Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Current Nutrition Reports 2/2017

Open Access 29.04.2017 | Food Acceptance and Nutrition in Infants and Young Children (H Coulthard, Section Editor)

Are There Sensitive Periods for Food Acceptance in Infancy?

verfasst von: Gillian Harris, Sarah Mason

Erschienen in: Current Nutrition Reports | Ausgabe 2/2017

Abstract

Purpose of Review

A sensitive period in development is one in which it is easier for learning to take place; the behaviour can however still be learned at a later stage, but with more difficulty. This is in contrast to a critical period, a time at which a behaviour must be learned, and if this window of opportunity is missed, then the behaviour can never be acquired. Both might determine food acceptance in childhood.

Recent Findings

There is evidence to support the idea of a sensitive period for the introduction of tastes, a critical period for the introduction of textures and for the development of oral motor function, and a possible critical period for the introduction of new foods but only in children where there is an innate disposition to develop early and extreme disgust responses.

Summary

There are both sensitive and critical periods in the acquisition of food preferences.
Hinweise
This article is part of the Topical Collection on Food Acceptance and Nutrition in Infants and Young Children

Introduction: Sensitive Periods and Critical Periods in Development

Firstly, what do we mean by a ‘sensitive period’, and how does this differ from a critical period in development? A sensitive period is a limited period during which the brain is particularly receptive to the effects of experience. Such periods ‘allow experience to instruct neural circuits to process or represent information in a way that is adaptive for the individual’ [1 p91]. After a sensitive period, learning may still occur but ‘extra energy’ is required for this learning to take place [1]. When this period provides experience essential for normal development ‘which alters performance permanently’, then this is described as a critical period [2], and the brain then becomes resistant to subsequent change [1]. However, there is now some evidence that even processes not attained during a critical period can be attained in adulthood [3]. Critical and sensitive periods both need an age of onset and an age or stage at which learning can no longer occur or occur with such ease.
We must also ask ourselves what might be the function of a sensitive, or even a critical period, for development in infancy, and the relevance of these stages to food acceptance. Given that all mammals must be open to learning about changes in their environment and changes in their role within a social group as they age, why would a crucial period for learning new skills be of advantage to a species? The answer must lie in the way in which the brain develops, with neural networks set up in the young to facilitate necessary learned behaviours as other unnecessary pathways die off [3]. However, there are very few critical periods for learning in infancy in which this occurs; the most cited one of these is within the visual system, that of acquired stereoscopic vision [4].
Critical periods are not seemingly advantageous and relate to the manner in which neural circuits develop, but why might sensitive periods be advantageous? This is because there are crucial periods during which the infant interacts with the environment; this interaction is determined by intrinsic factors, and the infant is primed to learn about stimuli that will facilitate development and contribute to subsequent survival. Learning about which foods to eat might therefore be subject to the ‘sensitive’ attribute. As human infants progressively need to eat food, then they need to learn very quickly which foods are safe, which foods are eaten by those around them, and their taste preferences need to be modified accordingly. Food acceptance per se cannot be hard wired given the wide range of environments inhabited by the human race and the wide range of foods which provide the diet of different cultural groups. In the beginning, all food preferences need to be learned. The presence of the timed ‘off switch’, which we will see does occur but not until late infancy, might be advantageous in that it directs the infant away from non-foods, once the initial learning has taken place and the infant is able to forage. This ‘off switch’ may, however, not always herald the end of availability of appropriate neural pathways but rather the presence of competing behaviours. Early research studies looking at isolation studies carried out on macaques seemed to show that there were critical periods during which social interaction skills had to develop. However, subsequent research [5] demonstrated that it was not that the infant macaque could not interact with age mates when introduced to them after periods of isolation, but that other fear behaviours interfered to prevent this interaction. So it could be that the limiter in our possible critical period is a fear response and that with ‘extra effort’, this response might be overcome.
There are three points of development and domains at which this easy acceptance or later refusal might occur in relation to feeding and eating behaviour: taste and smell, tactile stimulation and texture, and visual appearance. The mechanisms which are needed to sense taste and smell are more or less completely developed by birth and so acceptance post-natally in each of these domains [6] is related to exposure, with additional learned oral motor skills, and the development of the distaste-disgust response.

Taste

Infants are exposed to flavours in utero and via breast milk. However, the flavours to which the infant is exposed by these routes can vary widely from mother to mother and from feed to feed [7, 8], and although they might slightly prime the infant for subsequent acceptance of new foods, the advantages which they confer do not outweigh the mere introduction of tastes during the period of the introduction of complementary foods [9].
The infant is born with a preference for a sweet taste, and this may well be beneficial in that it directs the infant to ingest foods that might be a source of energy, and to reject water. The taste of bitter, however, is rejected at birth [10, 11] because of the possible association between a bitter taste and toxicity. All other taste preferences are subsequently learned through exposure [9, 10]. Early work carried out on the acceptance of tastes in food showed that there was an easy acceptance for a salt taste if this was added to a tasteless rice base and that this acceptance was dependent upon number of exposures rather than the amount eaten [12, 13]. A study carried out on a small sample of infants tested (15–26 weeks), for their preference for a tastant (salt) in the first food fed to them, cereal [14], additionally showed that intake for the food with the added tastant was significantly better for the younger infants (16–17 versus 18–25 weeks). However, Johnson [15] looked at the acceptance of tastants added to rice-based cereal. Infants were given a teaspoonful of a different tastant for 14 days in the first food fed to them (mean infant age 14 weeks). There was a clear and fast exposure effect for sour and salted rice; infants learned to like these flavours quickly and easily. However, by chance, the infants allocated to the condition in which they were given a bitter tastant added to cereal were slightly older (mean age 17 weeks) than were those exposed to the salt and sour tastes. For those infants given bitter tasting cereal, there was no exposure effect; those infants fed the bitter tasting cereal preferred the taste-free rice-based cereal. This effect might have been due to the bitter taste or due to the slightly later age of introduction, an effect noticed in infants fed bitter tasting (proteinhydrolysate) formulae [16]. Infants younger than 3–4 months would readily accept strong tasting bitter formulae; however, infants over 5–6 months of age refused them. In contrast, infants who had had experience with the taste prior to 5–6 months would accept the strong tasting formulae at the later age [17]. This easy acceptance of a bitter tasting formula after early exposure continues into later childhood [18]. The taste preference learning was also exactly specific to the formula experienced [19]. Therefore, what was learned in this early sensitive period was that flavours initially linked with possible toxicity, if paired with good nutritional outcome, could be accepted. Anecdotally, many parents report that it is difficult to change their infant from any formula in later infancy once they have learned to accept a specific formula taste in early infancy.
There was some generalization noted from this early acceptance of a bitter tasting formula to the acceptance of bitter tasting food. The infants given bitter hydrolysates preferred sour-flavoured juices at 4–5 years [20]. But such taste preferences do not seem to generalize from liquid to solid food. Infants fed protein hydrolysates did not prefer broccoli, which is bitter, to carrots which are sweet [21].

