism. 2002;6:287–97.CrossRefPubMed Söderström H, Råstam M, Gillberg C. Temperament and character in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism. 2002;6:287–97.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR. What makes people healthy, happy, and fulfilled in the face of current world challenges? Mens Sana Monograph. 2013;11:16–24.CrossRef Cloninger CR. What makes people healthy, happy, and fulfilled in the face of current world challenges? Mens Sana Monograph. 2013;11:16–24.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Zohar AH. Personality and the perception of health and happiness. J Affect Disord. 2011;128:24–32.CrossRefPubMed Cloninger CR, Zohar AH. Personality and the perception of health and happiness. J Affect Disord. 2011;128:24–32.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Garcia D, Kerekes N, Andersson-Arntén A-C, Archer T. Temperament, character, and adolescents’ depressive symptoms: focusing on affect. Depression Res Treat. 2012;. doi:10.1155/2012/925372. Garcia D, Kerekes N, Andersson-Arntén A-C, Archer T. Temperament, character, and adolescents’ depressive symptoms: focusing on affect. Depression Res Treat. 2012;. doi:10.​1155/​2012/​925372.
10.
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Josefsson K, Cloninger CR, Hintsanen M, Jokela M, Pulkki-Råback L, Keltikangas-Järvinen L. Associations of personality profiles with various aspects of well-being: a population-based study. J Affect Disord. 2011;133:265–73.CrossRefPubMed Josefsson K, Cloninger CR, Hintsanen M, Jokela M, Pulkki-Råback L, Keltikangas-Järvinen L. Associations of personality profiles with various aspects of well-being: a population-based study. J Affect Disord. 2011;133:265–73.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Gillespie NA, Cloninger CR, Heath AC, Martin NG. The genetic and environmental relationship between Cloninger’s dimensions of temperament and character. Personality Individ Differ. 2003;35:1931–46.CrossRef Gillespie NA, Cloninger CR, Heath AC, Martin NG. The genetic and environmental relationship between Cloninger’s dimensions of temperament and character. Personality Individ Differ. 2003;35:1931–46.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Garcia D, Lundström S, Brändström S, Råstam M, Cloninger CR, Kerekes N, Nilsson T, Anckarsäter H. Temperament and character in the child and adolescent twin study in sweden (catss): comparison to the general population, and genetic structure analysis. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(8):e70475. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0070475.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Garcia D, Lundström S, Brändström S, Råstam M, Cloninger CR, Kerekes N, Nilsson T, Anckarsäter H. Temperament and character in the child and adolescent twin study in sweden (catss): comparison to the general population, and genetic structure analysis. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(8):e70475. doi:10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0070475.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson CB, Joyce PR, et al. The effect of cognitive-behavioral therapy for bulimia nervosa on temperament and character as measured by the temperament and character inventory. Compr Psychiatry. 2002;43:182–8.CrossRefPubMed Anderson CB, Joyce PR, et al. The effect of cognitive-behavioral therapy for bulimia nervosa on temperament and character as measured by the temperament and character inventory. Compr Psychiatry. 2002;43:182–8.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Corchs F, Corregiari F, et al. Personality traits and treatment outcome in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria. 2008;30:246–50.CrossRefPubMed Corchs F, Corregiari F, et al. Personality traits and treatment outcome in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria. 2008;30:246–50.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Mortberg E, Andersson G. Predictors of response to individual and group cognitive behaviour therapy of social phobia. Psychol Psychother. 2013;87:32–43.CrossRefPubMed Mortberg E, Andersson G. Predictors of response to individual and group cognitive behaviour therapy of social phobia. Psychol Psychother. 2013;87:32–43.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Rowe S, Jordan J, et al. Dimensional measures of personality as a predictor of outcome at 5-year follow-up in women with bulimia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2010;185:414–20.CrossRefPubMed Rowe S, Jordan J, et al. Dimensional measures of personality as a predictor of outcome at 5-year follow-up in women with bulimia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2010;185:414–20.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Soto CJ, John OP. Ten facet scales for the big five inventory: convergence with NEO PI-R facets, self-peer agreement, and discriminant validity. J Res Pers. 2009;43:84–90.CrossRef Soto CJ, John OP. Ten facet scales for the big five inventory: convergence with NEO PI-R facets, self-peer agreement, and discriminant validity. J Res Pers. 2009;43:84–90.