Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Neurology 1/2015

Open Access 01.12.2015 | Research article

Coma recovery scale-r: variability in the disorder of consciousness

verfasst von: MD. Cortese, F. Riganello, F. Arcuri, ME. Pugliese, LF. Lucca, G. Dolce, WG. Sannita

Erschienen in: BMC Neurology | Ausgabe 1/2015

download
DOWNLOAD
print
DRUCKEN
insite
SUCHEN

Abstract

Background

Despite evidence from neuroimaging research, diagnosis and early prognosis in the vegetative (VS/UWS) and minimally conscious (MCS) states still depend on the observation of clinical signs of responsiveness. Multiple testing has documented a systematic variability during the day in the incidence of established signs of responsiveness. Spontaneous fluctuations of the Coma Recovery Scale-revised (CRS-r) scores are conceivable.

Methods

We retrospectively analyzed the CRS-r repeatedly administered to 7 VS/UWS and 12 MCS subjects undergoing systematic observation during a conventional 13 weeks. rehabilitation plan.

Results

The CRS-r global, visual and auditory scores were found higher in the morning than at the afternoon administration in both VS/UWS and MCS subgroups over the entire period of observation. The probability for a VS/UWS subject of being classified as MCS at the morning testing at least once during the 13 weeks. observation was as high as 30 %, i.e., compatible with the reported misdiagnosis rate between the two clinical conditions.

Conclusions

Multiple CRS-r testing is advisable to minimize the risk of misclassification; estimates of spontaneous variability could be used to characterize with greater accuracy patients with disorder of consciousness and possibly help optimize the rehabilitation plan.
Hinweise

Competing interests

The authors declare they have no financial or non-financial competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MD, FR, FA, and WGS, were involved in the conception and design of the study. FR performed the statistical analysis. MD, FR, FA, and WGS were involved in the analysis and interpretation of data. All authors were involved in the drafting and revision of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ information

Not applicable.
Abkürzungen
VS/UWS
Vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome
MCS
Minimally conscious state
CRS-r
Coma recovery scale-revised
SE
Standard error

Background

Diagnosis and the early prognosis of subjects in the vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (VS/UWS) [1, 2] or minimally conscious state (MCS) [37] still depend on the clinical evaluation of responsiveness [4, 5, 8], while functional assessment by neuroimaging remains mostly limited to research [5, 915]. The diagnostic error between the VS/UWS and MCS reportedly still hovers up to 25–45 %, with possible implications in the patients’ prognosis, treatment, allocation of resources, etc. [3, 4, 16]. Clinical scales have been developed to provide the attending physician and therapist with reliable criteria of behavioral assessment and to allow more accurate evaluations [3, 16, 17]. Among these, the Coma Recovery Scale - revised (CRS-r) is an established and widely used tool [18, 19], with higher percentage of MCS subjects correctly diagnosed and better overall classification accuracy than the current clinical criteria [7].
The within/between subject (spontaneous) variability of the signs in use to evaluate brain responsiveness in VS/UWS and MCS is a possible critical issue adding to the existing controversy on their pathophysiological meaning [17, 20]. The visual pursuit response, a major CRS-r item recognizable in 70–80 % of subjects in MCS [9, 21, 22] and key marker of evolution from VS/UWS [23, 24], has been also reported with lower incidence (~20–30 %) in subjects otherwise diagnosed as VS/UWS [20, 2528]. Multiple testing has documented systematic spontaneous fluctuations in the incidence of this response in the course of the day in both VS/UWS and MCS subjects; the overall chance of observing it at least once per day was ~33 % and ~62 % in the VS/UWS and MCS subgroups, respectively, with maxima at 10.30 am and 3.00 pm and no response at postprandial time in both subjects’ groups [29]. Individual variability may suggest limited diagnostic accuracy for the visual pursuit response and, by extension, risk of erroneous CRS-r classification when subjects are tested at a single time point of the day. Purpose of this study was to analyze the spontaneous fluctuation over time of the CRS-r scores.

Methods

We studied retrospectively the CRS-r scores obtained from two subgroups of subjects with disorder of consciousness undergoing a standard rehabilitation plan. They had been diagnosed by the attending physician as being in chronic VS/UWS (n = 7; age 51.8 ± 11.6 years.) or MCS (n = 12; age 51.4 ± 17.4 years.) at the beginning of treatment; diagnosis was based on the current clinical criteria and guidelines by the Aspen group [19]. The CRS-r global score was equal or lower than 7 (mean = 6.42 ± 0.49) in VS/UWS subjects and between 9 and 15 in the MCS patients (mean = 11.08 ± 1.75). Subjects clinically unstable, under treatment with (neuro)active drugs or beta-blockers, with anoxia, history of psychiatric disorder, concurrent systemic diseases, or evidence of recurrent pain were not considered. Subjects requiring Arousal Facilitation Protocol procedures (AFP) were also excluded in order to avoid the interference of these procedures on their responses to the CRS-r stimulus condition. The VS/UWS and MCS subjects’ subgroups did not differ as to age (Mann-Whitney’s test: z = −0.466, p > 0.05) or length of hospitalization (1144.3 ± 551.2 days in VS/UWS and 1711.4 ± 455 days in MCS; z = −1945, p > 0.05). Summary demographics and clinical information are reported in Tables 1 and 2.
Table 1
Summary demographics and clinical information at the beginning of the rehabilitation plan. (The patients’ relatives and caregivers gave their consent to the use of the patients’ data)
 
