Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 1/2015

Open Access 01.12.2015 | Research

Adolescents’ reactions to participating in ethically sensitive research: a prospective self-report study

verfasst von: Penelope Hasking, Ruth C. Tatnell, Graham Martin

Erschienen in: Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health | Ausgabe 1/2015

Abstract

Background

Conducting psychological research with adolescents is imperative for better understanding, prevention and treatment of mental illness. However there is concern that research addressing topics such as mental illness, substance use and suicidality has potential to distress participants, particularly youth.

Method

We administered a questionnaire to 1973 adolescents (13–18 years) at two time points, one year apart. Participants responded to items regarding nonsuicidal self-injury, psychological distress, history of physical and/or sexual abuse, adverse life events, alcohol use, suicidal behaviour, self-efficacy, and coping skills as well as two open-ended questions regarding whether they enjoyed participating in the research and whether participation worried or upset them.

Results

Most youth (74 %) enjoyed participation and cited altruistic reasons and a greater self-awareness as reasons. Those reporting being upset by the questionnaire (15 %) reported poorer psychological functioning than their peers. Youth who were upset by their participation at baseline, but who reported enjoying the questionnaire at follow-up reported improved psychosocial functioning over time, while the reverse was true for those who initially enjoyed participation but later reported the questionnaire upset them.

Conclusions

Results suggest researchers acknowledge benefits for young people who participate in research, but also be mindful of the potential for distress among the most at risk youth.

Background

Research regarding mental illness, substance use, nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI) and suicidal behaviour is crucial to evidence-based prevention, early intervention and treatment efforts. Many signs of psychological distress or risk-taking behaviour (e.g. NSSI) first emerge in adolescence, and one in five young people will be diagnosed with a mental disorder before they reach adulthood [1]. As such, research which seeks to model risk and protective factors for later psychological distress among currently healthy adolescents is vital to better understanding the aetiology of mental disorders, and to development of effective prevention and early intervention strategies to interrupt negative psychological trajectories resulting from early distress.
However such research raises significant ethical challenges. Even among psychologically healthy participants, questions regarding NSSI, psychological distress, substance use, and suicidal behaviour may cause emotional distress. Developmental psychologists argue that adolescence is a particularly important time for the development of emotional maturity, emotion recognition and regulation, and adaptive coping skills [2]. Thus, while the majority of young people are resilient, adolescents may have a limited capacity, relative to adults, to reflect on sensitive topics without experiencing some degree of distress [3].
Despite concerns of research ethics committees and institutional review boards (IRBs), rather than experiencing distress, it is possible that young people value participating in research and that they derive benefit from doing so [4, 5]. We explored these issues as part of a larger study which aimed to identify risk and protective factors for the development of later psychological distress and NSSI among adolescents.

Guidelines for the ethical conduct of research

In Australia, where the current study was completed, conduct of research is underpinned by the values set out in the National Statement on Ethical Conduct in Human Research [6], guided by the Declaration of Helsinki [7]. The National Statement articulates that research with young people “should provide for the child or young person’s safety, emotional and psychological security, and wellbeing” (p56). Inherent within this is that researchers should minimise any potential for distress to arise. The National Statement also distinguishes between harm, discomfort and inconvenience. Specific examples of psychological harm include: “feelings of worthlessness, distress, guilt, anger or fear…disclosure of sensitive or embarrassing information” (p16). While ethical review boards serve an important, and vital, role in protecting both participants and researchers, concern regarding the impact of asking young people mental health questions, with potential to cause distress, can result in research being hindered in obtaining approval from the appropriate governing bodies (i.e., IRBs) [8, 9].

Effects of participating in research

A growing body of research has explored the issues of harm and psychological distress as a result of research participation, most notably in relation to trauma and suicidal behaviour. The consensus has been that participation is not distressing, or is distressing only for a minority [10]. Among a sample who had recently been physically or sexually abused, Johnson and Benight [11] noted only 6 % of their participants regretted participating and 45 % found involvement beneficial. However these studies have primarily focused on what might be considered ‘high-risk’ groups of participants, including victims of child abuse [12], participants reporting recent physical or sexual abuse [11], or prison inmates who had attempted suicide [13].
Less research has explored the impact of asking sensitive questions of healthy adolescent samples. Gould et al. [14] and Robinson et al. [5] both assessed responses to a screening program to detect students at risk of psychological problems and suicide, administered to whole classrooms of school-based adolescents. Screening was not associated with change in suicidal ideation [14], or reported distress [5], suggesting asking these questions does not increase suicidality, or cause notable changes in mood. In fact, Gould et al. [14] observed that students identified as ‘high risk’ (those with depressive symptoms, substance abuse and/or previous suicide attempt) reported less distress and suicidal ideation following the screening than ‘high risk’ students who did not receive screening.

The current study

Our key aim was to establish whether adolescents who participate in research, designed to assess NSSI and related psychological constructs, find it enjoyable or upsetting, and reasons for this reaction. We also sought to establish whether reactions to participation were associated with psychosocial functioning among adolescents over time. To this end we assessed whether participants enjoyed participating in our study and whether anything worried or upset them, and explored how these responses were related to psychosocial functioning (e.g. self-esteem, coping skills, optimism, adverse life events, alcohol use, mental health, and NSSI). We then tested whether changes in these factors, over a 1-year period, were related to changes in whether participants reported enjoying the questionnaire or being upset by the study. To do this we examined changes in the psychosocial variables over time and how these differed for students who enjoyed participation at both time points, those who were worried or upset at both time points, and those who changed their responses over time (e.g. enjoyed the study initially but found it upsetting at time 2). Finally we assessed whether being upset by the questionnaire was associated with later onset of NSSI. We expected that participants who reported being worried or upset would report poorer psychosocial functioning. Further we expected deterioration in psychosocial functioning among students who initially enjoyed the questionnaire but later found it upsetting, and improvement in psychosocial functioning over time for those who were initially upset but later enjoyed participation.

Method

Participants

Participants were recruited from 40 secondary schools in five Australian states/territories to participate in a larger study on how adolescents cope with emotional problems. Parents of all students enrolled in grades 7–10 at participating schools were sent information about the study and invited to consent to their child’s participation (one information sheet addressed to parents/guardians was sent home with each child; n = 14,841). Consent forms were returned by 27.8 % (n = 4119) of parents and 75.6 % granted consent for study participation (n = 3116; 21.0 % total sample), a rate consistent with previous studies requiring active parental consent in Australia [15]. Of the students with parental consent, 2637 completed questionnaires at baseline.
Of these participants, 1973 completed the questionnaire one year later (M = 11.7 months, sd = 1.05), a retention rate (75 %) similar to other longitudinal studies with adolescents [16]. Only participants providing data at both time points were included in analyses. Reasons for attrition included transfer to another school (n = 96), school withdrawal (n = 114), parent/student withdrawal (n = 26), deceased (n = 1), or not present at second questionnaire administration (n = 428). All participants were between 12–18 years at baseline (M = 13.89, sd = 0.97).