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Anckarsäter H, Lundström S, Kollberg L, Kerekes N, Palm C, Carlström E, Långström N, Magnusson PKE, Bölte S, Gillberg C, Gumpert C, Råstam M, Lichtenstein P. The Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS). Twin Res Hum Genet. 2011;14:495–508. doi:10.1375/twin.14.6.495.CrossRefPubMed Anckarsäter H, Lundström S, Kollberg L, Kerekes N, Palm C, Carlström E, Långström N, Magnusson PKE, Bölte S, Gillberg C, Gumpert C, Råstam M, Lichtenstein P. The Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS). Twin Res Hum Genet. 2011;14:495–508. doi:10.​1375/​twin.​14.​6.​495.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Hansson SL, Svanström-Röjvall A, Råstam M, Gillberg C, Gillberg C, Anckarsater H. Psychiatric telephone interview with parents for screening of childhood autism-tics, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and other comorbidities (A-TAC) 1. Preliminary reliability and validity. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;187:262–7.CrossRefPubMed Hansson SL, Svanström-Röjvall A, Råstam M, Gillberg C, Gillberg C, Anckarsater H. Psychiatric telephone interview with parents for screening of childhood autism-tics, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and other comorbidities (A-TAC) 1. Preliminary reliability and validity. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;187:262–7.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Brändström S, Schlette P, Przybeck TR, Lundberg M, Forsgren T, Sigvardsson S, et al. Swedish normative data on personality using the temperament and character inventory. Compr Psychiatry. 1998;39:122–8.CrossRefPubMed Brändström S, Schlette P, Przybeck TR, Lundberg M, Forsgren T, Sigvardsson S, et al. Swedish normative data on personality using the temperament and character inventory. Compr Psychiatry. 1998;39:122–8.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Hannelius U, Gherman L, Mäkelä V, Lindstedt A, Zucchelli M, Lagerberg C, Tybring G, Lindgren CM. Large-scale zygosity testing using single nucleotide polymorphisms. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2007;10:604–25.CrossRefPubMed Hannelius U, Gherman L, Mäkelä V, Lindstedt A, Zucchelli M, Lagerberg C, Tybring G, Lindgren CM. Large-scale zygosity testing using single nucleotide polymorphisms. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2007;10:604–25.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Neale MC. Mx: Statistical Modeling. Box 710 MCV, Richmond, VA 23298: Department of Psychiatry. 2nd ed. 1994. Neale MC. Mx: Statistical Modeling. Box 710 MCV, Richmond, VA 23298: Department of Psychiatry. 2nd ed. 1994.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM, Wetzel RD. The temperament and character inventory (Temperament and character inventory): A guide to its development and use. St. Louis: Washington University Center for Psychobiology of Personality; 1994. Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM, Wetzel RD. The temperament and character inventory (Temperament and character inventory): A guide to its development and use. St. Louis: Washington University Center for Psychobiology of Personality; 1994.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Heath AC, Madden PA, Cloninger CR, Martin NG. Genetic and environmental structure of per- sonality. In: Cloninger CR, editor. Personality and psychopathology. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1994. Heath AC, Madden PA, Cloninger CR, Martin NG. Genetic and environmental structure of per- sonality. In: Cloninger CR, editor. Personality and psychopathology. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1994.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Stallings MC, Hewitt JK, Cloninger CR, Heath AC, Eaves LJ. Genetic and environmental structure of the tridimensional personality questionnaire: three or four temperament dimensions? J Personal Soc Psychol. 1996;70:127–40.CrossRef Stallings MC, Hewitt JK, Cloninger CR, Heath AC, Eaves LJ. Genetic and environmental structure of the tridimensional personality questionnaire: three or four temperament dimensions? J Personal Soc Psychol. 1996;70:127–40.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Josefsson K, Jokela M, Cloninger CR, Hintsanen M, Salo J, Hintsa T, Pilkki-Råback L, Keltikangas-Järvinen L. Maturity and change in personality: developmental trends of temperament and character in adulthood. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25:713–27.CrossRefPubMed Josefsson K, Jokela M, Cloninger CR, Hintsanen M, Salo J, Hintsa T, Pilkki-Råback L, Keltikangas-Järvinen L. Maturity and change in personality: developmental trends of temperament and character in adulthood. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25:713–27.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Josefsson K, Jokela M, Hintsanen M, Cloninger CR, Pilkki-Råback L, Merjonen P, Hutri-Kähönen N, Keltikangas-Järvinen L. Parental care-giving and home envinronment predicting offspring’s temperament and character traits after 18 years. Psychiatry Res. 2013;209:643–51.CrossRefPubMed Josefsson K, Jokela M, Hintsanen M, Cloninger CR, Pilkki-Råback L, Merjonen P, Hutri-Kähönen N, Keltikangas-Järvinen L. Parental care-giving and home envinronment predicting offspring’s temperament and character traits after 18 years. Psychiatry Res. 2013;209:643–51.