Sex
Age range
Aetiology
CRS-r
Time from brain injury (days)
Total score
Visual subscore
Auditory subscore
VS/UWS
Female
38–73
Vascular
6
1
1
662
Other
7
0
1
2278
Vascular
6
1
1
663
Vascular
7
0
1
958
Vascular
6
0
2
1256
Male
41–65
Traumatic
6
1
1
1199
Traumatic
7
1
1
1201
MCS
Male
31–65
Vascular
9
3
2
1199
Traumatic
10
3
2
1492
Traumatic
10
3
0
1958
Traumatic
13
3
1
2380
Traumatic
11
2
1
2455
Traumatic
11
2
1
2345
Traumatic
15
3
3
2399
Female
35–79
Traumatic
9
3
2
2251
Vascular
10
3
1
1393
Vascular
10
3
1
1773
Vascular
13
2
1
2298
Traumatic
12
3
1
1542
Table 2
Summary demographics and clinical information at the beginning of the rehabilitation plan. (The patients’ relatives and caregivers gave their consent to the use of the patients’ data)
Score
Auditory scale
Visual scale
Motor scale
Oromotor/Verbal scale
Communication scale
Arousal scale
6
-
-
Functional Object Use
-
-
-
5
-
Object Recognition
Automatic Motor Response
-
-
-
4
Consistent movement to command
Object Localization: Reaching
Object Manipulation
-
-
-
3
Reproducible Movement to Command
Visual Pursuit
Localization to Noxious Stimulation
Intelligible Verbalization
-
Attention
2
Localization to Sound
Fixation
Flexion Withdrawal
Vocalization/Oral Movement
Functional: Accurate
Eye Opening w/o Stimulation
1
Auditory Startle
Visual Startle
Abnormal Posturing
Oral Reflexive Movement
Non-Functional: Intentional
Eye Opening with Stimulation
0
None
None
None/Flaccid
None
None
Unarousable
The study has been approved by the local public health care Ethical Committee (Provincial Health Authority of Crotone). The patients’ relatives and caregivers were informed about the study aims and gave their consent to the use of the patients’ data, which have been always treated under condition of anonymity. The ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki (1964) by the World Medical Association concerning human experimentation were followed.
Patients were treated following a standard plan with alternating 2 weeks. periods of intensive rehabilitation (two sessions/day, morning and afternoon; hereafter Phase A) and 1 week. periods of standard rehabilitation (one session/day; Phase B) for a total of 13 weeks. The protocol began and ended with the phase B (Fig. 2). Subjects received nursing care before 9.00 a.m. and after 5.00 p.m. and were fed at noon compliant to the unit schedule; the rehabilitation sessions and CRS-r evaluation always began at least 30 min. after the morning nursing care in order to avoid induced arousal or other possible interference.
CRS-r was always administered before rehabilitation and in the two time windows of the day when responsiveness had previously proved highest in a previous study (9.30–11.00 a.m.; 3.00–4.00 p.m.) [29]. CRS-r administration was once a week in phases A (in the morning the first week and in the afternoon the second week); in phases B, it was administered in the morning and afternoon at the beginning and end of the week. The CRS-r was administered following the international guidelines by two expert examiners (a neuropsychologist and an occupational therapist) who were requested to examine each patient together and to reach an agreement on each measured item. When tested, patients were comfortably sitting on an armchair in a familiar setting with a constant temperature of 24 °C in the absence of environmental noise or interference; in no case, patients were reported to have showed discomfort in the presence of the examiners or during testing. Only patients with matching numbers of assessments were considered in the study. Scores from 342 CRS-r administrations were obtained in total (18 times per subject).
The CRS-r global scores and subscores at the morning and afternoon ratings and at the beginning and end of the protocol were compared. Statistical processing was by Wilcoxon signed-rank test, after collapsing the data from each patient into a pair of values to avoid alpha inflation due to the sample size when comparing conditions (morning vs afternoon; baseline vs. end of treatment). To this end, nine values from the morning assessment and the corresponding nine values at the afternoon administration (4 from phase A and 5 from phase B) were averaged. The effect size (r) (i.e., the index measuring the magnitude of difference or change between two conditions, in this case baseline vs. end of the protocol and morning vs. afternoon) [30] was calculated as the z/square root (N) (where N is the number of observations on which z is based) and will be hereafter formally referred to as not relevant (r < 0.1), small (0.1 < r < 0.3), medium (0.3 < r < 0.5), or large (r > 0.5) [31]. The Bonferroni correction for multiple testing was applied for a p < 0.005 level of statistical significance on ten comparisons between morning and afternoon. The risk of misclassification of subjects in VS/UWS or MCS to the other clinical condition when relying only on a single (morning or afternoon) assessment was tested by the Odds Ratio and Risk Ratio in both subjects’ groups.