Materials

Two items assessed reactions to completing the questionnaire. These were: “Did you enjoy completing the questionnaire (yes/no); Why or why not?” and “Did anything in this questionnaire worry or upset you (yes/no)”. If they were worried or upset participants were asked “What was it that worried or upset you?” In addition, participants completed the following measures:
Part A of the Self-Harm Behavior Questionnaire (SHBQ) [17], was used to assess NSSI, and, if present, the method, recency, frequency and severity (from not at all serious to life-threatening) of the behaviour. Participants were also asked whether they had thoughts of taking their life, and if they had ever tried to take their own life. The SHBQ has acceptable internal consistency across young adults and adolescents in community samples (0.89–0.96) [17, 18] and differentiates between suicidal and non-suicidal young people [18]. Cronbach’s alpha in the current sample was 0.88.
The Adolescent Life Events Scale (ALES) [19] asks about individuals’ experience of each of 20 items (e.g. problems keeping up with school work, death of a family member), with the response options; never, yes more than a year ago, and yes within the past 12 months. The scale has good face validity, and in this sample a Cronbach’s alpha = 0.75.
The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) [20] consists of three subscales assessing frequency and quantity of consumption, dependence, and alcohol-related problems respectively. The current research utilised only the consumption scale due to the participants’ age. Cronbach’s alpha in this sample was 0.91.
The General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) [21] is a 12 item measure to assess psychological distress, including anxiety and depression. Baksheev et al. [22] have demonstrated validity with a sample of Australian high school students. Cronbach’s alpha = 0.91 in the current sample.
The Rosenberg Self Esteem Scale (RSES) [23] comprises 10 items with equal numbers positively and negatively worded. This scale has good face validity, convergent and discriminant validity as well as internal consistency and reliability [23]. The Cronbach’s alpha in this sample was 0.89.
The General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSE) [24] was developed as a subjective measure to assess people’s perceptions of their own efficacy, in a broad, context free scenario. The GSE has been administered in many countries and shows high reliability, construct validity and stability over time and across different cultural groups. In this sample Cronbach’s alpha = 0.85.
The Life Orientation Test-Revised (LOT-R) [25] is a measure of participants’ optimism. The 10-item scale has acceptable stability and internal consistency. The scale also possesses good convergent and discriminant validity [26], and a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.71 in this sample.
The Adolescent Coping Scale (ACS) [27] short form consists of 18 items assessing three primary factors: problem solving, reference to others, and non-productive coping. The scale shows acceptable test–retest reliability, and predictive validity [27]. The Cronbach’s alpha’s for each of the subscales in the current sample were; problem solving 0.68, reference to others 0.72, and non-productive coping 0.36.

Procedure

Ethical approval to conduct the project was obtained from Human Research Ethics Committees at Monash University and the University of Queensland, as well as all educational jurisdictions involved. Explanatory statements and consent forms were distributed to prospective participants by schools. Adolescents who obtained parental consent provided their own written consent to complete the 1-h questionnaire at school, during school hours. All participants were informed they could withdraw from the study at any time. All participants consented to participation and publication of the findings. Participants were supplied a unique code to ensure confidentiality, but enable identification if responses indicated immediate concern for life. Upon completion, all participants were given an information pack with mental health resources. This procedure was repeated at follow-up.

Data analysis

Chi square tests confirmed that whether participants enjoyed the survey or were upset by it did not vary by geographic location, remoteness, SES of school or religiosity of students (all p > 0.05). Consequently these demographic variables were not controlled in subsequent analyses.
Two mixed model MANOVAs were performed to determine if changes in psychosocial functioning were associated with enjoying the questionnaire or being upset by the questionnaire. In the first MANOVA, participants were divided into four groups: (1) those who enjoyed the study at both times, (2) those who did not at either, (3) those who enjoyed it the first time, but not the second, and (4) those who did not enjoy the first administration, but did the second. The second MANOVA assessed the change in whether participants were worried or upset by the questionnaire (four groups: upset at both times, not upset at either time, those who upset at baseline but not follow-up, and those who were not upset at the first administration, but were at the second). Both MANOVAs assessed group differences at each time point (between group factor), changes over time on the psychological variables (within group factors), and any differential changes over time according to group membership (i.e. interaction between within- and between-group factors).
Chi Square statistics were used to explore the relationships between responses to the two key questions and specific life events. Finally, we explored whether enjoying the questionnaire or being worried about it was related to onset, maintenance or cessation of NSSI over the study period. Onset was recorded when participants reported no history of NSSI at baseline, but reported NSSI within the last 12 months at follow-up. Participants reporting no NSSI in the last 12 months at follow-up (but recorded a prior history) were considered to have ceased the behaviour. Participants reporting a history of NSSI at both times points (within last 12 months at follow-up) were considered to have maintained NSSI. To minimize the impact of Type I error alpha was set to 0.01 for all analyses.
Qualitative responses were coded using thematic analysis [28]. Initial codes were collated into themes, and refined through discussion among the authors. 20 % of responses were independently coded by a researcher unaware of the study aims. Kappa measure of agreement was used to assess inter-rater reliability at both time points for each question, and ranged from 0.60 to 0.84.

Results

Did you enjoy completing the questionnaire?

Quantitative results

Of the sample (n = 1973), 1462 (74.10 %) enjoyed the questionnaire at baseline (1430 at follow-up; 72.48 %). Participants who did not enjoy the questionnaire at baseline were more likely to be upset at follow-up, and males were more likely to report that they did not enjoy the questionnaire than females (Table 1). Differences across the four groups were observed on optimism, F(3, 1632) = 8.10, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.02, self-efficacy, F(3, 1598) = 6.15, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.01, problem solving F(3, 1649) = 4.86, p < 0.002, η2 = 0.01, and reference to others F(3, 1675) = 5.50, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.01. In all cases those who enjoyed the questionnaire at both time points reported healthier functioning (e.g. greater self-efficacy) than those who did not enjoy the questionnaire at either time point. Participants who enjoyed the questionnaire at both time points were more optimistic than those who did not enjoy it at follow-up, F(1, 1632) = 10.80, p = 0.006, and had higher self-efficacy than those who did not enjoy it at baseline, F(1, 1598) = 10.71, p = 0.006 (Table 2). There was no significant interaction between enjoying the questionnaire and time on any of the dependent variables.
Table 1
Differences in demographic factors and suicidal behaviour
 