First Foods

The early and easy acceptance of all new foods, rather than mere tastes, at a slightly younger age in the period of the introduction of complementary foods (4–6 months) has been observed in many studies [22••, 23]. It has been shown that the earlier fruit and vegetables were introduced in the infant’s diet, the better their acceptance, both in infancy and at a later age in childhood [2430]. This possibly reflects the difficult nature in terms of texture and tastes shared by many fruit and vegetables, both properties which children find aversive [6, 31••]. It would seem therefore that early experience with both difficult tastes and textures has a facilitating effect. However, it should also be remembered that those introduced early, and to a variety of fruit and vegetables, probably live in an environment which generally facilitates the acceptance of a wider diet.
There is also a generalization effect with all new foods and tastes. The more new tastes that are introduced early (4–5 months as opposed to 6 months) then the better the acceptance of a new food [32]. In addition, those infants fed home prepared foods possibly featuring a wider range of taste and textures were more likely to eat a range of fruit and vegetables at 7 years, than were those fed commercially produced foods [33]. Difficult tastes are therefore not only better accepted if introduced earlier to the infant but also if they are introduced frequently [34]. It may also be advantageous if the infant experiences a continuation from taste to texture to appearance; that is they learn to recognize the food in all of its different presentations, as may well be the case with home prepared rather than with commercially produced introductory foods.
Two studies of interest which add to this literature have been carried out with premature infants. It was observed that premature infants not only thrive better with the earlier introduction of complementary foods [22••, 35] but also more readily accepted novel foods than did age matched full-term infants (5–6 months) [22••]. In this latter study, the mean gestational age of the premature infants was 30.6 weeks and that of the term infants was 39.1 weeks; at the point of testing, the age of the premature infants was not corrected for gestational age. The authors hypothesized that the premature infants would display more negative emotion when exposed to novel foods than would the full-term infants, but analysis of facial expression in response to the introduction of new foods showed a more positive acceptance by the pre-term infants. The pre-term infants were essentially 2 months younger than were the full-term infants.
Although some new tastes quite often need more than one exposure, especially if the taste has not been experienced before [36], acceptance does occur, in general, quite easily and seemingly faster at a younger age [22••] when the infant is offered frequent small tastes of the food eaten by those around them. The acceptance of a bitter taste is, however, more problematic; this is a taste often associated with toxicity and is therefore a taste to which it is appropriate for the infant to react and possibly subsequently reject. However, if the taste is associated in very early infancy with the only source of good nutrition, then this taste will be accepted at the earlier age, but not at the later age when other sources of good nutrition have been experienced. To a great extent, then the acceptance of food tastes is easier at an earlier age but not totally dependent upon developmental stage. Tastes are learned in context and gradually associated with foods and food textures [37] which become increasing more complex. Unless the infant (and child) has innate taste aversions, such as an extreme response to bitter [6, 38], then tastes can be accepted and modified throughout life [39]. However, in an adult, the motivation to try a new taste may be the defining characteristic determining whether or not that taste or taste modification is accepted. And even in some children who have had foods withheld in the first year of life because of infantile milk allergies, such foods may well be refused in later childhood [40•].
One further innate characteristic also acts as a limiter on how many tastes an infant or child is willing to try, that of sensory hypersensitivity, a trait linked to both food ‘fussiness’, and a refusal to try new foods [41, 42]. One study has recently shown that the age of introduction of new foods in the 4–6-month introductory period is more salient for those infants rated higher on tactile hypersensitivity. Hypersensitive infants introduced to foods earlier within the introductory period were more likely to accept a new food than were infants introduced later (4–5 months rather than 5–6 months) [43].

