Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Experimental Brain Research 3/2008

01.04.2008 | Research Article

Goal-directed reaching: movement strategies influence the weighting of allocentric and egocentric visual cues

verfasst von: Kristina A. Neely, Ayla Tessmer, Gordon Binsted, Matthew Heath

Erschienen in: Experimental Brain Research | Ausgabe 3/2008

Einloggen, um Zugang zu erhalten

Abstract

The location of an object in peripersonal space can be represented with respect to our body (i.e., egocentric frame of reference) or relative to contextual features and other objects (i.e., allocentric frame of reference). In the current study, we sought to determine whether the frame, or frames, of visual reference supporting motor output is influenced by reach trajectories structured to maximize visual feedback utilization (i.e., controlled online) or structured largely in advance of movement onset via central planning mechanisms (i.e., controlled offline). Reaches were directed to a target embedded in a pictorial illusion (the induced Roelofs effect: IRE) and advanced knowledge of visual feedback was manipulated to influence the nature of reaching control as reported by Zelaznik et al. (J Mot Behav 15:217–236, 1983). When vision could not be predicted in advance of movement onset, trajectories showed primary evidence of an offline mode of control (even when vision was provided) and endpoints demonstrated amplified sensitivity to the illusory (i.e., allocentric) features of the IRE. In contrast, reaches performed with reliable visual feedback evidenced a primarily online mode of control and showed increased visuomotor resistance to the IRE. These findings suggest that the manner a reaching response is structured differentially influences the weighting of allocentric and egocentric visual information. More specifically, when visual feedback is unavailable or unpredictable, the weighting of allocentric visual information for the advanced planning of a reach trajectory is increased.
Fußnoten
1
We elected to use the induced Roelofs effect because the illusory features of this stimulus are thought constructed by later visual processing systems (i.e., the ventral visual pathway; see Milner and Dyde 2003 for a discussion of this issue). Moreover, our IRE configuration was oriented in the anteroposterior direction (see Fig. 1) and thus differs from the mediolateral orientation used in most previous work (e.g., Bridgeman et al. 1997, 2000, Dassonville et al. 2004). Importantly, however, the IRE orientation used here has been shown to robustly influence reaching endpoints (Coello et al. 2003; Neely et al. 2007; see Neely 2005 for discussion of IRE orientations and visuomotor susceptibility to allocentric visual cues).
 
2
The fact that R 2 values at 25% of movement time did not differentiate between the feedback schedule and visual condition combinations is congruent with earlier work arguing that at this time point the spatiotemporal features of an initial movement impulse do not have sufficient time to unfold to predict ultimate movement endpoints (see Heath et al. 2004b).
 
3
Of course, the hypothesis proposed here is specific to situations in which reaching/grasping responses are directed to a target within a structured visual background and not restrictive experimental contexts wherein allocentric visual cues are not available to the performer; e.g., when a performer reaches to an isolated target (i.e., point of light) presented in an otherwise neutral or empty visual background.
 