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Gardini S, Cloninger CR, et al. Individual differences in personality traits reflect structural variance in specific brain regions. Brain Res Bull. 2009;79:265–70.CrossRefPubMed Gardini S, Cloninger CR, et al. Individual differences in personality traits reflect structural variance in specific brain regions. Brain Res Bull. 2009;79:265–70.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaasinen V, Maguire RP, et al. Mapping brain structure and personality in late adulthood. Neuroimage. 2005;24:315–22.CrossRefPubMed Kaasinen V, Maguire RP, et al. Mapping brain structure and personality in late adulthood. Neuroimage. 2005;24:315–22.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Schuerbeek P, Baeken C, et al. Individual differences in local gray and white matter volumes reflect differences in temperament and character: a voxel-based morphometry study in healthy young females. Brain Res. 2011;1371:32–42.CrossRefPubMed Van Schuerbeek P, Baeken C, et al. Individual differences in local gray and white matter volumes reflect differences in temperament and character: a voxel-based morphometry study in healthy young females. Brain Res. 2011;1371:32–42.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Eisenberg N, Eggum ND. Empathy-related and prosocial responding. Conceptions and correlates during development. In: Sullivan BA, Snyder M, Sullivan JL (eds.), Cooperation. The political psychology of effective human interaction. Singapore: Blackwell Publishing; 2008. p. 53–74. Eisenberg N, Eggum ND. Empathy-related and prosocial responding. Conceptions and correlates during development. In: Sullivan BA, Snyder M, Sullivan JL (eds.), Cooperation. The political psychology of effective human interaction. Singapore: Blackwell Publishing; 2008. p. 53–74.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Sussman RW, Cloninger CR. Origins of altruism and cooperation. New York: Springer; 2011.CrossRef Sussman RW, Cloninger CR. Origins of altruism and cooperation. New York: Springer; 2011.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat d’Aquili ED, Newberg A. The Neuropsychological Basis of Religions, or Why God Won’t Go Away. Zygon. 1998;33:187–201.CrossRef d’Aquili ED, Newberg A. The Neuropsychological Basis of Religions, or Why God Won’t Go Away. Zygon. 1998;33:187–201.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Magen Z. Exploring adolescent happiness. Commitment, purpose and fulfillment. London: Sage Publications, Inc. 1998. Magen Z. Exploring adolescent happiness. Commitment, purpose and fulfillment. London: Sage Publications, Inc. 1998.
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Magen Z. Commitment beyond self and adolescence: the issue of happiness. Soc Indic Res. 1996;37:235–67.CrossRef Magen Z. Commitment beyond self and adolescence: the issue of happiness. Soc Indic Res. 1996;37:235–67.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Garcia D, Anckarsäter H, Kjell ONE, Archer T, Rosenberg P, Cloninger CR, Sikström S. Agentic, communal, and spiritual traits are related to the semantic representation of written narratives of positive and negative life events. Psychol Well-Being: Theory, Res Pract. 2015;5:1–20. doi:10.1186/s13612-015-0035-x.CrossRef Garcia D, Anckarsäter H, Kjell ONE, Archer T, Rosenberg P, Cloninger CR, Sikström S. Agentic, communal, and spiritual traits are related to the semantic representation of written narratives of positive and negative life events. Psychol Well-Being: Theory, Res Pract. 2015;5:1–20. doi:10.​1186/​s13612-015-0035-x.CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat McAdams DP. The psychology of life stories. Rev Gen Psychol. 2001;5:100–22.CrossRef McAdams DP. The psychology of life stories. Rev Gen Psychol. 2001;5:100–22.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat McLean KC, Pasupathi M, Pals JL. Selves creating stories creating selves: a process model of narrative self development in adolescence and adulthood. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2007;11:262–78.CrossRef McLean KC, Pasupathi M, Pals JL. Selves creating stories creating selves: a process model of narrative self development in adolescence and adulthood. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2007;11:262–78.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Weeks TL, Pasupathi M. Autonomy, identity, and narrative construction with parents and friends. In: McLean KC, Pasupathi M (eds.), Narrative development in adolescence, advancing responsible adolescent development. New York: Springer; 2010. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-89825-4_4. Weeks TL, Pasupathi M. Autonomy, identity, and narrative construction with parents and friends. In: McLean KC, Pasupathi M (eds.), Narrative development in adolescence, advancing responsible adolescent development. New York: Springer; 2010. doi: 10.​1007/​978-0-387-89825-4_​4.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Erikson EH. Identity: youth and crisis. New York: Norton; 1968. Erikson EH. Identity: youth and crisis. New York: Norton; 1968.
