Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2019

Open Access 01.12.2019 | Research article

Cultural differences in positive psychotic experiences assessed with the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences-42 (CAPE-42): a comparison of student populations in the Netherlands, Nigeria and Norway

verfasst von: Margriet Vermeiden, Mayke Janssens, Viviane Thewissen, Esther Akinsola, Sanne Peeters, Jennifer Reijnders, Nele Jacobs, Jim van Os, Johan Lataster

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Background

Previous studies have suggested that culture impacts the experience of psychosis. The current study set out to extend these findings by examining cultural variation in subclinical positive psychotic experiences in students from The Netherlands, Nigeria, and Norway. Positive psychotic experiences were hypothesized to (i) be more frequently endorsed by, and (ii) cause less distress in Nigerian vs. Dutch and Norwegian students.

Methods

Psychology students, aged 18 to 30 years, from universities in the Netherlands (n = 245), Nigeria (n = 478), and Norway (n = 162) were assessed cross-sectionally with regard to the frequency of subclinical positive psychotic experiences and related distress, using the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE-42). Multi-group confirmatory factor analysis and multivariate analysis of covariance were performed to assess measurement invariance of the positive symptom dimension (CAPE-Pos) and compare mean frequency and associated distress of positive psychotic experiences across study samples.

Results

Only CAPE-Pos items pertaining to the dimensions ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’ met assumptions for (partial) measurement invariance. Frequencies of these experiences were higher in the Nigerian sample, compared to both the Dutch and Norwegian samples, which were similar. In addition, levels of experience-related distress were similar or higher in the Nigerian sample compared to respectively the Dutch and Norwegian samples.

Conclusion

Although positive psychotic experiences may be more commonly endorsed in non-Western societies, our findings do not support the notion that they represent a more benign, and hence less distressing aspect of human experience. Rather, the experience of psychotic phenomena may be just as, if not more, distressing in African than in European culture. However, observed differences in CAPE-Pos frequency and distress between samples from different cultural settings may partly reflect differences in the measure rather than in the latent trait. Future studies may therefore consider further cross-cultural adaptation of CAPE-42, in addition to explicitly examining cultural acceptance of psychotic phenomena, and environmental and other known risk factors for psychosis, when comparing and interpreting subclinical psychotic phenomena across cultural groups.
Hinweise
Mayke Janssens and Viviane Thewissen contributed equally to this work.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
AIC
Akaike information criterion
BIC
Bayesian information criterion
BPRS
Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale
CAPE-42
Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences
CAPE-Pos
CAPE positive symptom dimension
CFA
Confirmatory factor analysis
CFI
Comparative fit index
DSM-(I)V
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV / V
FIML
Full information maximum likelihood
ICD-10
International Classification of Diseases-10
MANCOVA
Multivariate analysis of covariance
MGCFA
Multi-group confirmatory factor analysis
RMSEA
Root mean square error of approximation
SIS-R
Structured Interview for Schizotypy
SPSS
Statistical Package for the Social Sciences
SRMR
Standardized root mean square residual

Background

The psychosis phenotype appears to become manifest across a spectrum of severity, extending from subtle psychotic experiences to a clinically relevant disorder [1, 2]. Although epidemiological studies have emphasized similarities, rather than differences in the prevalence of psychotic disorders across cultures [38], there are cultural variations in the manifestation of psychotic experiences [9, 10] with as much as 15–30% of psychosis expression being suggested to be culture-dependent [11]. The sociocultural context in which psychotic symptoms manifest themselves, and the associated degree of preoccupation and distress are important factors determining clinical relevance, but have not been investigated much [12]. Given that subclinical psychotic experiences may predict transition to a clinically relevant psychotic disorder, knowledge of cultural diversity in manifestations of psychotic experiences is important for the proper contextual assessment and treatment of psychotic disorders [13, 14]. Thus, an increasing number of studies explore cultural mechanisms that play a role in the expression of psychopathology [15].
Cultural representations, such as values, beliefs, and attitudes, establish a general understanding of the concept of reality, and influence how members of a particular society respond to psychotic phenomena [16]. The common societal conception of values and beliefs appears to be particularly important for the theme of delusions and hallucinations [17]. Delusions of grandeur, for example, hardly exist in societies where striving for a certain social status is frowned upon [11], and visual and tactile hallucinations are reported more frequently by members of social groups that take unexplainable sensory experiences as evidence of the supernatural or divine [16, 18]. These examples illustrate that the likelihood and quality of psychotic phenomena is partly dependent on an individual’s cultural environment [13, 19, 20]. Moreover, the extent to which hallucinations and delusions are interpreted as appropriate or benign, and socially accepted, may have implications for the distress resulting from these experiences within a certain society (i.e., distress is likely lower when experiences are considered normative or appropriate; [16, 2024]).
To better comprehend cross-cultural differences, several studies suggest that positive psychotic experiences are not to be interpreted as homogeneous dimensions, but are better represented as a number of symptom clusters that may vary in prevalence across cultures [2531]. Indeed, a number of cross-national survey studies have reported considerable variation in the prevalence of particular psychotic experiences across groups of individuals from different parts of the world, in the absence of any relevant differences in the overall prevalence of psychotic disorder [3236]. Similar findings have been reported with regard to the prevalence of positive symptom clusters across different ethnic groups within certain countries: disparity in symptom prevalence between ethnic groups exceeds disparity in prevalence of psychotic disorder [3739]. This seems to be especially true for perceptual anomalies and paranoia [4042]. Moreover, discrepancies between positive symptom prevalence and prevalence of a diagnosis of psychosis have been shown to be more pronounced in some ethnic groups than in others [10, 37, 39, 43], pointing towards a complex interplay of cultural and social factors (e.g. ethnic minority status) in the manifestation of psychosis [4447].
The majority of studies assessing cross-cultural differences in prevalence of psychotic experiences have employed trained clinical interviewers or used highly structured interviews, developed in accordance with definitions and criteria of ICD-10 and/or DSM-(I)V classification systems for psychotic disorder [48]. The Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE-42) [49]–a 42-item self-report questionnaire–on the other hand, was developed specifically to measure psychosis proneness in the general population based on a severity spectrum view of the psychosis phenotype. The CAPE-42 has proven to be a stable, reliable and valid instrument [49]. A recent meta-analysis of studies employing CAPE-42 [25] supported three dimensions of positive psychotic experiences in the general population, namely ‘strange experiences’, ‘delusional ideations’ (including paranoia, grandiosity and paranormal beliefs), and ‘perceptual anomalies’ (i.e., hallucinations). Although this meta-analysis [25] included samples from different parts of the world, the focus was mainly on identifying general underlying symptom structures, rather than comparing symptom prevalence across cultural groups or study samples. Moreover, the reviewed samples were from the so-called Western World, and to date CAPE-42 psychotic experiences have been rarely investigated in African study samples [22, 42].
The current study, therefore, aimed at furthering insight into the cultural variation in positive psychotic experiences as measured by CAPE-42, by comparing prevalence and perceived distress of these experiences across student populations from universities in The Netherlands, Norway (i.e., Western and Northern Europe) and Nigeria (Yoruba homeland). The distinction between reality and fantasy has been described as being more rigid in Euro-American culture in comparison to non-Western societies, and reports of psychotic-like phenomena (e.g. paranormal beliefs, feeling the presence of and/or being controlled by a deceased family member or under the influence of a supernatural force) are therefore more likely to be labelled as pathological in Western settings [50]. A greater degree of cultural acceptance towards such phenomena outside of the Western setting may lead to more frequent endorsement of psychotic experiences [16], as supported previously by a number of cross-national reports on the prevalence of hallucinations and delusions [13, 5155]. Recently, two large studies [22, 42] demonstrated cultural variation in epidemiological expression of psychotic experiences across respectively 12 and 13 countries, with highest prevalence rates in African samples. In line with these findings, we hypothesized higher prevalence of subclinical CAPE-42 psychotic experiences and lower prevalence of related distress in the Nigerian sample, compared to the Dutch and Norwegian samples. With regard to differences between the latter two samples, in Northern Europe living in the high-latitude band has been associated with higher incidence rates of psychotic disorders (the ‘latitude effect’ [56]). However, this effect possibly represents an artefact of methodological inconsistencies between studies (especially, use of register-based vs. first-contact data [57]), as there is little evidence of actual country variation in prevalence and distress of mental disorders across Europe when these inconsistencies are accounted for (see critical review by [58]). Although findings are thus inconclusive, we did not expect (pronounced) differences in prevalence and distress rates of subclinical positive psychotic experiences between Dutch and Norwegian samples, based on the assumption that participants from these countries would hold similar beliefs regarding the distinction between reality and fantasy, despite other cultural differences. In sum, we thus expected reports of hallucinations, strange experiences, and delusional ideations to be more prevalent in the Nigerian sample and, as theorized previously [16, 22, 50], the average distress related to these experiences to be relatively lower in the Nigerian sample, compared to both the Dutch and Norwegian samples. The lower prevalence of distress was based on the idea that these experiences are more likely labelled as appropriate or benign within African cultures, and hence met with more constructive attitudes [16, 2024, 42, 50, 59, 60].