Accepting Texture and Learning the Appropriate Oral Motor Skills

The concept of a sensitive period for the introduction of solid food was first suggested by Illingworth [44 p840,] based on case studies of hospitalized infants.
‘Children should be given solids to chew at a time when they are developmentally ready: in an average child, this age is 6 to 7 months. If they are not given solids then (as distinct from thickened feedings, which can be given any time after birth), they are very apt to be difficult about taking them later, failing to chew, refusing the solids, or vomiting. Failure to give solids may be due to mismanagement in normal children, including over anxiety about their choking’.
There is evidence to support this suggestion that the early introduction of textures, as soon as possible after 6 months and certainly before 12 months, is essential.
A new born infant is born with a disposition to ingest, with sucking being the earliest means of receiving nutrition [45]. Early nutritive sucking comprises forward and backward movements of the tongue and is supported by both physiological and anatomical factors: rooting and sucking reflexes, the lips, tongue, and jaw acting as a single unit; the size of the oral cavity; sucking pads; and proximity of oral and laryngeal structures [4547]. It is generally reported that premature infants cannot suck nutritively until the age of 34 weeks gestation but non-nutritive sucking is reported earlier [46, 48]. The difference between nutritive and non-nutritive sucking is that nutritive sucking requires the coordination of bursts of suck-swallow-breathe cycles whereas non-nutritive sucking is a burst of sucking and breathing with intermittent swallows. Reviews of the literature on non-nutritive sucking have shown that the promotion of non-nutritive sucking in premature babies who require tube feeding, usually by the use of pacifiers, leads to a quicker transition to oral feeding and shorter stays in neonatal units [4951]. While the benefits of non-nutritive sucking on the premature population are well researched, it is less clear whether these benefits continue in older infants who are unable to take nutrition orally during the early months of life. Mizano and Ueda [52] noted that, in their experience, once a baby reached 6 months or more, it was very difficult to establish bottle feeding for the first time.
One factor that may be involved in the difficulty of introducing breast or bottle feeding after an interval of non-oral feeding is that the infant reflexes of rooting and sucking diminish over the first 4 months of life, an example of a critical period of learning. At first, sucking will be triggered automatically by any sort of stimulation of the lips and tongue but gradually, this reflex response comes under voluntary control. Morris and Klein [47] described how the baby develops separation of movement that enables the jaw, lips, and tongue to move separately and thus perform more complex oral motor tasks, an extension to the reflex suck and swallow.
The attainment of early oral motor skills needed to process more solid foods around the mouth shows a wide diversity across individuals. Carruth and Skinner [53] found that the mean age at which infants used the tongue to move food to the back of the tongue to swallow was 4.95 months, with a standard deviation of 1.27 and a range of 2.0–7.5 months. This variation may reflect normal differences in the development of the central nervous system and opportunities the infant has to practice skills [53, 54]. Weaning guidelines currently recommend that spoon feeding should not be commenced before 17 weeks of age but infants started taking solids from a spoon at this earlier age in previous decades [55], so the oral motor skills required to eat from a spoon precede the need for their use.
The late introduction of textured solids that is foods that need to be processed with a side to side tongue movement is associated with poorer acceptance of these foods, and it has been suggested that there is at least a sensitive period for the acquisition of chewing skills [44]. Northstone, Emmett, and Nethersole [56] found that the introduction of lumpy foods at or later than10 months was associated with more feeding difficulties at 15 months of age. These feeding difficulties and a poorer acceptance of fruit and vegetables (foods of complex texture) were also observed in these children at the later age of 7 years [57]. The studies were, however, reported observations and other factors could have contributed to the later feeding difficulties.
Mature chewing is a complex combination of movements of the lips, tongue, and jaw, and fully mature chewing can take several years to develop [58, 59]. It begins with a mixture of sucking, biting, and up/down ‘munching’ movements, characterized by regular patterns of reciprocal activation between antagonistic muscle pairs [58]. At the same time, the tongue begins to move laterally, from centre to side. Wickendon [48] described how this munching enables a baby to manage foods such as infant rusks, breadsticks, and toast, from about 6–7 months. A study of the development of chewing in children aged 6 months to 2 years found that while chewing efficiency increased over the entire period between 6 months and 2 years, the most marked changes occurred between 6 and 10 months [60]. It has been reported that infants as young as 7 months were able to adjust their jaw movements in response to the different consistencies of food [58, 59] and that the best predictor of acceptance of chopped carrots in 12-month-old infants was shown to be the infant’s previous experience with carrot pieces [61].
The poor acceptance of textured foods by children who are introduced to them after the first year of life would appear to have at least two components: firstly, a refusal to try unfamiliar foods as the child moves into the neophobic stage of food refusal in the second year of life and, secondly, an inability to manage the texture because chewing skills have not been acquired through practice [62]. Most researchers [63] agree that movement patterns, such as lateral tongue movements, are texture dependent and therefore do not emerge unless the child is given the particular textures requiring these skills.
Another component in the acceptance of textured foods is that the side to side processing of food within the mouth serves to desensitize the inner sides of the cheeks. Children who are introduced later than usual to textured foods often attempt to process these foods using a ‘liquid swallow’, in which foods are not processed at the sides of the mouth but are swallowed directly over the back of the tongue. Lumpy textured food which lands onto the back of the tongue provokes a gag and retch reflex, which is aversive to the child. In addition to this, children who do not process foods with a side to side tongue movement which positions food appropriately for processing become hypersensitive to any tactile stimulation and therefore find the experience of foods at the sides of the mouth aversive [64, 65]. Parents may also respond to this gag response by further delaying the introduction of solid-textured foods [66]. Sensory hypersensitivity may therefore, once again, be a factor in the salience of the ‘sensitive’ period, this time in the introduction of textured foods. Sensory-hypersensitive children are reluctant to allow food to go into the sides of the mouth and also reluctant to process the more difficult textures. Higher tactile sensitivity is particularly associated with rejection of foods of difficult texture [41, 65].

Appearance: the Neophobic Stage

Infants learn to accept the taste and then the texture of foods that are fed to them and that they learn to perceive as ‘safe’ foods. However, before they get to the stage of foraging, in their second year, they need to know what their safe foods look like and need to attend quite closely to the detail of the appearance of that food.
The neophobic stage, as it is defined for humans, describes a response of refusing food on sight [66]. The way that the food looks or the way in which the food is packaged predicts the safety of the food, and any deviation in the smallest detail will lead to this rejection [67]. Even foods that have been accepted prior to the onset of the neophobic stage might be refused if their appearance changes on subsequent presentations [68]. This stage is described as starting at around 20 months [69] and continuing until around 6 years [70], by which time the response has gradually diminished. The strength of the neophobic response is also related to the child’s degree of sensory hypersensitivity [41] and to some extent genetically determined [71, 72]. The onset of this stage also occurs at the time at which infants are showing contamination fears and therefore an early form of disgust [7375]. A food that is liked might be refused if touched by a food that is disliked. Although it is relatively easy to get acceptance of a new taste or new food when that food has not previously been experienced, where there is motivation to try the food and where others are modelling the food [7678], once a food has been deemed disgusting it would seem that it is almost impossible to get that food accepted into the diet [79, 80]. Even where new foods are tried, it takes more exposures to gain an acceptance of those foods in later infancy and childhood than it does in early infancy [22••, 81].
Those children who are sensory hypersensitive are more likely to show strong neophobic responses and extreme disgust responses. In the extreme, some children form a strong disgust response to all new foods or variants of known foods [82]. In these children, the fear of new foods can remain until adulthood; new foods can evoke a disgust and fear response and texture refusal can worsen as the child becomes more orally defensive [64, 83].
This may be an example, not of a critical period however, but of interfering or competing behaviours. The formation of a disgust response towards foods that have not been accepted during the neophobic stage, perhaps necessary in early humans to prevent the ingestion of non-foods, blocks learning about new food tastes and textures. When no disgust or fear response has formed, then older children and adults can be motivated to try new foods that they deem safe to eat. The defining factor here is the sensory hypersensitivity of the child; the experience of the food interacts with the innate disposition of the child to produce an extreme fear response and it is not that neural pathways can no longer form to promote food acceptance.

