Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2006

Open Access 01.12.2006 | Research article

Auditory change detection in schizophrenia: sources of activity, related neuropsychological function and symptoms in patients with a first episode in adolescence, and patients 14 years after an adolescent illness-onset

verfasst von: Robert D Oades, Nele Wild-Wall, Stephanie A Juran, Jan Sachsse, Ljubov B Oknina, Bernd Röpcke

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2006

Abstract

Background

The event-related brain response mismatch negativity (MMN) registers changes in auditory stimulation with temporal lobe sources reflecting short-term echoic memory and frontal sources a deviance-induced switch in processing. Impairment, controversially present at the onset of schizophrenia, develops rapidly and can remain independent of clinical improvement. We examined the characteristics of the scalp-recorded MMN and related these to tests of short-term memory and set-shifting. We assessed whether the equivalent dipole sources are affected already at illness-onset in adolescence and how these features differ after a 14-year course following an adolescent onset. The strength, latency, orientation and location of frontal and temporal lobe sources of MMN activity early and late in the course of adolescent-onset schizophrenia are analysed and illustrated.

Methods

MMN, a measure of auditory change-detection, was elicited by short deviant tones in a 3-tone oddball-presentation and recorded from 32 scalp electrodes. Four dipole sources were placed following hypothesis-led calculations using brain electrical source analysis on brain atlas and MR-images. A short neuropsychological test battery was administered. We compared 28 adolescent patients with a first episode of schizophrenia and 18 patients 14 years after diagnosis in adolescence with two age-matched control groups from the community (n = 22 and 18, respectively).

Results

MMN peaked earlier in the younger than the older subjects. The amplitude was reduced in patients, especially the younger group, and was here associated with negative symptoms and slow set-shifting. In first-episode patients the temporal lobe sources were more ventral than in controls, while the left cingular and right inferior-mid frontal sources were more caudal. In the older patients the left temporal locus remained ventral (developmental stasis), the right temporal locus extended more antero-laterally (illness progression), and the right frontal source moved antero-laterally (normalised).

Conclusion

From the start of the illness there were differences in the dipole-model between healthy and patient groups. Separate characteristics of the sources of the activity differences showed an improvement, stasis or deterioration with illness-duration. The precise nature of the changes in the sources of MMN activity and their relationship to selective information processing and storage depend on the specific psychopathology and heterogeneous course of the illness.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1471-244X-6-7) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

NWW carried out most of the recordings, and their analysis, and contributed to drafting the manuscript; SAJ assisted NWW with the recordings and contributed to the analysis. LBO carried out some early recordings and analyses. RDO conceived planned and organised the study, ran the initial recordings and drafted most of the MS. JS and BR were involved in recruitment, diagnosis and neuropsychological testing and initial analyses of these data. All authors read and approved the draft manuscript.
Abkürzungen
BESA
Brain electrical source activity
CGI
Clinical global impression inventory
DSM
Diagnostic and statistical manual (of the American Psychiatric Association)
EOS
Early-onset schizophrenia
ERP
Event-related potential
MEG
magnetoencephalography
MMN
Mismatch Negativity
MR(I)
magnetic resonance (imaging)
RV
Residual variance
SAPS/SANS
Scale for the assessment of positive/negative symptoms
SCID
Structured clinical interview for DSM schizophrenia
SD
Standard deviation

Background

The detection of a change in ongoing ambient auditory stimulation is an important preliminary requirement for the conscious organisation of an adaptive response to a significant event. The unusual sound could be an unexpected tone in a well-known piece of music, or the telephone ringing during a conversation. The change is detected automatically, but the altered behaviour requires controlled information processing beyond detection. The brain's response on detecting deviance is registered by an event-related potential (ERP) called mismatch negativity (MMN). This is recorded by subtracting the ERPs after a series of similar stimuli from that elicited by the unexpected tone. The procedure requires no task, and is thus well-suited for study in patients with schizophrenia. But what parts of the brain generate MMN activity and what mechanisms are involved?
Sources of neuronal activity have been reported for the auditory cortices and the frontal lobe [1, 2]. The frontal sources lie in the right inferior/mid-frontal and left anterior cingulate gyri [3, 4]. This is consistent with functional imaging of the activity generated by dissonant tones in music [5]. The activity of these sources represents the registration of a change and the mechanism for switching to a new mode of information processing [6, 7]. Sources in the superior temporal lobe represent the short-term sensory memory trace for the currently usual sound [8, 9]. This sensory memory has many features in common with an auditory working memory [10]. Information in working memory is organised in the inferior frontal region [11] where activity closely covaries with that in the superior temporal areas in imaging studies of auditory memory (e.g. in same-different judgments [12, 13]). Like the phonological loop in working memory [14], the auditory sensory trace can be reactivated for 11–15 seconds after a stimulus [15]. A monitoring function is widely attributed to both working memory and to the automatic process underlying MMN [16], for which a supervisory attention system [17] and a store are essential parts [18]. There is much evidence for impaired auditory [19] and non-verbal working memory in schizophrenia [20], but are both the memorial (temporal lobe) and switching (frontal lobe) components of MMN also impaired? If so, then an examination of the sources should show how the impairment is expressed. Associations with conventional neuropsychological indicators of working memory and switching were explored..
A reduced MMN is widely reported in patients with schizophrenia with or without antipsychotic medication [2123]. In contrast, borderline or non-significant reductions are reported for depressed and bipolar patients [23, 24], and MMN with a normal amplitude was recorded in obsessive compulsive disorders [25]. Significant [26] or nonsignificant [27] decreases described at onset may get more severe after a longer illness [24, 2729]. They are often more marked in patients with non-paranoid or negative symptoms [27, 30] and without hallucinatory or delusory symptoms [31]. Such negative features (e.g. flat-affect, alogia, social withdrawal, avolition) correlate with the severity of working memory difficulties [32].
Initial studies of MMN sources with magnetoencephalography (MEG: [33, 34]) and ERP and imaging techniques (e.g. [35, 36]) suggest impaired left-sided locations and source strengths. But these have concentrated on the temporal rather than the frontal lobe. As suggested above, such temporal lobe changes would be expected if the reduced MMN amplitude reflects in part impaired superior temporal function in categorization [37], sound identification [38] and auditory working-memory processes [39]. But if the supervisory attention system is also impaired, the frontal sources of activity may be altered. Evidence from MMN topography [40, 41] and source modelling [42] has implicated frontal generator impairments in schizophrenia, and that in the early stages of illness there may be some further deterioration of these impairments reflecting illness-progression [42].
In this current report two groups of patients were selected to examine the hypotheses of there being impaired frontal sources and a progressive deterioration with course in schizophrenia. Patients were either experiencing their first episode as adolescents, or had been diagnosed initially in adolescence 14 years before. (It has been suggested that an onset in adolescence can lead to a severer course of illness [43].) We sought to explain the topographic pattern of MMN activity with brain electrical source analysis locating dipoles bilaterally in the frontal and temporal lobes based on published models [3, 42]. This procedure describes 4 features (locus, orientation, strength and latency) for each of the 4 dipoles that could differ between groups. We concentrate on the MMN generated by stimuli of different durations for which impairments are frequently reported, and may even be impaired in the patients' relatives [44], making it a candidate for an endophenotype of change detection processes.

Methods

Participants

The study protocol was reviewed and approved by both the board of the University of Essen Psychiatry Clinics and the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine according to the criteria of the Declaration of Helsinki (00-2-1357-Y). After the procedures had been described all subjects and care-givers gave written informed consent. A first-episode of DSM-IV schizophrenia was diagnosed in 28 adolescent inpatients (early-onset: EOS) on the basis of a clinical interview and hospital records first on the ward and then by research group personnel. This was confirmed 6 months later to exclude affective, schizoaffective and schizophreniform psychoses. Eighteen outpatients who had their first break as adolescents in our clinic on average 14.4y before (SD 3.5, range 8.1–19.7: S-14Y) were also recruited: 11 showed a partial remission (CGI 3–5), and 7 a chronic course or no remission (CGI 6–8). They had a mean of 4.6 hospitalizations (range 1–12). Of 34 patients contacted, 9 declined and 7 were too ill: participants did not differ on mean age, gender, illness-severity or social function [45]. Patients were excluded for other major psychiatric or somatic illness, alcohol abuse in the last 5 years and current substance abuse other than nicotine. Two age-matched comparison groups were recruited by advertisement and paid for participation (C-EOS, C-14Y). None had used drugs affecting the central nervous system or had a history of neurological or psychiatric illness. Gender balance was not considered significant as gender does not influence MMN [1, 4648]. All had normal or corrected to normal vision, and normal hearing on audiometric testing. In the S-14Y group 15 were receiving stable doses of antipsychotic medication. Of the EOS patients 15 had received the same treatment for >3 days, and 13 were examined without medication (Table 1). Assessment of the positive and negative symptoms in patients [49], SCID-II interviews and testing took place in the same week. Neuropsychological testing included a short IQ (4 WAIS sub-tests [50]), trail-making, digit-span forwards and backwards, logical memories, and visual reproduction.
Table 1
Characteristics of two groups of patients with schizophrenia and two groups of healthy comparison subjects (means and standard deviations)
 
EOS (N = 28)
C-EOS (N = 22)
S-14Y (N = 18)
C-14Y(N = 18)
 
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Gender m/f
21/7
 
12/10
 
12/6
 
7/11
 
Mean Age (y)
17.5
(0.4)
17.6
(0.4)
32.1
(0.9)
30.4
(1.4)
Handedness*
27R, 1M
 
21R, 1L
 
16R, 1M, 1L
 
14R, 3M, (1 missing)
 
Socio-economic status**
3.9
(0.4)
2.6
(0.2)
4.2
(0.4)
3.5
0.3
Short-IQ***
93.3
(4.0)
115.8
(4.2)
91.1
(4.1)
109.9
(4.0)
DSMIV diagnosis:
        
   Paranoid
22
 
-
 
8
 
-
 
   Disorganised
5
 
-
 
2
 
-
 
   Undifferentiated
1
 
-
 
1
 
-
 
   Residual
-
 
-
 
5
 
-
 
   Schizoaffective
-
 
-
 
2
 
-
 
Ratings per question+
        
SANS
2.13
(0.49)
-
 
1.68
(0.93)
-
 
SAPS
1.10
(0.8.)
-
 
0.81
(0.75)
-
 
Antipsychotic
478.1
(317.4)
-
 
366.0
(157.6)
-
 
   Medication (CPZ)#
-
       
[N, Atypical/Mixed/Typical]
10/3/2
 
-
 
10/2/2
 
-
 
[N, without Medication]
13
 
-
 
4
 
-
 
Mean cigarettes/day/smoker
15
[14, 50%]
5
[20, 91%]
25
[7, 39%]
9
[16, 89%]
[Non-smokers: N, %])##
        
*Edinburgh inventory [91](-100/-50 (left- [L]), -50/+50 (mixed- [M]), +50/+100 (right-handed [R]). ** Parent occupation, scale 1–6 [92] (EOS vs. C-EOS, t40 = 3.6, P < .01). ***Short-IQ (information, arithmetic, digit-symbol, block-design [50]: patients<age-matched controls (t74 = -5.5, P < .01). #Chlorpromazine equivalents (CPZ: mg/day) for those on medication (t26 = -1.18, P = .25). ## More cigarettes consumed by the EOS vs. C-EOS (t46 = 3.6, P = .001) and by the S-14Y vs. C-14Y groups (t34 = 3.6, P = .002). + No group differences in the mean ratings/question for positive (SAPS: t37 = -1.2, P = .25) and negative symptoms (SANS: t37 = -1.9, P = .06).