Baseline
Follow up
χ2
df
N
p
V
χ2
df
N
p
V
Enjoy
 Worry/upset
0.45
1
1829
0.50
0.02
11.31
1
1869
0.00
0.08
 Gender
7.92
1
1835
0.01
0.07
0.39
1
1881
0.54
0.01
 Psych. diagnosis
7.01
1
1816
0.01
0.06
0.74
1
1872
0.39
0.02
 Thought of NSSI
0.08
1
1827
0.78
0.01
0.05
1
1872
0.83
0.01
 NSSI
3.76
1
1795
0.05
0.05
2.74
1
1875
0.10
0.04
 Thought about suicide
5.78
1
1781
0.02
0.06
0.13
1
1860
0.72
0.01
 Attempted suicide
1.92
1
1765
0.17
0.03
0.41
1
1858
0.52
0.02
Worry/upset
 Enjoy
0.45
1
1829
0.50
0.02
11.31
1
1869
0.00
0.08
 Gender
42.33
1
1906
0.00
0.15
26.54
1
1929
0.00
0.14
 Psych. diagnosis
22.29
1
1885
0.00
0.11
36.92
1
1920
0.00
0.14
 Thoughts of NSSI
100.65
1
1895
0.00
0.23
101.44
1
1919
0.00
0.23
 NSSI
129.25
1
1860
0.00
0.26
119.49
1
1922
0.00
0.25
 Thought about suicide
72.37
1
1848
0.00
0.20
81.05
1
1907
0.00
0.21
 Attempted suicide
42.64
1
1832
0.00
0.15
23.11
1
1903
0.00
0.11
NB: Girls were more likely to enjoy participation and boys more likely to report being worried or upset. Participants reporting poorer psychosocial functioning (e.g. more distress, suicidal thoughts and behaviour) were more likely to find the questionnaire worrying or upsetting
Table 2
Multivariate analysis of variance demonstrates differential relationships between enjoyment and psychosocial functioning over time
Group
Variable
Baseline M (SD)
Follow up M (SD)
F
p
η2
Enjoy baseline and follow up n = 1189
Life events
27.68 (5.03)
28.63 (5.41)
50.66
0.00
0.048
Self-esteem
30.69 (4.93)
30.25 (5.12)
12.21
0.00
0.011
Self-efficacy
30.29 (3.89)
30.66 (4.02)
10.01
0.00
0.009
Non-productive coping
48.88 (11.77)
50.05 (11.48)
14.01
0.00
0.013
Reference to others
50.66 (13.34)
50.93 (13.24)
0.49
0.49
0.000
Problem solving
66.84 (12.20)
66.24 (11.59)
3.57
0.06
0.003
Optimism
20.77 (4.27)
20.69 (4.53)
0.51
0.48
0.000
Alcohol use
1.91 (2.00)
2.66 (2.72)
147.46
0.00
0.113
Psych. distress
22.59 (6.04)
23.05 (6.26)
5.86
0.02
0.005
NSSI
0.84 (3.06)
1.12 (3.45)
8.96
0.00
0.008
Enjoy baseline, not follow up n = 223
Life events
28.17 (5.60)
28.97 (5.64)
6.61
0.01
0.033
Self-esteem
30.56 (4.86)
29.68 (5.21)
8.92
0.00
0.041
Self-efficacy
30.02 (4.02)
29.90 (3.94)
0.25
0.62
0.001
Non-productive coping
48.78 (11.55)
50.97 (11.42)
6.71
0.01
0.034
Reference to others
50.68 (12.61)
50.14 (13.10)
0.34
0.56
0.002
Problem solving
66.27 (10.78)
63.57 (11.50)
12.31
0.00
0.058
Optimism
20.17 (4.29)
19.30 (4.70)
9.55
0.00
0.044
Alcohol use
1.83 (2.07)
2.82 (2.90)
41.33
0.00
0.161
Psych. distress
22.80 (6.18)
23.71 (6.38)
2.82
0.09
0.013
NSSI
0.74 (2.94)
1.32 (3.74)
7.10
0.00
0.033
Not enjoy baseline, enjoy follow up n = 165
Life events
28.53 (6.09)
28.83 (6.04)
0.478
0.49
0.004
Self-esteem
30.09 (5.40)
30.00 (5.28)
0.06
0.80
0.000
Self-efficacy
29.14 (4.13)
29.79 (4.14)
3.75
0.06
0.024
Non-productive coping
49.47 (12.26)
50.56 (13.28)
1.29
0.26
0.009
Reference to others
48.87 (14.00)
49.25 (14.27)
0.12
0.73
0.001
Problem solving
64.82 (12.25)
64.82 (12.92)
0.00
1.00
0.000
Optimism
19.83 (4.04)
19.93 (4.46)
0.11
0.74
0.001
Alcohol use
2.26 (2.53)
3.16 (3.06)
16.70
0.00
0.096
Psych. distress
23.67 (7.34)
23.16 (6.43)
0.91
0.30
0.006
NSSI
1.47 (3.96)
1.67 (4.11)
0.62
0.43
0.004
Not enjoy baseline or follow up n = 189
Life events
28.66 (5.56)
29.85 (5.95)
10.12
0.00
0.063
Self-esteem
30.03 (4.70)
29.26 (5.30)
4.80
0.03
0.029
Self-efficacy
29.37 (4.70)
29.83 (4.20)
2.01
0.14
0.013
Non-productive coping
49.55 (11.46)
50.85 (12.26)
2.11
0.15
0.013
Reference to others
46.94 (14.20)
47.44 (13.64)
0.26
0.61
0.001
Problem solving
64.47 (14.17)
62.95 (13.13)
2.56
0.11
0.014
Optimism
19.71 (4.29)
19.33 (4.22)
1.77
0.19
0.010
Alcohol use
2.14 (2.24)
3.22 (3.13)
37.92
0.00
0.175
Psych. distress
22.23 (5.62)
23.68 (6.29)
8.55
0.00
0.047
NSSI
0.98 (3.31)
1.58 (4.02)
5.46
0.02
0.029
When exploring specific negative life events, at baseline, those who reported being in trouble with the police, or physically abused within the last 12 months were less likely to report enjoying the questionnaire (Table 3). No specific life events were associated with enjoying the questionnaire at follow-up. Enjoying the questionnaire at baseline, χ2(3) = 6.45, p = 0.09, or at follow-up, χ2(3) = 4.44, p = 0.22, was not related to onset, maintenance or cessation of NSSI over time.