4
Bridgeman et al’s (1997) original IRE experiment is frequently cited as providing direct evidence that the IRE does not influence visuomotor control. Careful examination of that experiment, however (see Experiment 1), shows that “5 subjects showed a highly significant Roelofs effect [F(2, 4) > 18, P < 0.01], whereas the other 5 showed no sign of an effect [F(2, 4) ≤ 3.16, P > 0.18]” (p. 460). Importantly, that finding combined with a more recent work (Neely et al. 2007) highlights the existence of a controversy surrounding the extent to which the IRE represents an exemplar illusion supporting the view that egocentric visual cues restrictively mediate visually derived reaches.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Adamovich SV, Berkinblit MB, Fookson O, Poizner H (1999) Pointing in 3D space to remembered targets. II. Effects of movement speed toward kinesthetically defined targets. Exp Brain Res 125:200–210PubMedCrossRef Adamovich SV, Berkinblit MB, Fookson O, Poizner H (1999) Pointing in 3D space to remembered targets. II. Effects of movement speed toward kinesthetically defined targets. Exp Brain Res 125:200–210PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Beggs WD, Howarth CI (1970) Movement control in a repetitive motor task. Nature 225:752–753PubMedCrossRef Beggs WD, Howarth CI (1970) Movement control in a repetitive motor task. Nature 225:752–753PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Binsted G, Brownell K, Vorontsova Z, Heath M, Saucier D (2007) Visuomotor system uses target information unavailable to conscious awareness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:12669–12672PubMedCrossRef Binsted G, Brownell K, Vorontsova Z, Heath M, Saucier D (2007) Visuomotor system uses target information unavailable to conscious awareness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:12669–12672PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Binsted G, Heath M (2004) Can the motor system use a stored representation to control movement? Behav Brain Sci 27:25–27CrossRef Binsted G, Heath M (2004) Can the motor system use a stored representation to control movement? Behav Brain Sci 27:25–27CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bridgeman B, Gemmer A, Forsman T, Huemer V (2000) Processing spatial information in the sensorimotor branch of the visual system. Vision Res 40:3539–3352PubMedCrossRef Bridgeman B, Gemmer A, Forsman T, Huemer V (2000) Processing spatial information in the sensorimotor branch of the visual system. Vision Res 40:3539–3352PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bridgeman B, Peery S, Anand S (1997) Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space. Percept Psychophys 59:456–469PubMed Bridgeman B, Peery S, Anand S (1997) Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space. Percept Psychophys 59:456–469PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Carlton LG (1981) Processing visual feedback information for movement control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 7:1019–1030PubMedCrossRef Carlton LG (1981) Processing visual feedback information for movement control. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 7:1019–1030PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Coello Y, Grealy MA (1997) Effect of size and frame of visual field on the accuracy of an aiming movement. Perception 26:287–300PubMedCrossRef Coello Y, Grealy MA (1997) Effect of size and frame of visual field on the accuracy of an aiming movement. Perception 26:287–300PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Coello Y, Richaud S, Magne P, Rossetti Y (2003) Vision for spatial perception and vision for action: a dissociation between the left–right and near–far dimensions. Neuropsychologia 41:622–633PubMedCrossRef Coello Y, Richaud S, Magne P, Rossetti Y (2003) Vision for spatial perception and vision for action: a dissociation between the left–right and near–far dimensions. Neuropsychologia 41:622–633PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Conti P, Beaubaton D (1980) Role of structured visual field and visual reafference in accuracy of pointing movements. Percept Mot Skills 50:239–244PubMed Conti P, Beaubaton D (1980) Role of structured visual field and visual reafference in accuracy of pointing movements. Percept Mot Skills 50:239–244PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Culham JC, Danckert SL, DeSouza JF, Gati JS, Menon RS, Goodale MA (2003) Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas. Exp Brain Res 153:180–189PubMedCrossRef Culham JC, Danckert SL, DeSouza JF, Gati JS, Menon RS, Goodale MA (2003) Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas. Exp Brain Res 153:180–189PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dassonvile P, Bridgeman B, Bala JK, Thiem P, Sampanes A (2004) The induced Roelofs effect: two visual systems or the shift of a single reference frame? Vision Res 44:603–611CrossRef Dassonvile P, Bridgeman B, Bala JK, Thiem P, Sampanes A (2004) The induced Roelofs effect: two visual systems or the shift of a single reference frame? Vision Res 44:603–611CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Diedrichsen J, Werner S, Schmidt T, Trommershäuser J (2004) Immediate spatial distortions of pointing movements induced by visual landmarks. Percept Psychophys 66:89–103PubMed Diedrichsen J, Werner S, Schmidt T, Trommershäuser J (2004) Immediate spatial distortions of pointing movements induced by visual landmarks. Percept Psychophys 66:89–103PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Elliott D (1988) The influence of visual target and limb information on manual aiming. Can J Psychol 42:57–68PubMed Elliott D (1988) The influence of visual target and limb information on manual aiming. Can J Psychol 42:57–68PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Elliott D, Allard F (1985) The utilization of visual feedback information during rapid pointing movements. Q J Exp Psychol 37:407–425 Elliott D, Allard F (1985) The utilization of visual feedback information during rapid pointing movements. Q J Exp Psychol 37:407–425
