Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2023

Open Access 01.12.2023 | Research article

Application of symbolic play test in identification of autism spectrum disorder without global developmental delay and developmental language disorder

verfasst von: Xuening Chang, Lingli Sun, Ruizhen Li

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2023

Abstract

Background

Children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) usually experience difficulty regarding symbolic play. However, studies on whether symbolic play test (SPT) can differentiate between ASD and other developmental disorders are inconsistent, and evaluating the application value of the SPT in the identification of ASD without global developmental delay (GDD) and developmental language disorder (DLD) is necessary.

Methods

A total of 200 children were selected as the research participants. There were 100 cases of ASD without GDD and 100 cases of DLD. All children were tested by SPT and Children Neuropsychological and Behavioral Scale-Revision (CNBS-R2016). Binomial logistic regression was used for multivariate analysis. The receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve was used to evaluate the value of SPT in identifying ASD without GDD and DLD.

Results

SPT equivalent age was lower than chronological age in the two groups, the difference between the ASD without GDD group was greater than that in the DLD group, and the proportion of SPT equivalent age retardation was higher than that in the DLD group; the differences were statistically significant. Logistic regression analysis showed that there was a difference in SPT equivalent age between DLD and ASD without GDD. When the cut-off value of the SPT was 8.5, the largest area under the ROC curve was 0.723, and the sensitivity and specificity for the diagnosis of ASD without GDD were 0.720 and 0.620 respectively.

Conclusions

Symbolic play ability in ASD children is worse than that of DLD children at comparable development levels. SPT may be helpful to distinguish ASD without GDD from children with DLD.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Background

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in the DSM is classified as a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized as social communication disorders, narrow interest, or repeated stereotyped behaviors [1]. The incidence of ASD has risen sharply in recent years. The prevalence of ASD has reached 1/44, according to the data of the US Centers for Disease Control and Prevention in 2021 [2]. The male-female ratio was 4.3:1 [3].
Symbolic play arises between 18 and 24 months of life [4]. Symbolic play test (SPT) mainly observes children’s ability to express their self-experience and imagination with the simplest symbols [5]. Some studies indicate that social tendencies, shared attention, and lack of symbolic play are the core deficits of ASD [6]. Children with ASD usually experience difficulty with symbolic play [7].
Research shows the existence of a remarkable relationship between child play and the development of language [8, 9] in children with both typical and atypical development. The delay in the development of language and receptive language predicts the difficulties of children with ASD in pretend play [10].
Language disorders are the most obvious features of children with ASD and also are the direct reason for their hospital visits [11]. Developmental language disorder (DLD) refers to when a child’s language skills are substantially and quantitatively lower than their peers while excluding ASD, global developmental delay (GDD), etc. [12]. ASD usually has some comorbidities, such as GDD [13]. GDD is defined as a delay in two or more developmental domains of gross/fine motor, speech/language, cognition, social/personal, and activities of daily living, affecting children under the age of 5 years [14, 15]. ASD without GDD and DLD are often confused with each other in the early stage of the disease and are difficult to identify [16]. In China, most studies of autism have only focused on children with autism who have intellectual disability (ID) [17]. This is despite the fact that 75% of individuals living with autism spectrum disorder have no intellectual disability [18]. Children with GDD often meet diagnostic criteria for ID and probably represent the same population [19].
Studies have shown that early screening for ASD is far more cost-effective than comprehensive diagnostic assessment without screening [20]. At present, the commonly used screening tools in clinical are the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT) and the Autism Behavioral Scale (ABC), but both of them are subjectively influenced by their parents, and the screening efficacy is poor [21, 22].
SPT is considered an early indicator for the diagnosis of autism [23] and its assessment [24]. Some studies [25, 26] found that children with ASD present less symbolic play than children with other neurodevelopmental disorders. In contrast, some studies [24, 27] did not find differences between the groups of children with ASD and other neurodevelopmental disorders.
The primary objective of the current study was to provide scientific evidence for the early detection of ASD by evaluating the application value of the SPT in children with ASD without GDD.

Methods

Participants

Two hundred children aged 1.5–4.5 years old who were diagnosed with ASD without GDD and DLD in the Child Health Department of Wuhan Children’s Hospital from August 2021 to December 2021 participated in this study. 100 children with ASD without GDD were enrolled, including 79 boys and 21 girls. Their mean chronological age is 28.45 ± 5.34 months. 100 children were diagnosed with DLD, including 87 boys and 13 girls with a mean chronological age of 27.33 ± 5.22 months. There were no statistical differences in the gender distribution (χ2 = 2.268, P = 0.132) and chronological age (t = 1.498, P = 0.136) in these two groups of children. All the clinicians participating in the study were uniformly trained, and all scale assessments were completed by the same professional. The examiner was blind to the participants’ group when completing the assessment. This study was approved by the medical ethical committee of Wuhan Children’s Hospital, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology (NO.2021R181). Informed and signed consent was obtained from all parent(s)/caregiver(s) of the participating children. All methods in the present study were performed following the relevant guidelines and regulations of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Procedure