ERP measurements

An auditory oddball sequence (3 sinusoidal tones, 76 dBspl) was presented over 1600 trials. There was a pseudorandom sequence of standards (800 Hz, 80 ms, 10 ms rise/fall, p = 80%), frequency deviants (600 Hz, p = 10%) and duration deviants (40 ms, p = 10%) with each deviant preceded by at least one standard (stimulus onset asynchrony 850–1050 ms, mean 950 ms). During 4 blocks of 200 trials (passive auditory condition) subjects performed a simple visual red/green circle discrimination on a PC (50:50; subtending 3.8° at 1.5 m, changing at random every 1100 ms). Responses to the green target alternated between hands between blocks. Tone detection is not suppressed during various concurrent visual processes [51], nonetheless the stimulus onsets were controlled so as not to coincide. Four further audio-visual trial-blocks were presented with response to the frequency deviant (active auditory condition). This permitted an analysis of duration-deviant MMN in a state of attention to the auditory modality.
An EEG was recorded (Neuroscan, El Paso) from 31 tin electrodes (Fpz, Fz, Cz, Pz, Oz, FCz, CPz, F3, F7, F4, F8, FT7, FT8, C3, C4, P3, P4, CP3, CP4, T3, T4, T5, T6, TP7, TP8, M1, M2) in an electrode cap (Electro-Cap International: modified 10–20 system) with impedance <5 kΩ. A vertical EOG was recorded from the supra-orbital ridge of the right eye and a horizontal EOG from the outer canthus of the right and left eye to monitor blink-and eye-movements for rejection of artefacts (>50 μV) in EOG leads. Electrodes were referenced to linked-earlobes and the average-reference recomputed offline. A band-pass filter was set at 0.1–100 Hz. Data were digitised with 16-bit resolution, sampled at 500 Hz and stored on a hard disk. Records were epoched separately for each tone type with 100 ms prestimulus baseline and linear detrended. A 30 Hz low-pass filter (24 dB/octave) was used offline.

ERP data analysis

The MMN waveform was derived by subtracting the ERP to standard tones from those elicited by the duration-deviant. Peaks were sought automatically from 90 to 225 ms after stimulus-onset. The MMN was averaged across blocks for each group and the mean amplitude was computed in successive 30 ms windows from average-refenced data (105–135–165–195–225 ms) for the active and passive condition, respectively. An initial repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was carried out for 4 subject groups (between subject factor group) using data from 29 electrodes. The 135–165 and 165–195 ms windows covering the MMN peak (factor window), where inspection showed there to be potential group differences (Figure 1), were analysed by an ANOVA on the data from an array of 12 electrodes. The electrodes were arranged in two within-factors of 3 saggital chains (factor side: i.e. left; F3, FC3, C3, CP3: midline Fz, FCz, Cz, CPz: right; F4, FC4, C4, CP4)) and 4 coronal rows (factor row:frontal; fronto-central; central; centro-parietal). This reduced the degrees of freedom that were also adjusted with the Greenhouse-Geisser epsilon. The MMN in the active condition as well as the latency were analysed by ANOVAs using the same factors (for the latter excluding the factor window). Where significant main effects or interactions involved more than two factor levels, additional ANOVAs were carried out to test simple effects. The influence of antipsychotic medication was examined with ANOVA and Spearman correlations in the EOS group. Exploratory correlations were sought for 3 sets of MMN measures (frontal, mastoid and dipole-moments) with the CGI sum scores, the main clusters of SANS/SAPS symptoms, and neuropsychological measures of processes putatively related to MMN measures (digit-span, trail-making)[52, 53]. Trends are described at P < .05 and type-1 corrected correlations at P < .002.

MMN source analysis

Brain electrical source analysis (BESA) using a four-shell head-model [54] was used to compute dipoles based on the average-referenced ERP from 20 ms before to 40 ms after the MMN peak. Modelling requires iterative fitting of the dipole location and orientation in a spherical head-model until the difference between the recorded and the calculated surface data is minimised (least square fit [55]). Efficacy was enhanced by seeding with previously published solutions [3, 42, 56]. The goodness of fit is expressed by the residual variance (RV), not explained by the model. Final solutions showed an RV of <2% (Figure 2).
To calculate statistical differences between groups, the individual solutions were re-calculated for each subject. Waveforms were high- (1.5 Hz, 6 db/octave) and low-pass filtered (15 Hz, 12 db/octave). Data were discarded if a criterion of RV<4% was not reached. This reflects the decreased signal-to-noise ratio of an individual's waveform. Two C-EOS, 6 EOS, 3 C-14Y, and 5 S-14Y subjects were removed (Ns = 20, 23, 15, 13 respectively). Dipole-locations were compared between groups with a MANOVA including the 4-level factor dipole as well as the between-subject factor group. The x, y, and z-coordinates of each dipole [57] and the phi and theta orientation angles were compared with post-hoc F-Tests. Group differences of peak dipole-strength and latency were determined by univariate ANOVAs using a 2-level between-subject factor for the younger and older groups and a 4-level within-subject factor dipole (left/right, frontal/temporal lobe loci).

Results

Demographic, clinical, performance and ERP measures are presented in Tables 1, 2, 3. There were no group differences for hit rates on the visual vigilance control task, although the S-14Y group responded slower than the controls (F3,79 = 5.8, P < .01). Slow reaction times were confirmed for S-14Y patients in the auditory task (F3,79 = 3.9, P < .02). Here both patient groups made fewer hits than their comparison groups (F3,81 = 10.8, P < .01: Table 2).
Table 2
Response time and accuracy (mean and standard deviation) on an adjunctive visual vigilance test and an auditory discrimination of frequency deviant tones in younger and older groups of patients with schizophrenia and age-matched healthy comparison subjects
 
EOS (N = 28)
C-EOS(N = 22)
S-14Y (N = 18)
C-14Y (N = 18)
 
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Visual vigilance
554
[162]
496
[125]
664
[211]
467
[82]
response time (ms)#
Visual vigilance
92
[26]
97
[8]
98
[4]
100
[1]
task (% hits)*
Auditory discrimination
446
[92]
409
[61]
478
[80]
401
[61]
response time(ms)##
Auditory
72
[23]
90
[11]
61
[29]
92
[12]
discrimination (% hits)*
Response latencies of the S-14Y group on the visual (t33 = 3.6, P < .01)# and the auditory discrimination (t32 = 3.2, P < .01)## were slower: both patient groups were less accurate than their respective controls (t47,34 = -3.7, -4.2, P < .01) on the auditory task (*).
Table 3
ERP Characteristics for the Four Subject Groups
 
EOS (n = 28)
C-EOS (n = 22)
S-14Y (n = 18)
C-14Y (n = 18)
 
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
Mean
SD
MMN trials accepted
64
10
68
9
65
13
63
10
MMN latency (at Fz, ms)
1581
30
1531
19
1872
26
164
32
MMN amplitude Fz, (135–165 ms)
-1.1a
0.9
-2.1
1.1
-0.9
1.0
-1.5b
0.9
Cz,
-0.7a
1.1
-1.6
0.9
-0.6
1.2
-0.6b
1.2
Right mastoid, μV
+0.9a
0.9
+1.8
1.1
+0.9
1.7
+1.2b
1.0
Patients with early-onset schizophrenia (EOS) and 14 years after initial diagnosis (S-14Y) and their age-matched controls (C-EOS, C-14Y) : A significant difference for MMN amplitudes (average-reference 135–165 ms) was found for 4 groups(F3,82 = 5.1, P = .003). The effect was accounted for by the comparison between young subjects (F1,48 = 15.2, P < .001) – with patient amplitudes reduced [a], and the comparison for young vs. older controls (F1,38 = 6.0, P = .019) with amplitudes smaller in the older subjects [b]. Peak MMN latencies were earlier in younger than older patients1, and later in S-14Y vs. C-14Y2 (see text).

Scalp recordings

Initial analyses with 29 electrodes showed group differences for MMN amplitude (F84,2296 = 3.5, P < .001, ε =.16) in the 135–165 ms window (Figure 1). A trend difference continued in the 165–195 ms window (p = 0.1). EOS patients had a smaller MMN than the young controls (F28,1344 = 6.6, P < .001, ε = .16). The reduction in the older patients vs. their controls attained trend significance (F28,952 = 1.9, P = .1, ε = .15), but it should be noted that the older controls had a smaller MMN than the younger controls (F28,1064 = 2.8, P < .02, ε = .18). The MMN amplitudes did not differ between patient groups, and thus provide no evidence for a deterioration nor an improvement between onset (EOS) and later stages of the illness (S-14Y).
The ANOVA using mean amplitudes in two consecutive time windows confirmed the fronto-centrally pronounced MMN topography (factor row: F3,246 = 56.8, P < .001). MMN amplitudes in the frontal and fronto-central rows did not differ but were larger than in the other rows (Fs1,82 > 4.8, Ps < .03). Further MMN amplitudes were largest at the midline electrodes (factor side: F2,164 = 3.3, P = .043). However, the topography did vary with the time window (window × side: F2,164 = 7.8, P = .001). The MMN was broadly distributed in the 135–165 ms window (no significant differences between the midline, left and right side): but in the 165–195 ms window the midline MMN amplitude was larger than on the left or right side (Fs1,82>9.7, Ps < .003).
Most importantly, the 4 subject groups differed significantly from each other (F3,82 = 5.1, P = .003). Simple effects confirmed differences between the EOS group and their adolescent controls (F1,48 = 15.2, P < .001) as well as between the adolescent and the adult control groups (F1,38 = 6.0, P = .019). Effects were specific to time window and row (window × row × group: F9,246 = 2.6, P = .041). The group effect was pronounced in the 135–165 ms interval (group: F3,82 = 7.2, P < .001) and reduced to a trend in the 165–195 ms window (group: F3,82 = 2.4, P = .072). At 135–165 ms tests of simple effects revealed differences between EOS patients and their controls at all but the centro-parietal row (Fs1,48>13.5, Ps < .002). Differences between the control groups were most pronounced at the frontal and fronto-central row (Fs1,38>7.5, Ps < .011). When compared to their adult controls the S-14Y group showed a smaller MMN amplitude only at the frontal row (F1,34 = 4.8, P = .036).
ANOVA revealed significant differences in MMN peak latency between groups (F3,82 = 7.6, P < .001). Especially the S-14Y group showed an increased MMN latency, both, when compared to their adult controls (F1,34 = 18.7, P < .001) and to the younger patient group (F1,44 = 12.1, P = .001: Table 3).