Results and discussion

Individual CRS-r scores at the morning and afternoon scale administration are shown in Fig. 1. In both VS/UWS and MCS subgroups and over the entire period of observation, the mean CRS-r global scores were higher at the morning assessment (7 ± 1.5 and 11 ± 1.9 for VS/UWS and MCS, respectively) than in the afternoon (6.3 ± 1.3 and 10.1 ± 1.9) (Wilcoxon’s z = −3.916, p < 0.0001, r = 1.04 and z = −5.195, p < 0.0001, r = 1.06) (Fig. 2). Scores were higher in the morning in both Phases A and B in the MCS subgroup (Wilcoxon’s z = −3.513, p[correct] < 0.001, r = 0.75 and z = −3.843, p[correct] = 0.018, r = 0.82, respectively), but only in Phase B the difference reached statistical significance in the VS/UWS subgroup (Wilcoxon’s z = −3.840, p[correct] < 0.001, r = 1.02). The morning vs. afternoon difference was observed also when considering posttraumatic subjects or patients with vascular brain injury separately (Wilcoxon’s z = 2.874, p[correct] = 0.024, r = 0.67 and Wilcoxon’ = s z = −4.415, p[correct] = 0.000, r = 0.75 respectively) (Fig. 1). It increased progressively over the 13 weeks. observation period in VS/UWS, while reaching a maximum between the 6th and 9th weeks of protocol to decline thereafter in the MCS subgroup (Figs. 2 and 3); as a result, the mean CRS-r global scores were higher at the end of the protocol compared to baseline in the VS/UWS (7.6 ± 2.4 and 5.9 ± 0.8, respectively; morning-afternoon average; Wilcoxon’s z: −2.347, p = 0.019, effect size: r = 0.63), but not in MCS (Figs. 2 and 3). The morning vs. afternoon difference in global score was accounted for by the visual and auditory items (Fig. 1) both in the MCS (Wilcoxon’s z = −3.062, p correct = 0.01, r = 0.65 and z = −3.721, p correct < 0.001, r = 0.79) and VS/UWS subjects’ groups (Wilcoxon’s z = −2.874, p correct = 0.02, r = 0.77 and z = −2.744, p correct = 0.03, r = 0.73). No significant contributions from the motor, oromotor/verbal, communication and arousal subscales to the differences in the CRS-r global score were observed.
The measure of association in VS/UWS subjects between the CRS-r scores at the morning or afternoon assessments and the risk of wrong attribution to the MCS condition was significant (odds ratio = 4.75; 95 % confidence interval: 1.73–12.7; p = 0.001; relative risk =1.4; 95 % coefficient interval: 1.15–1.7; p = 0.001). The risk was higher in the phases B (odds ratio = 5.09; 95 % confidence interval: 1.38–18.7; p = 0.014; relative risk =1.76; 95 % coefficient interval: 1.30–1.19; p = 0.014) than in phases A (odds ratio = 1.70; 95 % confidence interval: 10.51–2.55; p = 0.5; relative risk =0.78; 95 % coefficient interval: 2.04–1.86; p = 0.014). The risk of wrong classification was lower in MCS group (odds ratio = 0.49; 95 % confidence interval: 0.087–2.73; p = 0.44; relative risk =0.66; 95 % coefficient interval: 0.21–2.06; p = 0.44). The estimated probability of observing during the rehabilitation protocol CRS-r items compatible with MCS in subjects diagnosed as VS/UWS was 30 % at the morning (range: 0–55 %) and 9.5 % at the afternoon assessments (range: 0–22 %).