Summary

It is easier to get new tastes accepted from birth and new complex flavours accepted in the early months of the introduction of complementary foods, between 4 and 6 months. However, acceptance of new tastes can be learned throughout life if the older child or adult is motivated to accept the repeated tastings that are required, not only for acceptance but also for pleasurable anticipation of the taste. For some of course, those who are genetically programmed to dislike certain tastes such as bitter [38] or those who are sensory hypersensitive [84], then simply getting older will not change acceptance; there will be no increased motivation to try. The factor of motivation is, however, an important one. Early childhood food preferences and experience predict later childhood and adult food preferences and dietary range [33, 85, 86]. So the sensitive period here is one of setting up habitual patterns of eating which may or may not be subject to later lifetime change. Of greater significance is that of acceptance of texture and the oral motor skills which develop in response to tactile stimulation, and this does have features of a sensitive, or even critical, period with a clear ‘off switch’. The response to texture may well be a function of innate differences, in that sensory-hypersensitive infants are less likely to accept different textures with ease [65, 87], but it can also be delayed by lack of early experience. Those infants who have not experienced textured foods in the first year of life find such foods difficult to accept and to process within the mouth. Similarly, in some, the development of the disgust response to specific foods is also a heightened innate response. In others, however, it could be due to a lack of learning via exposure [40•] in that foods that might be seen to be inherently disgusting in the toddler period could well have been accepted had they been introduced in the early period of complementary food introduction. One example of this response is that of seal blubber acceptance in the Inuit child, a food which would not be accepted or even approached without an extreme disgust response in those reared in other environments. The onset of disgust in the second year of life does therefore have features of a critical period of food acceptance, as well as that of the acceptance of textured foods and the development of oral motor skills.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

Gillian Harris is a member of Infant and Toddler Forum funded by an educational grant from Danone and is an occasional event speaker for Danone/Nutricia.
Sarah Mason declares that she has no conflict of interest.

Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent

This article does not contain any studies with human or animal subjects performed by any of the authors.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Innere Medizin

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Innere Medizin erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Innere Medizin, den Premium-Inhalten der internistischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten internistischen Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Allgemeinmedizin

