Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Journal of Pediatrics 8/2018

Open Access 28.05.2018 | Original Article

The impact of social inequalities on children’s knowledge and representation of health and cancer

verfasst von: Véronique Régnier Denois, Aurelie Bourmaud, Mabrouk Nekaa, Céline Bezzaz, Véronique Bousser, Julie Kalecinski, Julia Dumesnil, Fabien Tinquaut, Dominique Berger, Franck Chauvin

Erschienen in: European Journal of Pediatrics | Ausgabe 8/2018

Abstract

Reducing inequalities in the field of cancer involves studying the knowledge and mental representations of cancer among children. A qualitative study was conducted on 191 children aged 9 to 12 using the “write and draw” technique to get spontaneous mental representations of “healthy things”, “unhealthy things” and “cancer”. We grouped the voluntary schools according to two deprivation levels. In response to the request to “write or draw anything you think keeps you healthy”, the main responses categories were physical activity, healthy food and basic needs. Smoking, drinking alcohol, sedentary lifestyles/lack of sport were identified as “unhealthy”. The first theme associated with “cancer” is the “cancer site” implying children have a segmented perception of cancer. Deprived children have radically different views about the key items representing cancer: they are more likely to believe the illness is systematically deadly. They are less likely to believe it is a treatable illness. They are less likely to associate cancer with risky behaviors, particularly alcohol consumption.
Conclusion: Social inequalities affect representations of cancer and health literacy from early childhood. Prevention programs taking into account these representations need to be introduced at school.
What is Known:
• Social inequalities for cancer mortality are observed in all European countries and are particularly pronounced in France.
• Reducing these inequalities in prevention programs implies studying the knowledge and mental representations of cancer among children.
What is New:
• This study identified representations of cancer in young children according to social level.
• At age 9, children living in deprived areas are less able to produce content in discussions about cancer and have narrower mental representations and a more fatalistic view.
Hinweise
Communicated by Mario Bianchetti

Introduction

France has a longer life expectancy than other European countries, but with significant inequalities: between 2000 and 2008, 35-year-old men in senior management roles had a life expectancy of 49 years, which is 6 years more than blue-collar workers [30]. Indeed, the mortality rate inequality index has risen from 1.5 to 2.5 in the last 20 years, comparing the lowest and highest levels of qualification [15]. This is the result of social inequalities in living conditions starting in childhood and concern education, jobs, housing, and social relationships, among others.

Health literacy and social inequalities

Another factor affecting behaviors and beliefs about health is health literacy [26, 27, 31]. Health literacy is embedded within the social, cultural, and environmental context and norms [25]. Poor health literacy has been associated with health inequalities [12]. The only one study assessing the impact of health literacy on children concluded that health literacy enhancement had a major positive effect on health outcomes for this specific population [24]. Elements of literacy can be found in representations. Representations are defined as “a system of values, ideas and practices having for primary function to establish an order which will enable individuals to orient themselves in their material and social world and to master it, and for secondary function to enable communication to take place among the members of a community by providing them with a collective elaboration of a social object” [7]. Studying representations allows us to anticipate the social distribution of the elements of literacy, to understand why some elements are selected whereas others are rejected and how the different elements make sense in a coherent system.

Health representations

Few studies about children’s health representations have been performed. The few studies existing focused mainly on children with a disease. The rare studies conducted on the general population mainly focused on in addiction among children and overweight [23]. Among those studies, the health representations identified were considered to be rich, and children mainly viewed health as a personal responsibility attributed to will-power. Yet, those representations were rarely connected to child health-related behaviors.

Cancer representations

Studies of cancer representations have chiefly focused on adult populations. They concluded that cancer is considered a “distinct” disease for which the diagnosis is always synonymous with death in the collective psyche and “hides solid enduring beliefs that can produce dangerous behaviors for health” [18]. Children of that age already appear to have representations of cancer [4]. But most of the representations identified were focused on risk factors [2, 28].Other dimensions of children’s representations remain unexplored: illness stigma, attribution, disclosure.
Reducing inequalities in the field of cancer prevention implies studying the knowledge and the representations of cancer among children. [10, 29]. The representations are activated when the child comes into contact with illness or with information on cancer. These representations create the core understanding that will influence the child’s attitudes and current/future decisions in reacting to prevention stimuli, such as a smoking cessation campaign or screening incentive. This study therefore has the dual aim of identifying representations of health and cancer among 9 to 12-year-old children in a school setting, and determining whether the prevalence of some cancer representations is associated with the schools’ deprivation status.

Methods

A qualitative study was conducted on children aged 9 to 12 in elementary and junior high schools. In order to help the children understand the research questions and communicate elements of their representations [21] even when the theme relates to a subject they might find difficult to discuss, we adopted a relatively non-threatening means of eliciting ideas: the “write and draw” technique, as used by A. Oakley in 1995 [17] in his research into cancer representations among children. The write and draw method was also used in other studies concerning children’s representations of similar subjects, as this enables the rapid production of large volumes of spontaneous, qualitative data. In addition, it avoids imposing overly academic means of self-expression [16]. Data collection was followed by a group discussion, the main aim of which being to relieve any anxiety among the children. The school nurses were able to spot children that may need specific support and this was organized in conjunction with their class teacher.