The enrollment criteria for children with ASD without GDD: ①met the diagnostic criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM-5) [28], and were diagnosed by 2 deputy chief physicians and above specialists. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2) and Children Neuropsychological and Behavioral Scale-Revision (CNBS-R2016) were conducted by one professional reviewer; ②no more than one functional domain with a Developmental Quotient (DQ) < 70 in the evaluation results of the CNBS-R2016 [29]. Exclusion criteria: ①Fragile X Syndrome, tuberous sclerosis, cerebral paralysis, epilepsy, schizophrenia, mood disorders, hearing impairment;②abnormal karyotype, appearance deformities, Brain MRI and EEG abnormalities, and other neurological or somatic diseases. The enrollment standard for children with DLD: ①met the diagnostic criteria for DSM-V; ②DQ of language ≤85, DQ of gross motor, fine motor, adaptability, and social behavior ≥70 [30]. ASD, developmental delay, genetic differences, vision, hearing abnormalities, central nervous system disorders, and second language development disorders were excluded [31].

Instruments included in the study

Symbolic play test (SPT)

Four sets are used to assess children’s spontaneous play behaviors in structured situations for a total of 10–15 min of play (3 min per set) with the professional investigator. The investigator cannot use verbal or other communication types to explain the play but can remind children to draw attention to a neglected toy. Four sets of miniature objects are presented in a specific order in different situations, enabling the investigator to observe if the child relates to them appropriately. Objects are purposefully chosen and can lend themselves to a variety of interrelationships. The scoring system is based on the number of meaningful responses and connections the child is able to make.
The scale was made up of 24 items. Set 1 evaluates the following items: Discriminate handing of doll; relates spoon to cup or saucer; feeds, combs, or brushes doll; feeds, combs or brushes the other person; places the cup on the saucer. Set 2 evaluates the following items: Discriminate handing of doll; relates doll to bed; relates blanket or pillow to doll; puts doll to bed; uses pillow correctly. Set 3 evaluates the following items: Relates knife or fork to plate; relates fork, knife, or plate to table; relates tablecloth to other objects; plates doll on the chair; relates fork, knife, or plate to doll; relates chair to table; relates doll to table; places tablecloth on the table. Set 4 evaluates the following items: Moves tractor or trailer along; relates logs to the tractor, trailer or man; places man in the tractor or trailer; places man in driver’s seat; lines up the tractor and trailer; attaches tractor or trailer.
Completion of each item counts as 1 point, otherwise, counts as 0 points, and the final score is the sum of 24 items. Both raw score and age equivalences were provided by the scale. No corresponding age in months was found when SPT raw score is less than 5 or greater than 23. The test has good reliability (Cronbach α = 0.90) and construct-related validity (r = 0.77) [5].

Children neuropsychological and behavioral scale-revision (CNBS-R2016)

CNBS-R2016 evaluates the neurodevelopmental levels and autism symptoms of children. The development quotients (DQs) of the six domains of CNBS-R2016 include gross motor, fine motor, adaptive behavior, language, personal-social, and autism warning behavior. The results are expressed by DQ. DQ greater than 80 is interpreted as the absence of neuropsychological and/or behavioral difficulties. DQ between 70 and 79 indicates a borderline deficiency, and DQ less than 70 indicates a developmental delay. The CNBS-R2016 and the Griffiths Mental Development Scales for China showed good consistency in the developmental assessment of children with ASD [29].

Autism diagnostic observation schedule, second edition (ADOS-2)

The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule 2nd Edition [32] is a standardized semi-structured interview recommended for the assessment of ASD, generally lasting from 30 to 60 min. It includes a range of questions and activities designed to evoke behaviors and cognitions associated with ASD. These visible behaviors and discussions are then scored from 0 to 3 for “autism severity”. Under the original algorithm, 11 items from the larger scoring matrix are then summed to create an ADOS score, where 7 is the cut-off for being designated as “on the autism spectrum,” and 10 is the cut-off for being designated as “autistic.” The algorithm has two subscales: social affect and restrictive and repetitive behaviors, and total scores of 8 or more indicate possible ASD.

Statistical methods

Continuous variables were described as mean ± standard deviation (SD) and tested with a rank-sum test; categorical variables were described as the proportion (%) and tested with a chi-square test. Finally, multiple logistic regression was used to assess the association between SPT and two groups, while adjusting for confounders. SPT was the dependent variable; disease categories were the primary independent variable, and chronological age, sex, gross motor function, fine motor function, adaptive behavior function, language function, and personal-social function were confounding variables. The odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) were estimated from the multiple logistic regression models. The receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve was used to analyze the cut-off value of SPT in screening for ASD without GDD. All P values were two-tailed with a significant level of 0.05. Statistical analyses were carried out using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Inc., Cary, NC).