Medication

An ANOVA comparing MMN amplitudes in 15 EOS patients with and 13 without medication was not significant (F8,192 = 1.3, P = .27, ε = .23). MMN amplitude tended to decrease more in the second window (165–195 ms) with dose of antipsychotic medication (CPZ equivalents: Fz, r = +.58, P = .03, FCz, r = +.52, P = .06). In other words there was a modest tendency for medication to sharpen the shape of the MMN waveform around its peak latency.

Dipole source analysis

The 60 ms of activity around the MMN peak was well explained by the 4-dipole model [3]. With minor adjustments of orientation and location, the group-solution model showed a RV<1.8% (best fit 0.9%) for each group (Table 4). The left temporal source was close to the border of the superior temporal gyrus with the insula and parietal cortex (BA 22, 29), and the right temporal source lay slightly more rostro-ventrally on the border of the medial and superior temporal gyri (BA 21, 22, 42). The left frontal source lay posteriorly in the anterior cingulate cortex (BA 24). The right frontal source was on the border of the inferior and mid-frontal gyri (BA 10, 44, 47: Figure 2).
Table 4
Location, strength, latency and orientation of modeled dipole sources calculated for patient groups with early onset schizophrenia (EOS), 14 years after onset (S-14Y) and their age-matched controls (C-EOS, C-14Y)
Subject Group
Interval (ms)
RV/Best-Fit (%)
Left/Right
Temporal/Frontal
Location
Strength (nAm)
Latency (ms)
Orientation
     
x
y
z
  
phi
theta
C-14Y (N = 18)
120–180
1.75/0.60
L
T (STG)
-46
-39
13
19.9
136
51
127
   
R
T (S/MTG)
49
-32
-2
20.9
136
67
109
   
L
F (ACG)
-11
-18
34
15.9
138
55
101
   
R
F (I/MFG)
34
52
-3
8.6
168
57
84
S-14Y (N = 18)
166–226
1.40/0.88
L
T (STG)
-52
-29
3
12.6
206
40
131
   
R
T (S/MTG)
61
-18
-3
13.8
184
59
140
   
L
F (ACG)
-2
-11
42
10.9
192
57
101
   
R
F (I/MFG)
32
49
-1
7.8
220
59
108
C-EOS (N = 22)
120–180
0.99/0.55
L
T (ITP)
-40
-32
24
24.2
154
54
129
   
R
T (STG)
61
-26
17
16.1
174
66
115
   
L
F (ACG)
-21
1
33
10.9
134
57
114
   
R
F (IFG)
49
45
-2
8.1
158
58
114
EOS (N = 28)
140–200
0.99/0.62
L
T (ITP)
-45
-32
23
18.5
170
49
121
   
R
T (STG)
52
-18
1
15.9
140
55
109
   
L
F (ACG)
-12
-17
33
12.6
136
50
99
   
R
F (I/MFG)
30
36
-4
7.7
174
51
112
ACG, cingulate gyrus; ITP, insula-temporo-parietal junction; I/MFG, inferior/mid-frontal gyrus; S/MTG, superior/medial temporal gyrus; STG, superior temporal gyrus
Source locations calculated from the individual solutions (similar to the group solutions) differed significantly between the groups (Wilks Λ = .16, F12,22 = 9.9, P < .001). Potentially reflecting post-adolescent maturation the left temporal source was more lateral (10 mm) and ventral (12 mm) in the older vs. the younger controls (Fs1,33>19.8, Ps < .001), while on the right it was more medial (9 mm) and ventral (14 mm: Fs1,33>13.3, Ps < .001). The frontal source in the older controls was more posterior in the left cingulate (12 mm), and on the right it was more anterior (10 mm) and medial (13 mm) vs. the younger controls (Fs1,33>26.2, Ps < .001).
Compared to the young controls each dipole source in the EOS group was shifted slightly (Λ = .14, F12,30 = 15.4, P < .001). The left temporal source was more lateral (8 mm), and that on the right was 15 mm more ventral (Fs1,41 = 14.9, 48.0, P < .001). This is reminiscent of the adult control loci, but 11 mm more rostral. In the EOS patients the left cingulate source lay 15 mm more posterior, and the right frontal source 18 mm more medial than in the C-EOS group (F1,41 = 50.6, 73.5, P < .001: Figure 3). These values exceeded those in the same direction for healthy adults, and were less lateral (left cingular) and less rostral (right frontal).
Differences in the dipole locations in the S-14Y vs. C-14Y subjects were, like the MMN waveform, less marked than those in the younger groups (Λ = .25, F12,15 = 3.9, P = .008: Figure 4). The right temporal source was 10 mm more anterolateral and the left cingular source 7 mm more medial than in the older controls (F1,26>15.6, P < .001). These changes lay on different dimensions from those described between young patients and controls. Differences between older and younger patients that could reflect maturation or illness progression were evident (Λ = .14, F12,23 = 11.8, P < .001). The left temporal source was markedly more ventral (18 mm) and the right temporal source slightly more lateral (7 mm) in the older patients (F1,34 = 70.6, 15.1, P < .001). In contrast to the temporal source, the left frontal locus was 13 mm more dorsal and the right frontal locus 10 mm more rostral in the older than the younger patients (F1,34 = 26.6, 14.5, P < .001: Figure 3).
Dipole moments appeared weaker in patients (F3,201 = 4.4, P = .008). But ANOVA showed there were interactions with location rather than group. The right frontal source was always the weakest (vs. left cingulum; t70 = 3.1, P = .003). Likewise the slightly later dipole-latencies in patients were not significant (F9,201 = 1.2, P = 0.34). This is attributable to the variation recorded (SD 19–31 ms). However across groups mean latencies were longer for the right frontal source (173 vs. left frontal 161, left temporal 161, right temporal 163 ms: t70>2.8, P < .05) illustrating a bottom up sequence for activation. Finally although the dipole-orientation for the whole group solutions clearly differed between groups (see frontal and cingulate sources in Figure 2) comparisons of the means of the individual solutions proved variable, and ANOVA showed no significant group differences (see group data in Table 4).

Active condition

Group differences similar to those in the passive condition, were found with attention to the auditory modality (F3,77 = 3.9, P = .012). An interaction of group × window indicated that a significant reduction of MMN amplitude in the EOS vs. C-EOS group was delayed to the 165–195 ms window (F1,43 = 5.2, P = .027). Differences for EOS vs. S-14Y (F1,39 = 7.1, P = .011) and C-EOS vs. C-15Y (F1,38 = 9.6, P = .004) were retained for the 135–165 ms window. But comparisons between the older subjects were non-significant.
Fitting the dipole-model from the passive to the active condition, resulted in a good fit for the young subjects (C-EOS, RV 2.1%, best fit 1.4%: EOS, RV 3.5%, best fit 2.1%). Peak-dipole activity in the older subjects also fitted well the 60 ms envelope around the peak (RV 5.1%, best fits 1.2–4.0%).

Psychopathologic and neuropsychologic associations

Correlations were sought between MMN amplitude at Fz and the mastoids with the CGI and the 9 clusters of SANS/SAPS symptom ratings. EOS but not S-14Y patients showed decreasing amplitude with increasing CGI severity at frontal (r = +0.58, P = .002) and mastoid sites (r = -0.04, P = .035). In both patient groups associations of MMN with symptoms were restricted to 3 clusters. In EOS patients reduced MMN at the mastoids related to anergia (r = -0.42 to -0.7, P = .003–.0001) and flat affect (r = -0.43, P = .03). Quite separately in S-14Y patients there were indications that hallucinations related to decreased MMN at frontal sites (r = -0.54, P = .02), while, unexpectedly, increased amplitudes at the mastoids related to anergia ratings (r = +0.5 to +0.6, P = .03-.008).
Correlations for MMN amplitude with z-scores for trail-making measures of set-switching and digit-span measures of working memory were explored. Trails B-A scores were unrelated to MMN amplitude in healthy adolescents, but were negatively related to MMN amplitude at both mastoid sites in both time windows in EOS patients (r = -0.36 to -0.53, p = .07-.005). No correlations were evident in the older groups. There were no relationships to dipole-moments in the young subjects. But an association with the left cingular dipole in healthy adults (r = +0.67, P = .006) is of interest in view of trends in both older groups for an association of trails performance with temporal lobe dipole-moments in the left hemisphere (r = +0.5, P = .06–.08).
Surprisingly, in the C-EOS group digit-span scores correlated negatively with left and right mastoid MMN amplitudes in both time windows (r = -0.50 to-0.61, P = .01–.04). In contrast in EOS patients the correlations tended in the opposite direction at mastoid sites (r = +0.43, P = .03) and FCz (r = -0.4, P = .05). These measures were not correlated in the older subjects. There was no evidence of an association of MMN characteristics with other indicators of short-term memory (logical memories or verbal reproduction). Isolated relationships with dipole-moments could not be distinguished from chance.

Discussion

Auditory change detection (MMN) was reduced in teenagers experiencing their first break of schizophrenia, compared to age-matched controls (mean 1.1 vs. 2.1 μV at Fz: Table 3). In outpatients, 14 years after their first episode in adolescence, the difference with young patients showed no clear signs of deterioration or improvement (mean 1.1 vs. 0.9 μV). The unexpected decrease of MMN amplitude in healthy adults compared to adolescent controls (from 2.1 to 1.5 μV) underlies the attenuated statistical difference between adult healthy and patient groups (see also [58]). The degree of reduction of MMN amplitude in the younger patients was similar to other reports, but the reduction in older patients compared to age-matched controls was slightly less than in other reports [21, 23, 44].
In all subjects MMN was calculated as reflecting bilateral source-activity in the temporal lobes and the left cingulate gyrus followed by the right inferior-mid frontal cortex in a bottom-up sequence. These source locations, originally described in Jemel et al. [3] have been broadly confirmed using independent components analysis, LORETA, fMRI and subdural recording [59]. Dipole strengths were decreased and their latencies delayed in the patient groups. But, significant illness-related changes in the strength of dipole activity could not be demonstrated due to the poor signal-to-noise ratio in the individual solutions derived for statistical testing for between group differences. However, source locations did vary significantly between groups (see sections below). These results, obtained during attention to a visual task, were well-replicated when subjects attended to the auditory modality. MMN expression was related to symptom severity, working memory and measures of set-switching (see below).