Sample

The data was collected from a sample of elementary and junior high school children aged 9–12. All children from the targeted school classes (corresponding to age 9–12) of included schools were asked to participate. The schools were selected as follows: the study protocol was presented at an annual county meeting for school nurses (Loire department). The project was then proposed to all attending school nurses, independently of the geographic location of their school and therefore independently of its deprivation status. Volunteer school nurses had to obtain the school headmaster’s agreement. The schools included in the study received the intervention consecutively, the latter being blind to their deprivation status. The schools and, by then, the children included in the study were then ranked according to the social deprivation level of their geographic location. A deprivation index was calculated for each school, using the French adaptation of European Deprivation Index (EDI). The EDI is an adaptable transnational ecological deprivation index. It has been developed according to a common definition of deprivation—physical and social—while maintaining the specificity of each country. This index combines individual data from a European survey on poverty launched by the European Commission (EU-SILC) and data from the population census of each country. Those characteristics allow this index to be transposable from one country to another. This ecological deprivation index was developed for France by Pornet et al. [20] and includes 11 variables such as educational level, access to heating, access to a car, nationality, occupational category, number of persons in household, tenure and employment status. This index ranks individuals by residential areas based on five quintiles: ranging from most deprived (5) to least deprived (1). In this report, we grouped the schools according to two deprivation levels, with one “deprived” group, covering quintiles four and five and a “non-deprived” group, covering quintiles three down to 1.

Data collection

The researcher was given access to at least one class in each school for a 90-min interview. The researchers addressed half classes each time. Children were asked to answer three questions using the write or draw method: “write or draw anything you think makes you healthy;” “write or draw anything you think makes you unhealthy;” and “write or draw anything you know about cancer.” They were informed that there were no “right or wrong” answers, and that they could answer any way they liked, including handing in a blank sheet with no answers.
Additional individual demographic data (age, sex, school year) were collected at the end of the interview.

Analysis

A qualitative-quantitative mixed method was chosen to perform the analyses.
Qualitative blind assessments of the children’s creations were conducted by two independent readers. In the event of discrepancies between the two interpretations, a third reader was asked to settle the issue of classification.
The creations associated with good/bad health, were classified using the themes developed by Oakley et al. [16]. The sub theme “good health” was subdivided into the following themes: healthy food, exercise and sports, hygiene, not smoking, and sleeping. The sub theme “bad health” was subdivided into the following themes: diet, environment, violence, hygiene, alcohol, and medication. New themes emanated from this analysis were not found in the Oakley study. They have been added to the Oakley themes in the present analysis.
The third set of creations, concerning cancer, was analyzed using grounded theory approach [5]. This method relies on the construction of theory through the methodological gathering and analysis of data. Here, the focus was on the children’s creations: each researcher reviewed the creations collected and identified repeated concepts that became apparent. Those concepts were elements of meaning representing an idea. The idea could be a word, a group of words having a semantic meaning, an element of a drawing or an association of drawings and words. Those concepts were then tagged with codes, which were extracted from the data. As more data is collected, and re-reviewed, codes can be grouped into “items.” Common items were then grouped and defined under a new theme or sub-theme. Here, themes and sub-themes were not defined in advance.
Two socio-anthropologists (VR and JK) conducted a double analysis of each creation to produce items. Then, items were grouped into themes. Those analyses were conducted using a blind-assessment approach. The lists of themes were then compared and discussed by a group of experts consisting of: the two socio-anthropologists, an expert in education science (DB), a medical doctor and methodologist (VB), and a statistician (FT). This produced a single list of themes and sub-themes. The two analysts (VR and JK) then conducted a blind reclassification of all the items based on the new list. Their analyses were then compared. Any creation about which the two analysts disagreed on the attribution of items was examined by the multi-disciplinary group of experts so as to reach a consensus on the items and themes to be attributed.

Statistical analysis

A descriptive analysis of the population of children concerned was established, followed by a descriptive analysis of each creation (frequency and percentage, median and inter-quartiles).
Univariate analyses were performed to compare the frequency of themes arising, according to the deprivation level of the child’s school. Multivariate analyses were performed using a Poisson regression model to identify the link between the number of themes per child and their deprivation status, adjusted for confounding factors (school class and gender). Statistical significance was denoted by an alpha of 0.05. All statistical analyses were done using the R system.

Results

Description

Data was collected from a total of 191 children from 8 classrooms in the 6 participating schools (Table 1). All of the children attending were aged between 9 and 12 and completed the study. Three of the 191 handed in a blank sheet. There were more boys than girls (101 vs 90). A majority of children were in fifth grade (106–55.5%), with the rest in sixth (61–31.9%) and fourth (24–12.6%) grades. With regard to social deprivation based on the EDI score, 42.4% of the children were classified as living in deprived areas and 57.6% of them were classified as living in non-deprived areas (Table 2).
Table 1
Description of the sample
School
Total no. school pupils
Rural/urban
European Deprivation Index
Deprived/non-deprived
 
REP +*
Classes taking part in the survey
No. of pupils taking part in the survey
1
218
Urban
3.77
Deprived
 
Yes
Fourth grade
24
2
228
Urban
0.88
Deprived
 
No
Fifth grade
22
3
176
Rural
− 1.92
Non-deprived
 
No
Fifth grade
24
4
233
Rural
− 1.33
Non-deprived
 
No
Fifth grade
60
5
844
Urban
− 0.304
Non-deprived
 
No
Sixth grade
26
6
226
Rural
1.66
deprived
 
Yes
Sixth grade
35
*The French Education Ministry ranks schools according to their “social index.” In schools ranked “REP,” 55% of pupils come from the least privileged socio-professional classes. In “REP+” ranked schools that percentage mounts to nearly 70% (the national average being around 40%)
Table 2
Initial socio-demographic characteristics
Child characteristics
N = 191
Frequency (%)
School class
Fourth grade
24 (12.6)
Fifth grade
106 (55.5)
Sixth grade
61 (31.9)
Gender
Girls
90 (47.1)
Boys
101 (52.9)
Mean age (SD)
 