Results

Comparison of chronological age and SPT equivalent age in the two groups

SPT equivalent age lagged behind chronological age in the two groups, and the differences were statistically significant. The difference between chronological age and SPT equivalent age in ASD without GDD group was greater than that in the DLD group, and the differences were statistically significant (P < 0.05) (Table 1).
Table 1
Comparison of chronological age and SPT equivalent age in ASD without GDD group and DLD group
project
18–24 months
P
ES
25–31 months
P
ES
≥32 months
P
ES
N
x ± s
N
x ± s
N
x ± s
ASD without GDD group
 Chronological age
32
22.27 ± 2.01
< 0.001
3.553
40
28.75 ± 1.70
< 0.001
4.431
28
35.09 ± 2.14
< 0.001
3.961
 SPT equivalent age
16
14.50 ± 2.35
 
34
16.56 ± 3.50
 
26
19.52 ± 5.13
 
DLD group
 Chronological age
31
22.11 ± 1.92
< 0.001
1.517
43
26.87 ± 1.48
< 0.001
1.525
26
34.32 ± 3.82
< 0.001
2.002
 SPT equivalent age
27
17.39 ± 3.96
 
41
20.78 ± 5.45
 
24
23.49 ± 6.63
 
ASD Autism spectrum disorders, GDD Global developmental delay, SPT Symbolic play test, DLD Developmental language disorder, ES Effect size

Comparison of symbolic abilities and developmental levels in the two groups

In the 18–24 months group, the proportion of SPT equivalent age seriously lagging (SPT equivalent age is less than 12 months) in the ASD without GDD group was higher than that in the DLD group. The adaptive behavior function, language function, and personal-social function of ASD without the GDD group lagged behind that of the DLD group, and the differences were statistically significant (P < 0.05) (Table 2).
Table 2
Comparison of symbolic abilities and development levels in ASD without GDD group and DLD group in children aged 18–24 months
project
18–24 months
χ2/t
P
ES
ASD without GDD group
 
DLD group
 
(N = 32)
(N = 31)
SPT equivalent age
 < 12 months
16
80.00%
4
20.00%
10.001
0.002
0.398
 ≥12 months
16
37.21%
27
62.79%
   
Gross motor function
100.09 ± 7.62
98.97 ± 5.68
0.664
0.509
0.167
Fine motor function
89.06 ± 13.40
92.55 ± 9.65
−1.182
0.242
0.299
Adaptive behavior function
86.73 ± 12.45
100.65 ± 11.15
−4.667
< 0.001
1.178
Language function
58.67 ± 11.89
67.48 ± 9.57
−3.236
0.002
0.816
Personal-social function
69.14 ± 7.09
73.48 ± 5.11
−2.782
0.007
0.702
ASD Autism spectrum disorders, GDD Global developmental delay, SPT symbolic play test, DLD Developmental language disorder, ES Effect size
There are statistically significant differences in the Language function and Personal-social function variables in the 25–31 months group (Table 3). Statistically significant differences are observed in the Fine motor function variable in the ≥32 months group (Table 4). In both the 25–31 months and ≥ 32 months groups, the Adaptive behavior function of ASD without GDD group lagged behind that of the DLD group, and the differences were statistically significant (Tables 3 and 4).
Table 3
Comparison of symbolic play ability and development level in ASD without GDD group and DLD group in children aged 25–31 months
project
25–31 months
χ2/t
P
ES
ASD without GDD group
 
DLD group
 
(N = 40)
(N = 43)
SPT equivalent age
 < 12 months
6
75.00%
2
25.00%
1.499
0.221
0.134
 ≥12 months
34
45.33%
41
54.67%
   
Gross motor function
93.95 ± 7.57
94.05 ± 7.27
−0.059
0.953
0.013
Fine motor function
84.30 ± 8.63
89.91 ± 15.87
−1.979
0.051
0.439
Adaptive behavior function
82.08 ± 8.87
94.67 ± 9.34
−6.290
< 0.001
1.382
Language function
61.19 ± 13.65
66.77 ± 9.80
−2.150
0.035
0.470
Personal-social function
69.54 ± 7.50
74.05 ± 5.69
−3.099
0.003
0.678
ASD Autism spectrum disorders, GDD Global developmental delay, SPT symbolic play test, DLD Developmental language disorder, ES Effect size
Table 4
Comparison of symbolic play ability and development level in ASD without GDD group and DLD group in children aged ≥32 months
project
≥32 months
χ2/t
P
ES
ASD without GDD group
 