Zurück zum Zitat Elliott D, Binsted G, Heath M (1999a) The control of goal-directed limb movements: correcting errors in the trajectory. Hum Mov Sci 18:121–136CrossRef Elliott D, Binsted G, Heath M (1999a) The control of goal-directed limb movements: correcting errors in the trajectory. Hum Mov Sci 18:121–136CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Elliott D, Heath M, Binsted G, Ricker KL, Roy EA, Chua R (1999b) Goal-directed aiming: correcting a force-specification error with the right and left hands. J Mot Behav 31:309–324PubMed Elliott D, Heath M, Binsted G, Ricker KL, Roy EA, Chua R (1999b) Goal-directed aiming: correcting a force-specification error with the right and left hands. J Mot Behav 31:309–324PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Elliott D, Helsen WF, Chua R (2001) A century later: Woodworth’s (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. Psychol Bull 127:342–57PubMedCrossRef Elliott D, Helsen WF, Chua R (2001) A century later: Woodworth’s (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. Psychol Bull 127:342–57PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Elliott D, Lee TD (1995) The role of target information on manual-aiming bias. Psychol Res 58:2–9 Elliott D, Lee TD (1995) The role of target information on manual-aiming bias. Psychol Res 58:2–9
Zurück zum Zitat Elliott D, Madalena J (1987) The influence of premovement visual information on manual aiming. Q J Exp Psychol 39A:541–559 Elliott D, Madalena J (1987) The influence of premovement visual information on manual aiming. Q J Exp Psychol 39A:541–559
Zurück zum Zitat Gentilucci M, Chieffi S, Daprati E, Saetti MC, Toni I (1996) Visual illusion and action. Neuropsychologia 34:369–376PubMedCrossRef Gentilucci M, Chieffi S, Daprati E, Saetti MC, Toni I (1996) Visual illusion and action. Neuropsychologia 34:369–376PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Glazebrook CM, Dhillon VP, Keetch KM, Lyons J, Amazeen E, Weeks DJ, Elliott D (2005) Perception-action and the Muller-Lyer illusion: amplitude or endpoint bias? Exp Brain Res 160:71–78PubMedCrossRef Glazebrook CM, Dhillon VP, Keetch KM, Lyons J, Amazeen E, Weeks DJ, Elliott D (2005) Perception-action and the Muller-Lyer illusion: amplitude or endpoint bias? Exp Brain Res 160:71–78PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15:20–25PubMedCrossRef Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15:20–25PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Goodale MA, Westwood DA (2004) An evolving view of duplex vision: separate but interacting cortical pathways for perception and action. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:203–211PubMedCrossRef Goodale MA, Westwood DA (2004) An evolving view of duplex vision: separate but interacting cortical pathways for perception and action. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:203–211PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Goodale MA, Westwood DA, Milner AD (2004) Two distinct modes of control for object-directed action. Prog Brain Res 144:131–144PubMed Goodale MA, Westwood DA, Milner AD (2004) Two distinct modes of control for object-directed action. Prog Brain Res 144:131–144PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Heath M (2005) Role of limb and target vision in the online control of memory-guided reaches. Motor Control 9:281–311PubMed Heath M (2005) Role of limb and target vision in the online control of memory-guided reaches. Motor Control 9:281–311PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Heath M, Neely K, Binsted G (2007) Allocentric visual cues influence online limb adjustments. Motor Control 11:54–70PubMed Heath M, Neely K, Binsted G (2007) Allocentric visual cues influence online limb adjustments. Motor Control 11:54–70PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Heath M, Rival C (2005) Role of the visuomotor system in on-line attenuation of a premovement illusory bias in grip aperture. Brain Cogn 57:111–114PubMedCrossRef Heath M, Rival C (2005) Role of the visuomotor system in on-line attenuation of a premovement illusory bias in grip aperture. Brain Cogn 57:111–114PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Heath M, Rival C, Binsted G (2004a) Can the motor system resolve a premovement bias in grip aperture? Online analysis of grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion. Exp Brain Res 158:378–384PubMedCrossRef Heath M, Rival C, Binsted G (2004a) Can the motor system resolve a premovement bias in grip aperture? Online analysis of grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion. Exp Brain Res 158:378–384PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Heath M, Rival C, Neely K (2006a) Visual feedback schedules influence visuomotor resistance to the Müller-Lyer figures. Exp Brain Res 168:348–56PubMedCrossRef Heath M, Rival C, Neely K (2006a) Visual feedback schedules influence visuomotor resistance to the Müller-Lyer figures. Exp Brain Res 168:348–56PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Heath M, Rival C, Neely K, Krigolson O (2006b) Müller-Lyer illusions influence the online reeorganziation