10.4 (0.8)
Geographic area
Rural
84 (44)
Urban
107 (56)
Deprivation Level
Non-deprived
110 (57.6)
Deprived
81 (42.4)

Children’s creations (Table 3)

Good health

In response to the request to “write or draw anything you think keeps you healthy,” physical activity was found to be the most important factor for good health among almost all children (81.2%). The second biggest category was eating healthy food (71.2%): fruit and vegetables or greens accounting for 47.1% and also low sugar and dairy products, and low-salt and low-fat foods. Others were concerned with basic needs (51.3% of drawings): drinking (30.9%), sleeping (24.6%), controlling waste elimination (16.8%), accommodation (8.9%), eating (4.2%), breathing (3.7%) and clothing (2.6%). Hygiene was identified as being good for health in 23% of the drawings, not smoking in 15.7% and healthcare/vaccines in 13.6%. Other miscellaneous references to health factors were socio-psycho-affective: food, pleasure, and entertainment.
Table 3
Description of children’s drawings and writings. (Oakley’s categories for good and bad health are underlined)
Major theme
Sub-themes
Number of children quoting the item N (%) out of 191 (100%)
 Good health
  
  Excercises and sports
 
155 (81.2%)
  Diet-healthy food
 
136 (71.2%)
Fruit and vegetables
Water
Low sugar
Dairy products
Low salt
Low fat
Balanced Diet
90 (47.1%)
32 (16.8%)
22 (11.5%)
16 (8.4%)
8 (4.2%)
6 (3.1%)
49 (25.7%)
  Basic needs
 
98 (51.3%)
Drink
Sleep
Poop and pee
Have a home- nice home
Eat
Breathe
Keeping warm-Clothes
59 (30.9%)
47 (24.6%)
32 (16.8%)
17 (8.9%)
8 (4.2%)
7 (3.7%)
5 (2.6%)
  Hygiene
 
44 (23%)
  Tobacco (not smoking)
 
30 (15.7%)
  Healthcare/vaccines
 
26 (13.6%)
  Psycho-socio-affective
Good mood
21 (11%)
  Pleasure
 
16 (8.4%)
Fun foods
15(7.9%)
  Entertainment (TV, movies)
 
(5.8%)
  Not being sick
 
8 (4.2%)
  Other
 
11 (5.8%)
  Mean (SD) number of themes identified per child; median
 
3.9 (2); 4
 Bad health
  
  Diet
 
94 (49.2%)
Too much sugar
Fatty food
Too salty
Fruit and vegetables
Palm oil
Overweight–“being fat”
Eating snacks
75 (39.3%)
30 (15.7%)
26 (13.6%)
7 (3.7%)
7 (3.7%)
29 (15.2%)
4 (2.1%)
  Smoking
 
134 (70.2%)
  Alcohol
 
114 (59.7%)
  Sedentariness-lack of exercise
 
59 (30.9%)
  Basic needs
 
60 (31.5%)
Protection from cold–winter
Not eating
Not sleeping
Not drinking enough
33 (17.3%)
15 (7.9%)
15 (7.9%)
5 (2.6%)
  Disease
 
37 (19.4%)
  Hygiene
 
33 (17.3%)
Not washing yourself
29 (15.2%)
  Taking drugs
 
33 (17.3%)
  Environment/pollution
 
24 (12.6%)
  Medicines
 
22 (11.5%)
  Taking risks–sex without condoms
 
20 (10.5)
  Seeking healthcare
 
16 (8.4%)
  Emotional factors
 
9 (4.7%)
  Death
 
4 (2.1%)
  Mean (SD) number of items identified per child; median
 
3.95 (2) 4
  Cancer
  
  Descriptions
  
143 (74.9%)
By cancer sites
 
117 (61.3%)
  
Lung
Breast
Heart
Liver
Hair
Skin
Everywhere
Willy (penis)
Brain
Stomach
Throat
Blood
other
73 (38.2%)
66 (34.6%)
38 (19.9%)
24 (12.6%)
14 (7.3%)
13 (6.8%)
12 (6.3%)
9 (4.7%)
8 (4.2%)
8 (4.2%)
6 (3.1%)
5 (2.6%)
23 (12%)
 
By definition
A disease
A serious disease
55 (28.8%)
32 (16.8%)
 
By epidemiology
Age related disease
Geographical location
9 (4.7%)
1 (0.5%)
 
By symptoms
(loss hair, heart failure, breathing problems)
9 (4.7%)
 
By biological process
Tumor/cells
6 (3.1%)
 
By level of knowledge
Do not know
3 (1.6%)
 Risk factors
 
89 (46.6%)
 
By behavioral causes
 
86 (45%)
Tobacco
Alcohol
Food
Drugs
Others
73 (38.2%)
23 (12%)
12 (6.3%)
9 (4.7%)
5 (2.6%)
 
By other causes
 
11 (5.8%)
 