DLD group
 
(N = 28)
(N = 26)
SPT equivalent age
 < 12 months
2
66.67%
1
33.33%
0.000
1.000
0.000
 ≥12 months
26
50.98%
25
49.02%
   
Gross motor function
92.46 ± 7.92
93.50 ± 6.27
−0.530
0.598
0.146
Fine motor function
78.89 ± 6.36
85.38 ± 9.65
−2.940
0.005
0.794
Adaptive behavior function
79.21 ± 7.32
87.38 ± 9.48
−3.560
0.001
0.965
Language function
64.89 ± 10.96
61.65 ± 8.63
1.201
0.235
0.328
Personal-social function
72.21 ± 6.53
73.12 ± 6.26
−0.517
0.607
0.142
ASD Autism spectrum disorders, GDD Global developmental delay, SPT symbolic play test, DLD Developmental language disorder, ES Effect size

The effect of SPT on the differentiation of ASD without GDD and DLD

Logistic regression analysis was performed with disease categories as dependent variables and chronological age, sex, gross motor function, fine motor function, adaptive behavior function, language function, personal-social function, and SPT equivalent age as independent variables. The results showed that there was a difference in SPT equivalent age between DLD and ASD without GDD (OR = 1.16, 95%CI = 1.06 ~ 1.27) (Table 5).
Table 5
Binomial logistic regression analysis of influencing factors on the differentiation of ASD without GDD and DLD
Factor
Estimate of parameter
Standardization of parameter estimates
Chi-square
P value
OR (95% CI)
Sex
0.148
0.564
0.069
0.793
1.16 (0.38–3.50)
Chronological age
−0.209
0.104
4.060
0.044
0.81 (0.66–0.99)
Gross motor function
−0.166
0.104
2.540
0.111
0.85 (0.69–1.04)
Fine motor function
−0.043
0.067
0.406
0.524
0.96 (0.84–1.09)
Adaptive behavior Function
0.301
0.088
11.534
0.001
1.35 (1.14–1.61)
Language function
0.048
0.071
0.468
0.494
1.05 (0.91–1.21)
Personal-social function
−0.036
0.088
0.167
0.682
0.96 (0.81–1.15)
SPT equivalent age
0.150
0.045
11.332
0.001
1.16 (1.06–1.27)
ASD Autism spectrum disorders, GDD Global developmental delay, SPT symbolic play test, DLD Developmental language disorder

Exploration of the optimal cut-off value for SPT to identify two groups

SPT scores were used as the test variable. Positive results of DSM-V and ADOS-2 were used as the state variable. Then ROC curve of the SPT score was plotted. The optimal cut-off value of SPT was determined by calculating the boundary point corresponding to the maximum of the Youden index. ROC curve showed that when the cut-off value of the SPT was 8.5, the largest area under the ROC curve was 0.723 (95% CI: 0.654 ~ 0.793, P < 0.001), and the sensitivity and specificity for the diagnosis of ASD without GDD were 0.720 and 0.620 respectively. Therefore, SPT had the best discriminatory effect on ASD without GDD and DLD when the SPT score is 8–9 (Fig. 1).

Discussion

Early intervention can improve the core symptoms of children with ASD [20], and early detection is the key to achieving early intervention [33]. However, the age of diagnosis of ASD is still relatively late. A U.S. survey showed that the average age at diagnosis of ASD was 4.2 years [34]. The early identification and diagnosis of ASD lag behind in China [35], and the average age at diagnosis was 4.5 years [36].
SPT is a screening tool for young children in the early language stage, with a short test time and a simple method. It has been validated in English-speaking children. However, the assessment can be adapted to other cultures, such as Chinese and Japanese cultures, where chopsticks and bowls rather than spoons, forks, and knives are used as eating utensils [37].
This study indicates that SPT equivalent age was lower than the chronological age of the two groups. Therefore, SPT can assist in the screening of children with language developmental delay and ASD [6, 24]. The symbolic ability of the ASD without GDD group was significantly lower than that of the DLD group, a finding consistent with previous studies [38, 39]. There are statistically significant differences in the Language function, Personal-social function and Adaptive behavior function variables in the 18–24 months and 25–31 months groups. Statistically significant differences are also observed in the Fine motor function and Adaptive behavior function variables in the ≥32 months group. Logistic regression analysis showed that there was a difference in SPT equivalent age between DLD and ASD without GDD, independent of sex, age, and DQs of the five domains of CNBS-R2016. ROC curve showed that when the cut-off value of the SPT was 8–9, the largest area under the ROC curve was 0.723. It suggests that children with ASD still have difficulties in the symbolic game even without GDD. Symbolic play is an important behavior in early childhood development, which is closely related to social communication and cognitive development [40]. The earliest core symptoms of ASD were usually impairments in social interaction such as imaginary games and impairments in communication, rather than rigid repetitive movements or specific interests [41]. Other studies suggested that children with ASD have rigid and repetitive behaviors, lack complexity and diversity, and are difficult to understand the connection between objects. Moreover, they often show defects in pretend games and have less playfulness [42].
Prospective studies found that SPT is associated with developmental level or intelligence; ASD and developmental level both affect symbolic ability [5, 43]. However, few studies have explored the relationship between SPT and ASD independently of developmental level, and the optimal cut-off value for SPT to identify ASD.

