Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 1/2015

Open Access 01.12.2015 | Research

Learning by observation and learning by doing in Prader-Willi syndrome

verfasst von: Francesca Foti, Deny Menghini, Enzo Orlandi, Cristina Rufini, Antonino Crinò, Sabrina Spera, Stefano Vicari, Laura Petrosini, Laura Mandolesi

Erschienen in: Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders | Ausgabe 1/2015

Abstract

Background

New competencies may be learned through active experience (learning by doing) or observation of others’ experience (learning by observation). Observing another person performing a complex action accelerates the observer’s acquisition of the same action, limiting the time-consuming process of learning by doing. Here, we compared learning by observation and learning by doing in individuals with Prader-Willi syndrome (PWS). It is hypothesized that PWS individuals could show more difficulties with learning by observation than learning by doing because of their specific difficulty in interpreting and using social information.

Methods

The performance of 24 PWS individuals was compared with that of 28 mental age (MA)- and gender-matched typically developing (TD) children in tasks of learning a visuo-motor sequence by observation or by doing. To determine whether the performance pattern exhibited by PWS participants was specific to this population or whether it was a nonspecific intellectual disability effect, we compared the PWS performances with those of a third MA- and gender-matched group of individuals with Williams syndrome (WS).

Results

PWS individuals were severely impaired in detecting a sequence by observation, were able to detect a sequence by doing, and became as efficient as TD children in reproducing an observed sequence after a task of learning by doing. The learning pattern of PWS children was reversed compared with that of WS individuals.

Conclusions

The observational learning deficit in PWS individuals may be rooted, at least partially, in their incapacity to understand and/or use social information.
Begleitmaterial
Additional file 1: Table S1. Statistical comparisons of performances of new WS and old WS. To verify the stability of WS participants’ data between the sample employed in the previously published study [30] and the present one, we compared the performances displayed by the 16 WS of the present study (new WS) and those by the 20 WS of the previously published study [30] (old WS) on the four main parameters by means of ANOVAs.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​s11689-015-9102-0) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

FF, DM, AC, SV, LP, and LM designed the research. FF, EO, CR, and SS performed the research. FF, EO, CR, and LP analyzed the data. FF, DM, LP, and LM wrote the paper. All authors read, revised, and approved the final manuscript.
Abkürzungen
AP
automatization phase
CA
chronological age
DP
detection phase
EP
exercise phase
ID
intellectual disability
MA
mental age
OBS
observational task
PWS
Prader-Willi syndrome
TD
typically developing
TE
trial and error task
WS
Williams syndrome

Background

Prader-Willi syndrome (PWS) is a genetic disorder with an incidence rate at birth of about 1:15,000 to 1:20,000 caused by paternal deletion within 15q11-q13 (70% to 75% of cases), maternal disomy of chromosome 15 (mUPD) (20% to 25%), or unbalanced translocation or imprinting center defect (2%) [1,2]. PWS is characterized by hyperphagia; early-onset and morbid obesity if appropriate treatments (growth hormone treatment, diet and exercise regimes) are not provided; hypogonadism; hypotonia; maladaptive behavior, such as repetitive and stereotypical behavior, mental rigidity, impulsiveness, temper outbursts, and resistance to change; and impaired social functioning [3-5]. PWS individuals are characterized by a downward shift in the distribution of IQ scores and mild to moderate intellectual disability (ID) [6]. Their cognitive profile is characterized by strengths in long-term memory, visual perception, simultaneous processing, reading skill, and visuo-spatial functions and weaknesses in attention, short-term memory, sequential processing, executive functions, action-based visual processing, auditory processing, mathematical skills, language abilities, and social cognition [7-16].
Although the behavioral phenotype of PWS individuals has been characterized with regard to maladaptive behavior and cognitive profile (for a review, see [3]), their social functioning has been only recently examined. Social impairment exhibited by PWS individuals represents a deficit that is not merely a consequence of their maladaptive behavior, but it may reflect their specific difficulty in interpreting and using social information, such as emotional and nonverbal cues, facial emotional expressions, other’s mental and feeling states, and visual information into a coherent social story [16-19]. Most reports describe PWS people as characterized by poor peer relationships, social withdrawal, and preference for solitary activities [20,21]. Furthermore, they often display aggressive behavior and a deficitary comprehension of other’s thoughts or perspective [17,22].
To date, no research has analyzed whether different learning modalities facilitate or hinder the acquisition of new skills in PWS individuals. New competencies may be learned through active experience (learning by doing) or through observation of others’ experience (learning by observation) [23,24]. While learning by doing involves direct experience, learning by observation involves social processing, with all the other variables (for example, motor and cognitive complexity) being equal. Observing another person performing a complex action and solving a problem accelerates the observer’s acquisition of the same action, limits the time-consuming process of learning by trial and error, and reduces the practice needed to learn the skill [24,25]. Thus, it represents a powerful learning mechanism that may be based also on social processing [26-28].
The present research compared learning by observation and learning by doing in PWS individuals. It is hypothesized that PWS individuals show more difficulties with learning by observation than learning by doing because of their specific difficulty in interpreting and using social information.
The participants learned a visuo-motor sequence by performing the task after observing an actor detect the sequence of correct items by trial and error (learning by observation) or by actually detecting the correct sequence by trial and error (learning by doing) (Figure 1). The same visuo-motor task was previously used in studies of individuals with Williams syndrome (WS), dyslexia, and autistic spectrum disorders [29-31]. The task is suitable for studying the declarative and procedural components of learning. The performances of PWS individuals were compared with those of a mental age (MA)- and gender-matched group of typically developing (TD) children. To determine whether the performance pattern exhibited by PWS participants was specific to this population or whether it was a nonspecific ID effect, we compared the PWS performances with those of a third MA- and gender-matched group of WS individuals [30]. The study design that matched three experimental samples allowed us to take into account whether the learning performance of PWS individuals was better or worse than expected given their general level of intellectual functioning indexed as MA (MA-matched control group) and whether the performances were due to the cognitive profile of their specific pathology considering IQ (ID-matched control group) [10,32].