On temporal lobe sources

In young first-episode patients sources in both the left and right auditory cortices appeared more to the left, and in the right hemisphere more ventral than in healthy teenagers. These are the same directions of change observed for the more caudally located sources in healthy 30- vs. healthy 17-year-olds. In these controls this likely reflects developmental white-matter expansion and grey-matter density increases [60, 61] that can continue in the post-adolescent period [62]. The superficially similar result in EOS patients was unexpected. But this could reflect asymmetric ventricle expansion [63, 64] that can be marked in early-onset patients [65].
In the older patients the left temporal source was more ventral than in controls or either group of young subjects. Indeed, their right temporal source changed in association with illness progression, being more anterolateral than in either the EOS or the C-14Y groups.
The reductions of MMN amplitude (significant), smaller dipole moments in the temporal lobe and slightly longer latencies (both non-significant) in adolescent patients are very similar to alterations reported for first-episode and adult patients [1, 66]. There are reports of a loss of right MMN lateralization and reduced dipole-moments associated with location changes on the left [9, 35, 36]. Such changes are consistent with grey-matter loss in the left superior temporal gyrus, and specific to patients with a first episode of schizophrenia rather than other psychoses [67]. The location changes on the right, reported here, might be associated with the adolescent onset of illness. Grey matter reductions in the right superior temporal gyrus have indeed been reported for early-onset patients [68]. This would be consistent with earlier right hemisphere neurodevelopment [69], with the anomalies remaining 14 years after the adolescent onset.

On frontal lobe sources

Frontal MMN sources varied more between groups on the rostrocaudal axis rather than on the lateral axis, as seen for sources in the temporal lobe. In the young patients the right frontal source was more caudo-medial than in their controls, but with similar lateral coordinates to the older controls. The dipoles were more rostrally located in younger and older controls. This more caudal locus in the patients is consistent with data for the frequency-deviant MMN [42] and was tentatively interpreted to reflect delayed development in that part of the forebrain which matures latest [60]. Indeed, prefrontal grey-matter is decreased specifically in adult first-episode patients with schizophrenia, rather than affective psychoses [70]. This could account for sources remaining in a caudal location. The right frontal source of the older patients was located similarly to that in the controls. Yet as this source lay more rostral to that in the EOS patients, it probably reflects normal developmental expansion in the third decade of life.
Possibly also reflecting the illness and its duration the left cingular source in the S-14Y group was more medial than the controls, and more dorsal than in the young patients. But like the younger patients the cingular source was more caudally located than in the controls. It is intriguing that the left cingular source in young patients was more medio-caudally located than in the young healthy controls, but in a similar locus to the older controls. This result has some similarity with the results for the other dipoles (above). But it differs from changes we reported for the detection of frequency-deviants [42], where the patients' dipoles were located rostral to the controls. Thus we re-examined the data with low resolution electromagnetic tomography (LORETA [71]). This confirmed a rather rostrally located bilateral activation of the medial frontal gyri, albeit at the expense of right frontal activity, precisely in the two analyses showing the most rostral BESA solutions (namely the frequency-deviant source in the older patients and the duration-deviant source in the controls). While the degree of blending of activated regions in the LORETA solution may be accounted for by the smoothing effect of the underlying algorhythm, it remains difficult to account for the different directions of source changes seen in patients with the two types of deviant stimulus.

Associations of MMN

Studies of normal subjects demonstrate that temporal lobe contributions to MMN represent the short-term memory templates for standard tones against which deviants can be contrasted. Frontal sources, active after deviant detection, initiate a switch to controlled processing to allow the significance for response to be assessed [6, 7]. The cingulate source, active simultaneously with those in the auditory cortices, may be involved in monitoring the ongoing situation prior to the later engagement of the inferior frontal regions when discord is detected. This is similar to the inferences from studies of the processing of incongruent stimuli [72]. Generically, the function is similar to the mismatch of expectations registered by the error-related negativity that also has BESA-calculated dipole-sources in the cingulate cortex [73].
Trail-making abilities to switch between sets are associated with prefrontal function [74]. Exploratory analyses indicated that in the more severely ill young patients a reduced MMN from the mastoids was associated with poor trail-making scores. This is taken as broadly supporting the attribution of the ability to switch between channels of information processing to one of the functions represented in MMN. (We note there is considerable reason to doubt whether MMN recorded at the mastoids uniquely reflects the output of supratemporal generators, discussion in [75].)
Imaging studies show that digit-span measures of working memory rely on recruiting activity in a network that includes the right dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices [76]. Despite one negative report for healthy subjects [77] correlations with MMN were anticipated (see introduction). Superficially the correlations for improved digit-span measures with decreased mastoid measures of MMN in healthy subjects and vice versa in patients are the opposite of what would be expected if one predicts an association with MMN's mnemonic role. However, if the main function of deviant detection by MMN (at the mastoid) is the facility for switching (previous paragraph), then in the context of remembering a series of digits, longer spans with fewer errors would be more likely in those showing a less marked MMN. The association of longer spans with larger MMN in patients could reflect either the impaired switching facility or a relatively improved mnemonic role for MMN, perhaps reflecting the function of the supratemporal generator. This interpretation accords with the negative association reported for verbal memory performance with MR-activity in the right prefrontal region just posterior to the dipole source discussed here [78].
It is remarkable that the associations for SANS/SAPS symptom-clusters with frontal and mastoid MMN were largely restricted to negative symptoms of anergia and positive hallucinations. The former was associated with reduced mastoid MMN in the EOS group, as reported elsewhere for duration-deviant MMN in the right hemisphere [1]. In the S-14Y patients hallucination ratings were related negatively to measures at frontal electrodes, also reported previously for left hemisphere MMN recordings [36]. This is consistent with the attribution of these symptoms to impaired functional connectivity between the temporal and frontal lobes [79, 80]. The prominent role for negative symptoms in the EOS group may reflect their prevalence and significance in the development of psychosis in adolescence, when the earlier onset has a poorer prognosis [81] and patients frequently show reduced frontal volumes [82]. However, we cannot exclude that this role would apply to chronic patients with prominent negative symptoms considering a report that MMN deficits in patients ill for 18 years accounted for 42% of the variance in general assessments of function [83]. Thus, here a positive association with MMN for the negative symptoms of anergia or apathy in the S-14Y group was unexpected. Speculatively this could be a sign of the sort of strategy (withdrawal) acquired by patients who have had to cope (relatively succesfully) with this illness for many years. This requires further investigation.

The MMN model

Sources of MMN activity in the frontal, cingulate and temporal cortices have been proposed without detailed localization [1, 2, 84]. Details were restricted to MEG studies of the auditory cortices [33] until the report from Jemel et al. [3]. The reliability of such models depends on the signal-to-noise ratio. A reasonable effect size requires data for 6 electrodes per dipole from a thousand trials [85, 86]. This was achieved here for the group solution (1174–1792 trials), where the stability (RV<2%) was assisted by being hypothesis led [3, 42]. For the individual solutions, that provided a measure of variance between subjects, the data from 80% of the subjects still explained 96% of the variance in this model. Accuracy was facilitated by the digitization of recording sites with respect to individual skull markers. Further, we checked the stability of the group solution over successive millimetres away from the point solution [87] and found they were usually stable over 2–6 mm along each dimension. This order of magnitude for activated tissue is physiologically more plausible than the calculated point solution. Another estimate of the volume activated, or of the error in calculating its centroid is provided by the variability between subjects. Despite the poor signal-to-noise ratio this was usually well below a centimetre (Table 3). This upper limit for the model's accuracy matches that obtained from known modelled sources [88, 89]. But, in addition to the problems of inter-individual stability of the model, error is introduced in the placing of sources on the brain atlas. Discrepancies between MRI anatomical representations of the locus of an individual's source and its localization on an averaged template can be marked. One study of this issue reported differences in nearly two thirds of the cases examined [90]. We have partly corrected for this by representing sources on the anatomy of one of our subjects (figure 4). But without constructing an average brain image from the participants, we have not accounted for the anatomical variability between our subjects.

Conclusion

In summary, superimposed on the post-adolescent brain expansion manifest on the lateral axis in the temporal lobes and rostro-caudally in the frontal lobes, separate anomalies of structural-functional relationships develop along these 2 axes in psychotic patients. We propose that in the temporal lobes a ventro-lateral migration of the MMN sources occurs secondarily to ventricle expansion. In the frontal lobes the lack of a rostral migration of MMN sources reflects delayed post-adolescent development of prefrontal regions in psychotic patients. An MMN impairment was marked in association with symptoms of withdrawal (anergia, flat-affect) and a lack of cognitive flexibility (attentional set-switching).
Scalp records of MMN show a modest recovery of change-detection between the EOS and S-14Y groups. However, in both the younger and in the older patients dipole moments and latencies were diffusely weaker and delayed compared to controls. These results may reflect a modicum of adaptive plasticity. The significant movement of the centres of activity giving rise to the MMN dipoles may reflect this as plasticity, expansion or migration of these functional centres. Alternatively the changed locations may represent a progressive illness-dependent impairment requiring adaptation elsewhere in central nervous function.

Acknowledgements

The authors gratefully acknowledge the technical assistance of Else Chan, Ute Pfueller, Kerstin Herwig, Raquel Torres. LBO and NWW were supported by the Alfried Krupp von Bohlen and Halbach Stiftung and SAJ by the IFORES, Essen. Preliminary data were presented at the 12th Winter Workshop on Schizophrenia, Davos (Feb. 2004) and at the 16th IACAPAP Congress, Berlin (Aug. 2004).
Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​2.​0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