Sun
Fate
Others
4 (2.1%)
2 (1%)
5 (2.6%)
  Outcomes
 
73 (38.2%)
Always deadly
Sometimes deadly
28 (14.7%)
45 (23.6%)
  Consequences
 
70 (36.6%)
Hair lost
Sadness
After-effects
Pain
Others
51 (26.7%)
20 (10.5%)
3 (1.6%)
3 (1.6%)
9 (4.8)
  Cure/treatment
 
54 (28.3%)
Hospital
Curable disease
Medics
Surgery
Doctor
30 (15.7%)
21 (11%)
17 (8.9%)
9 (4.7%)
3 (1.6%)
  Prevention
 
39 (20.4%)
Of smoking
Of drinking alcohol
Of other risks
General guidelines
28 (14.7%)
11 (5.8%)
8 (4.2%)
8 (4.2%)
  Personal history
Experience of friends or relatives having cancer
4 (2.1%)
  Research
 
4 (2.1%)
  Mean (SD) number of themes given per child; median
 
4.31 (2.2); 4
The values in italic emphasis refers to the major Themes

Bad health

In response to the request to “write or draw anything you think makes you unhealthy,” food featured in 49.2% of the children’s drawings with most of references to too much sugar, fat, and salt. There were again references to being overweight and snacking. The drawings also portrayed smoking, drinking alcohol (59.7%), sedentary lifestyles/lack of sport (30.9%), basic needs (31.5%), hygiene and drugs (31.5%), and pollution 12.6% as factors. Taking medication and seeking healthcare were also associated with bad health.

Cancer

In response to the request to “write or draw anything you know about cancer,” the analyses of the drawings revealed five major themes. The first theme associated with “cancer” is the description by “cancer site,” with 61.3% of children referring to the parts of the body in which cancer can develop. The affected organs were either represented anatomically or colored in/blackened out, and sometimes the drawings were of healthcare situations (Fig. 1a). Eighty-eight of the 117 children referring to cancer sites mentioned two or more sites. The most-frequently mentioned were lung cancer (38.2% of children), breast cancer (34.6%), heart cancer (19.9%), and liver cancer (12.2%). None of the children mentioned colorectal cancer. The notion of risk factors was the second most frequently addressed theme, and awareness among the children was high (46.6%), particularly in tobacco (38.2% of children), which was considered the main risk factor, followed by alcohol (12%), food (6.3%), and drugs (4.7%). The third most frequently addressed theme was “outcomes” (38.2%) among those who considered cancer to be deadly sometimes (23.6%) or always (14.7%), see Fig. 1b. Next, the consequences of the disease (36.6%) are associated with its side effects, for example, hair loss (26.7% of children) and sadness (10.5%) see Fig. 1c. The themes of “cure/treatment” and “prevention” were each respectively mentioned by 28.3% and 20.4% of the children. The issue of cancer prevention was addressed in message form, such as: “if you don’t want cancer don’t smoke,” and primarily focused on tobacco (14.7% of children).
The mean number of themes referenced per child was estimated at 4.31 (SD = 2.2).

Comparison between deprived and non-deprived populations

The average number of items produced by the children according to their deprivation status is estimated at 3.6 (SD = 1.68) among children in deprived areas, and 4.83 (SD = 2.24) for non-deprived areas, with a significant statistical difference, p < 0.001 (Table 3). The number of items produced per child is also much higher among girls (4.78, SD = 2.13 vs 3.90, SD = 2.12; p = 0.004). The number of items increases the higher the school grade (4th grade—3.2, SD = 1.1 vs 5th grade—4.13, SD = 2.1 vs 6th grade—5.07, SD = 2.4; p = 0.0026).
After adjusting for gender, school class level, the link between the child’s deprivation status, and the mean number of themes addressed remains intact: non-deprived children express an estimated 0.37 items more than deprived children, which is a significant gap (p < 0.001) (see Table 4). We then conducted a comparative analysis of themes and sub-themes according to deprivation status (Table 5). There was a significant difference in the cancer sites identified according to a child’s deprivation status, with: lung cancer (47.3% non-deprived vs 25.9% deprived p = 0.004), heart cancer (28.2% non-deprived vs 8.6% deprived p = 0.0008), and liver cancer only being mentioned by non-deprived populations (12.5%; p < 0.0001). Non-deprived children more often list alcohol as a risk factor (17.3 vs 4.9%, p = 0.01). Deprived populations consider cancer to be “always deadly” more often than non-deprived children (27.2 vs 5.5% p < 0.0001). Non-deprived children were more likely to consider cancer to be curable than deprived children (18.2 vs 1.2%, p < 0.0001). Deprived children identified more themes with a high stigmatizing meaning such as the theme “always deadly” or themes referring to side effects.
Table 4
Association between deprivation status, other confounding factors and number of themes identified per child. Univariate (Wilcoxon test) and multivariate analysis (Poisson regression)
Variables tested
Univariate analysis
Multivariate analysis
 
Themes identified, mean (SD)
p value
Point estimate
p value
 Deprivation status
  Deprived
3.6 (1.68)
 
ref.
 
  Non-deprived
4.83 (2.24)
< 0.001
0.37
< 0.001
 Gender
  Girls
4.78 (2.13)
 
ref.
 