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Allgemeinmedizin erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Premium-Inhalten der allgemeinmedizinischen Zeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Allgemeinmedizin-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Burgren WW, Mueller CA. Developmental critical windows and sensitive periods as three-dimensional constructs in time and space. Physiol Biochem Zool. 2015;88(2):91–102. doi:10.1086/679906.CrossRef Burgren WW, Mueller CA. Developmental critical windows and sensitive periods as three-dimensional constructs in time and space. Physiol Biochem Zool. 2015;88(2):91–102. doi:10.​1086/​679906.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Knudsen E. Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(8):1412–25.CrossRefPubMed Knudsen E. Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(8):1412–25.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Hartley C, Lee F. Sensitive periods in affective development: nonlinear maturation of fear learning. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:50–60.CrossRefPubMed Hartley C, Lee F. Sensitive periods in affective development: nonlinear maturation of fear learning. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:50–60.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Crawford MLJ, Harwerth RS, Smith EL, von Noorden GK. Loss of stereopsis in monkeys following prismatic binocular dissociation during infancy. Behav Brain Res. 1996;79:207–18.CrossRefPubMed Crawford MLJ, Harwerth RS, Smith EL, von Noorden GK. Loss of stereopsis in monkeys following prismatic binocular dissociation during infancy. Behav Brain Res. 1996;79:207–18.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Novak MA, Harlow HF. Social recovery of monkeys isolated for the first year of life: I. Rehabilitation and therapy. Dev Psychol. 1975;11(4):453–65.CrossRef Novak MA, Harlow HF. Social recovery of monkeys isolated for the first year of life: I. Rehabilitation and therapy. Dev Psychol. 1975;11(4):453–65.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Ventura AK, Woroby J. Early influences on the development of food review preferences. Curr Biol. 2013;23:R401–8.CrossRefPubMed Ventura AK, Woroby J. Early influences on the development of food review preferences. Curr Biol. 2013;23:R401–8.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Hausner H, Bredie W, Mølgaard C, Petersen MA, Møller P. Differential transfer of dietary flavour compounds into human breast milk. PhysBeh. 2008;95:118–24. Hausner H, Bredie W, Mølgaard C, Petersen MA, Møller P. Differential transfer of dietary flavour compounds into human breast milk. PhysBeh. 2008;95:118–24.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. The effects of repeated exposure to garlic flavored milk on the nursling’s behavior. Pediatrics Research. 1993;34(6):805–8.CrossRef Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. The effects of repeated exposure to garlic flavored milk on the nursling’s behavior. Pediatrics Research. 1993;34(6):805–8.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenstein D, Oster H. Differential facial responses to 4 basic tastes in newborns. Child Dev. 1988;59:1555–68.CrossRefPubMed Rosenstein D, Oster H. Differential facial responses to 4 basic tastes in newborns. Child Dev. 1988;59:1555–68.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Crook CK. Taste perception in the newborn infant. Infant Behav Dev. 1978;1:52–69.CrossRef Crook CK. Taste perception in the newborn infant. Infant Behav Dev. 1978;1:52–69.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Harris G, Booth DA. Sodium preference in food and previous dietary experience in six-month-old infants. IRCS Medical Science. 1985;13:1177–8. Harris G, Booth DA. Sodium preference in food and previous dietary experience in six-month-old infants. IRCS Medical Science. 1985;13:1177–8.
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Harris G, Booth DA. Infants’ preference for salt in food: its dependence upon recent dietary experience. Journal of Infant and Reproductive Psychology. 1987;5:97–104.CrossRef Harris G, Booth DA. Infants’ preference for salt in food: its dependence upon recent dietary experience. Journal of Infant and Reproductive Psychology. 1987;5:97–104.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Harris G, Thomas A, Booth DA. Development of salt taste in infancy. Dev Psychol. 1990;26(4):534–8.CrossRef Harris G, Thomas A, Booth DA. Development of salt taste in infancy. Dev Psychol. 1990;26(4):534–8.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson R (unpublished thesis) University of Birmingham, School of Psychology 1999. Johnson R (unpublished thesis) University of Birmingham, School of Psychology 1999.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Menella JA, Griffin C, Beauchamp GK. Flavor programming during infancy. Pediatrics. 2004;113(4):840–5.CrossRef Menella JA, Griffin C, Beauchamp GK. Flavor programming during infancy. Pediatrics. 2004;113(4):840–5.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. Developmental changes in the acceptance of protein hydrolysate. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1996;17:386–91.CrossRefPubMed Mennella JA, Beauchamp GK. Developmental changes in the acceptance of protein hydrolysate. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1996;17:386–91.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Sausenthaler S, Koletzko S, Koletzko B, Reinhardt D, Kramer U, Berg A, Berdel D, Bauer C, Grubl A, Wichmann HE, Heinrich J. Effect of hydrolysed formula feeding on taste preferences at 10years. Data from the German infant nutritional intervention program plus study. Clin Nutr. 2010;29:304–6.CrossRefPubMed Sausenthaler S, Koletzko S, Koletzko B, Reinhardt D, Kramer U, Berg A, Berdel D, Bauer C, Grubl A, Wichmann HE, Heinrich J. Effect of hydrolysed formula feeding on taste preferences at 10years. Data from the German infant nutritional intervention program plus study. Clin Nutr. 2010;29:304–6.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Menella JA, Beauchamp GK. Flavor experiences during formula feeding are related to preferences during childhood. Pediatrics. 2002;68(2):71–82. Menella JA, Beauchamp GK. Flavor experiences during formula feeding are related to preferences during childhood. Pediatrics. 2002;68(2):71–82.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Menella JA, Kennedy JM, Beauchamp GK. Vegetable acceptance by infants: effects of formula flavors. Early Hum Devel. 2006;82(7):463–8.CrossRef Menella JA, Kennedy JM, Beauchamp GK. Vegetable acceptance by infants: effects of formula flavors. Early Hum Devel. 2006;82(7):463–8.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat •• Longfier L, Soussignan R, Reissland N, Leconte M, Marret S, Schaal B, Mellier D. Emotional expressiveness of 5-6 month-old infants born very premature versus full-term at initial exposure to weaning foods. Appetite. 2016;107:494–500. Although it does not set out to support the idea of a sensitive period, this study, because there was no correction for age in the premature infants, seems to do just that. CrossRefPubMed •• Longfier L, Soussignan R, Reissland N, Leconte M, Marret S, Schaal B, Mellier D. Emotional expressiveness of 5-6 month-old infants born very premature versus full-term at initial exposure to weaning foods. Appetite. 2016;107:494–500. Although it does not set out to support the idea of a sensitive period, this study, because there was no correction for age in the premature infants, seems to do just that. CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Nicklaus S. Complementary feeding strategies to facilitate acceptance of fruits and vegetables: a narrative review of the literature. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2016;13:1160.CrossRefPubMedCentral Nicklaus S. Complementary feeding strategies to facilitate acceptance of fruits and vegetables: a narrative review of the literature. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2016;13:1160.CrossRefPubMedCentral