These observations derive from a retrospective analysis of the CRS-r scores obtained during a conventional rehabilitation protocol that had not been designed to test differences in the subjects’ responsiveness. In this respect, the differences between the protocol phases A and B or the possible effects of intensive treatment are not suitable of detailed investigation or pathophysiological interpretation. Prospective studies are needed for this purpose. This caveat notwithstanding, the results both invite speculation and suggest caution in the use of the available clinical assessment tools. The CRS-r was administered at the same two time windows during the day when responsiveness had previously proved highest in chronic VS/UWS and MCS subjects [29]. The global score and visual and auditory subscores nevertheless proved higher in the morning than in the afternoon, with the mean differences in the CRS-r global score increasing with time in the VS/UWS subjects’ subgroup; the morning vs. afternoon difference was about twice larger than the average improvement at the end of the protocol (54.2 % vs. 28.8 %) which confirms the large size effect estimated statistically. The variability of the CRS-r assessment was unrelated to etiology in our patients’ sample, but a relationship with etiology cannot be ruled out in principle.
A positive visual pursuit response (a major CRS-r item) results of activation in the anterior and posterior midline structures of the brain (mesiofrontal and precuneal cortices) [3235], which are metabolically impaired in the severe disorder of consciousness according to neuroimaging studies [36, 37]. Neuroimaging has also documented regional activation in response to stimulus conditions in VS/UWS [12, 14, 20, 38, 39]; responses to stimulus conditions purported to induce emotional reaction have been described [4042]. Whether any of these responses may indicate “automatic” subcortical processing atypical for the VS/UWS or it marks higher order cortical activation and partially recovered consciousness, remains an unsolved controversy [19]. Yet, the observation of responses such as these is conventionally regarded as indicative of surviving modules of the corticocortical and brainstem-cortex functional interaction that is thought to sustain consciousness in the awake subject [6, 4348] and is interfered with in the VS/UWS and MCS [2, 9, 10, 19, 21, 22, 36, 37, 39, 49, 50]. In this respect, the CRS-r reliability (as also documented by the relationship between EEG descriptors and CRS-r scores) [51] is not to be questioned on the ground of its variability during the day, nor is to be questioned the examiners’ accuracy. Instead, the observed CRS-r differences between morning and afternoon are likely to reflect individual changes in the subject’s level of visual, auditory and motor functioning conceivably due to changes in the neuronal/non-neuronal factors that modulate the brain state [52, 53]. Spontaneous fluctuations of any of these factors may be expected to result in random differences in responsiveness rather than in systematic difference during the day; effects of fragmentary circadian/ultradian or otherwise cyclic processes (e.g., in metabolism) are thus also conceivable, albeit not documentable in the absence of multiple assessments at short intervals over the 24 h. period [22].
The extent to which the within-day variability in the CRS-r global, visual and auditory scores observed in this study may have clinical relevance in the diagnosis and early prognosis of subjects with disorder of consciousness remains to be studied in large patients’ samples. Systematic investigation on the spontaneous variability over time of the relevant neuronal or non-neuronal parameters in the severe disorder of consciousness is also advisable and may help characterize these conditions in greater detail [52]. The possible source(s) of variability notwithstanding, the estimated risk of misclassification for a single random CRS-r testing appears compatible with the reported misdiagnosis between VS/UWS and MCS [3, 4, 6, 7].