Table 3
Differences in specific items on the Life Events Scale at baseline and follow-up
Life event
Baseline
Follow-up
Enjoy n (%)
Worry/upset n (%)
Enjoy n (%)
Worry/upset n (%)
Yesa
No
Yes
No
Yes
No
Yes
No
n = 1462
n = 373
n = 304
n = 1602
n = 1430
n = 451
n = 235
n = 1694
Problems with schoolwork
χ2 = 1.01, p = 0.06
χ2 = 24.66, p = 0.00
χ2 = 1.94, p = 0.38
χ2 = 14.94, p = 0.00
624 (42.68)
167 (44.77)
170 (55.92)
647 (40.38)
719 (50.28)
242 (53.66)
147 (62.55)
840 (49.59)
Making/keeping friends
χ2 = 1.03, p = 0.06
χ2 = 31.78, p = 0.00
χ2 = 0.66, p = 0.72
χ2 = 23.75, p = 0.00
293 (20.04)
83 (22.25)
89 (29.28)
298 (18.60)
307 (21.47)
102 (22.62)
79 (33.62)
337 (19.89)
Arguments/fights with friends
χ2 = 3.40, p = 0.02
χ2 = 22.05, p = 0.00
χ2 = 3.29, p = 0.19
χ2 = 17.61, p = 0.00
370 (25.31)
108 (28.95)
106 (34.87)
383 (23.91)
372 (26.01)
137 (30.38)
90 (38.30)
429 (25.32)
Problems with girl/boyfriend
χ2 = 1.46, p = 0.48
χ2 = 32.73, p = 0.00
χ2 = 0.55, p = 0.76
χ2 = 23.57, p = 0.00
126 (8.62)
38 (10.19)
53 (17.43)
117 (7.30)
172 (12.03)
60 (13.30)
49 (20.85)
189 (11.16)
Bullied at school
χ2 = 1.45, p = 0.48
χ2 = 14.40, p = 0.00
χ2 = 0.03, p = 0.98
χ2 = 31.92, p = 0.00
216 (14.77)
59 (15.82)
65 (21.38)
223 (13.92)
181 (12.66)
58 (12.86)
57 (24.26)
193 (11.39)
Parents separated or divorced
χ2 = 1.15, p = 0.56
χ2 = 6.82, p = 0.03
χ2 = 0.35, p = 0.84
χ2 = 8.53, p = 0.01
42 (2.87)
11 (2.95)
15 (4.93)
41 (2.56)
41 (2.87)
12 (2.66)
12 (5.11)
42 (2.48)
Serious arguments with parents
χ2 = 6.71, p = 0.04
χ2 = 55.76, p = 0.00
χ2 = 11.43, p = 0.03
χ2 = 23.26, p = 0.00
375 (25.65)
119 (31.90)
133 (43.75)
379 (23.66)
443 (30.98)
149 (33.04)
107 (45.53)
504 (29.75)
Parents fights
χ2 = 2.92, p = 0.40
χ2 = 49.66, p = 0.00
χ2 = 4.96, p = 0.08
χ2 = 22.92, p = 0.00
251 (17.17)
75 (20.11)
92 (30.26)
246 (15.36)
277 (15.87)
94 (20.84)
72 (30.64)
314 (18.54)
You or family member: serious illness or accident
χ2 = 2.28, p = 0.32
χ2 = 13.14, p = 0.00
χ2 = 1.40, p = 0.50
χ2 = 30.45, p = 0.00
322 (22.02)
91 (24.40)
90 (29.61)
339 (21.16)
377 (26.36)
131 (29.05)
98 (41.70)
425 (25.09)
Close friends: serious illness or accident
χ2 = 2.28, p = 0.32
χ2 = 14.59, p = 0.00
χ2 = 6.44, p = 0.04
χ2 = 30.00, p = 0.00
170 (11.63)
53 (14.21)
55 (18.09)
172 (10.74)
196 (13.71)
77 (17.07)
61 (25.96)
217 (12.91)
Death in immediate family
χ2 = 1.80, p = 0.41
χ2 = 4.90, p = 0.08
χ2 = 1.92, p = 0.37
χ2 = 6.80, p = 0.03
35 (2.40)
13 (3.49)
13 (4.28)
37 (2.31)
28 (1.96)
12 (2.66)
10 (4.26)
30 (1.77)
Death of someone close
χ2 = 3.47, p = 0.18
χ2 = 14.93, p = 0.00
χ2 = 2.21, p = 0.33
χ2 = 19.61, p = 0.00
235 (16.07)
69 (18.50)
72 (23.68)
240 (14.98)
242 (16.92)
86 (19.07)
64 (27.23)
267 (15.76)
Family or friends suicide
χ2 = 4.54, p = 0.10
χ2 = 22.08, p = 0.00
χ2 = 1.16, p = 0.56
χ2 = 20.16, p = 0.00
25 (1.71)
12 (3.22)
12 (3.95)
24 (1.50)
54 (3.78)
19 (4.21)
19 (8.09)
53 (3.13)
Family attempt suicide or self-harm
χ2 = 4.16, p = 0.13
χ2 = 86.62, p = 0.00
χ2 = 1.89, p = 0.39
χ2 = 54.23, p = 0.00
44 (3.01)
6 (1.61)
27 (8.88)
25 (1.56)
54 (3.78)
18 (3.99)
24 (10.21)
49 (2.89)
Friend attempt suicide or self-harm
χ2 = 2.21, p = 0.33
χ2 = 99.44, p = 0.00
χ2 = 1.51, p = 0.47
χ2 = 80.69, p = 0.00
220 (15.05)
56 (15.01)
101 (33.22)
186 (11.61)
288 (20.14)
93 (20.62)
93 (39.57)
298 (17.59)
Physically abused
χ2 = 9.50, p = 0.00
χ2 = 14.51, p = 0.00
χ2 = 1.48, p = 0.48
χ2 = 17.61, p = 0.00
16 (1.09)
8 (2.14)
9 (2.96)
14 (0.87)
21 (1.47)
6 (1.39)
8 (3.40)
20 (1.18)
Trouble with police
χ2 = 12.89, p = 0.00
χ2 = 17.72, p = 0.00
χ2 = 2.01, p = 0.37
χ2 = 1.89, p = 0.39
15 (1.03)
12 (3.22)
9 (2.96)
20 (1.25)
26 (1.81)
12 (2.66)
6 (2.55)
33 (1.95)
Worries about sexual orientation
χ2 = 3.37, p = 0.19
χ2 = 19.13, p = 0.00
χ2 = 0.77, p = 0.68
χ2 = 53.67, p = 0.00
88 (6.02)
18 (4.83)
32 (10.53)
75 (4.68)
29 (2.03)
32 (7.09)
43 (18.30)
104 (6.14)
Forced to engage in sexual activity
χ2 = 1.39, p = 0.50
χ2 = 20.05, p = 0.00
χ2 = 1.23, p = 0.54
χ2 = 54.70, p = 0.00
19 (1.30)
6 (1.61)
11 (3.62)
14 (0.87)
29 (2.03)
10 (2.22)
14 (5.96)
25 (1.48)
Other distressing event
χ2 = 0.41, p = 0.81
χ2 = 87.57, p = 0.00
χ2 = 0.33, p = 0.85
χ2 = 108.57, p = 0.00
 