NWW carried out most of the recordings, and their analysis, and contributed to drafting the manuscript; SAJ assisted NWW with the recordings and contributed to the analysis. LBO carried out some early recordings and analyses. RDO conceived planned and organised the study, ran the initial recordings and drafted most of the MS. JS and BR were involved in recruitment, diagnosis and neuropsychological testing and initial analyses of these data. All authors read and approved the draft manuscript.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Kasai K, Nakagome K, Itoh K, Koshida I, Hata A, Iwanami A, Fukuda M, Kato N: Impaired cortical network for preattentive detection of change in speech sounds in schizophrenia: a high resolution event-related potential study. Am J Psychiat. 2002, 159: 546-553. 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.4.546.CrossRefPubMed Kasai K, Nakagome K, Itoh K, Koshida I, Hata A, Iwanami A, Fukuda M, Kato N: Impaired cortical network for preattentive detection of change in speech sounds in schizophrenia: a high resolution event-related potential study. Am J Psychiat. 2002, 159: 546-553. 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.4.546.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Valkonen-Korhonen M, Purhonen M, Tarkka IM, Sipilä P, Partanen J, Karhu J, Lehtonen J: Altered auditory consciousness in acutely psychotic, never-medicated first-episode patients. Cogn Brain Res. 2003, 17: 747-758. 10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00199-X.CrossRef Valkonen-Korhonen M, Purhonen M, Tarkka IM, Sipilä P, Partanen J, Karhu J, Lehtonen J: Altered auditory consciousness in acutely psychotic, never-medicated first-episode patients. Cogn Brain Res. 2003, 17: 747-758. 10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00199-X.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Jemel B, Achenbach C, Müller B, Röpcke B, Oades RD: Mismatch negativity results from bilateral asymmetric dipole sources in the frontal and temporal lobes. Brain Topogr. 2002, 15: 13-27. 10.1023/A:1019944805499.CrossRefPubMed Jemel B, Achenbach C, Müller B, Röpcke B, Oades RD: Mismatch negativity results from bilateral asymmetric dipole sources in the frontal and temporal lobes. Brain Topogr. 2002, 15: 13-27. 10.1023/A:1019944805499.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Juran SA, Oknina LB, Oades RD, Torres R, Herwig K, Weisbrod M, Chan E, Chen EYH, Röpcke B: Brain sources for change detection: an ERP-study in patients at the outbreak of early-onset schizophrenia and 15-years later. Schizophr Res. 2004, 67: 129-130. Juran SA, Oknina LB, Oades RD, Torres R, Herwig K, Weisbrod M, Chan E, Chen EYH, Röpcke B: Brain sources for change detection: an ERP-study in patients at the outbreak of early-onset schizophrenia and 15-years later. Schizophr Res. 2004, 67: 129-130.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Tillmann B, Janata P, Bharucha JJ: Activation of the inferior frontal cortex in musical priming. Cogn Brain Res. 2003, 16: 145-161. 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00245-8.CrossRef Tillmann B, Janata P, Bharucha JJ: Activation of the inferior frontal cortex in musical priming. Cogn Brain Res. 2003, 16: 145-161. 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00245-8.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Schröger E: A neural mechanism for involuntary attention shifts to changes in auditory stimulation. J Cog Neurosci. 1996, 8: 527-539.CrossRef Schröger E: A neural mechanism for involuntary attention shifts to changes in auditory stimulation. J Cog Neurosci. 1996, 8: 527-539.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Escera C, Alho K, Winkler I, Näätänen R: Neural mechanisms of involuntary attention to acoustic novelty and change. J Cog Neurosci. 1998, 10: 590-604. 10.1162/089892998562997.CrossRef Escera C, Alho K, Winkler I, Näätänen R: Neural mechanisms of involuntary attention to acoustic novelty and change. J Cog Neurosci. 1998, 10: 590-604. 10.1162/089892998562997.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Javitt DC, Shelley AM, Grochowski S, Ritter W: Mismatch negativity as an index of impaired auditory sensory memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1995, 15: 179- Javitt DC, Shelley AM, Grochowski S, Ritter W: Mismatch negativity as an index of impaired auditory sensory memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1995, 15: 179-
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Kircher TTJ, Rapp A, Grodd W, Buchkremer G, Weiskopf N, Lutzenberger W, Ackermann H, Mathiak K: Mismatch negativity responses in schizophrenia: a combined fMRI and whole-head MEG study. Am J Psychiat. 2004, 161: 294-304. 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.294.CrossRefPubMed Kircher TTJ, Rapp A, Grodd W, Buchkremer G, Weiskopf N, Lutzenberger W, Ackermann H, Mathiak K: Mismatch negativity responses in schizophrenia: a combined fMRI and whole-head MEG study. Am J Psychiat. 2004, 161: 294-304. 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.294.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Pekkonen E: Auditory specific implicit memory in aging and Alzheimer's disease as reflected by the mismatch negativity. Proc 3rd Eur Congr Psychophysiol. 1997, May: Konstanz-Germany. Pekkonen E: Auditory specific implicit memory in aging and Alzheimer's disease as reflected by the mismatch negativity. Proc 3rd Eur Congr Psychophysiol. 1997, May: Konstanz-Germany.
11.
Zurück zum Zitat D'Esposito M, Postle BR, Rypma B: Prefrontal cortical contributions of working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies. Exp Brain Res. 1997, 133: 3-11. 10.1007/s002210000395.CrossRef D'Esposito M, Postle BR, Rypma B: Prefrontal cortical contributions of working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies. Exp Brain Res. 1997, 133: 3-11. 10.1007/s002210000395.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Zatorre RJ, Mondor TA, Evans AC: Auditory attention to space and frequency activates similar cerebral systems. Neuroimage. 1999, 10: 544-554. 10.1006/nimg.1999.0491.CrossRefPubMed Zatorre RJ, Mondor TA, Evans AC: Auditory attention to space and frequency activates similar cerebral systems. Neuroimage. 1999, 10: 544-554. 10.1006/nimg.1999.0491.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Zatorre RJ, Bouffard M, Belin P: Sensitivity to Auditory Object Features in Human Temporal Neocortex. J Neurosci. 2004, 24: 3637-3642. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5458-03.2004.CrossRefPubMed Zatorre RJ, Bouffard M, Belin P: Sensitivity to Auditory Object Features in Human Temporal Neocortex. J Neurosci. 2004, 24: 3637-3642. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5458-03.2004.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Baddeley AD: The magic number and the episodic buffer (Commentary in "The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity" by N. Cowan). Behav Brain Sci. 2001, 24: 117-118. 10.1017/S0140525X01253928.CrossRef Baddeley AD: The magic number and the episodic buffer (Commentary in "The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity" by N. Cowan). Behav Brain Sci. 2001, 24: 117-118. 10.1017/S0140525X01253928.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Cowan N, Winkler I, Teder W, Näätänen R: Memory prerequisites of mismatch negativity in the auditory event-related potential (ERP). J Exp Psychol (Learning, Memory & Cognition). 1993, 19: 909-921. 10.1037/0278-7393.19.4.909.CrossRef Cowan N, Winkler I, Teder W, Näätänen R: Memory prerequisites of mismatch negativity in the auditory event-related potential (ERP). J Exp Psychol (Learning, Memory & Cognition). 1993, 19: 909-921. 10.1037/0278-7393.19.4.909.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Escera C, Alho K, Schröger E, Winkler I: Involuntary attention and distractability as evaluated with event-related potentials. Audiol Neurootol. 2000, 5: 151-166. 10.1159/000013877.CrossRefPubMed Escera C, Alho K, Schröger E, Winkler I: Involuntary attention and distractability as evaluated with event-related potentials. Audiol Neurootol. 2000, 5: 151-166. 10.1159/000013877.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Baddeley AD: Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience. 2003, 4: 829-839. 10.1038/nrn1201.CrossRefPubMed Baddeley AD: Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience. 2003, 4: 829-839. 10.1038/nrn1201.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Mlakar J, Jensterle J, Frith CD: Central monitoring deficiency and schizophrenic symptoms. Psychol Med. 1994, 24: 557-564.CrossRefPubMed Mlakar J, Jensterle J, Frith CD: Central monitoring deficiency and schizophrenic symptoms. Psychol Med. 1994, 24: 557-564.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Strous RD, Cowan N, Ritter W, Javitt DC: Auditory sensory ("echoic") memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. 1995, 152: 1517-1519.CrossRefPubMed Strous RD, Cowan N, Ritter W, Javitt DC: Auditory sensory ("echoic") memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. 1995, 152: 1517-1519.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Hoff AL, Kremen WS: Neuropsychology in schizophrenia: an update. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2003, 16: 149-155. 10.1097/00001504-200303000-00003.CrossRef Hoff AL, Kremen WS: Neuropsychology in schizophrenia: an update. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2003, 16: 149-155. 10.1097/00001504-200303000-00003.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Javitt DC, Doneshka P, Grochowski S, Ritter W: Impaired mismatch negativity generation reflects widespread dysfunction of working memory in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiat. 1995, 52: 550-558.CrossRefPubMed Javitt DC, Doneshka P, Grochowski S, Ritter W: Impaired mismatch negativity generation reflects widespread dysfunction of working memory in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiat. 1995, 52: 550-558.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Oades RD, Zerbin D, Eggers C: Stimulus-Vergleichsprozesse bei psychotischen Jugendlichen mit paranoiden Symptomen: "Mismatch Negativity" deutet auf differenzierte Beeinträchtigungen hin. Biologische Psychiatrie der Gegenwart. Edited by: Baumann P. 1993, Vienna, Springer-Verlag, 69-73.CrossRef Oades RD, Zerbin D, Eggers C: Stimulus-Vergleichsprozesse bei psychotischen Jugendlichen mit paranoiden Symptomen: "Mismatch Negativity" deutet auf differenzierte Beeinträchtigungen hin. Biologische Psychiatrie der Gegenwart. Edited by: Baumann P. 1993, Vienna, Springer-Verlag, 69-73.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Catts SV, Shelley AM, Ward PB, Liebert B, McConaghy N, Andrews S, Michie PT: Brain potential evidence for an auditory sensory memory deficit in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. 1995, 152: 213-219.CrossRefPubMed Catts SV, Shelley AM, Ward PB, Liebert B, McConaghy N, Andrews S, Michie PT: Brain potential evidence for an auditory sensory memory deficit in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. 1995, 152: 213-219.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Umbricht D, Koller R, Schmid L, Skrabo A, Grübel C, Huber T, Stassen H: How specific are deficits in mismatch negativity generation to schizophrenia?. Biol Psychiat. 2003, 53: 1120-1131. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01642-6.CrossRefPubMed Umbricht D, Koller R, Schmid L, Skrabo A, Grübel C, Huber T, Stassen H: How specific are deficits in mismatch negativity generation to schizophrenia?. Biol Psychiat. 2003, 53: 1120-1131. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01642-6.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Oades RD, Zerbin D, Dittmann-Balcar A, Eggers C: Auditory event-related potential (ERP) and difference-wave topography in schizophrenic patients with/without active hallucinations and delusions: a comparison with young OCD and healthy subjects. Int J Psychophysiol. 1996, 22: 185-214. 10.1016/0167-8760(96)00026-8.CrossRefPubMed Oades RD, Zerbin D, Dittmann-Balcar A, Eggers C: Auditory event-related potential (ERP) and difference-wave topography in schizophrenic patients with/without active hallucinations and delusions: a comparison with young OCD and healthy subjects. Int J Psychophysiol. 