  Boys
3.9 (2.12)
0.004
− 0.15
0.04
 School class
  6
5.07 (2.4)
 
ref
 
< 0.001
  5
4.13 (2.1)
 
− 0.24
0.08
  4
3.2 (1.1)
0.003
− 0.32
< 0.001
 Geographical area
  Rural
4.53 (2.0)
   
  Urban
4.14 (2.3)
0.09
Table 5
Association between deprivation status and frequency of themes identified. Univariate analysis (Wilcoxon test), significant results only. PR translates the number of themes identified by deprived children, compared to the non-deprived. For example, for the lung cancer, deprived children identify half less items than non-deprived children
Themes identified
Whole population N (%)
Non-deprived population N (%)
Deprived population N (%)
Crude PR [IC95%]
p value
Cancer site: lung
73 (38.2)
52 (47.3)
21 (25.9)
0.55 [0.37–0.81]
0.004
Cancer site: heart
38 (19.8)
31 (28.2)
7 (8.6)
0.31 [0.15–0.61]
0.0008
Cancer site: liver
24 (12.5)
24 (12.5)
0 (0)
 
< 0.0001
A disease
55 (28.7)
43 (39.1)
12 (14.8)
0.38 [0.23–0.64]
0.0003
A serious disease
32 (16.7)
27 (24.5)
5 (6.2)
0.25 [0.11–0.56]
0.0007
Behavioral causes: alcohol
23 (12)
19 (17.3)
4 (4.9)
0.29 [0.11–0.74]
0.01
Outcomes: always deadly
28 (14.6)
6 (5.5)
22 (27.2)
4.98 [2.35–10.55]
< 0.0001
Outcomes: sometimes deadly
45 (23.5)
41 (37.3)
4 (4.9)
0.13 [0.06–0.28]
< 0.0001
Visible side-effects
63 (32.9)
28 (25.5)
35 (43.2)
1.7 [1.14–2.54]
0.01
Visible side-effects: hair loss
51 (26.7)
19 (17.3)
32 (39.5)
2.29 [1.43–3.67]
0.0008
Invisible side-effects
69 (36.1)
31 (28.2)
38 (46.7)
1.66 [1.14–2.42]
0.0095
Cure/treatment: a curable disease
21 (10.9)
20 (18.2)
1 (1.2)
0.07 [0.02–0.28]
< 0.0001
PR prevalence ratio

Discussion

This is the first study to involve such a large cohort of 9–12 year olds and to identify significant differences in representations of cancer between deprived and non-deprived children. From the age of nine, deprived children have radically different views about the key representations of cancer: they are more likely to believe the illness is systematically deadly (27.2 vs. 5.5%, p < 0.0001). They are less likely to believe it is a treatable illness (1.2 vs 18.2%, p < 0.0001). They are less likely to associate cancer with risky behaviors, particularly alcohol consumption. All of which reflects a fatalistic outlook. Non-deprived children are able to generate more ideas and content on the subject of cancer than deprived children (number of items mentioned: 4.8 vs 3.6 p < 0.001). This difference also persists independently of other factors that could potentially influence a child’s capacity for self-expression. The wider the variety of items comprising the representation, the more easily the latter is connected to other items of knowledge [6]. Inequalities can exacerbated by the fact that deprived children may live in environments leaving them more exposed to risks and tend to be less able to integrate information about prevention.

Other lessons from this study

An examination of the main representations of cancer featured in the children’s drawings/writings reveals that the disease is primarily seen as an illness of the organs, implying that they have a segmented, anatomo-clinical perception of cancer. This representation may conflict with the global approach to the body and health that is widely being promoted in programs as the cornerstone of the government’s National Health and Nutrition plan. Indeed, this global view is visible in the children’s drawings of good and bad health, which show physical and eating behaviors consistent with the catchphrases associated with those programs. Strictly healthcare-related items, such as doctors, hospitals, and vaccines, were under-represented in the drawings. Another strand of data confirms this observation: although physical activity is identified by children as the leading factor for good health and is seen as influencing bad health in 30% of the children, the concept of sedentariness is completely absent from the cancer risk factors identified by the children. They therefore seem to struggle to connect the idea of general prevention in healthcare with that of an illness described as attacking a specific organ. Representing cancer more as an illness of blood cells than of an organ, or as an illness linked with general aging in the body would be more consistent with the representative base children already have with regard to health.
As regards knowledge of risk factors, our research therefore leads us to consider that most of the population is health literate when it comes to tobacco-related risks. Indeed, this knowledge was identified in younger children (4–8 year olds) in the Porcellato study [19]. However, alcohol consumption is spontaneously identified as a risk factor among 38% of non-deprived children, but in just 12% of deprived children, and the trend is similar when it comes to food (21.6 vs. 6.1%). Accordingly, non-deprived children present richer representations of risk factors that are more consistent with scientific data than those of deprived children. Specifically, the association between alcohol and liver cancer has only been reported by non-deprived children. This may be connected to representations of female alcoholism, which tends to affect women with higher levels of education. More importantly, however, representations of these women are predominated by the idea of personal consequences. [3].
Finally, with regard to the illness itself, which was a highly visible theme in the children’s creations, the differences were striking and clearly suggest that children in deprived areas equate cancer with death. This deterministic view is consistent with secondary prevention avoidance behaviors, as seen in publications [14]. It is therefore understandably difficult for such populations to participate in cancer screening programs when the announcement of an “anomaly” would be translated as one of imminent and inevitable death.
This study provides statistical validation of the data suggested in the research conducted by Oakley [17] and Knighting [9] using the same methodology and addressing the same age bracket. With respect to representations of cancer, we found the same key themes (cancer site, risks, outcomes, and consequences) ranked in the same order of appearance. The sample used in the Knighting study included social differences based on the Carstairs and Morris Index of Deprivation. While we reproduced the structure of this sample, applying the European deprivation index, unlike Knighting, we were able to demonstrate statistically that social inequalities, in terms of representations and health literacy, are present from childhood. These factors stand to play an important role in maintaining social inequality over the years to come, when those children become adults and will need to make decisions about proposed primary and secondary prevention measures. Several other conclusions reached in our study will need to be taken into consideration in drawing up and implementing education programs in schools.
Physical activity is now widely recognized as a prevention practice that should be promoted for every human being. The WHO developed the “Global Recommendations on Physical Activity for Health” [8] in order to provide, for all the age groups, guidance on the type and amount of physical activity needed for the prevention of non-communicable diseases. Although those guidelines are widely disseminated in mass media and recognized by all, they remain less frequently implemented according to a socio-economic gradient. This has been observed in a specific child population type: those living in the most deprived circumstances are less likely to undergo physical activity than those living in the least deprived [13].
Promotion of physical activity should be performed adequately, through intervention aiming at addressing targeted population issues and preferably in the workplace. That intervention is even more likely to tackle inequalities when implemented at a high social or policy decision-making level [11]. For children, such results highlight the need to develop educational efforts, endorsed by the national school system, performed in the school environment, aiming at promoting healthy behaviors such as physical activity and fighting negative health representations, such as the image of cancer as being systematically deadly. Indeed, studies of adults have found the representation of cancer as a deadly disease among the more deprived classes [22]. Our study demonstrates that the representation exists even before adolescence, which means that early intervention is to be favored.
Our study was limited by a number of factors. One of these stems from the method chosen, i.e. “write and draw,” which involved interpreting and ranking the drawings by theme. Had we not been limited by the time available for collecting data, we could have added the “Draw, write and tell” system, as described by Angell [1]. That said the bias created in ranking the themes was offset by having several readers validate the themes. Similarly, the deprived/non-deprived classification method used is based on environmental indicators. Individual indicators, such as the socio-professional category of the children’s parents, would have yielded more reliable results, but require additional authorizations and, a high level of quality control of the data collected. Another limit of this work is that the ethnicity and immigration status of the children could not have been collected, this being forbidden under French law. Those variables could have helped to more precisely identify differences in children’s representations.