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Lange C, Visalli M, Jacob S, Chabanet C, Schlich P, Nicklaus S. Maternal feeding practices during thefirst year and their impact on infants’ acceptance of complementary food. Food Qual Prefer. 2013;29:89–98.CrossRef Lange C, Visalli M, Jacob S, Chabanet C, Schlich P, Nicklaus S. Maternal feeding practices during thefirst year and their impact on infants’ acceptance of complementary food. Food Qual Prefer. 2013;29:89–98.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Cooke LJ, Wardle J, Gibson EL, Sapochnik M, Sheiham A, Lawson M. Demographic, familialand trait predictors of fruit and vegetable consumption by pre-school children. Public Health Nutr. 2004;7:295–302.CrossRefPubMed Cooke LJ, Wardle J, Gibson EL, Sapochnik M, Sheiham A, Lawson M. Demographic, familialand trait predictors of fruit and vegetable consumption by pre-school children. Public Health Nutr. 2004;7:295–302.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Bounds W, Ziegler P, Reidy K. Do food-related experiences in the first 2 years of life predict dietary variety in school-aged children? J Nutr Educ Behav. 2002;34:310–5.CrossRefPubMed Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Bounds W, Ziegler P, Reidy K. Do food-related experiences in the first 2 years of life predict dietary variety in school-aged children? J Nutr Educ Behav. 2002;34:310–5.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Gibson EL, Wardle J. Energy density predicts preferences for fruit and vegetables in 4-year-oldchildren. Appetite. 2003;41:97–8.CrossRefPubMed Gibson EL, Wardle J. Energy density predicts preferences for fruit and vegetables in 4-year-oldchildren. Appetite. 2003;41:97–8.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Issanchou S. Food choices at lunch during the third year of life: highselection of animal and starchy foods but avoidance of vegetables. Acta Paediatr. 2005;94:943–51.CrossRefPubMed Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Issanchou S. Food choices at lunch during the third year of life: highselection of animal and starchy foods but avoidance of vegetables. Acta Paediatr. 2005;94:943–51.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Barends C, de Vries J, Mojet J, de Graaf C. Effects of repeated exposure to either vegetables orfruits on infant’s vegetable and fruit acceptance at the beginning of weaning. Food Qual Prefer. 2013;29:157–65.CrossRef Barends C, de Vries J, Mojet J, de Graaf C. Effects of repeated exposure to either vegetables orfruits on infant’s vegetable and fruit acceptance at the beginning of weaning. Food Qual Prefer. 2013;29:157–65.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Sullivan SA, Birch LL. Infant dietary experience and acceptance of solid foods. Pediatrics. 1994;93:271–7.PubMed Sullivan SA, Birch LL. Infant dietary experience and acceptance of solid foods. Pediatrics. 1994;93:271–7.PubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat •• Werthmann J, Jansen A, Havermans R, Nederkoorn C, Kremers S, Roefs A. Bits and pieces. Food texture influences food acceptance in young children. Appetite. 2015;84:181. The first paper to show that children really don’t like lumps in food. CrossRefPubMed •• Werthmann J, Jansen A, Havermans R, Nederkoorn C, Kremers S, Roefs A. Bits and pieces. Food texture influences food acceptance in young children. Appetite. 2015;84:181. The first paper to show that children really don’t like lumps in food. CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Coulthard H, Harris G, Fogel A. Vegetable acceptance in children weaned early and late. Appetite. 2014;78:89–94.CrossRefPubMed Coulthard H, Harris G, Fogel A. Vegetable acceptance in children weaned early and late. Appetite. 2014;78:89–94.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Coulthard H, Harris G, Emmett P, the ALSPAC team. Long term consequences of early fruit and vegetable feeding practices. Pub Health Nutr. 2010;13(12):2044–51.CrossRef Coulthard H, Harris G, Emmett P, the ALSPAC team. Long term consequences of early fruit and vegetable feeding practices. Pub Health Nutr. 2010;13(12):2044–51.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Maier AS, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Leathwood PD, Issanchou SN. Breastfeeding and experience with variety early in weaning increase infantsacceptance of new foods for up to two months. ClinNutr. 2008;27:849–57. Maier AS, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Leathwood PD, Issanchou SN. Breastfeeding and experience with variety early in weaning increase infantsacceptance of new foods for up to two months. ClinNutr. 2008;27:849–57.
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Foote K, Marriott LM, Kimber AC, et al. A randomised controlled trial ofan evidence based weaning strategy designed specifically to meet theneeds of preterm infants. Arch Dis Child. 2002;86(suppl 1):A1. Foote K, Marriott LM, Kimber AC, et al. A randomised controlled trial ofan evidence based weaning strategy designed specifically to meet theneeds of preterm infants. Arch Dis Child. 2002;86(suppl 1):A1.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Maier A, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Issanchou S, Leathwood P. Effects of repeated exposure on acceptanceof initially disliked vegetables in 7-month old infants. Food Qual Prefer. 2007;18:1023–32.CrossRef Maier A, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Issanchou S, Leathwood P. Effects of repeated exposure on acceptanceof initially disliked vegetables in 7-month old infants. Food Qual Prefer. 2007;18:1023–32.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Hetherington M, Schwartz C, Madrelle J, Croden F, Nekitsing C, Vereijken C, Weenen H. A step-by-step introduction to vegetables at the beginning of complementary feeding. The effects of early and repeated exposure. Appetite. 2015;84:280–90.CrossRefPubMed Hetherington M, Schwartz C, Madrelle J, Croden F, Nekitsing C, Vereijken C, Weenen H. A step-by-step introduction to vegetables at the beginning of complementary feeding. The effects of early and repeated exposure. Appetite. 2015;84:280–90.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Bell KI, Tepper BJ. Short-term vegetable intake by young children classified by 6-n-propylthoiuracil bitter-taste phenotype. Am J ClinNutr. 2006;84:245–51. Bell KI, Tepper BJ. Short-term vegetable intake by young children classified by 6-n-propylthoiuracil bitter-taste phenotype. Am J ClinNutr. 2006;84:245–51.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Rollins BY, Loken E, Birch LL. Stability and change in snackfood likes and dislikes from 5 to 11 years. Appetite. 2010;55:371–3.CrossRefPubMed Rollins BY, Loken E, Birch LL. Stability and change in snackfood likes and dislikes from 5 to 11 years. Appetite. 2010;55:371–3.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat • Maslin K, Grundy J, Glasbey G, Dean T, Hasan S, Grimshaw K, Oliver E, Roberts G, Venter C. Cows’ milk exclusion diet during infancy: is there a long term effect on children’s eating behaviour and food preferences? Pediatr Allergy Immunol. 2015;27:141–6. Observes that infants who have had milk exclusion diets would not eat some calorie dense milk products in mid childhood. CrossRef • Maslin K, Grundy J, Glasbey G, Dean T, Hasan S, Grimshaw K, Oliver E, Roberts G, Venter C. Cows’ milk exclusion diet during infancy: is there a long term effect on children’s eating behaviour and food preferences? Pediatr Allergy Immunol. 2015;27:141–6. Observes that infants who have had milk exclusion diets would not eat some calorie dense milk products in mid childhood. CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Coulthard H, Blissett J. Fruit and vegetable consumption in children and their mothers: moderating effects of child sensory sensitivity. Appetite. 2009;52:410–5.CrossRefPubMed Coulthard H, Blissett J. Fruit and vegetable consumption in children and their mothers: moderating effects of child sensory sensitivity. Appetite. 