1996, 22: 185-214. 10.1016/0167-8760(96)00026-8.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Oades RD: Connections between studies of the neurobiology of attention, psychotic processes and event-related potentials. Perspectives of Event-Related Potentials Research: Electroencephalography and clinical Neurophysiology suppl. 44. Edited by: Karmos G, Molnar M, Csepe V, Czigler I and Desmedt JE. 1995, Amsterdam, Elsevier, 428-438. Oades RD: Connections between studies of the neurobiology of attention, psychotic processes and event-related potentials. Perspectives of Event-Related Potentials Research: Electroencephalography and clinical Neurophysiology suppl. 44. Edited by: Karmos G, Molnar M, Csepe V, Czigler I and Desmedt JE. 1995, Amsterdam, Elsevier, 428-438.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, Griggs CB, Bonner-Jackson A, McCarley RW: Mismatch negativity in chronic schizophrenia and first-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2002, 59: 686-694. 10.1001/archpsyc.59.8.686.CrossRefPubMed Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, Griggs CB, Bonner-Jackson A, McCarley RW: Mismatch negativity in chronic schizophrenia and first-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2002, 59: 686-694. 10.1001/archpsyc.59.8.686.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Grzella I, Müller BW, Oades RD, Bender S, Schall U, Zerbin D, Wolstein J, Sartory G: Novelty-elicted mismatch negativity (MMN) in patients with schizophrenia on admission and discharge. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2001, 26: 235-246.PubMedPubMedCentral Grzella I, Müller BW, Oades RD, Bender S, Schall U, Zerbin D, Wolstein J, Sartory G: Novelty-elicted mismatch negativity (MMN) in patients with schizophrenia on admission and discharge. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2001, 26: 235-246.PubMedPubMedCentral
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Umbricht D, Javitt DC, Bates J, Kane J, Lieberman JA: Auditory event-related potentials (ERP): indices of both premorbid and illness-related progressive neuropathology in schizophrenia?. Schizophr Res. 2002, 53: 18- Umbricht D, Javitt DC, Bates J, Kane J, Lieberman JA: Auditory event-related potentials (ERP): indices of both premorbid and illness-related progressive neuropathology in schizophrenia?. Schizophr Res. 2002, 53: 18-
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Javitt DC, Shelley AM, Ritter W: Associated deficits in mismatch negativity generation and tone matching in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol. 2000, 111: 1733-1737. 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00377-1.CrossRefPubMed Javitt DC, Shelley AM, Ritter W: Associated deficits in mismatch negativity generation and tone matching in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol. 2000, 111: 1733-1737. 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00377-1.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Oades RD, Dittmann-Balcar A, Zerbin D, Grzella I: Impaired attention-dependent augmentation of MMN in nonparanoid vs paranoid schizophrenic patients: a comparison with obsessive-compulsive disorder and healthy subjects. Biol Psychiat. 1997, 41: 1196-1210. 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00214-4.CrossRefPubMed Oades RD, Dittmann-Balcar A, Zerbin D, Grzella I: Impaired attention-dependent augmentation of MMN in nonparanoid vs paranoid schizophrenic patients: a comparison with obsessive-compulsive disorder and healthy subjects. Biol Psychiat. 1997, 41: 1196-1210. 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00214-4.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Sharma T, Antonova L: Cognitive function in schizophrenia: deficits, functional consequences and future treatment. Psychiat Clin N Am. 2003, 26: 25-40. 10.1016/S0193-953X(02)00084-9.CrossRef Sharma T, Antonova L: Cognitive function in schizophrenia: deficits, functional consequences and future treatment. Psychiat Clin N Am. 2003, 26: 25-40. 10.1016/S0193-953X(02)00084-9.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Pekkonen E, Katila H, Ahveninen J, Karhu J, Houtilainen M, Tiihonen J: Impaired temporal lobe processing of preattentive auditory discrimination in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2002, 28: 467-474.CrossRefPubMed Pekkonen E, Katila H, Ahveninen J, Karhu J, Houtilainen M, Tiihonen J: Impaired temporal lobe processing of preattentive auditory discrimination in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2002, 28: 467-474.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Kasai K, Yamada H, Kamio S, Nakagome K, Iwanami A, Fukuda M, Yumoto M, Itoh K, Koshida I, Abe O, Kato N: Neuromagnetic correlates of impaired automatic categorical perception of speech sounds in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2003, 59: 159-172. 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00382-6.CrossRefPubMed Kasai K, Yamada H, Kamio S, Nakagome K, Iwanami A, Fukuda M, Yumoto M, Itoh K, Koshida I, Abe O, Kato N: Neuromagnetic correlates of impaired automatic categorical perception of speech sounds in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2003, 59: 159-172. 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00382-6.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Park HJ, Kwon JS, Youn T, Pae JS, Kim JJ, Kim MS, Ha KS: Statistical parametric mapping of LORETA using high density EEG and individual MRI: application to mismatch negativities in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002, 17: 168-178. 10.1002/hbm.10059.CrossRefPubMed Park HJ, Kwon JS, Youn T, Pae JS, Kim JJ, Kim MS, Ha KS: Statistical parametric mapping of LORETA using high density EEG and individual MRI: application to mismatch negativities in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002, 17: 168-178. 10.1002/hbm.10059.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Youn T, Park HJ, Kim JJ, Kim MS, Kwon JS: Altered hemispheric asymmetry and positive symptoms in schizophrenia: equivalent current dipole of auditory mismatch negativity. Schizophr Res. 2003, 59: 253-260. 10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00154-8.CrossRefPubMed Youn T, Park HJ, Kim JJ, Kim MS, Kwon JS: Altered hemispheric asymmetry and positive symptoms in schizophrenia: equivalent current dipole of auditory mismatch negativity. Schizophr Res. 2003, 59: 253-260. 10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00154-8.CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Antonova E, Sharma T, Morris R, Kumari V: The relationship between brain structure and neurocognition in schizophrenia: a selective review. Schizophr Res. 2004, 70: 117-145. 10.1016/j.schres.2003.12.002.CrossRefPubMed Antonova E, Sharma T, Morris R, Kumari V: The relationship between brain structure and neurocognition in schizophrenia: a selective review. Schizophr Res. 2004, 70: 117-145. 10.1016/j.schres.2003.12.002.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Mesulam MM: From sensation to cognition. Brain. 1998, 121: 1013-1052. 10.1093/brain/121.6.1013.CrossRefPubMed Mesulam MM: From sensation to cognition. Brain. 1998, 121: 1013-1052. 10.1093/brain/121.6.1013.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Colombo M, Rodman HR, Gross CG: The effects of superior temporal cortex lesions on the processing and retention of auditory information in monkeys (Cebus apella). J Neurosci. 1996, 4501-4517. Colombo M, Rodman HR, Gross CG: The effects of superior temporal cortex lesions on the processing and retention of auditory information in monkeys (Cebus apella). J Neurosci. 1996, 4501-4517.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Sato Y, Yabe H, Todd J, Michie PT, Shinozaki N, Sutoh T, Hiruma T, Nashida T, Matsuoka T, Kaneko S: Impairment in activation of a frontal attention-switch mechanism in schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychol. 2003, 62: 49-63. 10.1016/S0301-0511(02)00113-8.CrossRefPubMed Sato Y, Yabe H, Todd J, Michie PT, Shinozaki N, Sutoh T, Hiruma T, Nashida T, Matsuoka T, Kaneko S: Impairment in activation of a frontal attention-switch mechanism in schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychol. 2003, 62: 49-63. 10.1016/S0301-0511(02)00113-8.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Todd J, Michie PT, Jablensky AV: Duration mismatch negativity (MMN) reduction in schizophrenia indicates abnormalities in frontal relative to supratemporal generators. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2001, 2: 46- Todd J, Michie PT, Jablensky AV: Duration mismatch negativity (MMN) reduction in schizophrenia indicates abnormalities in frontal relative to supratemporal generators. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2001, 2: 46-
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Oknina LB, Wild-Wall N, Oades RD, Juran SA, Röpcke B, Pfueller U, Weisbrod M, Chan E, Chen EYH: Frontal and temporal sources of mismatch negativity in healthy controls, patients at onset of schizophrenia in adolescence and others at 15 years after onset. Schizophr Res. 2005, 76: 25-41. 10.1016/j.schres.2004.10.003.CrossRefPubMed Oknina LB, Wild-Wall N, Oades RD, Juran SA, Röpcke B, Pfueller U, Weisbrod M, Chan E, Chen EYH: Frontal and temporal sources of mismatch negativity in healthy controls, patients at onset of schizophrenia in adolescence and others at 15 years after onset. Schizophr Res. 2005, 76: 25-41. 10.1016/j.schres.2004.10.003.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Ballageer T, Malla A, Manchanda R, Takhar J, Haricharan R: Is Adolescent-Onset First-Episode Psychosis Different From Adult Onset?. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiat. 2005, 44: 782-789. 10.1097/01.chi.0000164591.55942.ea.CrossRef Ballageer T, Malla A, Manchanda R, Takhar J, Haricharan R: Is Adolescent-Onset First-Episode Psychosis Different From Adult Onset?. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiat. 2005, 44: 782-789. 10.1097/01.chi.0000164591.55942.ea.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Michie PT, Innes-Brown H, Todd J, Jablensky AV: Duration mismatch negativity in biological relatives of patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiat. 2003, 52: 749-758. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01379-3.CrossRef Michie PT, Innes-Brown H, Todd J, Jablensky AV: Duration mismatch negativity in biological relatives of patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiat. 2003, 52: 749-758. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01379-3.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Eggers C, Bunk D, Volberg G, Röpcke B: The ESSEN study of childhood onset schizophrenia: Selected results. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiat. 1999, 8: 21-28. 10.1007/s007870050123.CrossRef Eggers C, Bunk D, Volberg G, Röpcke B: The ESSEN study of childhood onset schizophrenia: Selected results. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiat. 1999, 8: 21-28. 10.1007/s007870050123.CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Nagy E, Potts GF, Loveland KA: Sex-related ERP differences in deviance detection. Int J Psychophysiol. 2003, 48: 285-292. 10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00042-4.CrossRefPubMed Nagy E, Potts GF, Loveland KA: Sex-related ERP differences in deviance detection. Int J Psychophysiol. 2003, 48: 285-292. 10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00042-4.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Aaltonen O, Eerola O, Lang A, Uusipaikka E, Tuomainen J: Automatic discrimination of phonetically relevant and irrelevant vowel parameters as reflected by mismatch negativity. J Acoust Soc Am. 1994, 96: 1489-1493. 10.1121/1.410291.CrossRefPubMed Aaltonen O, Eerola O, Lang A, Uusipaikka E, Tuomainen J: Automatic discrimination of phonetically relevant and irrelevant vowel parameters as reflected by mismatch negativity. J Acoust Soc Am. 1994, 96: 1489-1493. 10.1121/1.410291.CrossRefPubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosburg T: Left hemispheric dipole locations of the neuromagnetic mismatch negativity to frequency, intensity and duration deviants. Cogn Brain Res. 2003, 16: 83-90. 