of visually guided grasping movements. Exp Brain Res 169:473–481PubMedCrossRef Heath M, Rival C, Neely K, Krigolson O (2006b) Müller-Lyer illusions influence the online reeorganziation of visually guided grasping movements. Exp Brain Res 169:473–481PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Heath M, Rival C, Westwood DA, Neely K (2005) Time course analysis of closed- and open-loop grasping of the Müller-Lyer illusion. J Mot Behav 37:179–185PubMedCrossRef Heath M, Rival C, Westwood DA, Neely K (2005) Time course analysis of closed- and open-loop grasping of the Müller-Lyer illusion. J Mot Behav 37:179–185PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Heath M, Westwood DA (2003) Can a visual representation support the online control of memory-dependent reaching? Evidence from a variable spatial mapping paradigm. Motor Control 7:346–361PubMed Heath M, Westwood DA (2003) Can a visual representation support the online control of memory-dependent reaching? Evidence from a variable spatial mapping paradigm. Motor Control 7:346–361PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Heath M, Westwood DA, Binsted G (2004b) The control of memory-guided reaching movements in peripersonal space. Motor Control 8:76–106PubMed Heath M, Westwood DA, Binsted G (2004b) The control of memory-guided reaching movements in peripersonal space. Motor Control 8:76–106PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Henry FM (1986) Development of the motor memory trace and control program. J Mot Behav 18:77–100PubMed Henry FM (1986) Development of the motor memory trace and control program. J Mot Behav 18:77–100PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Henry FM, Rogers DE (1960) Increased response latency for complication movements and a “memory drum” theory of neuromotor reaction. Res Q Exerc Sport 31:448–458 Henry FM, Rogers DE (1960) Increased response latency for complication movements and a “memory drum” theory of neuromotor reaction. Res Q Exerc Sport 31:448–458
Zurück zum Zitat Held R, Gottlieb N (1958) Technique for studying adaptation to disarranged hand–eye coordination. Percept Mot Skills 8:83–86CrossRef Held R, Gottlieb N (1958) Technique for studying adaptation to disarranged hand–eye coordination. Percept Mot Skills 8:83–86CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hu Y, Goodale MA (2000) Grasping after a delay shifts size-scaling from absolute to relative metrics. J Cogn Neurosci 12:856–868PubMedCrossRef Hu Y, Goodale MA (2000) Grasping after a delay shifts size-scaling from absolute to relative metrics. J Cogn Neurosci 12:856–868PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hu Y, Eagleson R, Goodale MA (1999) The effects of delay on the kinematics of grasping. Exp Brain Res 126:109–116PubMedCrossRef Hu Y, Eagleson R, Goodale MA (1999) The effects of delay on the kinematics of grasping. Exp Brain Res 126:109–116PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jakobson LS, Goodale MA (1991) Factors affecting higher-order movement planning: a kinematic analysis of human prehension. Exp Brain Res 86:199–208PubMedCrossRef Jakobson LS, Goodale MA (1991) Factors affecting higher-order movement planning: a kinematic analysis of human prehension. Exp Brain Res 86:199–208PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat James TW, Culham J, Humphrey GK, Milner AD, Goodale MA (2003) Ventral occipital lesions impair object recognition but not object-directed grasping: an fMRI study. Brain 126:2463–2475PubMedCrossRef James TW, Culham J, Humphrey GK, Milner AD, Goodale MA (2003) Ventral occipital lesions impair object recognition but not object-directed grasping: an fMRI study. Brain 126:2463–2475PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Khan MA, Elliott D, Coull J, Chua R, Lyons J (2002) Optimal control strategies under different feedback schedules: kinematic evidence. J Mot Behav 32:45–57 Khan MA, Elliott D, Coull J, Chua R, Lyons J (2002) Optimal control strategies under different feedback schedules: kinematic evidence. J Mot Behav 32:45–57
Zurück zum Zitat Keetch KM, Glazebrook CM, Lyons J, Lam MY, Weeks DJ, Elliott D (2006) The effect of response uncertainty on illusory biases of perception and action. Neurosci Lett 406:117–121PubMedCrossRef Keetch KM, Glazebrook CM, Lyons J, Lam MY, Weeks DJ, Elliott D (2006) The effect of response uncertainty on illusory biases of perception and action. Neurosci Lett 406:117–121PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Keele SW (1968) Movement control in skilled motor performance. Psychol Bull 70:387–403CrossRef Keele SW (1968) Movement control in skilled motor performance. Psychol Bull 70:387–403CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Klapp ST (1975) Feedback versus motor programming in the control of aimed movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 104:161–169PubMed Klapp ST (1975) Feedback versus motor programming in the control of aimed movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 104:161–169PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Krigolson O, Clark N, Heath M, Binsted G (2007) The proximity of visual landmarks impacts reaching performance. Spat Vis 20:317–336PubMedCrossRef Krigolson O, Clark N, Heath M, Binsted G (2007) The proximity of visual landmarks impacts reaching performance. Spat Vis 20:317–336PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Krigolson O, Heath M (2004) Background visual cues and memory-guided reaching. Hum Mov Sci 23:861–877PubMedCrossRef Krigolson O, Heath M (2004) Background visual cues and memory-guided reaching. Hum Mov Sci 