199 (13.61)
53 (14.21)
90 (29.61)
170 (10.61)
247 (17.27)
75 (16.63)
93 (39.57)
237 (13.99)
NB: At both time points, being worried or upset is associated with experiencing more negative life events
aPercentages represent the proportion of participants responding yes or no to this question, who reported experiencing the given life event in the previous 12 months

Thematic analysis

Five themes were evident among those who enjoyed the questionnaire; (1) understanding and reflection, (2) help is available/helping others, (3) that it was fun or enjoyable, (4) self-expression, (5) getting out of class. Two major themes emerged among those that did not enjoy the questionnaire; (1) boredom/inconvenience, or (2) negative experiences (Table 4).
Table 4
Exemplars reflecting key themes extracted in qualitative analysis
Theme
Exemplars
Enjoy the questionnaire
 Understanding and reflection
It allowed me to reflect on my positive thoughts and how I view myself as an individual
Knowing I haven’t tried any illegal substances or at least reading I haven’t made me feel good
On the last page my head started running over all the pleasant memories I was writing about
It gave me a better understanding of myself
It helped me to understand more about the mental health problems of young people and helped me understand more about me
 Help is available/helping others
I hope that what I have answered can contribute even just a little to the research
I know it will help research into emotional problems with teenagers providing solutions to overcome their issues
Made me feel that there is help out there, we just need to talk about it
I did enjoy this because it makes me know that someone is trying to help
 Fun and enjoyable
It was interesting and I enjoy filling out questionnaires
Because I like being asked questions about me
Yes, it was enjoyable. More fun than what I thought
It was a change and I enjoyed it
 Expressing themselves
Because I can release my feelings, emotions and thoughts and know they will stay anonymous
I can tell the truth and not get into trouble
I knew I could be completely honest and it felt good to express some things that I had not in any other way
 Getting out of class
I got out of geography class and got a show bag
It meant we don’t have to do work
Because I won’t have to do schoolwork and listen to the teacher…
Not enjoy the questionnaire
 Boredom/inconvenience
I didn’t have a problem with my life so it wasn’t really effective for me
Didn’t really relate to any of my problems
I sometimes found it hard to understand what the question was asking for example, I have never thought about committing suicide so I found it strange answering questions about that topic
Cause it was boring…
It took ages. I was meant to be in the library doing assignments and studying
Missed studying for science test coming up
 Negative experience
Shows the bad side of me
It made me think about my problems too much
I did not realise other people had some of the worries mentioned
It made me sad for others
Worried or upset
 Personal experience/feelings
It was just upset remembering things that have happened
I realised that maybe I do need help
I feel like there might be something wrong with me
Recently someone in my community committed suicide so the questions relating to that were upsetting
The self-harm/suicide part and the bullying part and the sexuality part
 Relationships with others
Brought back memories of awkwardness with my ex-girlfriend
I don’t have a relationship with my dad, which makes me sad
I didn’t like the questions about family because my relationship with my family is great
When I shared thoughts about my friends, I wished hard that I had better friends
…it’s hard because I have a really close friend who does it [self-injures] and I can’t stop him
 Awareness of others’ issues
Some people my age actually have these problems
That there are a lot of mental problems out there
The thought that young people take their lives
The drinking and drugs, it just worried me that people do that to themselves
 Confidentiality
I was worried this questionnaire wouldn’t be confidential
… I might be identified by my answers
… you might contact the school about me
I… am worried about my parents finding out

Positive responses

1.
Understanding and reflection At both baseline and follow-up the most common reason given for enjoying the questionnaire was that participants experienced a greater understanding of themselves and others (baseline n = 511; follow-up n = 488; ~13 % of total sample). These participants stated that they enjoyed the questionnaire because they had given positive responses and felt good about themselves. Others found that the questionnaire helped them to experience good memories, and reminded them of good things in life.
 
2.
Help is available/helping others 12.8 % (n = 234) at baseline and 15.5 % (n = 286) at follow-up reported they felt they were helping others, specifically the researchers and other adolescents in need. In addition, some responded that through the questionnaire, they now felt that help was available to them.
 
3.
Enjoyable and interesting At both baseline (n = 268, 14.6 %) and follow-up (n = 237, 12.8 %) several participants enjoyed the questionnaire simply because it was fun or interesting. Many stated that “I found it interesting”. Several reported enjoying the questionnaire because they like questionnaires.
 
4.
Self-expression At baseline 176 (9.6 %) and at follow-up 126 (6.8 %) indicated that the questionnaire gave them the opportunity to express their feelings without fear of people finding out about their behaviour, or judging them for it.
 
5.
Getting out of class Only 5.2 % (n = 96) at baseline and 7.7 % (n = 143) at follow-up reported enjoying the questionnaire because they got out of class.
 

Negative responses

1.
Inconvenience/irrelevance At both baseline and follow-up, the most common reason for not enjoying the questionnaire was that students found it inconvenient or irrelevant to them (baseline n = 255, 13.9 %; follow-up n = 300, 16.2 %). Some felt that they didn’t have anything to talk about, where others clearly felt that whilst they had problems, the questionnaire didn’t address them. Finally, others commented that the questionnaire was too long, or that they didn’t enjoy it because they would have preferred to be doing something else.
 
2.
Negative experiences Only 5.4 % (n = 98) at baseline and 5 % (n = 93) at follow-up reported a negative experience associated with participation. Some indicated answering questions made them upset about their own problems and concerns, whereas others gained an understanding of what others go through and this made them feel uncomfortable or unhappy.
 

Did anything in this questionnaire worry or upset you?