Methods

Sample

The non-probability sample for this study, to which we had access, consisted of 885 psychology students from the Open University of the Netherlands (n = 245), the University of Lagos, Nigeria (n = 478), and the Universities of Oslo, Trondheim and Bergen, Norway (n = 162). Inclusion criteria were: (i) aged 18–30 years old, (ii) sufficient command of the Dutch, English or Norwegian language, respectively, to understand instructions and give informed consent, and (iii) no current or lifetime history of psychotic disorder. The total sample consisted of 307 males and 576 females, ranging in age from 18 to 30 years (Mean = 23.3, SD = 3.3). Further demographic characteristics of the sample are displayed in Table 1.
Table 1
Demographic characteristics and unadjusted descriptives of CAPE-Pos by sample, and tests for between-group differences
 
Netherlands
(n = 245)
Nigeria
(n = 478)
Norway
(n = 162)
F / χ2a
(R2)
p
Age, M (SD)
25.7 (3.0)
range 18–30
22.0 (2.9)
range 18–30
23.3 (2.8)
range 18–30
F(2,882) = 135.36
(R2 = 0.235)
<  0.001
Gender, n (%)
   
χ2(2, n = 883) = 74.55b
<  0.001
 Male
50 (20.4%)
227 (47.5%)
30 (18.5%)
  
 Female
193 (78.8%)
251 (52.5%)
132 (81.5%)
  
 Missing
2 (0.8%)
  
Marital status, n (%)
   
χ2(2, n = 883) = 54.56b
<  0.001
 Not Married
195 (79.6%)
461 (96.4%)
151 (93.2%)
  
 Married
48 (19.6%)
17 (3.6%)
11 (6.8%)
  
 Missing
2 (0.8%)
  
Educationc, n (%)
   
χ2(2, n = 878) = 219.60
<  0.001
 Secondary
26 (10.6%)
318 (67.5%)
101 (62.3%)
  
 Tertiary
219 (89.4%)
153 (32.5%)
61 (37.7%)
  
Ethnic group, n (%)d
 
 Yoruba
327 (68.4%)
  
 Igbo
80 (16.7%)
  
 Hausa
8 (1.7%)
  
 Other / missing
63 (13.2%)
  
CAPE-Pos total
 Frequency, M (SD)
1.37 (0.21)
1.98 (0.40)
1.31 (0.19)
F(2,882) = 431.29
<  0.001
 (min – max)
(1.00–2.20)
(1.13–3.16)
(1.05–2.10)
(R2 = 0.494)
 
 Distresse, M (SD)
1.53 (0.42)
1.78 (0.53)
1.36 (0.42)
F(2,874) = 50.35
<  0.001
 (min – max)
(1.00–3.20)
(1.00–3.21)
(1.00–3.00)
(R2 = 0.103)
 
 Distressf, M (SD)
1.14 (0.14)
1.54 (0.47)
1.12 (0.14)
F(2,881) = 141.42
<  0.001
 (min – max)
(1.00–1.70)
(1.00–3.50)
(1.00–1.65)
(R2 = 0.243)
 
aChi-square and ANOVA tests were performed to test whether the distribution of demographic and CAPE-Pos scores was comparable across the three study samples, bBased on 2 × 3 table (not including missing category), cHighest educational level completed, dOnly assessed in Nigerian sample, eDistress scores when frequency of experience was rated as at least ‘sometimes’, fDistress including scores when frequency of experience was rated as ‘never’ (following [25, 61, 62])

Study design and procedure

The study employed a cross-sectional survey design, asking participants to fill out a paper (Nigeria) or online self-report questionnaire (Netherlands and Norway). A lecturer of the University of Lagos directly approached Nigerian participants in 2011, handing out surveys during classes and giving participants the opportunity to directly give verbal consent and fill out the questions, which resulted in a 100% return rate. Dutch participants were recruited in 2009, and received course credits for their participation. The survey return rate was 100%. Norwegian participants were recruited through local university email services, electronic advertisements and Facebook in 2010 and, as a consequence, the total sampling frame and survey response rate are unknown. A total number of 246 Dutch, and 162 Norwegian psychology students accessed the online survey webpage, of which respectively 243 (98.8%), and 158 (97.5%) provided a complete survey response (i.e., all demographic and CAPE-42 items answered). A total number of 501 Nigerian psychology students handed in a paper survey document, of which 316 (63.1%) provided a complete response. We followed previous work on CAPE psychotic-like experiences [30], and excluded all participants with ≥ 25% CAPE-42 data missing, thereby eliminating data rows from 24 participants (< 3% of total recruited sample; one Dutch, and 23 Nigerian participants). The final dataset for analysis therefore contained data from N = 885 individuals, of which 771 (87.1%) provided complete demographic and CAPE-42 data, 109 (12.3%) had no more than five missing values on the set of CAPE frequency variables for analysis, and five individuals (0.6%) had more than five but less than 25% missing data points.