23:861–877PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Krigolson O, Heath M (2006) A lower visual field advantage for endpoint stability but no advantage for online movement precision. Exp Brain Res 170:127–135PubMedCrossRef Krigolson O, Heath M (2006) A lower visual field advantage for endpoint stability but no advantage for online movement precision. Exp Brain Res 170:127–135PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McIntosh RD, McClements KI, Schindler I, Cassidy TP, Birchall D, Milner AD (2004) Avoidance of obstacles in the absence of visual awareness. Proc Biol Sci 7:15–20CrossRef McIntosh RD, McClements KI, Schindler I, Cassidy TP, Birchall D, Milner AD (2004) Avoidance of obstacles in the absence of visual awareness. Proc Biol Sci 7:15–20CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McIntyre J, Stratta F, Lacquaniti F (1997) Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space. J Neurophysiol 78:1601–1618PubMed McIntyre J, Stratta F, Lacquaniti F (1997) Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space. J Neurophysiol 78:1601–1618PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Mendoza JE, Elliott D, Meegan DV, Lyons JL, Welsh TN (2006) The effect of the Muller-Lyer illusion on the planning and control of manual aiming movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32:413–422PubMedCrossRef Mendoza JE, Elliott D, Meegan DV, Lyons JL, Welsh TN (2006) The effect of the Muller-Lyer illusion on the planning and control of manual aiming movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32:413–422PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mendoza J, Hansen S, Glazebrook CM, Keetch KM, Elliott D (2005) Visual illusions affect both movement planning and on-line control: a multiple cue position on bias and goal-directed action. Hum Mov Sci 24:760–773PubMedCrossRef Mendoza J, Hansen S, Glazebrook CM, Keetch KM, Elliott D (2005) Visual illusions affect both movement planning and on-line control: a multiple cue position on bias and goal-directed action. Hum Mov Sci 24:760–773PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Merigan WH, Maunsell JH (1993) How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annu Rev Neurosci 16:369–402PubMedCrossRef Merigan WH, Maunsell JH (1993) How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annu Rev Neurosci 16:369–402PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Messier J, Kalaska JF (1999) Comparison of variability of initial kinematics and endpoints of reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 125:139–152PubMedCrossRef Messier J, Kalaska JF (1999) Comparison of variability of initial kinematics and endpoints of reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 125:139–152PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Meyer DA, Abrams RA, Kornblum S, Wright CE, Smith JEK (1988) Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol Rev 95:340–370PubMedCrossRef Meyer DA, Abrams RA, Kornblum S, Wright CE, Smith JEK (1988) Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol Rev 95:340–370PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Milner AD, Goodale MA (1995) The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press, Oxford Milner AD, Goodale MA (1995) The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Zurück zum Zitat Milner D, Dyde R (2003) Why do some perceptual illusions affect visually guided action, when others don’t? Trends Cogn Sci 7:10–11PubMedCrossRef Milner D, Dyde R (2003) Why do some perceptual illusions affect visually guided action, when others don’t? Trends Cogn Sci 7:10–11PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Neely KA (2005) The induced Roelofs effect: evidence for an interaction between allocentric and egocentric visual information. Unpublished master’s thesis, Indiana University, Bloomington Neely KA (2005) The induced Roelofs effect: evidence for an interaction between allocentric and egocentric visual information. Unpublished master’s thesis, Indiana University, Bloomington
Zurück zum Zitat Neely KA, Binsted G, Heath M (2007) Allocentric and egocentric visual cues influence the specification of movement distance and direction. J Mot Behav (in press) Neely KA, Binsted G, Heath M (2007) Allocentric and egocentric visual cues influence the specification of movement distance and direction. J Mot Behav (in press)
Zurück zum Zitat Obhi SS, Goodale MA (2005) The effects of landmarks on the performance of delayed and real-time pointing movements. Exp Brain Res 167:335–344PubMedCrossRef Obhi SS, Goodale MA (2005) The effects of landmarks on the performance of delayed and real-time pointing movements. Exp Brain Res 167:335–344PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pisella L, Grea H, Tilikete C, Vighetto A, Desmurget M, Rode G, Boisson D, Rossetti Y (2000) An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia. Nat Neurosci 3:729–736PubMedCrossRef Pisella L, Grea H, Tilikete C, Vighetto A, Desmurget M, Rode G, Boisson D, Rossetti Y (2000) An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia. Nat Neurosci 3:729–736PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Plamondon R (1995) A kinematic theory of rapid human movements. Part II. Movement time and control. Biol Cybern 72:309–720PubMedCrossRef Plamondon R (1995) A kinematic theory of rapid human movements. Part II. Movement time and control. Biol Cybern 72:309–720PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Proteau L, Marteniuk RG, Levesque L (1992) A sensorimotor basis for motor learning: evidence indicating specificity of practice. Q J Exp Psychol 44A:557–575 Proteau L, Marteniuk RG, Levesque L (1992) A sensorimotor basis for motor learning: evidence indicating specificity of practice. Q J Exp Psychol 44A:557–575