Quantitative results

At baseline, 304 participants (15.40 % of total sample) found the questionnaire worrying or upsetting which decreased to 235 (12.18 %) participants at follow-up. Males were less likely to be upset by the questionnaire than females. At both baseline and follow-up participants were more likely to report feeling worried or upset if they had a previous diagnosis of emotional or behavioural problems. Those who were worried or upset were also more likely to have thought about harming themselves, to have told someone about it, to have self-injured, thought about and attempted suicide at both baseline and follow-up (Table 1).
When considering changes in the reaction to the questionnaire over time, between group effects were evident for all variables except reference to others (all p < 0.01). People who were upset at baseline but not follow-up had poorer functioning on all variables except alcohol use and relying on others to cope, (all p < 0.001), than those who were not upset at either time point. Participants upset at baseline only also reported more psychological distress, less self-esteem, more life events, and higher self-injury scores (all p < 0.001) than those who were upset at both time points. Participants upset at both time points reported less problem solving, more drinking, and higher self-injury scores, than those not upset at either time (all p < 0.001). Participants upset at follow-up but not baseline exhibited less problem solving, F(1, 1742) = 18.87, p < 0.001, than participants who were not upset by the questionnaire at either time (Table 5).
Table 5
Multivariate analysis of variance demonstrates differential relationships between being worried or upset and psychosocial functioning over time
Group
Variable
Baseline M (SD)
Follow up M (SD)
F
p
η2
Upset baseline and follow up n = 96
Life events
33.23 (6.96)
34.38 (6.65)
4.15
0.05
0.050
Self-esteem
26.68 (5.75)
25.73 (6.02)
3.55
0.06
0.038
Self-efficacy
27.40 (4.44)
28.84 (4.50)
11.36
0.00
0.115
Non-productive coping
56.23 (12.34)
58.79 (11.15)
50.4
0.03
0.056
Reference to others
49.39 (12.69)
51.67 (13.22)
2.42
0.12
0.026
Problem solving
60.40 (13.12)
60.00 (13.76)
0.09
0.77
0.001
Optimism
17.66 (4.69)
17.66 (5.63)
0.000
1.00
0.000
Alcohol use
2.30 (2.67)
3.72 (3.33)
15.97
0.00
0.154
Psych. distress
29.14 (8.15)
29.44 (7.92)
0.11
0.75
0.001
NSSI
4.14 (5.90)
5.58 (6.00)
6.75
0.01
0.074
Upset baseline, not follow up n = 197
Life events
30.51 (5.61)
30.70 (5.73)
0.28
0.60
0.002
Self-esteem
28.50 (5.15)
28.87 (4.89)
1.63
0.20
0.009
Self-efficacy
28.94 (4.24)
29.78 (4.03)
7.29
0.01
0.040
Non-productive coping
53.79 (12.28)
53.18 (10.79)
0.57
0.45
0.003
Reference to others
50.24 (13.58)
51.16 (13.62)
1.10
0.30
0.006
Problem solving
63.40 (11.78)
63.39 (11.54)
0.000
0.98
0.000
Optimism
18.82 (4.41)
18.86 (4.58)
0.02
0.90
0.000
Alcohol use
2.26 (2.23)
3.18 (2.89)
30.15
0.00
0.136
Psych. distress
25.64 (7.00)
24.24 (6.16)
7.02
0.01
0.036
NSSI
1.81 (4.35)
2.00 (4.35)
0.47
0.49
0.003
Not upset baseline, upset follow up n = 131
Life events
28.81 (4.97)
31.85 (5.75)
37.67
0.00
0.260
Self-esteem
29.82 (4.81)
27.33 (5.44)
32.37
0.00
0.217
Self-efficacy
30.04 (4.19)
29.19 (3.88)
4.92
0.03
0.042
Non-productive coping
50.17 (10.87)
54.51 (11.88)
15.67
0.00
0.127
Reference to others
50.83 (13.65)
50.79 (12.19)
0.001
0.98
0.000
Problem solving
64.35 (12.34)
60.66 (12.07)
11.90
0.00
0.086
Optimism
20.34 (4.07)
18.22 (4.39)
31.18
0.00
0.206
Alcohol use
1.90 (2.10)
3.02 (3.01)
25.30
0.00
0.167
Psych. distress
23.86 (5.86)
27.58 (7.42)
28.51
0.00
0.192
NSSI
0.72 (2.93)
2.54 (4.86)
19.87
0.00
0.140
Not upset baseline or follow up n = 1446
Life events
27.09 (4.79)
27.88 (5.03)
42.07
0.00
0.034
Self-esteem
31.14 (4.67)
30.69 (4.88)
15.80
0.00
0.012
Self-efficacy
30.37 (3.86)
30.66 (3.97)
8.12
0.00
0.006
Non-productive coping
47.61 (11.37)
48.85 (11.39)
16.98
0.00
0.013
Reference to others
49.87 (13.38)
49.87 (13.44)
0.00
0.99
0.000
Problem solving
67.28 (12.06)
66.30 (11.76)
10.48
0.00
0.008
Optimism
20.90 (4.12)
20.81 (4.37)
0.84
0.36
0.001
Alcohol use
1.86 (2.00)
2.65 (2.76)
189.53
0.00
0.119
Psych. distress
21.74 (5.44)
22.32 (5.70)
12.03
0.00
0.009
NSSI
0.57 (2.53)
0.76 (2.95)
8.16
0.00
0.006
Changes in the variables over time varied according to whether participants were upset by the questionnaire for optimism, psychological distress, self-esteem, self-efficacy, life events, non-productive coping, and self-injury (Table 5; all p < 0.001). Of note, those who were not upset at baseline but were upset by the questionnaire at follow-up demonstrated deterioration in functioning (e.g. less adaptive coping, more psychological distress) over time. Conversely those who were upset by the questionnaire at baseline but not at follow-up demonstrated increased self-efficacy and decreased psychological distress over time.
With regard to specific life events, at both time points, those who reported feeling worried or upset were more likely to report having experienced a range of events, primarily to do with close relationships (e.g. problems with friends and parents) as well as suicidal behaviour among friends and family (Table 3). Finding the questionnaire upsetting at baseline was also related to the course of NSSI over the study period, χ2(3) = 155.37, p < 0.001. Specifically, participants reporting maintenance (standardised residual = 8.4) or cessation of NSSI (standardised residual = 6.4) were more likely to find the questionnaire upsetting at baseline. Of those who were upset at baseline, 8 % later engaged in NSSI, but this was not a significant relationship (1.3 % of the total sample reported being upset at baseline and onset of NSSI over the study period; 3.4 % who were not upset at baseline reported onset of NSSI). Similar results were observed for participants who were upset by the questionnaire at follow-up, χ2(3) = 152.51, p < 0.001. Specifically, adolescents who maintained their NSSI over time (standardised residual = 6.5), and those who first commenced self-injury between baseline and follow-up (standardised residual = 8.0) were more likely to be upset by the questionnaire at follow-up, while cessation of NSSI was not associated with being upset by the questionnaire.

Thematic analysis

When asked why they felt worried or upset, four themes emerged; (1) personal experience/feelings, (2) relationships with others, (3) awareness of other people’s issues, (4) confidentiality.
1.
Personal experience/feelings At both time points (baseline n = 80; follow-up n = 85; 4 % of total sample), the most common reason for reporting feeling upset or worried was that the questionnaire prompted memories they would prefer to avoid. Most frequently, these life events were related to death or suicide. Many felt that the questions asked were too personal, but others felt concerned that they themselves might need help. Finally, several participants expressed a dislike for themselves, and were worried that they might be judged based on their responses.
 
2.
Relationships with others Participants reported feeling worried or upset in relation to their interpersonal relationships (baseline n = 72; follow-up n = 59; 3 % of total sample), including relationships with friends, family, and romantic partners. Some expressed concerns about their friends’ behaviour, or felt that the friends they had were not very good friends. Romantic relationships, while less frequently identified than family or friend relationships, caused some participants concern.
 
3.
Awareness of others’ issues A small number of participants reported increased awareness of the problems or emotional concerns that others might have (baseline n = 42; follow-up n = 23; 1.6 % of total sample). Most of these responses indicated a new awareness that some young people engage in harmful behaviour, including drinking, taking drugs and NSSI, and that some consider suicide.
 
4.
Confidentiality A relatively small proportion of participants expressed concerns that others may find out about their responses, 17 at baseline and 9 at follow-up (0.6 % of total sample).
 

Discussion

Research exploring risk and protective factors for the development of later mental health problems, among currently healthy adolescents, is imperative to the development of evidence-based prevention and early intervention programs for mental illness. However ethical concerns arise regarding the potential for sensitive questions to evoke distress, particularly in young samples. We explored whether participation in sensitive research is perceived as enjoyable or upsetting to young people, and the reasons for these reactions. We also examined whether changes in psychosocial functioning over time were related to changes in perceptions of research participation. Finally we assessed whether enjoying the questionnaire or finding it upsetting was associated with the course of NSSI over time (i.e. onset, maintenance or cessation of NSSI).
More participants reported positive outcomes than negative, and these were largely related to experiencing a greater understanding of self and others, or feeling altruistic about helping other people. Where participants reported negative outcomes, these were related to disinterest, although some students also reported being upset by the content of questions related to recent life events. Changes in response to the questionnaire were accompanied by predictable changes in psychosocial functioning; greater enjoyment was accompanied by improved psychosocial functioning while negative reactions were accompanied by deteriorating functioning. We failed to ask young people whether they regretted participation, but fewer than 1 % of participants (or their parents) failed to consent to participation at follow-up suggesting that even those who were upset at baseline were willing to complete the questionnaire a second time [11, 12].
That fewer participants reported being worried or upset by the questionnaire at follow-up may indicate that the adaptive coping and emotion regulation skills typically developed throughout adolescence protect older students from adverse emotional reactions. If so, additional care should be taken to minimise potential distress and address duty of care with younger adolescents. Conversely, prior exposure to the questions may have minimised the emotional impact of the items.