Conclusion

Our research shows that most children have good knowledge and appropriate representations of health, cancer, and risk factors. Tobacco particularly, is adequately identified as the most important and avoidable risk factor. More importantly, however, differences highlighted between child deprivation levels alert us in showing that social inequalities affect those representations as early as childhood.
This study strongly advocates the development of early cancer prevention programs in schools and particularly targeted programs in schools in deprived areas, to avoid inequalities between deprived and non-deprived children becoming ingrained. It encourages decision-makers to develop and implement specific educational strategies that take better account of individual child perceptions in order to foster healthy behavior, especially in relation to cancer. Researchers are legitimate in providing their expertise in educational and behavioral sciences in order to help those intervention efforts fulfill this goal.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the teachers, all the children for their participation, and the school nurses of Loire Department for their ongoing support with our research and our traducer Tracey Seva.

Compliance with ethical standards

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in this study were in accordance with ethical standards. The study has been declared with the French National Commission on Information Technology and Liberties (CNIL No. 1688834v0). The relevant ethics committee (CHU–St Etienne) has delivered its favorable opinion on the investigation plan (IRBN 602015/CHUSTE).

Conflict of interest

Véronique Régnier Denois declares that she has no conflict of interest. Aurelie Bourmaud received non-financial support and research travel cover from Abbie Pharmaceuticals. Mabrouk Nekaa declares that he has no conflict of interest. Céline Bezzaz declares that she has no conflict of interest. Véronique Bousser declares that she has no conflict of interest. Julie Kalecinski declares that she has no conflict of interest. Julia Dumesnil declares that she has no conflict of interest. Fabien Tinquaut declares that he has no conflict of interest. Dominique Berger declares that he has no conflict of interest. Franck Chauvin declares that he has no conflict of interest. All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest or financial relationship with the organization that sponsored the research.
In accordance with ethical approval requirements, individual informed consent was not required for the children. An agreement has been signed with the National Education administration. Information about the study was given to the participating school inspector and school leaders, as well as to the teaching team, parents and children involved.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Pädiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Pädiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Pädiatrie, den Premium-Inhalten der pädiatrischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Pädiatrie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Angell C, Alexander J, Hunt JA (2011) How are babies fed? A pilot study exploring primary school children’s perceptions of infant feeding. Birth 38:346–353CrossRefPubMed Angell C, Alexander J, Hunt JA (2011) How are babies fed? A pilot study exploring primary school children’s perceptions of infant feeding. Birth 38:346–353CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Dixon HG, Scully ML, Wakefield MA, White VM, Crawford DA (2007) The effects of television advertisements for junk food versus nutritious food on children’s food attitudes and preferences. Soc Sci Med 65:1311–1323CrossRefPubMed Dixon HG, Scully ML, Wakefield MA, White VM, Crawford DA (2007) The effects of television advertisements for junk food versus nutritious food on children’s food attitudes and preferences. Soc Sci Med 65:1311–1323CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Erol A, Karpyak VM (2015) Sex and gender-related differences in alcohol use and its consequences: contemporary knowledge and future research considerations. Drug Alcohol Depend 156:1–13CrossRefPubMed Erol A, Karpyak VM (2015) Sex and gender-related differences in alcohol use and its consequences: contemporary knowledge and future research considerations. Drug Alcohol Depend 156:1–13CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Gray WN, Szulczewski LJ, Regan SM, Williams JA, Pai AL (2014) Cultural influences in pediatric cancer: from diagnosis to cure/end of life. J Pediatr Oncol Nurs 31:252–271CrossRefPubMed Gray WN, Szulczewski LJ, Regan SM, Williams JA, Pai AL (2014) Cultural influences in pediatric cancer: from diagnosis to cure/end of life. J Pediatr Oncol Nurs 31:252–271CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Jeoffrion C, Dupont P, Tripodi D, Roland-Lévy C (2016) Social representations of illness: comparison of “expert” knowledge and “naïve” knowledge. Encéphale 42:226–233CrossRefPubMed Jeoffrion C, Dupont P, Tripodi D, Roland-Lévy C (2016) Social representations of illness: comparison of “expert” knowledge and “naïve” knowledge. Encéphale 42:226–233CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Jodelet D (2008) Social representations: the beautiful invention. J Theory Soc Behav 38:411–430CrossRef Jodelet D (2008) Social representations: the beautiful invention. J Theory Soc Behav 38:411–430CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Kahlmeier S, Wijnhoven