Zurück zum Zitat Redon C, Hay L (2005) Role of visual context and oculomotor conditions in pointing accuracy. Neuroreport 16:2065–2067PubMedCrossRef Redon C, Hay L (2005) Role of visual context and oculomotor conditions in pointing accuracy. Neuroreport 16:2065–2067PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schluter ND, Rushworth MF, Mills KR, Passingham RE (1999) Signal-, set-, and movement-related activity in the human premotor cortex. Neuropsychologia 37:233–243PubMedCrossRef Schluter ND, Rushworth MF, Mills KR, Passingham RE (1999) Signal-, set-, and movement-related activity in the human premotor cortex. Neuropsychologia 37:233–243PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schmidt RA, Zelaznik H, Hawkins B, Frank JS, Quinn JT (1979) Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychol Rev 47:415–451PubMedCrossRef Schmidt RA, Zelaznik H, Hawkins B, Frank JS, Quinn JT (1979) Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. Psychol Rev 47:415–451PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schindler I, Rice NJ, McIntosh RD, Rossetti Y, Vighetto A, Milner AD (2004) Automatic avoidance of obstacles is a dorsal stream function: evidence from optic ataxia. Nat Neurosci 7:779–784PubMedCrossRef Schindler I, Rice NJ, McIntosh RD, Rossetti Y, Vighetto A, Milner AD (2004) Automatic avoidance of obstacles is a dorsal stream function: evidence from optic ataxia. Nat Neurosci 7:779–784PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Smeets JBJ, Brenner E, de Grave DDJ, Cuijpers RH (2002) Illusions in action: consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes. Exp Brain Res 147:135–144PubMedCrossRef Smeets JBJ, Brenner E, de Grave DDJ, Cuijpers RH (2002) Illusions in action: consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes. Exp Brain Res 147:135–144PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Smyrnis N, Taira M, Ashe J, Georgopoulos AP (1992) Motor cortical activity in a memorized delay task. Exp Brain Res 92:139–151PubMedCrossRef Smyrnis N, Taira M, Ashe J, Georgopoulos AP (1992) Motor cortical activity in a memorized delay task. Exp Brain Res 92:139–151PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Ungerleider LG, Courtney SM, Haxby JV (1998) A neural system for human visual working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:883–90PubMedCrossRef Ungerleider LG, Courtney SM, Haxby JV (1998) A neural system for human visual working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:883–90PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Velay JL, Beaubaton D (1986) Influence of visual context on pointing movement accuracy. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive 6:447–456 Velay JL, Beaubaton D (1986) Influence of visual context on pointing movement accuracy. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive 6:447–456
Zurück zum Zitat Westwood DA, Heath M, Roy EA (2000) The effect of a pictorial illusion on closed-loop and open-loop prehension. Exp Brain Res 134:456–463PubMedCrossRef Westwood DA, Heath M, Roy EA (2000) The effect of a pictorial illusion on closed-loop and open-loop prehension. Exp Brain Res 134:456–463PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Westwood DA, Heath M, Roy EA (2001) The accuracy of reaching movements in brief delay conditions. Can J Exp Psychol 55:304–10PubMed Westwood DA, Heath M, Roy EA (2001) The accuracy of reaching movements in brief delay conditions. Can J Exp Psychol 55:304–10PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Westwood DA, Heath M, Roy EA (2003) No evidence for accurate visuomotor memory: systematic and variable error in memory-guided reaching. J Mot Behav 35:127–133PubMedCrossRef Westwood DA, Heath M, Roy EA (2003) No evidence for accurate visuomotor memory: systematic and variable error in memory-guided reaching. J Mot Behav 35:127–133PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Westwood DA, Goodale MA (2003) Perceptual illusion and the real-time control of action. Spat Vis 16:243–254PubMedCrossRef Westwood DA, Goodale MA (2003) Perceptual illusion and the real-time control of action. Spat Vis 16:243–254PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Whitney D, Westwood DA, Goodale MA (2003) The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object. Nature 423:869–873PubMedCrossRef Whitney D, Westwood DA, Goodale MA (2003) The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object. Nature 423:869–873PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Woodworth RS (1899) The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychol Rev 3:1–114 Woodworth RS (1899) The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychol Rev 3:1–114
Zurück zum Zitat Zelaznik HZ, Hawkins B, Kisselburgh L (1983) Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. J Mot Behav 15:217–236PubMed Zelaznik HZ, Hawkins B, Kisselburgh L (1983) Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. J Mot Behav 15:217–236PubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Goal-directed reaching: movement strategies influence the weighting of allocentric and egocentric visual cues
verfasst von
Kristina A. Neely
Ayla Tessmer
Gordon Binsted
Matthew Heath
Publikationsdatum
01.04.2008
Verlag
Springer-Verlag
Erschienen in
Experimental Brain Research / Ausgabe 3/2008
Print ISSN: 0014-4819
Elektronische ISSN: 1432-1106
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1238-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2008