Implications

Our findings suggest that young people do enjoy participating in psychological research. School staff may take comfort in knowing that not only are their students contributing to scientific knowledge, but that they also gain insight about their own behaviour, and that of their peers. In our study students became more aware of their own emotional struggles, and some indicated that although they did not know where to seek support for emotional difficulties prior to the study, through participation (and arguably our provision of mental health materials) they now knew that help was available, and where to seek it. Conducting research with adolescents could promote opportunities for help-seeking, delivering intervention earlier than might otherwise have occurred, and improving the prognosis for young people who are distressed.
Our results also suggest that young people can, and do, become upset when asked to respond to questions about their mental health, particularly if the questions touch on recent negative life events. In addition to minimising the potential for such distress to occur, resources need to be made available to young people who experience distress as a result of questionnaire completion. The fact that some participants expressed concerns regarding confidentiality, despite our provision of both written and verbal assurance, suggests researchers need to take additional steps to ensure that young people clearly understand how the researchers will utilise their data. Arguably, if participants were not clear regarding confidentiality, they may also have been unclear regarding other aspects of the study. This has important implications for the ability to provide informed consent and stresses the responsibility of researchers to ensure all participants are fully informed about the nature of the research.

Limitations and future research

The relatively low response rate, while consistent with similar Australian research requiring both active parental and active child consent [15], is lower than other longitudinal research and suggests bias in the sample. Of note, given the sensitive nature of many of the questions we asked, the lack of representativeness of the sample may mask important relationships between NSSI, psychological distress and reflections on survey participation. Comments from parents and teachers suggest that parents who were concerned about their child’s mental health were reluctant to consent to their child’s participation. That the vast majority of the sample enjoyed the questionnaire and were not upset by it may be an artefact of this selection bias.
While one of the key concerns in conducting research with youth is the potential for iatrogenic effects we are not able to directly assess this. Alcohol use and NSSI did increase over time, for all groups, but we cannot determine whether this is a consequence of being exposed to the questionnaire items or whether it is a natural artefact of progression through adolescence. Our analyses suggest that people who are upset by research participation are more likely to self-injure, and that a percentage do later engage in NSSI. However it is not possible with the current design to establish a causal relationship. Onset of NSSI over the study period is likely an accumulation of adverse life events, poor psychosocial functioning and perhaps reflection on this through answering these questions. Future research is needed to explicitly determine whether responding to questionnaire items regarding high risk behaviour confers risk of NSSI and other adverse effects among participants, under what conditions iatrogenic effects might be observed, and how to minimise these effects.
Although the majority of our measures demonstrated reliability, the ‘reference to others’ subscale of the Adolescent Coping Scale did not, which may account for the failure to find significant effects. Similarly, the global assessment of distress or enjoyment of the questionnaire precludes examination of whether students might respond differentially to different parts of the questionnaire. Further work regarding which type of questions might have most potential to cause distress, and whether this overrides any benefits of participation, is warranted. Finally, our question regarding distress combined the terms worry and upset, arguably two different emotional reactions. Disentangling how participants experience distress would be important for better assessing the risk of psychological harm as a result of research participation.

Conclusions

In exploring the reactions of adolescents to responding to sensitive research questions, we have confirmed previous findings that research can have both benefits and risks for participants. Encouragingly, the majority of school-based adolescents enjoy research participation and cite altruistic reasons for participation. However, a minority of youth, particularly those who are already experiencing distress, are temporarily distressed by responding to questions about mental health. Researchers are encouraged to emphasise the benefits of research participation, while also continuing to work to minimise the potential for distress and implement appropriate duty of care protocols.

Authors’ contributions

PH and GM designed the study, oversaw data collection and assisted with manuscript preparation. RT analysed data and assisted with manuscript preparation. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by the Australian Research Council. We are grateful for the assistance of the following people in data collection and entry: Sophie Aitken, Tori Andrews, Emily Berger, Teryn Callaway, Lauren Friend, Cassandara Rotolone, and Grace Skrzypiec.