2009;52:410–5.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Farrow CV, Coulthard H. Relationships between sensory sensitivity, anxiety and selective eating in children. Appetite. 2012;58:842–6.CrossRefPubMed Farrow CV, Coulthard H. Relationships between sensory sensitivity, anxiety and selective eating in children. Appetite. 2012;58:842–6.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Coulthard H, Harris G, Fogel A. Tactile over-responsivity and early vegetable consumption; moderating effect of age of introduction to solid foods. Maternal and Child Nutrition. 2016; doi:10.1111/mcn.12228.PubMed Coulthard H, Harris G, Fogel A. Tactile over-responsivity and early vegetable consumption; moderating effect of age of introduction to solid foods. Maternal and Child Nutrition. 2016; doi:10.​1111/​mcn.​12228.PubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Illingworth RS, Lister J. The critical or sensitive period, with special reference to certain feeding problems in infants and children. J Pediatr. 1964;65:839–48.CrossRefPubMed Illingworth RS, Lister J. The critical or sensitive period, with special reference to certain feeding problems in infants and children. J Pediatr. 1964;65:839–48.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Booth DA. Psychology of nutrition. London: Taylor & Francis; 1994. Booth DA. Psychology of nutrition. London: Taylor & Francis; 1994.
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolf LS, Glass RP. Feeding and swallowing disorders in infancy. Assessment and management. Arizona: Therapy Skill Builders; 1992. Wolf LS, Glass RP. Feeding and swallowing disorders in infancy. Assessment and management. Arizona: Therapy Skill Builders; 1992.
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Morris SE, Klein MD. Pre-feeding skills. 2nd ed. USA: Therapy Skill Builders; 2000. Morris SE, Klein MD. Pre-feeding skills. 2nd ed. USA: Therapy Skill Builders; 2000.
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Wickendon AD. The development and disruption of feeding skills: how speech & language therapists can help. In: Southall A, Schwartz A, editors. Feeding problems in children. Abington, Oxon: Radcliffe Medical Press; 2000. p. 3–23. Wickendon AD. The development and disruption of feeding skills: how speech & language therapists can help. In: Southall A, Schwartz A, editors. Feeding problems in children. Abington, Oxon: Radcliffe Medical Press; 2000. p. 3–23.
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Arvedson J, Clark H, Lazarus C, Schooling T, Frymark T. Evidence-based systematic review: effects of oral motor interventions on feeding and swallowing in preterm infants. American Journal of Speech - Language Pathology (Online). 2010;19(4):321–340A.CrossRef Arvedson J, Clark H, Lazarus C, Schooling T, Frymark T. Evidence-based systematic review: effects of oral motor interventions on feeding and swallowing in preterm infants. American Journal of Speech - Language Pathology (Online). 2010;19(4):321–340A.CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Pinelli J, Symington A. Non-nutritive sucking for promoting physiologic stability and nutrition in preterm infants (Cochrane Review). In: Cochrane Neonatal Group, editors. The Cochrane Library, Issue 2, Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., 2004. Pinelli J, Symington A. Non-nutritive sucking for promoting physiologic stability and nutrition in preterm infants (Cochrane Review). In: Cochrane Neonatal Group, editors. The Cochrane Library, Issue 2, Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., 2004.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Bazyk S. Factors associated with the transition to oral feeding in infants fed by nasogastric tubes. Am J OccupTher. 1990;44:1070–8.CrossRef Bazyk S. Factors associated with the transition to oral feeding in infants fed by nasogastric tubes. Am J OccupTher. 1990;44:1070–8.CrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Mizuno K, Ueda A. Development of sucking behavior in infants who have not been fed for 2 months after birth. Pediatr Int. 2001;43:251–5.CrossRefPubMed Mizuno K, Ueda A. Development of sucking behavior in infants who have not been fed for 2 months after birth. Pediatr Int. 2001;43:251–5.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Carruth BR, Skinner JD. Feeding behaviors and other motor development in healthy children (2-24 months). J Am CollNutr. 2002;21(2):88–96. Carruth BR, Skinner JD. Feeding behaviors and other motor development in healthy children (2-24 months). J Am CollNutr. 2002;21(2):88–96.
55.
Zurück zum Zitat COMA report Committee on Medical Aspects of Food Policy – COMA. Department of Health: 45 weaning and the weaning diet—report of working group. London: HMSO; 1994. COMA report Committee on Medical Aspects of Food Policy – COMA. Department of Health: 45 weaning and the weaning diet—report of working group. London: HMSO; 1994.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Northstone K, Emmett P, Nethersole F. The effect of age of introduction to lumpy solids on foods eaten and reported feeding difficulties at 6 and 15 months. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2001;14:43–54.CrossRefPubMed Northstone K, Emmett P, Nethersole F. The effect of age of introduction to lumpy solids on foods eaten and reported feeding difficulties at 6 and 15 months. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2001;14:43–54.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Coulthard H, Harris G, Emmett P. Delayed introduction of lumpy foods to children during the complementary feeding period affects child’s food acceptance and feeding at 7 years of age. Matern Child Nutr. 2009;5(1):75–85.CrossRefPubMed Coulthard H, Harris G, Emmett P. Delayed introduction of lumpy foods to children during the complementary feeding period affects child’s food acceptance and feeding at 7 years of age. Matern Child Nutr. 2009;5(1):75–85.CrossRefPubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Wilson EM, Green JR, Weismer GA. Kinematic description of the temporal characteristics of jaw motion for early chewing: preliminary findings. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2012;55(2):626–63.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Wilson EM, Green JR, Weismer GA. Kinematic description of the temporal characteristics of jaw motion for early chewing: preliminary findings. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2012;55(2):626–63.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Gisel EG. Effect of food texture on the development of chewing of children between six months and two years of age. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1991;33:69–79.CrossRefPubMed Gisel EG. Effect of food texture on the development of chewing of children between six months and two years of age. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1991;33:69–79.CrossRefPubMed
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Blossfield I, Collins A, Kiely M, Delahunty C. Texture preferences of 12-month-old infants and the role of early experiences. Food Qual Pref. 2007;18:396–404.CrossRef Blossfield I, Collins A, Kiely M, Delahunty C. Texture preferences of 12-month-old infants and the role of early experiences. Food Qual Pref. 2007;18:396–404.CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Mason SJ, Harris G, Blissett J. Tube feeding in infancy: implications for the development of normal eating and drinking skills. Dysphagia. 2005;20:46–61.CrossRefPubMed Mason SJ, Harris G, Blissett J. Tube feeding in infancy: implications for the development of normal eating and drinking skills. Dysphagia. 2005;20:46–61.CrossRefPubMed
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Reilly S, Skuse D, Mathisen B, Wolke D. The objective rating of oral-motor functions during feeding. Dysphagia. 1995;10:177–91.CrossRefPubMed Reilly S, Skuse D, Mathisen B, Wolke D. The objective rating of oral-motor functions during feeding. Dysphagia. 1995;10:177–91.CrossRefPubMed