Measures

The Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences is a 42-item self-report instrument that measures the lifetime frequencies and distress of positive, negative and depressive subclinical psychotic experiences [63]. CAPE-42 has proven to be an appropriate tool to assess these experiences in clinical and subclinical populations [25], and has been cross-validated with the Structured Interview for Schizotypy (SIS-R) and the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS), demonstrating good stability, reliability and (discriminant) validity [49]. The current study used validated Dutch and English versions of the instrument [64, 65], and a validated Norwegian translation [66]. The Norwegian [66] and English validation [65] of CAPE-42 suggested sufficient internal consistency of positive, negative and depressive experiences, with Cronbach’s alpha values of 0.87, 0.87, 0.82 (Norway), and 0.83, 0.81, 0.84 (Canada), respectively. The Dutch version of CAPE-42 showed acceptable 3-factor model fit (positive, negative, depressive experiences) with RMSEA = 0.05 and TLI = 0.94, though not fully supported by CFI = 0.81 [64].
The positive symptom dimension (hereafter: CAPE-Pos) that is more closely examined in the current study consists of scores on 20 items, asking participants about subclinical positive psychotic experiences (e.g. “Do you ever hear voices when you are alone?”, “Do you ever feel as if you are under the control of some power other than yourself?”, “Do you ever feel as if there is a conspiracy against you?”). The frequency of each experience is rated on a four-point scale, ranging from 1 (never), 2 (sometimes), 3 (often), to 4 (nearly always), and a four-point scale from 1 (not distressed), 2 (a bit distressed), 3 (quite distressed) to 4 (very distressed) measures the degree of associated distress for each experience that is labelled as present (frequency of at least sometimes). The CAPE-Pos scale was initially conceptualized as unidimensional [67], but later studies have suggested either a three-dimensional structure that distinguishes ‘hallucinations’ (4 items), ‘strange experiences’ (7 items), and ‘delusional ideations’ (9 items; [25]), or a five-dimensional structure, in which the delusional ideation dimension is further decomposed into the subdimensions ‘paranoia’ (5 items), ‘grandiosity’ (2 items), and ‘magical thinking’ (2 items; [26]).

Analysis

Data were analysed with R version 3.5.3 [68], using the psych (version 1.8.12 [69]) and lavaan packages (version 0.6–3 [70]), and SPSS Statistics version 25.0 [71]. Data were inspected for anomalies, but outliers were not removed, as extreme scores were argued to indicate actual severe psychotic experiences.
Demographic variability in frequency and distress of CAPE-42 positive psychotic experiences was preliminary explored using multiple regression analyses with weighted mean frequency and distress scores for all CAPE-Pos items as dependent variables and age, gender (1 = male; 2 = female), marital status (1 = not married; 2 = married), and educational level (1 = secondary education; 2 = tertiary education) as independent variables.
Tests for configural, metric, and scalar invariance were performed to investigate the degree of measurement invariance for CAPE-Pos frequency ratings across groups, with (partial) invariance being considered as prerequisite for validly interpreting mean comparisons between samples as reflecting differences in the latent construct or trait rather than the measure [7274]. In line with previous work [64], measurement invariance testing for the distress ratings was omitted, as these values were selectively available only for those experiences labelled as present (frequency of at least sometimes).
Configural invariance was assessed by performing maximum likelihood confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to investigate the fit of one-, three-, and five-dimensional models of CAPE-Pos [25, 26, 67] in the whole sample, and in the Dutch, Nigerian, and Norwegian samples separately. Comparative fit index (CFI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) were computed for absolute goodness of fit exploration (minimally acceptable fit criteria: CFI ≥ 0.90, [75]; RMSEA ≤ 0.08, [76]; SRMR ≤ 0.08, [77]), and Akaike information criterion (AIC) and sample size adjusted Bayesian information criterion (BIC) were used to compare relative goodness of fit (smaller values indicate better fit). Internal consistency for the initial CFA-supported CAPE-Pos dimensions was assessed in each sample using McDonald’s ω [78], and inter-item correlations (IIC) for dimensions consisting of two items.
Configural, metric and scalar invariance of CAPE-Pos scores across Dutch, Nigerian, and Norwegian samples were further assessed by performing maximum likelihood multi-group confirmatory factor analysis (MGCFA [79]), in which several invariance models were compared sequentially, each model adding more restrictions and being tested against the less constrained model [73, 74, 80]. Specification of the baseline configural model was based on CFA results. Metric invariance was tested by constraining factor loadings, and comparing fit to the baseline model, in which loadings were allowed to be freely estimated across groups. The scalar invariance model additionally constrained item intercepts, and was tested against the metric invariance model. χ2 difference tests and ΔCFI were used to examine differences between the nested models, with ΔCFI < 0.01 implying that the invariance assumption holds [81]. When full invariance was not supported, modification indices were explored to identify non-invariant items, and constraints for these items were lifted, thus allowing partial invariance [72, 82].
When at least partial scalar invariance was supported, multivariate analyses of covariance (MANCOVAs) were performed to test for mean differences in the frequency and associated distress of CAPE-Pos experiences between Dutch, Nigerian and Norwegian participants. In order to do so, for those CAPE-Pos dimensions meeting criteria for (partial) measurement invariance, raw scores on CAPE-Pos frequency and distress items were transformed into weighted means (i.e. sum of item scores divided by the number of items with valid responses). Weighted means for experience related distress were only calculated when at least one positive psychotic experience within the respective CAPE-Pos dimension was labelled as present (frequency of at least sometimes). Covariates in the MANCOVA models were age, gender, marital status, and educational level. Whenever the MANCOVA tests yielded significant results, pairwise comparisons of estimated marginal means were conducted to explore group differences with Bonferroni corrected statistics, and Hedges’ g values were calculated by dividing mean differences between groups by the pooled and weighted standard deviation [83].
All findings were interpreted against a significance threshold of p <  0.05, and the respective n for each analysis is presented along with model estimates. (MG)CFAs were performed with full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation to account for missing data points [70, 84], but were additionally repeated using listwise deletion (results presented in Appendix) to ensure that findings were not systematically biased by missing data processing.

Results

Sample characteristics and demographic variability in CAPE-Pos frequency and distress

Table 1 presents a demographic description of the Dutch, Nigerian, and Norwegian samples, together with mean frequency and distress scores for CAPE-Pos. Dutch participants were oldest, most often married, and had generally completed tertiary educational level. By contrast, Nigerian participants were youngest, generally not married, and had typically not (yet) completed the tertiary educational level. Women were overrepresented in the Dutch and Norwegian samples, whereas in the Nigerian sample male and female participants were represented equally. Among the Nigerian participants, 68.4, 16.7, and 1.7% considered themselves as belonging to the Yoruba, Igbo, or Hausa ethnic group, respectively.
In the total sample, preliminary multiple regression analysis revealed that CAPE-Pos frequency scores were significantly associated with age, gender, and educational level (F(4,871) = 49.37, p <  0.001; R2 = 0.185): higher CAPE-Pos frequency was linked to younger (vs. older; B(SE) = − 0.04(0.005), p <  0.001), male (vs. female; B(SE) = − 0.201(0.030), p <  0.001) and lower (vs. higher; B(SE) = − 0.076(0.031), p = 0.014) educated participants. CAPE-Pos distress scores were significantly associated with age and gender (F(4,870) = 15.98, p <  0.001; R2 = 0.069): higher CAPE-Pos distress was linked to younger (vs. older; B(SE) = − 0.025(0.005), p <  0.001), and male (vs. female; B(SE) = − 0.106(0.029), p <  0.001) participants.