Conclusions

Multiple CRS-r assessments at different times of the day and monitoring over time are advisable. Diagnostic criteria taking also individual variability into a proper account may help reduce misdiagnosis between conditions sharing the underlying pathophysiology, but differing as to clinical picture, prognosis, required medical care and logistics, legal or popular perception of bioethical issues, allocated resources, healthcare policies, etc. [1, 6, 40, 5456]. These considerations aside, the evidence of higher responsiveness in the morning compared to the afternoon invites discussion on whether the time window dedicated to treatment is irrelevant to rehabilitation or it should be planned accordingly.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Competing interests

The authors declare they have no financial or non-financial competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MD, FR, FA, and WGS, were involved in the conception and design of the study. FR performed the statistical analysis. MD, FR, FA, and WGS were involved in the analysis and interpretation of data. All authors were involved in the drafting and revision of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ information

Not applicable.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, León-Carrión J, Sannita WG, et al. Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med. 2010;8:68.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, León-Carrión J, Sannita WG, et al. Unresponsive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome. BMC Med. 2010;8:68.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Giacino JT, Kalmar K. Diagnostic and prognostic guidelines for the vegetative and minimally conscious states. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2005;15:166–74.CrossRefPubMed Giacino JT, Kalmar K. Diagnostic and prognostic guidelines for the vegetative and minimally conscious states. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2005;15:166–74.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Andrews K, Murphy L, Munday R, Littlewood C. Misdiagnosis of the vegetative state: retrospective study in a rehabilitation unit. BMJ. 1996;313:13–6.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Andrews K, Murphy L, Munday R, Littlewood C. Misdiagnosis of the vegetative state: retrospective study in a rehabilitation unit. BMJ. 1996;313:13–6.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Bosco A, Lancioni GE, Belardinelli MO, Singh NN, O’Reilly MF, Sigafoos J. Vegetative state: efforts to curb misdiagnosis. Cogn Process. 2010;11:87–90.CrossRefPubMed Bosco A, Lancioni GE, Belardinelli MO, Singh NN, O’Reilly MF, Sigafoos J. Vegetative state: efforts to curb misdiagnosis. Cogn Process. 2010;11:87–90.CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Celesia GG, Sannita WG. Can patients in vegetative state experience pain and have conscious awareness? Neurology. 2013;80:328–9.CrossRefPubMed Celesia GG, Sannita WG. Can patients in vegetative state experience pain and have conscious awareness? Neurology. 2013;80:328–9.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Dolce G, Sannita WG, European Task Force On The Vegetative State. The vegetative state: A syndrome seeking revision? Brain Inj. 2010;24:1628–9.CrossRefPubMed Dolce G, Sannita WG, European Task Force On The Vegetative State. The vegetative state: A syndrome seeking revision? Brain Inj. 2010;24:1628–9.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Schnakers C, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Giacino J, Ventura M, Boly M, Majerus S, et al. Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment. BMC Neurol. 2009;9:35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Schnakers C, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Giacino J, Ventura M, Boly M, Majerus S, et al. Diagnostic accuracy of the vegetative and minimally conscious state: clinical consensus versus standardized neurobehavioral assessment. BMC Neurol. 2009;9:35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Bodart O, Laureys S, Gosseries O. Coma and disorders of consciousness: scientific advances and practical considerations for clinicians. Semin Neurol. 2013;33:083–90.CrossRef Bodart O, Laureys S, Gosseries O. Coma and disorders of consciousness: scientific advances and practical considerations for clinicians. Semin Neurol. 2013;33:083–90.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Giacino JT, Hirsch J, Schiff N, Laureys S. Functional neuroimaging applications for assessment and rehabilitation planning in patients with disorders of consciousness. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2006;87(12, Supplement):67–76. Neuroplasticity and Brain Imaging Research: Implications for Rehabilitation.CrossRef Giacino JT, Hirsch J, Schiff N, Laureys S. Functional neuroimaging applications for assessment and rehabilitation planning in patients with disorders of consciousness. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2006;87(12, Supplement):67–76. Neuroplasticity and Brain Imaging Research: Implications for Rehabilitation.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Laureys S, Lemaire C, Maquet P, Phillips C, Franck G. Cerebral metabolism during vegetative state and after recovery to consciousness. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1999;67:121–2.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Laureys S, Lemaire C, Maquet P, Phillips C, Franck G. Cerebral metabolism during vegetative state and after recovery to consciousness. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1999;67:121–2.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Monti MM, Coleman MR, Owen AM. Neuroimaging and the vegetative state. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1157:81–9.CrossRefPubMed Monti MM, Coleman MR, Owen AM. Neuroimaging and the vegetative state. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1157:81–9.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Owen AM, Coleman MR. Functional neuroimaging of the vegetative state. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:235–43.CrossRefPubMed Owen AM, Coleman MR. Functional neuroimaging of the vegetative state. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:235–43.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Schiff ND, Rodriguez-Moreno D, Kamal A, Kim KHS, Giacino JT, Plum F, et al. fMRI reveals large-scale network activation in minimally conscious patients. Neurology. 2005;64:514–23.CrossRefPubMed Schiff ND, Rodriguez-Moreno D, Kamal A, Kim KHS, Giacino JT, Plum F, et al. fMRI reveals large-scale network activation in minimally conscious patients. Neurology. 2005;64:514–23.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Monti MM, Rosenberg M, Finoia P, Kamau E, Pickard JD, Owen AM. Thalamo-frontal connectivity mediates top-down cognitive functions in disorders of consciousness. Neurology. 2014; doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001123. Monti MM, Rosenberg M, Finoia P, Kamau E, Pickard JD, Owen AM. Thalamo-frontal connectivity mediates top-down cognitive functions in disorders of consciousness. Neurology. 2014; doi: 10.​1212/​WNL.​0000000000001123​.
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Fernández-Espejo D, Bekinschtein T, Monti MM, Pickard JD, Junque C, Coleman MR, et al. Diffusion weighted imaging distinguishes the vegetative state from the minimally conscious state. NeuroImage. 2011;54:103–12.CrossRefPubMed Fernández-Espejo D, Bekinschtein T, Monti MM, Pickard JD, Junque C, Coleman MR, et al. Diffusion weighted imaging distinguishes the vegetative state from the minimally conscious state. NeuroImage. 2011;54:103–12.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Childs NL, Mercer WN, Childs HW. Accuracy of diagnosis of persistent vegetative state. Neurology. 1993;43:1465–5.CrossRefPubMed Childs NL, Mercer WN, Childs HW. Accuracy of diagnosis of persistent vegetative state. Neurology. 1993;43:1465–5.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Majerus S, Gill-Thwaites H, Andrews K, Laureys S. Behavioral evaluation of consciousness in severe brain damage. In Progress in brain research. Volume 150. Edited by Laureys S. Elsevier: Liege (BL); 2005.397–413. The Boundaries of Consciousness: Neurobiology and Neuropathology. Majerus S, Gill-Thwaites H, Andrews K, Laureys S. Behavioral evaluation of consciousness in severe brain damage. In Progress in brain research. Volume 150. Edited by Laureys S. Elsevier: Liege (BL); 2005.397–413. The Boundaries of Consciousness: Neurobiology and Neuropathology.
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The JFK coma recovery scale-revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2004;85:2020–9.CrossRefPubMed Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J. The JFK coma recovery scale-revised: measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2004;85:2020–9.CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz DI, et al. The minimally conscious state definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 2002;58:349–53.CrossRefPubMed Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz DI, et al. The minimally conscious state definition and diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 2002;58:349–53.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Bruno M-A, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers C, Boly M, Gosseries O, Demertzi A, et al. Visual fixation in the vegetative state: an observational case series PET study. BMC Neurol. 2010;10:35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Bruno M-A, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers C, Boly M, Gosseries O, Demertzi A, et al. Visual fixation in the vegetative state: an observational case series PET study. BMC Neurol. 2010;10:35.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Giacino JT. Disorders of consciousness: differential diagnosis and neuropathologic features. In: Seminars in neurology, vol. 17. New York: Thieme-Stratton Inc; 1997. p. 105–12. Giacino JT. Disorders of consciousness: differential diagnosis and neuropathologic features. In: Seminars in neurology, vol. 17. New York: Thieme-Stratton Inc; 1997. p. 105–12.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Bruno M-A, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Thibaut A, Moonen G, Laureys S. From unresponsive wakefulness to minimally conscious PLUS and functional locked-in syndromes: recent advances in our understanding of disorders of consciousness. J Neurol. 2011;258:1373–84.CrossRefPubMed Bruno M-A, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Thibaut A, Moonen G, Laureys S. From unresponsive wakefulness to minimally conscious PLUS and functional locked-in syndromes: recent advances in our understanding of disorders of consciousness. J Neurol. 2011;258:1373–84.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Dolce G, Quintieri M, Serra S, Lagani V, Pignolo L. Clinical signs and early prognosis in vegetative state: a decisional tree, data-mining study. Brain Inj. 2008;22:617–23.CrossRefPubMed Dolce G, Quintieri M, Serra S, Lagani V, Pignolo L. Clinical signs and early prognosis in vegetative state: a decisional tree, data-mining study. Brain Inj. 2008;22:617–23.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers C, Brédart S, Laureys S. Assessment of visual pursuit in post-comatose states: use a mirror. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008;79:223–3. Vanhaudenhuyse A, Schnakers C, Brédart S, Laureys S. Assessment of visual pursuit in post-comatose states: use a mirror. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008;79:223–3.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Dolce G, Sazbon L. The post-traumatic vegetative state. Stuttgart: Thieme; 2002.CrossRef Dolce G, Sazbon L. The post-traumatic vegetative state. Stuttgart: Thieme; 2002.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Dolce G, Lucca LF, Candelieri A, Rogano S, Pignolo L, Sannita WG. Visual pursuit in the severe disorder of consciousness. J Neurotrauma. 