Limitations

The limitations of this study include the following aspects. First, the severity of ASD symptoms was not grouped. Therefore, it is difficult to further determine the application value of the SPT in different severities of ASD without GDD. Second, lack of neurotypical control samples. Setting a neurotypical control group may reduce the influence of confounding factors. Finally, we should also explore the discriminative role of SPT in children with ASD and GDD versus children with GDD alone, as the sample was comprised of children with ASD without GDD, the findings of this study may not be applicable to the entire ASD population.

Conclusions

To sum up, symbolic play ability in ASD children is worse than that of DLD children at comparable development levels. In addition, SPT had the best discriminatory effect on ASD without GDD and DLD when the SPT score is 8–9. Our results, therefore, highlight the importance of assessing symbolic play. These findings may facilitate better identification of individuals at risk and the development of effective interventions to help children with ASD in China. Thus, SPT should be routinely used to screen for ASD in children who present with language delay without obvious developmental problems, and SPT should be used as an early warning signal of ASD to identify children suspected of ASD more sensitive and quickly so that they can be referred to hospitals at a higher level for comprehensive evaluation and diagnosis, and win the time of early intervention.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all patients and students for their participation in the study and the clinic for its collaboration.

Declarations

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The present study was approved by the medical ethical committee of Wuhan Children’s Hospital, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology (NO.2021R181). Informed and signed consent was obtained from all parent(s)/caregiver(s) of the participating children.
Written informed consent for publication was obtained from all participants.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Siu AL. Force atUPST: screening for autism Spectrum disorder in young children: US preventive services task force recommendation statement. JAMA. 2016;315(7):691–6.PubMedCrossRef Siu AL. Force atUPST: screening for autism Spectrum disorder in young children: US preventive services task force recommendation statement. JAMA. 2016;315(7):691–6.PubMedCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Maenner MJ, Shaw KA, Bakian AV, Bilder DA, Durkin MS, Esler A, et al. Prevalence of autism Spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years — autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2018. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2021;70(11):1–16.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Maenner MJ, Shaw KA, Bakian AV, Bilder DA, Durkin MS, Esler A, et al. Prevalence of autism Spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years — autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2018. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2021;70(11):1–16.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Levy SE, Giarelli E, Lee L-C, Schieve LA, Kirby RS, Cunniff C, et al. Autism Spectrum disorder and co-occurring developmental, psychiatric, and medical conditions among children in multiple populations of the United States. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2010;31(4):267–75.PubMedCrossRef Levy SE, Giarelli E, Lee L-C, Schieve LA, Kirby RS, Cunniff C, et al. Autism Spectrum disorder and co-occurring developmental, psychiatric, and medical conditions among children in multiple populations of the United States. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2010;31(4):267–75.PubMedCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Lillard AS, Lerner MD, Hopkins EJ, Dore RA, Smith ED, Palmquist CM. The impact of pretend play on children's development: a review of the evidence. Psychol Bull. 2013;139(1):1–34.PubMedCrossRef Lillard AS, Lerner MD, Hopkins EJ, Dore RA, Smith ED, Palmquist CM. The impact of pretend play on children's development: a review of the evidence. Psychol Bull. 2013;139(1):1–34.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Thiemann-Bourque K, Johnson LK, Brady NC. Similarities in functional play and differences in symbolic play of children with autism Spectrum disorder. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2019;124(1):77–91.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Thiemann-Bourque K, Johnson LK, Brady NC. Similarities in functional play and differences in symbolic play of children with autism Spectrum disorder. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2019;124(1):77–91.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat González-Sala F, Gómez-Marí I, Tárraga-Mínguez R, Vicente-Carvajal A, Pastor-Cerezuela G. Symbolic play among children with autism Spectrum disorder: a scoping review. Children. 2021;8(9):801.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef González-Sala F, Gómez-Marí I, Tárraga-Mínguez R, Vicente-Carvajal A, Pastor-Cerezuela G. Symbolic play among children with autism Spectrum disorder: a scoping review. Children. 2021;8(9):801.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Barton EE. Teaching generalized pretend play and related behaviors to young children with disabilities. Except Child. 2015;81(4):489–506.CrossRef Barton EE. Teaching generalized pretend play and related behaviors to young children with disabilities. Except Child. 2015;81(4):489–506.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Lewis V, Boucher J, Lupton L, Watson S. Relationships between symbolic play, functional play, verbal and non-verbal ability in young children. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2000;35(1):117–27.PubMedCrossRef Lewis V, Boucher J, Lupton L, Watson S. Relationships between symbolic play, functional play, verbal and non-verbal ability in young children. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2000;35(1):117–27.PubMedCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Musatti T, Veneziano E, Mayer S. Contributions of language to early pretend play. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Curr Psychol Cogn. 1998;17(2):155–84. Musatti T, Veneziano E, Mayer S. Contributions of language to early pretend play. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Curr Psychol Cogn. 1998;17(2):155–84.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Sa B. Bourchier-Sutton a: the decontextualization of form and function in the development of pretence. Br J Dev Psychol. 2007;25(3):335–51.CrossRef Sa B. Bourchier-Sutton a: the decontextualization of form and function in the development of pretence. Br J Dev Psychol. 2007;25(3):335–51.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Barsotti J, Mangani G, Nencioli R, Pfanner L, Tancredi R, Cosenza A, et al. Grammatical comprehension in Italian children with autism Spectrum disorder. Brain Sci. 2020;10(8):510.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Barsotti J, Mangani G, Nencioli R, Pfanner L, Tancredi R, Cosenza A, et al. Grammatical comprehension in Italian children with autism Spectrum disorder. Brain Sci. 2020;10(8):510.