Confirmatory factor analysis and internal consistency estimates

Fit indices for one-, three-, and five-dimensional models of CAPE-Pos are presented in Table 2. In the total sample, only the five-dimensional model fulfilled all three fit criteria for CFI, SRMR and RMSEA. In the Dutch and Nigerian samples, the five-dimensional model best fit the data: AIC and BIC values were lower compared to those of the three- and one-dimensional models, SRMR and RMSEA reached threshold values, and CFIs were 0.845 (Dutch sample) and 0.902 (Nigerian sample). In the Norwegian sample, none of the tested models reached threshold values for CFI, SRMR and RMSEA, indicating poor or suboptimal fit of previously suggested CAPE-Pos factor structures in the Norwegian sample. Thus, CFA largely supported five-factor structures of CAPE-Pos in the Dutch and Nigerian samples, but not in the Norwegian sample. Repeating CFAs using listwise deletion instead of FIML returned slightly lower CFI-values, but yielded similar results overall (see Table 7 in Appendix).
Table 2
Confirmatory Factor Analysis for the total sample, and for the three study samples separately
CAPE-Pos frequency
χ2a,b
dfc
p
Absolute goodness of fit
Comparative fit
RMSEAd (90% CI)
CFId
SRMR
AICb
BICb,e
Factor loadings
Total sample (n = 885)f
 1-factor modelg
789
152
<  0.001
0.084 (0.078; 0.090)
0.821
0.059*
34089
34181
0.315–0.697
 3-factor modelh
543
149
<  0.001
0.066 (0.060; 0.072)*
0.891
0.049*
33715
33812
0.328–0.810
 5-factor modeli
356
142
<  0.001
0.050 (0.043; 0.056)*
0.941*
0.040*
33457
33565
0.350–0.902
Dutch sample (n = 245)f
 1-factor modelg
344
152
<  0.001
0.082 (0.071; 0.094)
0.573
0.085
4035
4048
0.029–0.541
 3-factor modelh
317
149
<  0.001
0.074 (0.063; 0.086)*
0.656
0.083
3973
3987
0.278–0.679
 5-factor modeli
218
142
<  0.001
0.051 (0.037; 0.064)*
0.845
0.068*
3868
3884
0.299–0.988
Nigerian sample (n = 478)f
 1-factor modelg
473
152
<  0.001
0.073 (0.066; 0.081)*
0.770
0.064*
20477
20534
0.039–0.615
 3-factor modelh
363
149
<  0.001
0.060 (0.052; 0.068)*
0.849
0.058*
20344
20404
0.062–0.756
 5-factor modeli
281
142
<  0.001
0.049 (0.041; 0.058)*
0.902*
0.048*
20260
20327
0.242–0.761
Norwegian sample (n = 162)f
 1-factor modelg
362
152
<  0.001
0.101 (0.088; 0.114)
0.452
0.099
3954
3950
0.182–0.534
 3-factor modelh
319
149
<  0.001
0.087 (0.074; 0.101)
0.596
0.090
3876
3871
0.181–0.945
 5-factor modeli
303
142
<  0.001
0.086 (0.073; 0.100)
0.624
0.088
3866
3862
0.212–0.971
aYuan-Bentler scaled test-statistic, bRounded to nearest integer, cItem no. 41 dropped, due to (near-)zero variance in Dutch and Norwegian samples (i.e. (virtually) all scores = 1), dRobust RMSEA and CFI from the scaled test-statistic, eSample size adjusted Bayesian BIC, fMissing datapoints were processed using full information maximum likelihood estimation, gCAPE-Pos one-factor structure as originally reported by [67], hCAPE-Pos three-factor structure as reported by [25], iCAPE-Pos five-factor structure as reported by [26], *Meeting minimally acceptable fit criteria: CFI ≥ 0.90 [75], RMSEA ≤ 0.08 [76], SRMR ≤ 0.08 [77]
Internal consistency estimates for the five CAPE-Pos dimensions–‘hallucinations’, ‘strange experiences’, ‘paranoia’, ‘grandiosity’, and ‘magical thinking’–that were largely supported by CFA in the Dutch and Nigerian samples were, respectively, ω = 0.53, ω = 0.74, ω = 0.30, IIC = 0.58, IIC = 0.40 in the Dutch sample, ω = 0.75, ω = 0.82, ω = 0.63, IIC = 0.36, IIC = 0.09 in the Nigerian sample, and ω = 0.69, ω = 0.68, ω = 0.59, IIC = 0.38, IIC = 0.22 in the Norwegian sample.

Metric and scalar invariance

Fit statistics for different invariance models of CAPE-Pos across study samples are presented in Table 3. In line with CFA results, the five-dimensional configural invariance (baseline) model did not fit the data well, and assumptions of metric and scalar invariance were violated. Modification indices were explored to identify non-invariant items, and it was concluded that only those items assigned to the dimensions ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’ were (largely) invariant across samples. The configural (baseline) model was respecified accordingly, yielding improved fit, and full metric and partial scalar invariance were established across study samples for the redefined, reduced model (see Table 3 for details). Repeating MGCFA using listwise deletion instead of FIML returned slightly lower fit values for all models, but provided similar results regarding metric and scalar invariance between samples (see Table 8 in Appendix).
Table 3
Fit statistics for different invariance models of CAPE-42 positive psychotic experiences across study samples
CAPE-Pos frequency
χ2
(Δχ2)a
df
(Δdf)
p
p)
CFI
(ΔCFI)b
RMSEA
(ΔRMSEA)b
SRMR
(ΔSRMR)
Comparison
Decision
5-dimensional modelc; n = 245d (Netherlands); n = 478d (Nigeria); n = 162d (Norway).
 M1 Configural invariance
798
426
<  0.001
0.843
0.058
0.060
Reject
 M2 Metric invariance
840 (42)
454 (28)
<  0.001 (0.012)
0.825 (0.018)
0.060 (−0.002)
0.071 (−0.011)
M1 vs. M2
Reject
 M3 Scalar invariance
1053 (213)
482 (28)
<  0.001 (<  0.001)
0.740 (0.085)
0.071 (− 0.011)
0.087 (− 0.016)
M2 vs. M3
Reject
2-dimensions reduced model (Strange experiences – items 5, 17, 24, 26, 28, 30, 31; Paranoia – items 2, 6, 7, 22); n = 245d (Netherlands); n = 478d (Nigeria); n = 162d (Norway).
 M1 Configural invariance
198
129
<  0.001
0.924
0.048
0.048
Accept
 M2 Metric invariance
217 (19)
147 (18)
<  0.001 (0.206)
0.915 (0.009)
0.048 (0.00)
0.062 (−0.014)
M1 vs. M2
Accept
 M3 Scalar invariance
341 (124)
165 (18)
<  0.001 (<  0.001)
0.787 (0.128)
0.071 (−0.023)
0.086 (−0.024)
M2 vs. M3
Reject
 M3a Partial scalar invariancee
231 (14)
159 (12)
<  0.001 (0.300)
0.912 (0.003)
0.046 (0.002)
0.065 (−0.003)
M2 vs. M3a
Accept
ΔCFI < 0.01 implies that invariance assumption holds [81], aYuan-Bentler scaled test-statistic (rounded to nearest integer), bRobust RMSEA and CFI from the scaled test-statistic, cItem no. 41 dropped, due to (near-)zero variance in Dutch and Norwegian samples (i.e. (virtually) all scores = 1), dMissing data points were processed using full information maximum likelihood estimation, eNo intercept constraints for items 6,7,17