2011;28:1149–54.CrossRefPubMed Dolce G, Lucca LF, Candelieri A, Rogano S, Pignolo L, Sannita WG. Visual pursuit in the severe disorder of consciousness. J Neurotrauma. 2011;28:1149–54.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Riganello F, Cortese MD, Dolce G, Sannita WG. Visual pursuit response in the severe disorder of consciousness: modulation by the central autonomic system and a predictive model. BMC Neurol. 2013;13:164.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Riganello F, Cortese MD, Dolce G, Sannita WG. Visual pursuit response in the severe disorder of consciousness: modulation by the central autonomic system and a predictive model. BMC Neurol. 2013;13:164.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Candelieri A, Cortese MD, Dolce G, Riganello F, Sannita WG. Visual pursuit: within-Day variability in the severe disorder of consciousness. J Neurotrauma. 2011;28:2013–7.CrossRefPubMed Candelieri A, Cortese MD, Dolce G, Riganello F, Sannita WG. Visual pursuit: within-Day variability in the severe disorder of consciousness. J Neurotrauma. 2011;28:2013–7.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenthal R. Effect sizes: Pearson’s correlation, its display via the BESD, and alternative indices. Am Psychol. 1991;46:1086–7.CrossRef Rosenthal R. Effect sizes: Pearson’s correlation, its display via the BESD, and alternative indices. Am Psychol. 1991;46:1086–7.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Hemphill JF. Interpreting the magnitudes of correlation coefficients. Am Psychol. 2003;58:78–9.CrossRefPubMed Hemphill JF. Interpreting the magnitudes of correlation coefficients. Am Psychol. 2003;58:78–9.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Krauzlis RJ. Neuronal activity in the rostral superior colliculus related to the initiation of pursuit and saccadic eye movements. J Neurosci. 2003;23:4333–44.PubMed Krauzlis RJ. Neuronal activity in the rostral superior colliculus related to the initiation of pursuit and saccadic eye movements. J Neurosci. 2003;23:4333–44.PubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Krauzlis RJ, Lisberger SG. Temporal properties of visual motion signals for the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys. J Neurophysiol. 1994;72:150–62.PubMed Krauzlis RJ, Lisberger SG. Temporal properties of visual motion signals for the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys. J Neurophysiol. 1994;72:150–62.PubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Newsome WT, Wurtz RH, Dursteler MR, Mikami A. Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. J Neurosci. 1985;5:825–40.PubMed Newsome WT, Wurtz RH, Dursteler MR, Mikami A. Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. J Neurosci. 1985;5:825–40.PubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Tian JR, Lynch JC. Corticocortical input to the smooth and saccadic eye movement subregions of the frontal eye field in Cebus monkeys. J Neurophysiol. 1996;76:2754–71.PubMed Tian JR, Lynch JC. Corticocortical input to the smooth and saccadic eye movement subregions of the frontal eye field in Cebus monkeys. J Neurophysiol. 1996;76:2754–71.PubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Laureys S, Perrin F, Brédart S. Self-consciousness in non-communicative patients. Conscious Cogn. 2007;16:722–41. Subjectivity and the Body.CrossRefPubMed Laureys S, Perrin F, Brédart S. Self-consciousness in non-communicative patients. Conscious Cogn. 2007;16:722–41. Subjectivity and the Body.CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Laureys S, Owen AM, Schiff ND. Brain function in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3:537–46.CrossRefPubMed Laureys S, Owen AM, Schiff ND. Brain function in coma, vegetative state, and related disorders. Lancet Neurol. 2004;3:537–46.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Cruse D, Chennu S, Chatelle C, Bekinschtein TA, Fernández-Espejo D, Pickard JD, et al. Bedside detection of awareness in the vegetative state: a cohort study. Lancet. 2012;378:2088–94.CrossRef Cruse D, Chennu S, Chatelle C, Bekinschtein TA, Fernández-Espejo D, Pickard JD, et al. Bedside detection of awareness in the vegetative state: a cohort study. Lancet. 2012;378:2088–94.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Dolce G, Riganello F, Quintieri M, Candelieri A, Conforti D. Personal interaction in the vegetative state: a data-mining study. J Psychophysiol. 2008;22:150–6.CrossRef Dolce G, Riganello F, Quintieri M, Candelieri A, Conforti D. Personal interaction in the vegetative state: a data-mining study. J Psychophysiol. 2008;22:150–6.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Riganello F, Candelieri A, Quintieri M, Conforti D, Dolce G. Heart rate variability: An index of brain processing in vegetative state? an artificial intelligence, data mining study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2010;121:2024–34.CrossRefPubMed Riganello F, Candelieri A, Quintieri M, Conforti D, Dolce G. Heart rate variability: An index of brain processing in vegetative state? an artificial intelligence, data mining study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2010;121:2024–34.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Machado C, Korein J, Aubert E, Bosch J, Alvarez MA, Rodríguez R, et al. Recognizing a Mother’s voice in the persistent vegetative state. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2007;38:124–6.CrossRefPubMed Machado C, Korein J, Aubert E, Bosch J, Alvarez MA, Rodríguez R, et al. Recognizing a Mother’s voice in the persistent vegetative state. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2007;38:124–6.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Hassler R, Rolle Y, Streicher H. Pathophisiologie Der Bewustseintatigkeit. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme; 1972. Hassler R, Rolle Y, Streicher H. Pathophisiologie Der Bewustseintatigkeit. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme; 1972.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Boussaoud D, Ungerleider LG, Desimone R. Pathways for motion analysis: cortical connections of the medial superior temporal and fundus of the superior temporal visual areas in the macaque. J Comp Neurol. 