TM, Alpiger P, Schweizer C, Breda J, Martin BW (2015) National physical activity recommendations: systematic overview and analysis of the situation in European countries. BMC Public Health 15:133CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kahlmeier S, Wijnhoven TM, Alpiger P, Schweizer C, Breda J, Martin BW (2015) National physical activity recommendations: systematic overview and analysis of the situation in European countries. BMC Public Health 15:133CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Knighting K, Rowa-Dewar N, Malcolm C, Kearney N, Gibson F (2011) Children’s understanding of cancer and views on health-related behaviour: a “draw and write” study. Child Care Health Dev 37:289–299CrossRefPubMed Knighting K, Rowa-Dewar N, Malcolm C, Kearney N, Gibson F (2011) Children’s understanding of cancer and views on health-related behaviour: a “draw and write” study. Child Care Health Dev 37:289–299CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Lloyd JJ, Logan S, Greaves CJ, Wyatt KM (2011) Evidence, theory and context—using intervention mapping to develop a school-based intervention to prevent obesity in children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 8:73CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lloyd JJ, Logan S, Greaves CJ, Wyatt KM (2011) Evidence, theory and context—using intervention mapping to develop a school-based intervention to prevent obesity in children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 8:73CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Lorenc T, Petticrew M, Welch V, Tugwell P (2013) What types of interventions generate inequalities? Evidence from systematic reviews. J Epidemiol Community Health 67:190–193CrossRefPubMed Lorenc T, Petticrew M, Welch V, Tugwell P (2013) What types of interventions generate inequalities? Evidence from systematic reviews. J Epidemiol Community Health 67:190–193CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Mantwill S, Monestel-Umaña S, Schulz PJ (2015) The relationship between health literacy and health disparities: a systematic review. PLoS One 10:e0145455CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Mantwill S, Monestel-Umaña S, Schulz PJ (2015) The relationship between health literacy and health disparities: a systematic review. PLoS One 10:e0145455CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
13.
Zurück zum Zitat McCormack GR, Giles-Corti B, Timperio A, Wood G, Villanueva K (2011) A cross-sectional study of the individual, social, and built environmental correlates of pedometer-based physical activity among elementary school children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 8:30CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral McCormack GR, Giles-Corti B, Timperio A, Wood G, Villanueva K (2011) A cross-sectional study of the individual, social, and built environmental correlates of pedometer-based physical activity among elementary school children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 8:30CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Meissner HI, Breen N, Klabunde CN, Vernon SW (2006) Patterns of colorectal cancer screening uptake among men and women in the United States. Cancer Epidemiol Biomark Prev 15:389–394CrossRef Meissner HI, Breen N, Klabunde CN, Vernon SW (2006) Patterns of colorectal cancer screening uptake among men and women in the United States. Cancer Epidemiol Biomark Prev 15:389–394CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Menvielle G, Chastang JF, Luce D, Leclerc A; Groupe EDISC (2007) Changing social disparities and mortality in France (1968−1996): cause of death analysis by educational level. Epidemiology and Public Health/Rev Epidemiol Sante Publique 55:97–105 Menvielle G, Chastang JF, Luce D, Leclerc A; Groupe EDISC (2007) Changing social disparities and mortality in France (1968−1996): cause of death analysis by educational level. Epidemiology and Public Health/Rev Epidemiol Sante Publique 55:97–105
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Mulvihill C, Rivers K, Aggleton P (2000) A qualitative study investigating the views of primary-age children and parents on physical activity. Health Education J 59:166–179CrossRef Mulvihill C, Rivers K, Aggleton P (2000) A qualitative study investigating the views of primary-age children and parents on physical activity. Health Education J 59:166–179CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Oakley A, Bendelow G, Barnes J, Buchanan M, Husain OA (1995) Health and cancer prevention: knowledge and beliefs of children and young people. BMJ 310:1029–1033CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Oakley A, Bendelow G, Barnes J, Buchanan M, Husain OA (1995) Health and cancer prevention: knowledge and beliefs of children and young people. BMJ 310:1029–1033CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Peretti-Watel P, Constance J, Seror V, Beck F (2009) Cigarettes and social differentiation in France: is tobacco use increasingly concentrated among the poor? Addiction 104:1718–1728CrossRefPubMed Peretti-Watel P, Constance J, Seror V, Beck F (2009) Cigarettes and social differentiation in France: is tobacco use increasingly concentrated among the poor? Addiction 104:1718–1728CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Porcellato L, Dugdill L, Springett J, Sanderson FH (1999) Primary schoolchildrens’ perceptions of smoking: implications for health education. Health Educ Res 14:71–83CrossRefPubMed Porcellato L, Dugdill L, Springett J, Sanderson FH (1999) Primary