Mean differences in CAPE-pos frequency and associated distress between groups

Based on results from MGCFA, tests for mean differences in the frequency and associated distress of CAPE-Pos experiences between samples were limited to the CAPE-Pos dimensions ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’. In nearly all MANCOVA models, the assumption of homogeneity of variance was violated (Levene’s tests were significant), and bootstrapping was used to correct the confidence interval for bias. Samples differed significantly with regard to the mean frequency of ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’ (Wilk’s Λ = 0.748, F(4, 1736) = 67.74, p <  0.001; see Table 5). Post-hoc analyses showed that estimated marginal means for the frequency of ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’ were significantly higher in the Nigerian sample (all comparisons p <  0.001), compared to the Dutch and Norwegian samples, which were similar (see Tables 4 and 6).
Table 4
Estimated marginal means for frequency and distress of CAPE-42 strange experiences and paranoia
CAPE-Pos
Frequency
Distressa
Netherlands
Nigeria
Norway
Netherlands
Nigeria
Norway
n
M (SE) b
n
M (SE) b
n
M (SE) b
n
M (SE) b
n
M (SE) b
n
M (SE) b
Strange experiencesc
243
1.146 (0.032)
471
1.738 (0.021)
162
1.188 (0.033)
106
1.326 (0.074)
365
1.842 (0.035)
80
1.299 (0.072)
Paranoiac
243
1.654 (0.045)
471
2.004 (0.030)
162
1.806 (0.047)
106
1.999 (0.083)
365
1.836 (0.039)
80
1.588 (0.080)
aOnly including distress scores when frequency of experience was rated as at least ‘sometimes’, bAdjusted for age, gender, marital status, and educational level, cPartial scale
The multivariate model of CAPE-Pos distress associated with ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’ also revealed significant differences between samples, Wilk’s Λ = 0.869, F(4, 1086) = 19.81, p <  0.001 (See Table 5). Post-hoc comparison of estimated marginal means showed that distress associated with ‘strange experiences’ was significantly higher in the Nigerian sample (all comparisons p <  0.001), compared to the Dutch and Norwegian samples, which were similar. Distress associated with ‘paranoia’ was similar in Dutch and Nigerian participants, and significantly higher compared to Norwegian participants (see Tables 4 and 6).
Table 5
Between-group analysis of frequency and associated distress of CAPE-42 strange experiences and paranoia
CAPE-Pos
Frequency
Distress a
F b
df
p
η p 2
F b
df
p
η p 2
Strange experiencesc
143.16
(2,869)
<  0.001
0.248
31.85
(2,544)
<  0.001
0.105
Paranoiac
18.72
(2,869)
<  0.001
0.041
6.82
(2,544)
0.001
0.024
All models adjusted for age, gender, marital status, and educational level, aOnly including distress scores when frequency of experience was rated as at least ‘sometimes’, bMultivariate F ratios were generated from Wilk’s statistics, cPartial scale
Table 6
Pairwise comparisons for frequency and associated distress of CAPE-42 strange experiences and paranoia between samples
CAPE-Pos
Netherlands (reference) vs. Nigeria
Netherlands (reference) vs. Norway
Nigeria (reference) vs. Norway
ΔM (SE)
(95% CI)a
p b
Hedges’ g
ΔM (SE)
(95% CI)a
p b
Hedges’ g
ΔM (SE)
(95% CI)a
p b
Hedges’ g
Frequency
 Strange experiencesc
0.593 (0.042)
(0.492; 0.694)
<  0.001
1.26
0.042 (0.047)
(−0.070; 0.154)
0.999
0.09
−0.551 (0.040)
(−0.645; − 0.455)
<  0.001
1.23
 Paranoiac
0.351 (0.059)
(0.209; 0.492)
<  0.001
0.52
0.152 (0.065)
(−0.005; 0.309)
0.061
0.23
−0.198 (0.056)
(− 0.333; − 0.064)
0.001
0.31
Distressd
 Strange experiencesc
0.516 (0.089)
(0.304; 0.729)
<  0.001
0.75
−0.027 (0.103)
(− 0.274; 0.221)
0.999
0.04
−0.543 (0.080)
(− 0.736; − 0.350)
<  0.001
0.82
 Paranoiac
−0.163 (0.099)
(− 0.400; 0.074)
0.295
0.21
− 0.411 (0.115)
(− 0.687; − 0.135)
0.001
0.51
−0.248 (0.090)
(− 0.463; − 0.033)
0.018
0.34
aBased on estimated marginal means, and adjusted for age, gender, marital status, and educational level, bBonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons, cPartial scale, dOnly including distress scores when frequency of experience was rated as at least ‘sometimes’