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00222-7.CrossRef Rosburg T: Left hemispheric dipole locations of the neuromagnetic mismatch negativity to frequency, intensity and duration deviants. Cogn Brain Res. 2003, 16: 83-90. 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00222-7.CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Andreasen NC, Olsen SA: Negative versus positive schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiat. 1982, 39: 789-794.CrossRefPubMed Andreasen NC, Olsen SA: Negative versus positive schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiat. 1982, 39: 789-794.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Blyler CR, Gold JM, Iannone VN, Buchanan RW: Short form of the WAIS-III for use with patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2000, 46: 209-215. 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00017-7.CrossRefPubMed Blyler CR, Gold JM, Iannone VN, Buchanan RW: Short form of the WAIS-III for use with patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2000, 46: 209-215. 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00017-7.CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Harris LR, Lieberman L: Auditory stimulus detection is not suppressed during saccadic eye movements. Percept. 1996, 25: 999-1004.CrossRef Harris LR, Lieberman L: Auditory stimulus detection is not suppressed during saccadic eye movements. Percept. 1996, 25: 999-1004.CrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Brebion G, Smith MJ, Gorman JM, Malaspina D, Sharif Z, Amador XF: Memory and schizophrenia: differential link of processing speed and selective attention with two levels of encoding. J Psychiatr Res. 2000, 34: 121-127. 10.1016/S0022-3956(99)00050-3.CrossRefPubMed Brebion G, Smith MJ, Gorman JM, Malaspina D, Sharif Z, Amador XF: Memory and schizophrenia: differential link of processing speed and selective attention with two levels of encoding. J Psychiatr Res. 2000, 34: 121-127. 10.1016/S0022-3956(99)00050-3.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Arbuthnott K, Frank J: Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: validation using a set-switching paradigm. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000, 22: 518-528.CrossRefPubMed Arbuthnott K, Frank J: Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: validation using a set-switching paradigm. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000, 22: 518-528.CrossRefPubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Berg P, Scherg M: A fast method for forward computation of multiple-shell spheical head models. Electroencephalogr clin Neurophysiol. 1994, 90: 58-64. 10.1016/0013-4694(94)90113-9.CrossRefPubMed Berg P, Scherg M: A fast method for forward computation of multiple-shell spheical head models. Electroencephalogr clin Neurophysiol. 1994, 90: 58-64. 10.1016/0013-4694(94)90113-9.CrossRefPubMed
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Scherg M, Picton TW: Separation and identification of event-related brain potential components by brain electric source analysis. Electroencephalogr clin Neurophysiol. 1991, Suppl.42: 24-37. Scherg M, Picton TW: Separation and identification of event-related brain potential components by brain electric source analysis. Electroencephalogr clin Neurophysiol. 1991, Suppl.42: 24-37.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Scherg M, Berg P: Use of a priori knowledge in brain electromagnetic brain activity. Brain Topogr. 1991, 4: 143-150. 10.1007/BF01132771.CrossRefPubMed Scherg M, Berg P: Use of a priori knowledge in brain electromagnetic brain activity. Brain Topogr. 1991, 4: 143-150. 10.1007/BF01132771.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Crottaz-Herbette S, Ragot R: Perception of complex sounds: N1 latency codes pitch, and topography codes spectra. Clin Neurophysiol. 2000, 111: 1759-1766. 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00422-3.CrossRefPubMed Crottaz-Herbette S, Ragot R: Perception of complex sounds: N1 latency codes pitch, and topography codes spectra. Clin Neurophysiol. 2000, 111: 1759-1766. 10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00422-3.CrossRefPubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Michie PT, Todd J, Schall U, Karayanidis F: Duration of illness and mismatch negativity in schizophrenia. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2005, 6: 52- Michie PT, Todd J, Schall U, Karayanidis F: Duration of illness and mismatch negativity in schizophrenia. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2005, 6: 52-
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Marco-Pallares J, Grau C, Ruffini G: Combined ICA-LORETA analysis of mismatch negativity. Neuroimage. 2005, 25: 471-477. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.028.CrossRefPubMed Marco-Pallares J, Grau C, Ruffini G: Combined ICA-LORETA analysis of mismatch negativity. Neuroimage. 2005, 25: 471-477. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.028.CrossRefPubMed
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Tessner KD, Toga AW: Mapping continued brain growth and gray matter density reduction in dorsal frontal cortex: Inverse relationships during postadolescent brain maturation. J Neurosci. 2001, 21: 8819-8829.PubMed Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Tessner KD, Toga AW: Mapping continued brain growth and gray matter density reduction in dorsal frontal cortex: Inverse relationships during postadolescent brain maturation. J Neurosci. 2001, 21: 8819-8829.PubMed
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Bartzokis G, Meckson M, Lu PH, Nuechterlein KH, Edwards N, Mintz J: Age-related changes in frontal and temporal lobe volumes in men: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2001, 58: 461-465. 10.1001/archpsyc.58.5.461.CrossRefPubMed Bartzokis G, Meckson M, Lu PH, Nuechterlein KH, Edwards N, Mintz J: Age-related changes in frontal and temporal lobe volumes in men: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2001, 58: 461-465. 10.1001/archpsyc.58.5.461.CrossRefPubMed
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Gogtay N, Sporn A, Clasen LS, Nugent TF, Greenstein D, Nicolson R, Giedd JN, Lenane M, Gochman P, Evans A, Rapoport JL: Comparison of progressive cortical gray matter loss in childhood-onset schizophrenia with that in childhood-onset atypical psychoses. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2004, 61: 17-22. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.1.17.CrossRefPubMed Gogtay N, Sporn A, Clasen LS, Nugent TF, Greenstein D, Nicolson R, Giedd JN, Lenane M, Gochman P, Evans A, Rapoport JL: Comparison of progressive cortical gray matter loss in childhood-onset schizophrenia with that in childhood-onset atypical psychoses. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2004, 61: 17-22. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.1.17.CrossRefPubMed
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Puri BK, Richardson AJ, Oatridge A, Hajnal JV, Saeed N: Cerebral ventricular asymmetry in schizophrenia: a high resolution 3D magnetic resonance imaging study. Int J Psychophysiol. 1999, 34: 207-211. 10.1016/S0167-8760(99)00078-1.CrossRefPubMed Puri BK, Richardson AJ, Oatridge A, Hajnal JV, Saeed N: Cerebral ventricular asymmetry in schizophrenia: a high resolution 3D magnetic resonance imaging study. Int J Psychophysiol. 1999, 34: 207-211. 10.1016/S0167-8760(99)00078-1.CrossRefPubMed
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Narr KL, Thompson PM, Sharma T, Moussai J, Blanton RE, Anvar B, Edris A, Krupp R, Rayman J, Khaledy M, Toga AW: Three-dimensional mapping of temporo-limbic regions and the lateral ventricles in schizophrenia: gender effects. Biol Psychiat. 2001, 50: 84-97. 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01120-3.CrossRefPubMed Narr KL, Thompson PM, Sharma T, Moussai J, Blanton RE, Anvar B, Edris A, Krupp R, Rayman J, Khaledy M, Toga AW: Three-dimensional mapping of temporo-limbic regions and the lateral ventricles in schizophrenia: gender effects. Biol Psychiat. 2001, 50: 84-97. 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01120-3.CrossRefPubMed
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Collinson SL, McKay CE, James AC, Quested DJ, Phillips T, Roberts N, Crow TJ: Brain volume, asymmetry and intellectual impairment in relation to sex in early-onset schizophrenia. Br J Psychiat. 2003, 183: 114-120. 10.1192/bjp.183.2.114.CrossRef Collinson SL, McKay CE, James AC, Quested DJ, Phillips T, Roberts N, Crow TJ: Brain volume, asymmetry and intellectual impairment in relation to sex in early-onset schizophrenia. Br J Psychiat. 2003, 183: 114-120. 10.1192/bjp.183.2.114.CrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Yamasue H, Yamada H, Yumoto M, Kamio S, Kudo N, Uetsuki M, Abe O, Fukuda R, Aoki S, Ohtomo K, Iwanami A, Kato N, Kasai K: Abnormal associations between reduced magnetic mismatch field to speech sounds and smaller left planum temporale volume in schizophrenia. Neuroimage. 2004, 22: 720-727. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.042.CrossRefPubMed Yamasue H, Yamada H, Yumoto M, Kamio S, Kudo N, Uetsuki M, Abe O, Fukuda R, Aoki S, Ohtomo K, Iwanami A, Kato N, Kasai K: Abnormal associations between reduced magnetic mismatch field to speech sounds and smaller left planum temporale volume in schizophrenia. Neuroimage. 2004, 22: 720-727. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.042.CrossRefPubMed
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Kasai K, Shenton ME, Salisbury DF, Hirayasu Y, Lee CU, Ciszewski AA, Yurgelun-Todd D, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, McCarley RW: Progressive decrease of left superior temporal gyrus gray matter volume in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. 2003, 160: 156-164. 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.156.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kasai K, Shenton ME, Salisbury DF, Hirayasu Y, Lee CU, Ciszewski AA, Yurgelun-Todd D, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, McCarley RW: Progressive decrease of left superior temporal gyrus gray matter volume in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. 2003, 160: 156-164. 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.156.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Matsumoto H, Simmon A, Williams S, Hadjulis M, Pipe R, Murray R, Frangou S: Superior temporal gyrus abnormalities in early-onset schizophrenia: similarities and differences with adult-onset schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. 2001, 158: 1299-1304. 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1299.CrossRefPubMed Matsumoto H, Simmon A, Williams S, Hadjulis M, Pipe R, Murray R, Frangou S: Superior temporal gyrus abnormalities in early-onset schizophrenia: similarities and differences with adult-onset schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. 2001, 158: 1299-1304. 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1299.CrossRefPubMed
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Geschwind N, Galaburda AM: Cerebral lateralization: biological mechanisms, associations and pathology. 1. A hypothesis and a program for research. Arch Neurol. 1985, 42: 428-459.CrossRefPubMed Geschwind N, Galaburda AM: Cerebral lateralization: biological mechanisms, associations and pathology. 1. A hypothesis and a program for research. Arch Neurol. 1985, 42: 428-459.CrossRefPubMed
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Hirayasu Y, Tanaka S, Shenton ME, Salisbury DF, DeSantis MA, Levitt JJ, Wible CG, Yurgelun-Todd DA, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, McCarley RW: Prefrontal gray matter volume reduction in first episode schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex. 2001, 11: 374-381. 10.1093/cercor/11.4.374.CrossRefPubMed Hirayasu Y, Tanaka S, Shenton ME, Salisbury DF, DeSantis MA, Levitt JJ, Wible CG, Yurgelun-Todd DA, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, McCarley RW: Prefrontal gray matter volume reduction in first episode schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex. 2001, 11: 374-381. 10.1093/cercor/11.4.374.CrossRefPubMed