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Myers SM, Voigt RG, Colligan RC, Weaver AL, Storlie CB, Stoeckel RE, et al. Autism Spectrum disorder: incidence and time trends over two decades in a population-based birth cohort. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49(4):1455–74.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Myers SM, Voigt RG, Colligan RC, Weaver AL, Storlie CB, Stoeckel RE, et al. Autism Spectrum disorder: incidence and time trends over two decades in a population-based birth cohort. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49(4):1455–74.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang H, Du Y, Mao Z, Che Y, Li H, Ding L, et al. Use of the Griffiths mental development scale-Chinese in the assessment of children with autism spectrum disorder and global developmental delay/intellectual disability. Medicine. 2021;100(13):e25407.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wang H, Du Y, Mao Z, Che Y, Li H, Ding L, et al. Use of the Griffiths mental development scale-Chinese in the assessment of children with autism spectrum disorder and global developmental delay/intellectual disability. Medicine. 2021;100(13):e25407.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Majnemer A, Shevell MI. Diagnostic yield of the neurologic assessment of the developmentally delayed child. J Pediatr. 1995;127(2):193–9.PubMedCrossRef Majnemer A, Shevell MI. Diagnostic yield of the neurologic assessment of the developmentally delayed child. J Pediatr. 1995;127(2):193–9.PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Shevell M, Ashwal S, Donley D, Flint J, Gingold M, Hirtz D, et al. Practice parameter: evaluation of the child with global developmental delay [RETIRED]. Rep Qual Standards Subcommittee Am Acad Neurol Pract Committee Child Neurol Soc. 2003;60(3):367–80. Shevell M, Ashwal S, Donley D, Flint J, Gingold M, Hirtz D, et al. Practice parameter: evaluation of the child with global developmental delay [RETIRED]. Rep Qual Standards Subcommittee Am Acad Neurol Pract Committee Child Neurol Soc. 2003;60(3):367–80.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim SW, Jeon HR, Jung HJ, Kim JA, Song JE, Kim J. Clinical characteristics of developmentally delayed children based on interdisciplinary evaluation. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):8148.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kim SW, Jeon HR, Jung HJ, Kim JA, Song JE, Kim J. Clinical characteristics of developmentally delayed children based on interdisciplinary evaluation. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):8148.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Sun X, Allison C, Wei L, Matthews FE, Auyeung B, Wu YY, et al. Autism prevalence in China is comparable to Western prevalence. Mol Autism. 2019;10:7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Sun X, Allison C, Wei L, Matthews FE, Auyeung B, Wu YY, et al. Autism prevalence in China is comparable to Western prevalence. Mol Autism. 2019;10:7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Sun XAC, Matthews FE, Sharp SJ, Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Brayne C. PrevalenceofautisminmainlandChina,HongKongandTaiwan:asystematicreviewandmeta-analysis. Mol Autism. 2013;4(1):7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Sun XAC, Matthews FE, Sharp SJ, Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Brayne C. PrevalenceofautisminmainlandChina,HongKongandTaiwan:asystematicreviewandmeta-analysis. Mol Autism. 2013;4(1):7.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Belanger SA, Caron J. Evaluation of the child with global developmental delay and intellectual disability. Paediatr Child Health. 2018;23(6):403–19.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Belanger SA, Caron J. Evaluation of the child with global developmental delay and intellectual disability. Paediatr Child Health. 2018;23(6):403–19.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Dawson G, Bernier R. A quarter century of progress on the early detection and treatment of autism spectrum disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25(4 Pt 2):1455–72.PubMedCrossRef Dawson G, Bernier R. A quarter century of progress on the early detection and treatment of autism spectrum disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25(4 Pt 2):1455–72.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Gong X, Li X, Wang Q, Hoi SP, Yin T, Zhao L, et al. Comparing visual preferences between autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and normal children to explore the characteristics of visual preference of ASD children by improved visual preference paradigm: a case-control study. Transl Pediatr. 2021;10(8):2006–15.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Gong X, Li X, Wang Q, Hoi SP, Yin T, Zhao L, et al. Comparing visual preferences between autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and normal children to explore the characteristics of visual preference of ASD children by improved visual preference paradigm: a case-control study. Transl Pediatr. 2021;10(8):2006–15.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Wetherby AMWJ, Allen L, Cleary J, Dickinson H, Lord C. Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34(5):473–93.CrossRef Wetherby AMWJ, Allen L, Cleary J, Dickinson H, Lord C. Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34(5):473–93.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Thiemann-Bourque KS, Brady NC, Fleming KK. Symbolic play of preschoolers with severe communication impairments with autism and other developmental delays: more similarities than differences. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(5):863–73.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Thiemann-Bourque KS, Brady NC, Fleming KK. Symbolic play of preschoolers with severe communication impairments with autism and other developmental delays: more similarities than differences. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(5):863–73.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Hobson JA, Hobson RP, Malik S, Bargiota K, Caló S. The relation between social engagement and pretend play in autism. Br J Dev Psychol. 2013;31(1):114–27.PubMedCrossRef Hobson JA, Hobson RP, Malik S, Bargiota K, Caló S. The relation between social engagement and pretend play in autism. Br J Dev Psychol. 2013;31(1):114–27.PubMedCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Hobson JA, Hobson RP, Cheung Y, Caló S. Symbolizing as interpersonally grounded shifts in meaning: social play in children with and without autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(1):42–52.PubMedCrossRef Hobson JA, Hobson RP, Cheung Y, Caló S. Symbolizing as interpersonally grounded shifts in meaning: social play in children with and without autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(1):42–52.PubMedCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Bentenuto A, De Falco S, Venuti P. Mother-child play: a comparison of autism Spectrum disorder, Down syndrome, and typical development. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1829.