Discussion

This study examined cultural variation in manifestations of positive psychotic experiences as measured by CAPE-42 in student populations from universities in the Netherlands, Nigeria (Yoruba homeland) and Norway. Findings showed that only those items of CAPE-Pos pertaining to the dimensions ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’ (largely) met assumptions for measurement invariance across samples, whereas those that were labelled as belonging to dimensions of ‘hallucinations’, ‘grandiosity’, and ‘magical thinking’ in previous studies did not. For those positive psychotic experiences that could be validly compared between samples (i.e., ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’), frequencies were higher in the Nigerian sample compared to the Dutch and Norwegian samples, which were similar in this regard, as hypothesized. However, contrary to expectations, Nigerian participants reported comparable or even higher levels of psychotic experience related distress than did Dutch and Norwegian participants, not fitting the notion of psychotic phenomena as constituting a relatively more normative and hence less distressing aspect of human experience in Nigerian compared to Dutch and Norwegian culture.
Our findings suggest, first of all, that caution may be warranted when comparing frequency and distress of CAPE-Pos experiences between general population samples from different countries using available translations of CAPE-42, as observed differences may partly reflect differences in the measure rather than in the latent construct(s). Measurement non-invariance may be due to a variety of differences between samples, e.g. differences in the applicability of items, the conceptual meaning or understanding of constructs, the extent of social desirability, the nature of personal reference points and extreme item responses, item translations, or due to different administration methods [79]. Previous work [64] has suggested that individuals with psychosis vulnerability may show a tendency of scoring slightly lower on CAPE-42 when the measure is administered online versus on paper, but concluded this to be of negligible consequence for research in general population samples. Nonetheless, considering the CAPE-42 questionnaire was assessed online by Dutch and Norwegian participants, but on paper by Nigerian participants, differences in the administration method may have represented a source of measurement non-invariance. Moreover, CAPE-42 was developed in the Netherlands and it cannot be ruled out that Dutch values, beliefs, and attitudes have influenced the idiom of the questions [22], and may have been understood differently by Norwegian and Nigerian participants presented with translated versions of the instrument. Assessment tools may not always be applicable cross-culturally [85], and our findings may imply that further cross-cultural adaptation of CAPE-42 for non-Western samples may thus be necessary [86] (see also ITC Guidelines for Translating and Adapting Tests [87]).
Despite differences in the measurement of CAPE-42 positive psychotic experiences across cultural settings, similarities were observed for the measurement of ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’, of which the observed higher frequency in Nigerian participants aligns with previous work showing higher psychotic symptom prevalence of non-Western compared to Western societies [13, 22, 33, 42, 53, 54]. As suggested previously [16, 50], a more pliable way of differentiating between reality and fantasy in non-Western societies–expressed, for example, through recognition of a supernatural or divine realm–may give rise to more frequent endorsement of psychotic-like experiences, such as imagery, or altered states of consciousness, than in Euro-American societies, in which distinctions between what is real and what is not tend to be more rigid [50]. Experiences such as communicating with a deceased family member or feeling the presence of a supernatural force may in fact be highly valued and culturally meaningful in non-Western societies [88], and may therefore be more frequently noticed and more easily communicated to others [50]. A higher prevalence of positive psychotic symptoms in African societies may thus, in part, reflect a greater tendency to share culturally sanctioned experiences that signify contact with the supernatural realm or spirit world [38, 50, 60]. In Western societies, on the other hand, negative attitudes towards experiences that could signal dissociation from what is sensible or physically perceivable (i.e., what is considered as ‘real’), may reduce the tendency to report such experiences, out of fear for stigma of mental illness [50].
However, while the Nigerian participants included in our study reported a higher frequency of positive psychotic experiences, they also reported equal or even higher levels of distress due to these experiences compared to Dutch and Norwegian participants, respectively. This does not seem to fit well with the idea of psychotic-like experiences representing a more normative and benign, and hence less distressing aspect of human experience in non-Western societies [16, 20, 22, 50]. Rather, this finding suggests that the experience of psychotic phenomena may be just as, if not more, distressing in African than in European culture. This contrasts with previous reports, describing for instance that hallucinations are considered less troublesome and in general are experienced positively in African culture [20], in addition to suggesting more sympathetic attitudes within the social environment, a lower demand for clinical support, a more transient nature, and favourable course and outcome of psychosis in non-Western societies [85, 8993], although some studies suggest that symptom severity rather than associated distress is linked to need for care [94]. However, other work suggests that prevalence-distress associations for psychotic symptoms do not necessarily differ across cultural groups [95], and certain culture-specific spiritual or supernatural interpretations of psychotic-like experiences, such as beliefs of possession by higher order malevolent entities (typically: devils, demons) as described for certain social groups in Africa, may actually induce severe distress [96, 97]. Reversely, the experience of psychological distress may be expressed or communicated differently across cultures [98, 99], and the endorsement of psychotic-like symptoms in Africans may in fact reflect a culturally sanctioned expression of distress, in line with the observation of higher rates of brief reactive psychoses following stressful events in non-Western compared to Western cultures [100].
Because the current study did not tap directly into the belief system of participants, it remains unclear exactly in which way and to what extent culturally based beliefs drive the findings on psychotic symptom frequency and distress. Several studies have suggested, for instance, that while on the one hand Nigerians are in general acculturated in the sense that they support attempts to emulate western economic and social developments, they are on the other hand very protective of traditional cultural values and norms [101, 102]. This may explain why caregivers of schizophrenia patients in Nigeria endorse both supernatural (‘traditional’), as well as natural (‘acculturated’) causes as important in the aetiology of the disorder [103], and why Nigerian patients with schizophrenia seek help from both spiritual healers and physicians [104]. Nigerians may thus represent a relatively heterogeneous group in terms of beliefs and attitudes regarding psychotic experiences, with the possibility of contrasting or bicultural belief systems at play. Educational level may play a role, as supernatural beliefs about disease causation are less commonly endorsed by individuals with formal education [103]. This may well be relevant for Nigerian participants in the current study, given that all had completed secondary or tertiary education. Moreover, respondents were psychology students with expectedly higher tendency towards medical and scientific explanations for their experiences, making them more aware of possible psychopathology.
Apart from cultural factors, the participant samples included in the current study may have differed in terms of demographic and exposure to environmental risk factors for psychosis. The Nigerian sample, for instance, consisted of proportionally more men, was significantly younger, lower educated, and less often married than the Dutch and Norwegian samples. Therefore, although statistical analyses were corrected for demographic variability, it cannot be ascertained that between-group differences do not in part reflect different demographic risk profiles for psychosis [8]. In addition, previously identified environmental risk factors for psychosis, such as childhood trauma, cannabis use, and urbanicity may have differed between samples but were not assessed [44]. Whereas lifetime cannabis use is likely more common in Norway, and particularly in The Netherlands, compared to Nigeria (see e.g. [105]), reports of childhood abuse may be more commonly reported by African patients with a psychiatric diagnosis compared to patients from Europe [106, 107]. With regard to urbanicity, it is important to recognize that Nigerian participants were from the city of Lagos, which is considered the fastest growing and largest city of Africa, with an estimated population of over 20 million residents [108]. Moreover, the city has been ranked among the least liveable cities in the world, due to issues regarding political stability, security, healthcare, education, and infrastructure [109]. Urban residency was previously shown to be associated with delusion-like experiences in young Ugandan adults, regardless of age, gender and social class [52]. Thus, although speculative, between-group differences in reported rates of psychotic experiences and associated distress may be partly attributable to differences in exposure to environmental risk rather than differences in cultural background.
Although reported frequencies of positive psychotic experiences were comparable between Dutch and Norwegian participants, our data suggest that experiences of paranoia caused less distress for Norwegian than for Dutch students. Although this finding agrees with previous work showing that psychological distress in general tends to be lower in Norwegian students compared to students from other Western countries [110], our study is the first to compare psychosis proneness-related distress between Dutch and Norwegian adults, and further investigations are required to test the robustness of this result, and elucidate possible involvement of cultural factors.

Strengths and limitations

This study adds to the existing literature on subclinical psychotic experiences in community-based samples, by assessing measurement invariance and differences in frequency and distress of CAPE-42 positive psychotic experiences across student samples from different cultural settings. In particular, the current study responds to the dearth of CAPE-42 data for non-Western study samples (see [25] for recent overview), by exploring CAPE-42 positive psychotic experiences in African students. Although the current study thereby provides new insights in the extended psychosis phenotype across sociocultural groups, a number of limitations require consideration when interpreting results, and deserve attention in future studies.
First, as pointed out above, the Nigerian study sample differed significantly from the Dutch and Norwegian samples regarding demographic characteristics, and possibly also regarding exposure to environmental (e.g. trauma, cannabis use, urbanicity [44]) and other risk factors for psychosis, such as co-occurring phenomena like anxiety and depression [111, 112]. Therefore, although findings were statistically adjusted for demographic characteristics, caution is warranted when attributing between-group differences to cultural factors. Second, higher educated individuals were overrepresented in all samples, and the Dutch and Norwegian samples consisted largely of women. In addition, recruitment procedures may have led to selective underrepresentation of certain groups in our study sample, and results thus possibly lack accuracy regarding generalization to the population level (see e.g. [113, 114] for potential limitations of non-probability sampling). Third, although we assumed similarities between Dutch and Norwegian participants with respect to their cultural belief system in context of distinctions between reality and fantasy, they cannot be interpreted as culturally equal. Fourth, the current study did not distinguish between self-reported ethnic group membership of Nigerian participants. Fifth, (partial) measurement invariance of CAPE-Pos across samples was only established for items pertaining to ‘strange experiences’ and ‘paranoia’, thus limiting between-group comparisons to these dimensions of CAPE-Pos. In addition, because reports of experience related distress were selectively available–only for those experiences labelled as present–measurement invariance testing for the distress dimension was omitted, in line with previous work [64]. As discussed above, differences in administration, language, and interpretation of CAPE-42 between samples all represent potential sources of measurement non-invariance in our study, and subject to further investigation in future cross-cultural studies using CAPE-42. Sixth, internal consistency of CAPE-Pos subdimensions supported by CFA varied from ω = 0.30 to ω = 0.82, and findings must be interpreted in this context. Seventh, the current study did not include an infrequency scale (e.g. [115]) or other measures for identifying inconsistent survey responders, thus the possibility of dishonesty, survey fatigue, or other response biases affecting our data cannot be ruled out. Eighth and lastly, this study focused on the positive dimension of psychotic experiences, because it was considered most sensitive to cross-cultural differences in belief systems [17, 50]. However, recent work by [116] suggested a multidimensional nature of the extended psychosis phenotype in which positive and negative psychotic experiences, disorganization, mania and depression complement a general transdiagnostic psychosis factor (i.e., a factor that is relevant across a range of mental disorders). Future cross-cultural studies may thus consider a more comprehensive assessment of the extended psychosis phenotype that takes this multidimensionality into account.