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Harris G. Food refusal in the sensory sensitive child. Paed Child Health. 2009;19(9):435–6.CrossRef Harris G. Food refusal in the sensory sensitive child. Paed Child Health. 2009;19(9):435–6.CrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith AM, Roux S, Naidoo NT, Venter DJL. Food choices of tactile defensive children. Nutr. 2005;21(1):14–9.CrossRef Smith AM, Roux S, Naidoo NT, Venter DJL. Food choices of tactile defensive children. Nutr. 2005;21(1):14–9.CrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Cooke L, Wardle J, Gibson E. Relationship between parental report of food neophobia and everyday food consumption in 2-6-year-old children. Appetite. 2003;41:205–6.CrossRefPubMed Cooke L, Wardle J, Gibson E. Relationship between parental report of food neophobia and everyday food consumption in 2-6-year-old children. Appetite. 2003;41:205–6.CrossRefPubMed
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown S, Harris G. A theoretical proposal for a perceptually driven, food-based disgust that can influence food acceptance during early childhood. International Journal of Child Health and Nutrition. 2012a;1(1):1–10. Brown S, Harris G. A theoretical proposal for a perceptually driven, food-based disgust that can influence food acceptance during early childhood. International Journal of Child Health and Nutrition. 2012a;1(1):1–10.
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown S, Harris G. Rejection of known and previously accepted foods during early childhood: an extension of the neophobic response? International Journal of Child Health and Nutrition. 2012b;1:72–81. Brown S, Harris G. Rejection of known and previously accepted foods during early childhood: an extension of the neophobic response? International Journal of Child Health and Nutrition. 2012b;1:72–81.
69.
70.
71.
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Cooke LJ, Haworth CMA, Wardle J. Genetic and environmental influences on children’s food neophobia. Am J ClinNutr. 2007;86(2):428–33. Cooke LJ, Haworth CMA, Wardle J. Genetic and environmental influences on children’s food neophobia. Am J ClinNutr. 2007;86(2):428–33.
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown S, Harris G. Disliked food acting as a contaminant during infancy; a disgust based motivation for rejection. Appetite. 2012c;58(2):535–58.CrossRefPubMed Brown S, Harris G. Disliked food acting as a contaminant during infancy; a disgust based motivation for rejection. Appetite. 2012c;58(2):535–58.CrossRefPubMed
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Toyama N. Young children’s awareness of socially mediated rejection offood. Why is food dropped at the table ‘dirty’? Cogn Dev. 2000;15:523–41.CrossRef Toyama N. Young children’s awareness of socially mediated rejection offood. Why is food dropped at the table ‘dirty’? Cogn Dev. 2000;15:523–41.CrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Rozin P, Hammer L, Oster H, Horowitz T, Marmora V. The child’s conception of food. Differentiation of categories of rejected substances in the 16 months to 5 year age range. Appetite. 1986a;7:141–51.CrossRefPubMed Rozin P, Hammer L, Oster H, Horowitz T, Marmora V. The child’s conception of food. Differentiation of categories of rejected substances in the 16 months to 5 year age range. Appetite. 1986a;7:141–51.CrossRefPubMed
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Hendy HM. Effectiveness of trained peer models to encourage food acceptance in preschool children. Appetite. 2002;39(3):217–25.CrossRefPubMed Hendy HM. Effectiveness of trained peer models to encourage food acceptance in preschool children. Appetite. 2002;39(3):217–25.CrossRefPubMed
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Hendy HM, Raudenbusch B. Effectiveness of teachermodeling to encourage food acceptance in preschool children. Appetite. 2000;34:61–76.CrossRefPubMed Hendy HM, Raudenbusch B. Effectiveness of teachermodeling to encourage food acceptance in preschool children. Appetite. 2000;34:61–76.CrossRefPubMed
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Cooke L. The importance of exposure for healthy eating in childhood: a review. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2007;20:294–301.CrossRefPubMed Cooke L. The importance of exposure for healthy eating in childhood: a review. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2007;20:294–301.CrossRefPubMed
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Brown S, Harris G, Bell L, Lines LM. Disliked food acting as a contaminant in a sample of young children. Appetite. 2012d;58(3):991–6.CrossRefPubMed Brown S, Harris G, Bell L, Lines LM. Disliked food acting as a contaminant in a sample of young children. Appetite. 2012d;58(3):991–6.CrossRefPubMed
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Batsell RW, Brown AS, Ansfield ME, Paschall GY. You will eat all of that!: a retrospective analysis of forced consumptionepisodes. Appetite. 2002;38(3):211–9.CrossRefPubMed Batsell RW, Brown AS, Ansfield ME, Paschall GY. You will eat all of that!: a retrospective analysis of forced consumptionepisodes. Appetite. 2002;38(3):211–9.CrossRefPubMed
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Lakkaluka A, Geaghan J, Zanovec M, Pierce S, Tuuri G. Ten tastes does improve the liking of disliked vegetables in older children, but the effect is stronger with a sweet tasting vegetable. Appetite. 2010;55(2):226–31.CrossRef Lakkaluka A, Geaghan J, Zanovec M, Pierce S, Tuuri G. Ten tastes does improve the liking of disliked vegetables in older children, but the effect is stronger with a sweet tasting vegetable. Appetite. 2010;55(2):226–31.CrossRef
82.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Bryant-Waugh R. Avoidant and restrictive food intake disorder: an illustrative case example. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2013;46(5):420–3.CrossRefPubMed Bryant-Waugh R. Avoidant and restrictive food intake disorder: an illustrative case example. The International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2013;46(5):420–3.CrossRefPubMed
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Chabanet C, Issanchou S. A prospective study of food preferences inchildhood. Food Qual Prefer. 2004;15:805–18.CrossRef Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Chabanet C, Issanchou S. A prospective study of food preferences inchildhood. Food Qual Prefer. 2004;15:805–18.CrossRef
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Chabanet C, Issanchou S. A prospective study of food variety seeking in childhood, adolescence and early adult life. Appetite. 2005;44:289–97.CrossRefPubMed Nicklaus S, Boggio V, Chabanet C, Issanchou S. A prospective study of food variety seeking in childhood, adolescence and early adult life. Appetite. 2005;44:289–97.CrossRefPubMed
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Coulthard H, Harris G. Early food refusal; the role of maternal mood. J Reproductive & Infant Psych. 2003;21:335–45.CrossRef Coulthard H, Harris G. Early food refusal; the role of maternal mood. J Reproductive & Infant Psych. 2003;21:335–45.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Are There Sensitive Periods for Food Acceptance in Infancy?
verfasst von
Gillian Harris
Sarah Mason
Publikationsdatum
29.04.2017
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Current Nutrition Reports / Ausgabe 2/2017
Elektronische ISSN: 2161-3311
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s13668-017-0203-0

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2017

Current Nutrition Reports 2/2017 Zur Ausgabe

Food Acceptance and Nutrition in Infants and Young Children (H Coulthard, Section Editor)

A Systematic Review of Methods for Increasing Vegetable Consumption in Early Childhood

Food Acceptance and Nutrition in Infants and Young Children (H Coulthard, Section Editor)

Baby-Led Weaning: The Evidence to Date

Leitlinien kompakt für die Innere Medizin

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Update Innere Medizin

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.