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Rice J, et al. Multifactorial inheritance with cultural transmission and assortative mating. II. a general model of combined polygenic and cultural inheritance. Am J Hum Genet. 1979;31:176–98.PubMedPubMedCentral Cloninger CR, Rice J, et al. Multifactorial inheritance with cultural transmission and assortative mating. II. a general model of combined polygenic and cultural inheritance. Am J Hum Genet. 1979;31:176–98.PubMedPubMedCentral
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Rice J, et al. Multifactorial inheritance with cultural transmission and assortative mating. III. Family structure and the analysis of separation experiments. Am J Hum Genet. 1979;31:366–88.PubMedPubMedCentral Cloninger CR, Rice J, et al. Multifactorial inheritance with cultural transmission and assortative mating. III. Family structure and the analysis of separation experiments. Am J Hum Genet. 1979;31:366–88.PubMedPubMedCentral
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Arnedo J, Svrakic DM, del Val C, Romero-Zaliz R, Hernandez-Cuervo H, Fanous AH, Pato MT, Pato CN, de Erausquin GA, Cloninger CR, Zwir I. Uncovering the hidden risk architecture of the schizophrenias: confirmation in three independent genome-wide association studies. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;172:139–53.CrossRefPubMed Arnedo J, Svrakic DM, del Val C, Romero-Zaliz R, Hernandez-Cuervo H, Fanous AH, Pato MT, Pato CN, de Erausquin GA, Cloninger CR, Zwir I. Uncovering the hidden risk architecture of the schizophrenias: confirmation in three independent genome-wide association studies. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;172:139–53.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Marty P. Personalizing crime. Dispute Resolut Mag. 2000;7:8–11. Marty P. Personalizing crime. Dispute Resolut Mag. 2000;7:8–11.
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Cloninger KM. Person-centered Therapeutics. Int J Pers Centered Med. 2011;1(1):43–52.CrossRef Cloninger CR, Cloninger KM. Person-centered Therapeutics. Int J Pers Centered Med. 2011;1(1):43–52.CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Cloninger KM. Development of instruments and evaluative procedures on contributors to illness and health. Int J Pers Centered Med. 2011;1(3):446–55. Cloninger CR, Cloninger KM. Development of instruments and evaluative procedures on contributors to illness and health. Int J Pers Centered Med. 2011;1(3):446–55.
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Salloum IM, Mezzich JE. The dynamic origins of positive health and wellbeing. Int J Pers Centered Med. 2012;2(2):179–87. Cloninger CR, Salloum IM, Mezzich JE. The dynamic origins of positive health and wellbeing. Int J Pers Centered Med. 2012;2(2):179–87.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Falhgren E, Nima AA, Archer T, Garcia D. Person-centered osteopathic practice: patients’ personality (body, mind, and soul) and health (ill-being and well-being). PeerJ. 2015;3:e1349. doi:10.7717/peerj.1349.CrossRef Falhgren E, Nima AA, Archer T, Garcia D. Person-centered osteopathic practice: patients’ personality (body, mind, and soul) and health (ill-being and well-being). PeerJ. 2015;3:e1349. doi:10.​7717/​peerj.​1349.CrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Svrakic NM, Svrakic DM. Role of personality self-organization in development of mental order and disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 1997;9:881–906.CrossRefPubMed Cloninger CR, Svrakic NM, Svrakic DM. Role of personality self-organization in development of mental order and disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 1997;9:881–906.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Hopkins B. Restorative justice in schools. Support Learn. 2002;17:144–9.CrossRef Hopkins B. Restorative justice in schools. Support Learn. 2002;17:144–9.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
The genetic and environmental structure of the character sub-scales of the temperament and character inventory in adolescence
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2016
Erschienen in
Annals of General Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2016
Elektronische ISSN: 1744-859X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12991-016-0094-2

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2016

Annals of General Psychiatry 1/2016 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.