Conclusion

Our findings show cultural variation in the extended psychosis phenotype, and support previous work suggesting that culture may profoundly affect various dimensions of psychometric measures of psychosis proneness [16]. The results emphasize the importance of investigating cross-cultural variants in symptom definition, and behavioural and symptomatic manifestations of psychosis [117]. Awareness of the sociocultural context in which psychotic experiences occur is required in order to adequately interpret these experiences and respond appropriately [12, 14, 15, 118120]. Prevention, screening, identification, and treatment of psychosis in non-Western cultural settings may require involvement of and collaboration between traditional healers and health professionals, as already operationalized in certain African countries for HIV/AIDS and related illnesses [121123].
Future studies are advised to further investigate and minimize sources of measurement non-invariance, explicitly examine cultural acceptance towards psychotic phenomena, and assess environmental and other known risk factors for psychosis when comparing and interpreting subclinical psychotic phenomena across different cultural groups.

Acknowledgements

We thank Annemarije Busch and Karin Hilbink for their contributions to the data collection in Norway and The Netherlands, respectively.
The initial study protocol was developed by researchers from Open University in The Netherlands, and was later adapted for use in Norway and Nigeria with permission of the participating institutions. As there is no judicial coverage of ethical review of non-medical research in The Netherlands, faculties of social sciences together developed the ‘code of ethics for research in the social and behavioural sciences’, which was accepted in January 2016 by the deans of social sciences in The Netherlands. From then on, approval of research protocols from an ethics review committee needs to be established prior to the start of a study. The current study was conducted before this date, which made it not obligatory to obtain approval of an IRB/ethics panel. For that reason, the research protocol was not reviewed by such a board, and Dutch ethics committees are prohibited from retroactively granting ethical approval. However, data were collected under responsibility of the researchers, who confirm that the study was carried out in accordance with The Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) for medical research involving humans. It is further emphasized that the Dutch law on Medical Research in Humans (Wet Medisch-wetenschappelijk onderzoek met mensen [WMO]) was not applicable to the current study, because the research question was not medical, did not involve patients, and participants were not randomly assigned to an intervention.
All participants were informed that participation was on a voluntary basis, and gave digital (in case of online survey; The Netherlands, Norway) or verbal informed consent (in case of paper and pencil survey; Nigeria) at study entry.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors confirm that there are no conflicts of interest associated with this publication.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Anhänge

Appendix

Table 7
Confirmatory Factor Analysis for the total sample, and for the three study samples separately
CAPE-Pos frequency
 