Experimental Brain Research 3/2008 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Hirnblutung unter DOAK und VKA ähnlich bedrohlich

17.05.2024 Direkte orale Antikoagulanzien Nachrichten

Kommt es zu einer nichttraumatischen Hirnblutung, spielt es keine große Rolle, ob die Betroffenen zuvor direkt wirksame orale Antikoagulanzien oder Marcumar bekommen haben: Die Prognose ist ähnlich schlecht.

Thrombektomie auch bei großen Infarkten von Vorteil

16.05.2024 Ischämischer Schlaganfall Nachrichten

Auch ein sehr ausgedehnter ischämischer Schlaganfall scheint an sich kein Grund zu sein, von einer mechanischen Thrombektomie abzusehen. Dafür spricht die LASTE-Studie, an der Patienten und Patientinnen mit einem ASPECTS von maximal 5 beteiligt waren.

Schwindelursache: Massagepistole lässt Otholiten tanzen

14.05.2024 Benigner Lagerungsschwindel Nachrichten

Wenn jüngere Menschen über ständig rezidivierenden Lagerungsschwindel klagen, könnte eine Massagepistole der Auslöser sein. In JAMA Otolaryngology warnt ein Team vor der Anwendung hochpotenter Geräte im Bereich des Nackens.

Schützt Olivenöl vor dem Tod durch Demenz?

10.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Konsumieren Menschen täglich 7 Gramm Olivenöl, ist ihr Risiko, an einer Demenz zu sterben, um mehr als ein Viertel reduziert – und dies weitgehend unabhängig von ihrer sonstigen Ernährung. Dafür sprechen Auswertungen zweier großer US-Studien.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.