Compliance with ethical guidelines

Competing interests The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Merikangas KR, He JP, Burstein M, Swanson SA, Avenevoli S, Cui L, Benjet C, Georgiades K, Swendsen J (2010) Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Questionnaire replication adolescent supplement (NCS-A). J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 49:980–989CrossRef Merikangas KR, He JP, Burstein M, Swanson SA, Avenevoli S, Cui L, Benjet C, Georgiades K, Swendsen J (2010) Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Questionnaire replication adolescent supplement (NCS-A). J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 49:980–989CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Gullone E, Hughes EK, King NJ, Tonge B (2010) The normative development of emotion regulation strategy use in children and adolescents: a 2-year follow-up study. J Child Psychol Psychol 51:567–574CrossRef Gullone E, Hughes EK, King NJ, Tonge B (2010) The normative development of emotion regulation strategy use in children and adolescents: a 2-year follow-up study. J Child Psychol Psychol 51:567–574CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Spear LP (2000) The adoelscent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci Biobehav R 24:417–463CrossRef Spear LP (2000) The adoelscent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci Biobehav R 24:417–463CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Kelly AB, Halford WK (2007) Responses to ethical challenges in conducting research with Australian adolescents. Aust J Pychol 59:24–33CrossRef Kelly AB, Halford WK (2007) Responses to ethical challenges in conducting research with Australian adolescents. Aust J Pychol 59:24–33CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson J, Pan Yuen H, Martin C, Hughes A, Baksheev GN, Dodd S, Bapat S, Schwass W, McGorry P, Yung AR (2011) Does screening high school students for psychological distress, deliberate self-harm, or suicidal ideation cause distress and is it acceptable? An Australian-based study. Crisis 32:254–263CrossRefPubMed Robinson J, Pan Yuen H, Martin C, Hughes A, Baksheev GN, Dodd S, Bapat S, Schwass W, McGorry P, Yung AR (2011) Does screening high school students for psychological distress, deliberate self-harm, or suicidal ideation cause distress and is it acceptable? An Australian-based study. Crisis 32:254–263CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) (2007) National Statement on Ethical Conduct in Human Research. Australian Government, Canberra National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) (2007) National Statement on Ethical Conduct in Human Research. Australian Government, Canberra
7.
Zurück zum Zitat World Medical Association (1964) The Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. 18th World Medical Assembly, Helsinki World Medical Association (1964) The Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. 18th World Medical Assembly, Helsinki
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Ceci SJ, Peters D, Plotkin J (1985) Human subjects review, personal values, and the regulation of social science research. Am Psychol 40:994–1002CrossRef Ceci SJ, Peters D, Plotkin J (1985) Human subjects review, personal values, and the regulation of social science research. Am Psychol 40:994–1002CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Cukrowicz K, Smith P, Poindexter E (2010) The effect of participating in suicide research: Does participating in a research protocol on suicide and psychiatric symptoms increase suicide ideation and attempts? Suicide Life Threat 40:535–543CrossRef Cukrowicz K, Smith P, Poindexter E (2010) The effect of participating in suicide research: Does participating in a research protocol on suicide and psychiatric symptoms increase suicide ideation and attempts? Suicide Life Threat 40:535–543CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Jaffe AE, DiLillo D, Hoffman L, Haikalis M, Dykstra RE (2015) Does it hurt to ask? A meta-analysis of participant reactions to trauma research. Clin Psych Rev 40:40–56CrossRef Jaffe AE, DiLillo D, Hoffman L, Haikalis M, Dykstra RE (2015) Does it hurt to ask? A meta-analysis of participant reactions to trauma research. Clin Psych Rev 40:40–56CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson L, Benight C (2003) Effects of trauma-focused research on recent domestic violence survivors. J Trauma Stress 16:567–571CrossRefPubMed Johnson L, Benight C (2003) Effects of trauma-focused research on recent domestic violence survivors. J Trauma Stress 16:567–571CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Walker EA, Newman E, Koss M, Bernstein D (1997) Does the study of victimization revictimize the victims? Gen Hosp Psychiatry 19:403–410CrossRefPubMed Walker EA, Newman E, Koss M, Bernstein D (1997) Does the study of victimization revictimize the victims? Gen Hosp Psychiatry 19:403–410CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Rivlin A, Marzano L, Hawton K, Fazel S (2012) Impact on prisoners of participating in research interviews related to near-lethal suicide attempts. J Affect Disord 136:54–62CrossRefPubMed Rivlin A, Marzano L, Hawton K, Fazel S (2012) Impact on prisoners of participating in research interviews related to near-lethal suicide attempts. J Affect Disord 136:54–62CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Gould MS, Marrocco FA, Kleinman M, Thomas JG, Mostkoff K, Cote J, Davies M (2005) Evaluating iatrogenic effects of youth suicide screening programs: a randomized control trial. J Am Med Assoc 293:1635–1643CrossRef Gould MS, Marrocco FA, Kleinman M, Thomas JG, Mostkoff K, Cote J, Davies M (2005) Evaluating iatrogenic effects of youth suicide screening programs: a randomized control trial. J Am Med Assoc 293:1635–1643CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Hasking P, Coric S, Swannell S, Martin G, Thomspon HK, Frost ADJ (2010) Emotion regulation and coping as moderators in the relationship between personality and self-injury. J Adolesc 33:767–773CrossRefPubMed Hasking P, Coric S, Swannell S, Martin G, Thomspon HK, Frost ADJ (2010) Emotion regulation and coping as moderators in the relationship between personality and self-injury. J Adolesc 33:767–773CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Boergers J, Spirinto A (2003) Follow-up studies of child and adolescent suicide attempters. In: King RA, Apter A (eds) Suicide in Children and Adolescents. Cambridge University Press, New York Boergers J, Spirinto A (2003) Follow-up studies of child and adolescent suicide attempters. In: King RA, Apter A (eds) Suicide in Children and Adolescents. Cambridge University Press, New York
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Gutierrez PM, Osman A, Barrios FX, Kopper BA (2001) Development and initial validation of the Self-Harm Behavior Questionnaire. J Personal Assess 77:475–490CrossRef Gutierrez PM, Osman A, Barrios FX, Kopper BA (2001) Development and initial validation of the Self-Harm Behavior Questionnaire. J Personal Assess 77:475–490CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Brausch AM, Gutierrez PM (2010) Differences in non-suicidal self-injury and suicide attempts in adolescents. J Youth Adolesc 39:233–242CrossRefPubMed Brausch AM, Gutierrez PM (2010) Differences in non-suicidal self-injury and suicide attempts in adolescents. J Youth Adolesc 39:233–242CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Hawton K, Rodham K (2006) By their own young hand. Deliberate self-harm and suicidal ideas in adolescents. Jessica Kingsley, London Hawton K, Rodham K (2006) By their own young hand. Deliberate self-harm and suicidal ideas in adolescents. Jessica Kingsley, London
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Babor TF, Higgins-Biddle JC, Saunders JB, Monteiro MG (2001) The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test: Guidelines for use in primary care. World Health Organisation Babor TF, Higgins-Biddle JC, Saunders JB, Monteiro MG (2001) The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test: Guidelines for use in primary care. World Health Organisation
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Goldberg D, Williams P (1988) A user’s guide to the General Health Questionnaire. NFER-Nelson, Windor Goldberg D, Williams P (1988) A user’s guide to the General Health Questionnaire. NFER-Nelson, Windor
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Basheev GN, Robinson J, Cosgrave EM, Baker K, Yung AR (2011) Validity of the 12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) in detecting depressive and anxiety disorders among high school students. Psychiatry Res 187:291–296CrossRef Basheev GN, Robinson J, Cosgrave EM, Baker K, Yung AR (2011) Validity of the 12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) in detecting depressive and anxiety disorders among high school students. Psychiatry Res 187:291–296CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenberg M (1965) Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton University Press, Princeton Rosenberg M (1965) Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton University Press, Princeton
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwarzer R, Jerusalem M (1995) Generalized Self-Efficacy scale. In: Weinman J, Wright S, Johnston M (eds) Measures in health psychology: a user’s portfolio Causal and control beliefs. Nfer-Nelson, Windsor, pp 35–37 Schwarzer R, Jerusalem M (1995) Generalized Self-Efficacy scale. In: Weinman J, Wright S, Johnston M (eds) Measures in health psychology: a user’s portfolio Causal and control beliefs. Nfer-Nelson, Windsor, pp 35–37
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Scheier MF, Carver CS (1985) Optimism, coping, and health: assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychol 4:219–247CrossRefPubMed Scheier MF, Carver CS (1985) Optimism, coping, and health: assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychol 4:219–247CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Scheier MF, Carver CS, Bridges MW (1994) Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): a re-evaluation of the Life Orientation Test. J Personal Soc Psychol 67:1063–1078CrossRef Scheier MF, Carver CS, Bridges MW (1994) Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): a re-evaluation of the Life Orientation Test. J Personal Soc Psychol 67:1063–1078CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Frydenberg E, Lewis R (1993) Adolescent Coping Scale. Australian Council for Educational Research, Melbourne Frydenberg E, Lewis R (1993) Adolescent Coping Scale. Australian Council for Educational Research, Melbourne
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Braun V, Clarke V (2006) Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual Res Psychol 3:77–101CrossRef Braun V, Clarke V (2006) Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual Res Psychol 3:77–101CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Adolescents’ reactions to participating in ethically sensitive research: a prospective self-report study
verfasst von
Penelope Hasking
Ruth C. Tatnell
Graham Martin
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2015
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health / Ausgabe 1/2015
Elektronische ISSN: 1753-2000
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-015-0074-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2015

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 1/2015 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.