Absolute goodness of fit
Comparative fit
χ2a,b
dfc
p
RMSEAd (90% CI)
CFId
SRMR
AICb
BICb,e
Factor loadings
Whole sample (n = 786)f
 1-factor modelg
677
152
<  0.001
0.082 (0.075; 0.088)
0.824
0.060*
29,905
29,962
0.307–0.681
 3-factor modelh
459
149
<  0.001
0.063 (0.056; 0.069)*
0.898
0.050*
29,566
29,627
0.316–0.786
 5-factor modeli
320
142
<  0.001
0.048 (0.041; 0.056)*
0.942*
0.041*
29,372
29,443
0.348–0.904
Dutch sample (n = 245)f
 1-factor modelg
344
152
<  0.001
0.082 (0.071; 0.094)
0.573
0.085
4035
4048
0.029–0.541
 3-factor modelh
317
149
<  0.001
0.074 (0.063; 0.086)*
0.656
0.083
3973
3987
0.278–0.679
 5-factor modeli
218
142
<  0.001
0.051 (0.037; 0.064)*
0.845
0.068*
3868
3884
0.299–0.988
Nigerian sample (n = 380)f
 1-factor modelg
407
152
<  0.001
0.073 (0.064; 0.081)*
0.742
0.070*
16,591
16,620
0.103–0.580
 3-factor modelh
314
149
<  0.001
0.059 (0.049; 0.068)*
0.836
0.063*
16,478
16,510
0.143–0.728
 5-factor modeli
254
142
<  0.001
0.050 (0.040; 0.059)*
0.888
0.053*
16,423
16,460
0.217–0.737
Norwegian sample (n = 161)f
 1-factor modelg
359
152
<  0.001
0.100 (0.087; 0.114)
0.455
0.103
3904
3901
0.095–0.533
 3-factor modelh
316
149
<  0.001
0.087 (0.074; 0.100)
0.600
0.094
3825
3822
0.185–0.944
 5-factor modeli
301
142
<  0.001
0.086 (0.072; 0.099)
0.626
0.091
3816
3812
0.211–0.971
aYuan-Bentler scaled test-statistic, bRounded to nearest integer, cItem no. 41 dropped, due to (near-)zero variance in Dutch and Norwegian samples (i.e. [virtually] all scores = 1), dRobust RMSEA and CFI from the scaled test-statistic, eSample size adjusted Bayesian BIC, fMissing data points were processed using listwise deletion, gCAPE-Pos one-factor structure as originally reported by [67], hCAPE-Pos three-factor structure as reported by [25], iCAPE-Pos five-factor structure as reported by [26], *Meeting minimally acceptable fit criteria: CFI ≥ 0.90 [75], RMSEA ≤ 0.08 [76], SRMR ≤ 0.08 [77]
Table 8
Fit statistics for different invariance models of CAPE-42 positive psychotic experiences across study samples
CAPE-Pos frequency
χ2
(Δχ2)a
df
(Δdf)
p
p)
CFI
(ΔCFI)b
RMSEA
(ΔRMSEA)b
SRMR
(ΔSRMR)
Comparison
Decision
5-dimensional modelc; n = 245d (Netherlands); n = 478d (Nigeria); n = 162d (Norway).
 M1 Configural invariance
768
426
<  0.001
0.823
0.059
0.063
Reject
 M2 Metric invariance
809 (41)
454 (28)
<  0.001 (0.018)
0.804 (0.019)
0.061 (−0.002)
0.074 (−0.011)
M1 vs. M2
Reject
 M3 Scalar invariance
1004 (195)
482 (28)
<  0.001 (<  0.001)
0.711 (0.093)
0.071 (− 0.010)
0.088 (− 0.014)
M2 vs. M3
Reject
2-dimensions reduced model (Strange experiences – items 5, 17, 24, 26, 28, 30, 31; Paranoia – items 2, 6, 7, 22); n = 245d (Netherlands); n = 478d (Nigeria); n = 162d (Norway).
 M1 Configural invariance
197
129
<  0.001
0.917
0.049
0.050
Accept
 M2 Metric invariance
213 (16)
147 (18)
<  0.001 (0.291)
0.911 (0.006)
0.048 (0.001)
0.062 (−0.012)
M1 vs. M2
Accept
 M3 Scalar invariance
334 (121)
165 (18)
<  0.001 (<  0.001)
0.775 (0.136)
0.071 (−0.023)
0.086 (−0.024)
M2 vs. M3
Reject
 M3a Partial scalar invariancee
226 (13)
159 (12)
<  0.001 (0.375)
0.910 (0.001)
0.046 (0.002)
0.065 (−0.003)
M2 vs. M3a
Accept
ΔCFI < 0.01 implies that invariance assumption holds [81], aYuan-Bentler scaled test-statistic (rounded to nearest integer), bRobust RMSEA and CFI from the scaled test-statistic; cItem no. 41 dropped, due to (near-)zero variance in Dutch and Norwegian samples (i.e. [virtually] all scores = 1); dMissing data points were processed using listwise deletion, eNo intercept constraints for items 6, 7, 17
Literatur
9.
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Stompe T, Karakula H, Rudaleviciene P, Okribelashvili N, Chaudhry HR, Idemudia EE, et al. The pathoplastic effect of culture on psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia. World Cult Psychiatry Res Rev. 2006;1(3/4):157–63. Stompe T, Karakula H, Rudaleviciene P, Okribelashvili N, Chaudhry HR, Idemudia EE, et al. The pathoplastic effect of culture on psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia. World Cult Psychiatry Res Rev. 2006;1(3/4):157–63.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Lim C, Chong SA, Keefe RS. Psychosocial factors in the neurobiology of schizophrenia: a selective review. Ann Acad Med Singap. 2009;38(5):402–6.PubMed Lim C, Chong SA, Keefe RS. Psychosocial factors in the neurobiology of schizophrenia: a selective review. Ann Acad Med Singap. 2009;38(5):402–6.PubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Sartorius N, Jablensky A, Korten A, Ernberg G, Anker M, Cooper JE, et al. Early manifestations and first-contact incidence of schizophrenia in different cultures: a preliminary report on the initial evaluation phase of the WHO collaborative study on determinants of outcome of severe mental disorders. Psychol Med. 1986. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291700011910.PubMedCrossRef Sartorius N, Jablensky A, Korten A, Ernberg G, Anker M, Cooper JE, et al. Early manifestations and first-contact incidence of schizophrenia in different cultures: a preliminary report on the initial evaluation phase of the WHO collaborative study on determinants of outcome of severe mental disorders. Psychol Med. 1986. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1017/​S003329170001191​0.PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Pull CB, Cloos JM, Pull-Erpelding MC. Clinical assessment instruments in psychiatry. In: Maj M, Gaebel MW, Lopez-Ibor JJ, Sartorius N, editors. Psychiatric diagnosis and classification: Wiley; 2002. p. 177–218. https://doi.org/10.1002/047084647X. Pull CB, Cloos JM, Pull-Erpelding MC. Clinical assessment instruments in psychiatry. In: Maj M, Gaebel MW, Lopez-Ibor JJ, Sartorius N, editors. Psychiatric diagnosis and classification: Wiley; 2002. p. 177–218. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​047084647X.
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Busch AN, Jacobs N. Is there a continuum of psychotic symptoms in the Norwegian population? Validation of the Norwegian translation of the CAPE. Heerlen: Open University of the Netherlands; 2012. Busch AN, Jacobs N. Is there a continuum of psychotic symptoms in the Norwegian population? Validation of the Norwegian translation of the CAPE. Heerlen: Open University of the Netherlands; 2012.
68.
Zurück zum Zitat R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing, R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna; 2018. Available from: http://www.R-project.org/. [version 3.5.3; cited 2019 May 09] R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing, R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna; 2018. Available from: http://​www.​R-project.​org/​. [version 3.5.3; cited 2019 May 09]
71.
Zurück zum Zitat IBM Corp. IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 25.0. IBM Corp: Armonk; 2017. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 25.0. IBM Corp: Armonk; 2017.
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Hirschfeld G, von Brachel R. Multiple-group confirmatory factor analysis in R-a tutorial in measurement invariance with continuous and ordinal indicators. Prac Assess Res Eval. 2014;19:7. Hirschfeld G, von Brachel R. Multiple-group confirmatory factor analysis in R-a tutorial in measurement invariance with continuous and ordinal indicators. Prac Assess Res Eval. 2014;19:7.
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. 1st ed. Orlando: Academic press; 2014. Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. 1st ed. Orlando: Academic press; 2014.
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Allison PD. Handling missing data by maximum likelihood. Paper presented at: SAS Global Forum. Orlando: SAS Institute; 2012. Allison PD. Handling missing data by maximum likelihood. Paper presented at: SAS Global Forum. Orlando: SAS Institute; 2012.
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Bentall R, Boyle M, Chadwick P, Cooke A, Garety P, Gelsthorpe P, et al. Understanding psychosis and schizophrenia. Leicester: The British Psychological Society Division of Clinical Psychology; 2017. p. 175. Bentall R, Boyle M, Chadwick P, Cooke A, Garety P, Gelsthorpe P, et al. Understanding psychosis and schizophrenia. Leicester: The British Psychological Society Division of Clinical Psychology; 2017. p. 175.
93.
Zurück zum Zitat Castillo RJ. Effects of culture on recovery from transient psychosis. Psychiatr Times. 2006;23(14):46–8. Castillo RJ. Effects of culture on recovery from transient psychosis. Psychiatr Times. 2006;23(14):46–8.
97.
Zurück zum Zitat Niehaus DJ, Oosthuizen P, Lochner C, Emsley RA, Jordaan E, Mbanga NI, et al. A culture-bound syndrome ‘amafufunyana’ and a culture-specific event ‘ukuthwasa’: differentiated by a family history of schizophrenia and other psychiatric disorders. Psychopathology. 2004. https://doi.org/10.1159/000077579.PubMedCrossRef Niehaus DJ, Oosthuizen P, Lochner C, Emsley RA, Jordaan E, Mbanga NI, et al. A culture-bound syndrome ‘amafufunyana’ and a culture-specific event ‘ukuthwasa’: differentiated by a family history of schizophrenia and other psychiatric disorders. Psychopathology. 2004. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1159/​000077579.PubMedCrossRef
100.
Zurück zum Zitat Kleinman A. Do psychiatric disorders differ in different cultures? In: Brown JP, Closser S, editors. Understanding and applying medical anthropology: biosocial and cultural approaches. 3rd ed. London and New York: Routledge; 2016. p. 300–10. Kleinman A. Do psychiatric disorders differ in different cultures? In: Brown JP, Closser S, editors. Understanding and applying medical anthropology: biosocial and cultural approaches. 3rd ed. London and New York: Routledge; 2016. p. 300–10.
102.
119.
Zurück zum Zitat Bentall RP. Madness explained: psychosis and human nature. London: Penguin UK; 2004. Bentall RP. Madness explained: psychosis and human nature. London: Penguin UK; 2004.
123.
Zurück zum Zitat Colvin M, Gumede L, Grimwade K, Maher D, Wilkinson D. Contribution of traditional healers to a rural tuberculosis control programme in Hlabisa, South Africa. Int J Tuberc Lung Dis. 2003;7(9 Suppl 1):S86–91.PubMed Colvin M, Gumede L, Grimwade K, Maher D, Wilkinson D. Contribution of traditional healers to a rural tuberculosis control programme in Hlabisa, South Africa. Int J Tuberc Lung Dis. 2003;7(9 Suppl 1):S86–91.PubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Cultural differences in positive psychotic experiences assessed with the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences-42 (CAPE-42): a comparison of student populations in the Netherlands, Nigeria and Norway
verfasst von
Margriet Vermeiden
Mayke Janssens
Viviane Thewissen
Esther Akinsola
Sanne Peeters
Jennifer Reijnders
Nele Jacobs
Jim van Os
Johan Lataster
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2019
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2019
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-019-2210-8

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

BMC Psychiatry 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).