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bentenuto A, De Falco S, Venuti P. Mother-child play: a comparison of autism Spectrum disorder, Down syndrome, and typical development. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1829.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat US APAD-TFAV. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5™ (5th ed.). Codas. 2013;25(2):191. US APAD-TFAV. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5™ (5th ed.). Codas. 2013;25(2):191.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Li HH, Feng JY, Wang B, Zhang Y, Wang CX, Jia FY. Comparison of the children neuropsychological and behavior scale and the Griffiths mental development scales when assessing the development of children with autism. Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2019;12:973–81.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Li HH, Feng JY, Wang B, Zhang Y, Wang CX, Jia FY. Comparison of the children neuropsychological and behavior scale and the Griffiths mental development scales when assessing the development of children with autism. Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2019;12:973–81.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Liu XM. A clinical framework for the comprehensive evaluation and differential diagnosis of language delay language disorders and other childhood developmental disorders. Zhonghua Er Ke Za Zhi. 2021;59(11):901–4.PubMed Liu XM. A clinical framework for the comprehensive evaluation and differential diagnosis of language delay language disorders and other childhood developmental disorders. Zhonghua Er Ke Za Zhi. 2021;59(11):901–4.PubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Bishop DVM, Snowling MJ, Thompson PA, Greenhalgh T, the C-c. Phase 2 of CATALISE: a multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: terminology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58(10):1068–80.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bishop DVM, Snowling MJ, Thompson PA, Greenhalgh T, the C-c. Phase 2 of CATALISE: a multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: terminology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58(10):1068–80.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Lord CRM, DiLavore PC, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism diagnostic observation schedule, second edition (ADOS-2) manual (part I): modules 1–4. Torrance: Western Psychological Services; 2012. Lord CRM, DiLavore PC, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism diagnostic observation schedule, second edition (ADOS-2) manual (part I): modules 1–4. Torrance: Western Psychological Services; 2012.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Grzadzinski R, Amso D, Landa R, Watson L, Guralnick M, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. Pre-symptomatic intervention for autism spectrum disorder (ASD): defining a research agenda. J Neurodev Disord. 2021;13(1):49.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Grzadzinski R, Amso D, Landa R, Watson L, Guralnick M, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. Pre-symptomatic intervention for autism spectrum disorder (ASD): defining a research agenda. J Neurodev Disord. 2021;13(1):49.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Daniels AM, Mandell DS. Explaining differences in age at autism spectrum disorder diagnosis: a critical review. Autism. 2014;18(5):583–97.PubMedCrossRef Daniels AM, Mandell DS. Explaining differences in age at autism spectrum disorder diagnosis: a critical review. Autism. 2014;18(5):583–97.PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Cholewicki JDE, Chezan LC. Parental perception of quality of life for children with autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Phys Disabil. 2019;31:575–92.CrossRef Cholewicki JDE, Chezan LC. Parental perception of quality of life for children with autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Phys Disabil. 2019;31:575–92.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Li C, Xu X. Early screening and diagnosis for ASD children in China. Pediatr Med. 2019;2:35. Li C, Xu X. Early screening and diagnosis for ASD children in China. Pediatr Med. 2019;2:35.
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Au YLE, Ma KM, Sy WM, Lee WC, Leung LSJ, Au Yeung YC, et al. Use of developmental language scales in Chinese children. Brain Dev. 2004;26(2):127–9.PubMedCrossRef Au YLE, Ma KM, Sy WM, Lee WC, Leung LSJ, Au Yeung YC, et al. Use of developmental language scales in Chinese children. Brain Dev. 2004;26(2):127–9.PubMedCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Lee GT, Xu S, Guo S, Gilic L, Pu Y, Xu J. Teaching “imaginary objects” symbolic play to young children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49(10):4109–22.PubMedCrossRef Lee GT, Xu S, Guo S, Gilic L, Pu Y, Xu J. Teaching “imaginary objects” symbolic play to young children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019;49(10):4109–22.PubMedCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Pedersen AL, Pettygrove S, Lu Z, Andrews J, Meaney FJ, Kurzius-Spencer M, et al. DSM criteria that best differentiate intellectual disability from autism spectrum disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2017;48(4):537–45.PubMedCrossRef Pedersen AL, Pettygrove S, Lu Z, Andrews J, Meaney FJ, Kurzius-Spencer M, et al. DSM criteria that best differentiate intellectual disability from autism spectrum disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2017;48(4):537–45.PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Quinn S, Kidd E. Symbolic play promotes non-verbal communicative exchange in infant–caregiver dyads. Br J Dev Psychol. 2019;37(1):33–50.PubMedCrossRef Quinn S, Kidd E. Symbolic play promotes non-verbal communicative exchange in infant–caregiver dyads. Br J Dev Psychol. 2019;37(1):33–50.PubMedCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Wetherby AM, Woods J, Allen L, Cleary J, Dickinson H, Lord C. Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34(5):473–93.PubMedCrossRef Wetherby AM, Woods J, Allen L, Cleary J, Dickinson H, Lord C. Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34(5):473–93.PubMedCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Jung S, Sainato DM. Teaching play skills to young children with autism. J Intellect Develop Disabil. 2013;38(1):74–90.CrossRef Jung S, Sainato DM. Teaching play skills to young children with autism. J Intellect Develop Disabil. 2013;38(1):74–90.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Stanley GC, Konstantareas MM. Symbolic play in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(7):1215–23.PubMedCrossRef Stanley GC, Konstantareas MM. Symbolic play in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(7):1215–23.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Application of symbolic play test in identification of autism spectrum disorder without global developmental delay and developmental language disorder
verfasst von
Xuening Chang
Lingli Sun
Ruizhen Li
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2023
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2023
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-04647-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2023