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Pascual-Marqui RD, Esslen M, Kochi K, Lehmann D: Functional imaging with low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA): A review. Methods & Findings in Exp Clin Pharmacol. 2002, 24(C): 91-95. Pascual-Marqui RD, Esslen M, Kochi K, Lehmann D: Functional imaging with low resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA): A review. Methods & Findings in Exp Clin Pharmacol. 2002, 24(C): 91-95.
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Carter CS, MacDonald AW, Ross LL, Stenger VA: Anterior cingulate cortex activity and impaired self-monitoring of performance in patients with schizophrenia: an event-related fMRI study. Am J Psychiat. 2001, 158: 1423-1428. 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.9.1423.CrossRefPubMed Carter CS, MacDonald AW, Ross LL, Stenger VA: Anterior cingulate cortex activity and impaired self-monitoring of performance in patients with schizophrenia: an event-related fMRI study. Am J Psychiat. 2001, 158: 1423-1428. 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.9.1423.CrossRefPubMed
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Nieuwenhuis S, Holroyd CB, Mol N, Coles MGH: Reinforcement-related brain potentials from medial frontal cortex: origins and significance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004, 28: 441-448. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.05.003.CrossRefPubMed Nieuwenhuis S, Holroyd CB, Mol N, Coles MGH: Reinforcement-related brain potentials from medial frontal cortex: origins and significance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004, 28: 441-448. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.05.003.CrossRefPubMed
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Stuss DT, Levine B: Adult clinical neuropsychology: lessons from studies of the frontal lobes. Ann Rev Psychol. 2002, 53: 401-433. 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135220.CrossRef Stuss DT, Levine B: Adult clinical neuropsychology: lessons from studies of the frontal lobes. Ann Rev Psychol. 2002, 53: 401-433. 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135220.CrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Todd J, Michie PT, Jablensky AV: Association between reduced duration mismatch negativity (MMN) and raised temporal discrimination thresholds in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003, 114: 2061-2070. 10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00246-3.CrossRefPubMed Todd J, Michie PT, Jablensky AV: Association between reduced duration mismatch negativity (MMN) and raised temporal discrimination thresholds in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003, 114: 2061-2070. 10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00246-3.CrossRefPubMed
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Gerzon BK, Brown TT, Meyer-Lindenberg AS, Kohn P, Holt JL, Olsen JL, Berman KF: Shared and distinct neurophysiological components of the digits forward and backward tasks as revealed by functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia. 2004, 42: 1781-1787. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.023.CrossRef Gerzon BK, Brown TT, Meyer-Lindenberg AS, Kohn P, Holt JL, Olsen JL, Berman KF: Shared and distinct neurophysiological components of the digits forward and backward tasks as revealed by functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia. 2004, 42: 1781-1787. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.023.CrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Pekkonen E, Jääskeläinen IP, Kaakkola S, Ahveninen J: Cholinergic modulation of preattentive auditory processing in aging. Neuroimage. 2005, 27: 387-392. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.018.CrossRefPubMed Pekkonen E, Jääskeläinen IP, Kaakkola S, Ahveninen J: Cholinergic modulation of preattentive auditory processing in aging. Neuroimage. 2005, 27: 387-392. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.018.CrossRefPubMed
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Ino T, Doi T, Kimura T, Ito J, Fukuyama H: Neural substrates of the performance of an auditory verbal memory: between-subjects analysis by fMRI. Brain Res Bull. 2004, 64: 115-126. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2004.04.018.CrossRefPubMed Ino T, Doi T, Kimura T, Ito J, Fukuyama H: Neural substrates of the performance of an auditory verbal memory: between-subjects analysis by fMRI. Brain Res Bull. 2004, 64: 115-126. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2004.04.018.CrossRefPubMed
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Lawrie SM, Büchel C, Whalley HC, Frith CD, Friston KJ, Johnstone EC: Reduced frontotemporal functional connectivity in schizophrenia associated with auditory hallucinations. Biol Psychiat. 2002, 51: 1008-1013. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01316-1.CrossRefPubMed Lawrie SM, Büchel C, Whalley HC, Frith CD, Friston KJ, Johnstone EC: Reduced frontotemporal functional connectivity in schizophrenia associated with auditory hallucinations. Biol Psychiat. 2002, 51: 1008-1013. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01316-1.CrossRefPubMed
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Shergill SS, Brammer MJ, Willias SCR, Murray RM, McGuire PK: Mapping auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2000, 57: 1033-1038. 10.1001/archpsyc.57.11.1033.CrossRefPubMed Shergill SS, Brammer MJ, Willias SCR, Murray RM, McGuire PK: Mapping auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2000, 57: 1033-1038. 10.1001/archpsyc.57.11.1033.CrossRefPubMed
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Hollis C: Developmental precursors of child- and adolescent-onset schizophrenia and affective psychoses: diagnostic specificity and continuity with symptom dimensions. Br J Psychiat. 2003, 182: 37-44. 10.1192/bjp.182.1.37.CrossRef Hollis C: Developmental precursors of child- and adolescent-onset schizophrenia and affective psychoses: diagnostic specificity and continuity with symptom dimensions. Br J Psychiat. 2003, 182: 37-44. 10.1192/bjp.182.1.37.CrossRef
82.
Zurück zum Zitat James AC, James S, Smith DM, Javaloyes A: Cerebellar, Prefrontal Cortex, and Thalamic Volumes Over Two Time Points in Adolescent-Onset Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. 2004, 161: 1023-1029. 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.6.1023.CrossRefPubMed James AC, James S, Smith DM, Javaloyes A: Cerebellar, Prefrontal Cortex, and Thalamic Volumes Over Two Time Points in Adolescent-Onset Schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. 2004, 161: 1023-1029. 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.6.1023.CrossRefPubMed
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Light GA, Braff DL: Mismatch Negativity Deficits Are Associated With Poor Functioning in Schizophrenia Patients. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2005, 62: 127-136. 10.1001/archpsyc.62.2.127.CrossRefPubMed Light GA, Braff DL: Mismatch Negativity Deficits Are Associated With Poor Functioning in Schizophrenia Patients. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2005, 62: 127-136. 10.1001/archpsyc.62.2.127.CrossRefPubMed
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Waberski TD, Kreitschmann-Andermahr I, Kawohl W, Darvas F, Ryang Y, Gobbele R, Buchner H: Spatio-temporal source imaging reveals subcomponents of the human auditory mismatch negativity in the cingulum and right inferior temporal gyrus. Neurosci Lett. 2001, 308: 107-110. 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01988-7.CrossRefPubMed Waberski TD, Kreitschmann-Andermahr I, Kawohl W, Darvas F, Ryang Y, Gobbele R, Buchner H: Spatio-temporal source imaging reveals subcomponents of the human auditory mismatch negativity in the cingulum and right inferior temporal gyrus. Neurosci Lett. 2001, 308: 107-110. 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01988-7.CrossRefPubMed
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Kamijo KI, Yamazaki T, Kiyuna T, Takaki Y, Kuriowa Y: Visual event-related potentials during movement imagery and the dipole analysis. Brain Topogr. 2002, 14: 279-292. 10.1023/A:1015733127075.CrossRefPubMed Kamijo KI, Yamazaki T, Kiyuna T, Takaki Y, Kuriowa Y: Visual event-related potentials during movement imagery and the dipole analysis. Brain Topogr. 2002, 14: 279-292. 10.1023/A:1015733127075.CrossRefPubMed
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Phillips C, Rugg MD, Friston KJ: Systematic regularization of linear inverse solutions of the EEG source localization problem. Neuroimage. 2002, 17: 287-301. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1175.CrossRefPubMed Phillips C, Rugg MD, Friston KJ: Systematic regularization of linear inverse solutions of the EEG source localization problem. Neuroimage. 2002, 17: 287-301. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1175.CrossRefPubMed
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Jemel B, Oades RD, Oknina L, Achenbach C, Röpcke B: Frontal and temporal lobe sources for a marker of controlled auditory attention: the negative difference (Nd) event-related potential. Brain Topogr. 2003, 15: 249-262. 10.1023/A:1023915730566.CrossRefPubMed Jemel B, Oades RD, Oknina L, Achenbach C, Röpcke B: Frontal and temporal lobe sources for a marker of controlled auditory attention: the negative difference (Nd) event-related potential. Brain Topogr. 2003, 15: 249-262. 10.1023/A:1023915730566.CrossRefPubMed
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Yvert B, Bertrand O, Thevenet M, Echallier JF, Pernier J: A systematic evaluation of the spherical model accuracy in EEG dipole localization. Electroencephalogr clin Neurophysiol. 1997, 102: 452-459. 10.1016/S0921-884X(97)96611-X.CrossRefPubMed Yvert B, Bertrand O, Thevenet M, Echallier JF, Pernier J: A systematic evaluation of the spherical model accuracy in EEG dipole localization. Electroencephalogr clin Neurophysiol. 1997, 102: 452-459. 10.1016/S0921-884X(97)96611-X.CrossRefPubMed
89.
Zurück zum Zitat Laarne PH, Tenhunen-Eskelinen ML, Hyttinen JK, Eskola HJ: Effect of EEG electrode density on dipole localization accuracy using two realistically shaped skull resistivity models. Brain Topogr. 2000, 12: 249-254. 10.1023/A:1023422504025.CrossRefPubMed Laarne PH, Tenhunen-Eskelinen ML, Hyttinen JK, Eskola HJ: Effect of EEG electrode density on dipole localization accuracy using two realistically shaped skull resistivity models. Brain Topogr. 2000, 12: 249-254. 10.1023/A:1023422504025.CrossRefPubMed
90.
Zurück zum Zitat Ha KS, Youn T, Kong SW, Park HJ, Ha TH, Kim MS, Kwon JS: Optimized individual mismatch negativity source localization using a realistic head model and the Talairach coordinate system. Brain Topogr. 2003, 15: 233-238. 10.1023/A:1023910108759.CrossRefPubMed Ha KS, Youn T, Kong SW, Park HJ, Ha TH, Kim MS, Kwon JS: Optimized individual mismatch negativity source localization using a realistic head model and the Talairach coordinate system. Brain Topogr. 2003, 15: 233-238. 10.1023/A:1023910108759.CrossRefPubMed
91.
Zurück zum Zitat Oldfield RC: The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971, 9: 97-113. 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4.CrossRefPubMed Oldfield RC: The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971, 9: 97-113. 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4.CrossRefPubMed
92.
Zurück zum Zitat Erikson R, Goldthorpe JH: Individual or family? Results from two approaches to class assignment. Acta Sociol. 2005, 35: 95-106.CrossRef Erikson R, Goldthorpe JH: Individual or family? Results from two approaches to class assignment. Acta Sociol. 2005, 35: 95-106.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Auditory change detection in schizophrenia: sources of activity, related neuropsychological function and symptoms in patients with a first episode in adolescence, and patients 14 years after an adolescent illness-onset
verfasst von
Robert D Oades
Nele Wild-Wall
Stephanie A Juran
Jan Sachsse
Ljubov B Oknina
Bernd Röpcke
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2006
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2006
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-6-7

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2006

BMC Psychiatry 1/2006 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).