1990;296:462–95.CrossRefPubMed Boussaoud D, Ungerleider LG, Desimone R. Pathways for motion analysis: cortical connections of the medial superior temporal and fundus of the superior temporal visual areas in the macaque. J Comp Neurol. 1990;296:462–95.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Gottlieb JP, MacAvoy MG, Bruce CJ. Neural responses related to smooth-pursuit eye movements and their correspondence with electrically elicited smooth eye movements in the primate frontal eye field. J Neurophysiol. 1994;72:1634–53.PubMed Gottlieb JP, MacAvoy MG, Bruce CJ. Neural responses related to smooth-pursuit eye movements and their correspondence with electrically elicited smooth eye movements in the primate frontal eye field. J Neurophysiol. 1994;72:1634–53.PubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim JS, Sharpe JA. The vertical vestibulo-ocular reflex, and its interaction with vision during active head motion: effects of aging. J Vestib Res. 2001;11:3–12.PubMed Kim JS, Sharpe JA. The vertical vestibulo-ocular reflex, and its interaction with vision during active head motion: effects of aging. J Vestib Res. 2001;11:3–12.PubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Zeman A. Consciousness. In Encyclopedia of cognitive science. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; Chichester (UK); 2006. Zeman A. Consciousness. In Encyclopedia of cognitive science. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; Chichester (UK); 2006.
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosano C, Krisky CM, Welling JS, Eddy WF, Luna B, Thulborn KR, et al. Pursuit and saccadic Eye movement subregions in human frontal Eye field: a high-resolution fMRI investigation. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12:107–15.CrossRefPubMed Rosano C, Krisky CM, Welling JS, Eddy WF, Luna B, Thulborn KR, et al. Pursuit and saccadic Eye movement subregions in human frontal Eye field: a high-resolution fMRI investigation. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12:107–15.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Gusnard DA, Raichle ME. Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:685–94.CrossRefPubMed Gusnard DA, Raichle ME. Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:685–94.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor CM, Aird VH, Tate RL, Lammi MH. Sequence of recovery during the course of emergence from the minimally conscious state. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2007;88:521–5.CrossRefPubMed Taylor CM, Aird VH, Tate RL, Lammi MH. Sequence of recovery during the course of emergence from the minimally conscious state. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2007;88:521–5.CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Lechinger J, Bothe K, Pichler G, Michitsch G, Donis J, Klimesch W, et al. CRS-R score in disorders of consciousness is strongly related to spectral EEG at rest. J Neurol. 2013;260:2348–56.CrossRefPubMed Lechinger J, Bothe K, Pichler G, Michitsch G, Donis J, Klimesch W, et al. CRS-R score in disorders of consciousness is strongly related to spectral EEG at rest. J Neurol. 2013;260:2348–56.CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Sannita WG. Individual variability, end-point effects and possible biases in electrophysiological research. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006;117:2569–83.CrossRefPubMed Sannita WG. Individual variability, end-point effects and possible biases in electrophysiological research. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006;117:2569–83.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Cruse D, Thibaut A, Demertzi A, Nantes JC, Bruno M-A, Gosseries O, et al. Actigraphy assessments of circadian sleep-wake cycles in the vegetative and minimally conscious states. BMC Med. 2013;11:18.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cruse D, Thibaut A, Demertzi A, Nantes JC, Bruno M-A, Gosseries O, et al. Actigraphy assessments of circadian sleep-wake cycles in the vegetative and minimally conscious states. BMC Med. 2013;11:18.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Racine E, Amaram R, Seidler M, Karczewska M, Illes J. Media coverage of the persistent vegetative state and end-of-life decision-making. Neurology. 2008;71:1027–32.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Racine E, Amaram R, Seidler M, Karczewska M, Illes J. Media coverage of the persistent vegetative state and end-of-life decision-making. Neurology. 2008;71:1027–32.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Katz-Leurer M, Rotem H, Keren O, Meyer S. Heart rate and heart rate variability at rest and during exercise in boys who suffered a severe traumatic brain injury and typically-developed controls. Brain Inj. 2010;24:110–4.CrossRefPubMed Katz-Leurer M, Rotem H, Keren O, Meyer S. Heart rate and heart rate variability at rest and during exercise in boys who suffered a severe traumatic brain injury and typically-developed controls. Brain Inj. 2010;24:110–4.CrossRefPubMed
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Luauté J, Maucort-Boulch D, Tell L, Quelard F, Sarraf T, Iwaz J, et al. Long-term outcomes of chronic minimally conscious and vegetative states. Neurology. 2010;75:246–52.CrossRefPubMed Luauté J, Maucort-Boulch D, Tell L, Quelard F, Sarraf T, Iwaz J, et al. Long-term outcomes of chronic minimally conscious and vegetative states. Neurology. 2010;75:246–52.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Coma recovery scale-r: variability in the disorder of consciousness
verfasst von
MD. Cortese
F. Riganello
F. Arcuri
ME. Pugliese
LF. Lucca
G. Dolce
WG. Sannita
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2015
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Neurology / Ausgabe 1/2015
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2377
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-015-0455-5

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2015

BMC Neurology 1/2015 Zur Ausgabe