BMC Psychiatry 1/2023 Zur Ausgabe

„Übersichtlicher Wegweiser“: Lauterbachs umstrittener Klinik-Atlas ist online

17.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Sie sei „ethisch geboten“, meint Gesundheitsminister Karl Lauterbach: mehr Transparenz über die Qualität von Klinikbehandlungen. Um sie abzubilden, lässt er gegen den Widerstand vieler Länder einen virtuellen Klinik-Atlas freischalten.

ADHS-Medikation erhöht das kardiovaskuläre Risiko

16.05.2024 Herzinsuffizienz Nachrichten

Erwachsene, die Medikamente gegen das Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-Hyperaktivitätssyndrom einnehmen, laufen offenbar erhöhte Gefahr, an Herzschwäche zu erkranken oder einen Schlaganfall zu erleiden. Es scheint eine Dosis-Wirkungs-Beziehung zu bestehen.

Klinikreform soll zehntausende Menschenleben retten

15.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Gesundheitsminister Lauterbach hat die vom Bundeskabinett beschlossene Klinikreform verteidigt. Kritik an den Plänen kommt vom Marburger Bund. Und in den Ländern wird über den Gang zum Vermittlungsausschuss spekuliert.

Typ-2-Diabetes und Depression folgen oft aufeinander

14.05.2024 Typ-2-Diabetes Nachrichten

Menschen mit Typ-2-Diabetes sind überdurchschnittlich gefährdet, in den nächsten Jahren auch noch eine Depression zu entwickeln – und umgekehrt. Besonders ausgeprägt ist die Wechselbeziehung laut GKV-Daten bei jüngeren Erwachsenen.