schoolchildrens’ perceptions of smoking: implications for health education. Health Educ Res 14:71–83CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Pornet C, Delpierre C, Dejardin O, Grosclaude P, Launay L, Guittet L, Lang T, Launoy G (2012) Construction of an adaptable European transnational ecological deprivation index: the French version. J Epidemiol Community Health 66:982–989CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pornet C, Delpierre C, Dejardin O, Grosclaude P, Launay L, Guittet L, Lang T, Launoy G (2012) Construction of an adaptable European transnational ecological deprivation index: the French version. J Epidemiol Community Health 66:982–989CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Punch S (2002) Research with children: the same or different from research with adults? Childhood 9:321–341 Punch S (2002) Research with children: the same or different from research with adults? Childhood 9:321–341
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Quaife SL, Winstanley K, Robb KA, Simon AE, Ramirez AJ, Forbes LJ, Brain KE, Gavin A, Wardle J (2015) Socioeconomic inequalities in attitudes towards cancer: an international cancer benchmarking partnership study. Eur J Cancer Prev 24:253–260CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Quaife SL, Winstanley K, Robb KA, Simon AE, Ramirez AJ, Forbes LJ, Brain KE, Gavin A, Wardle J (2015) Socioeconomic inequalities in attitudes towards cancer: an international cancer benchmarking partnership study. Eur J Cancer Prev 24:253–260CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Rees R, Oliver K, Woodman J, Thomas J (2011) The views of young children in the UK about obesity, body size, shape and weight: a systematic review. BMC Public Health 11:188CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Rees R, Oliver K, Woodman J, Thomas J (2011) The views of young children in the UK about obesity, body size, shape and weight: a systematic review. BMC Public Health 11:188CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson LD Jr, Calmes DP, Bazargan M (2008) The impact of literacy enhancement on asthma-related outcomes among underserved children. J Natl Med Assoc 100:892–896 Robinson LD Jr, Calmes DP, Bazargan M (2008) The impact of literacy enhancement on asthma-related outcomes among underserved children. J Natl Med Assoc 100:892–896
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Sanders LM, Federico S, Klass P, Abrams MA, Dreyer B (2009) Literacy and child health: a systematic review. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 163:131–140CrossRefPubMed Sanders LM, Federico S, Klass P, Abrams MA, Dreyer B (2009) Literacy and child health: a systematic review. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 163:131–140CrossRefPubMed
26.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Sørensen K, Van den Broucke S, Fullam J, Doyle G, Pelikan J, Slonska Z, Brand H; (HLS-EU) (2012) Health literacy and public health: a systematic review and integration of definitions and models. BMC Public Health 12:80 Sørensen K, Van den Broucke S, Fullam J, Doyle G, Pelikan J, Slonska Z, Brand H; (HLS-EU) (2012) Health literacy and public health: a systematic review and integration of definitions and models. BMC Public Health 12:80
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Stjerna M-L, Lauritzen SO, Tillgren P (2004) “Social thinking” and cultural images: teenagers’ notions of tobacco use. Soc Sci Med 59:573–583CrossRefPubMed Stjerna M-L, Lauritzen SO, Tillgren P (2004) “Social thinking” and cultural images: teenagers’ notions of tobacco use. Soc Sci Med 59:573–583CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Sullivan HW, Rutten LJ, Hesse BW, Moser RP, Rothman AJ, McCaul KD (2010) Lay representations of cancer prevention and early detection: associations with prevention behaviors. Prev Chronic Dis 7:A14PubMed Sullivan HW, Rutten LJ, Hesse BW, Moser RP, Rothman AJ, McCaul KD (2010) Lay representations of cancer prevention and early detection: associations with prevention behaviors. Prev Chronic Dis 7:A14PubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Vernay M, Bonaldi C, Grémy I (2015) Chronic diseases: recent trends, challenges and prospects for change. Sante Publique 27:S189–S197CrossRefPubMed Vernay M, Bonaldi C, Grémy I (2015) Chronic diseases: recent trends, challenges and prospects for change. Sante Publique 27:S189–S197CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Wardle J, Steptoe A (2003) Socioeconomic differences in attitudes and beliefs about healthy lifestyles. J Epidemiol Community Health 57:440–443CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wardle J, Steptoe A (2003) Socioeconomic differences in attitudes and beliefs about healthy lifestyles. J Epidemiol Community Health 57:440–443CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
The impact of social inequalities on children’s knowledge and representation of health and cancer
verfasst von
Véronique Régnier Denois
Aurelie Bourmaud
Mabrouk Nekaa
Céline Bezzaz
Véronique Bousser
Julie Kalecinski
Julia Dumesnil
Fabien Tinquaut
Dominique Berger
Franck Chauvin
Publikationsdatum
28.05.2018
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
European Journal of Pediatrics / Ausgabe 8/2018
Print ISSN: 0340-6199
Elektronische ISSN: 1432-1076
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00431-018-3173-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 8/2018

European Journal of Pediatrics 8/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.