Methods

Participants

Twenty-four individuals with PWS, 24 individuals with WS (syndromic control group), and 28 TD children (control group) matching the PWS and WS groups for MA and gender were examined. Eight individuals with WS and five TD children participated in both the present study as well as our recent research [30]. The stability of data displayed by WS participants was verified as described in Additional file 1: Table S1. The present study encompassed two experimental conditions: learning by doing followed by learning by observation (Condition 1) and learning by observation followed by learning by doing (Condition 2) (Figure 1; Table 1). The participants were randomly assigned to the two experimental conditions. Chronological age (CA) and MA, as well as IQ, of all participants are compared in Table 1.
Table 1
Statistical comparisons (one-way ANOVA) of CA, MA, and IQ between PWS, WS, and TD groups
Group
CA Mean (±SEM)
F (fd) P η P 2
MA Mean (±SEM)
F (fd) P η P 2
IQ Mean (±SEM)
F (fd) P η P 2
PWS1 (Condition 1)
21.08 (±2.06)
F 1, 22 = 1.16, P = 0.29, η P 2 = 0.05
6.02 (±0.01)
F 1, 22 = 0.70, P = 0.41, η P 2 = 0.03
51.8 (±2.6)
F 1, 22 = 0.07, P = 0.79, η P 2 = 0.003
PWS2 (Condition 2)
18.05 (±1.08)
6.05 (±0.03)
52.7 (±2.9)
WS1 (Condition 1)
20.05 (±2.04)
F 1, 22 = 0.59, P = 0.45, η P 2 = 0.03
6.04 (±0.02)
F 1, 22 = 0.07, P = 0.78, η P 2 = 0.003
54.2 (±2.7)
F 1, 22 = 0.01, P = 0.90, η P 2 = 0.0006
WS2 (Condition 2)
17.06 (±2.02)
6.05 (±0.03)
53.8 (±2.2)
TD1 (Condition 1)
6.06 (±0.02)
F 1, 26 = 0.03, P = 0.85, η P 2 = 0.001
6.06 (±0.03)
F 1, 26 = 0.69, P = 0.41, η P 2 = 0.03
103 (±3.1)
F 1, 26 = 2.27, P = 0.14, η P 2  = 0.08
TD2 (Condition 2)
6.07 (±0.02)
6.04 (±0.03)
109.1 (±2.7)
All pathological participants were part of a larger pool of individuals attending the Children’s Hospital Bambino Gesù for clinical and rehabilitative follow-up. In the PWS and WS participants, clinical diagnosis was confirmed by genetic investigation (fluorescence in situ hybridization (FISH)), which showed paternal deletion on chromosome band 15q11-q13 in the PWS group and deletion on chromosome band 7q11.23 in the WS group. All PWS participants had been receiving growth hormone therapy for ≥3 years and were in euthyroidism. All participants lived with their own families.
While TD children were individually tested in a quiet room at their schools, all syndromic participants were tested in a quiet room at the Children’s Hospital Bambino Gesù. The study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The parents of participants gave written informed consent.

Intelligence evaluation and neuropsychological assessment

The brief version of the Leiter International Performance Scale–Revised (four out of 10 subtests: figure ground, form completion, sequential order, and repeated patterns) was used to compute brief IQ and the corresponding MA [33]. Visuo-motor integration and memory functions were assessed by visuo-motor integration (VMI) [34], visuo-spatial short-term memory (VSS), and visuo-object short-term memory (VOS) tests [35]. Description of tests and statistical comparisons among groups are reported in Table 2 and Additional file 2: Table S2.
Table 2
Statistical comparisons (one-way ANOVA) of performances of PWS, WS, and TD participants
Cognitive domain
PWS Mean (±SEM)
WS Mean (±SEM)
TD Mean (±SEM)
Group effect F (fd) P η P 2
Post hoc Newman-Keuls’s test
     
P ; Cohen’s d ; r
VMI
13.08 (±0.54)
12.79 (±0.52)
15.14 (±0.28)
F 2, 73 = 8.51
PWS vs. WS
P = 0.0005
P = 0.65; d = 0.11; r = 0.05
η P 2 = 0.19
PWS vs. TD
P = 0.002; d = −0.96; r = −0.43
WS vs. TD
P = 0.001; d = −1.12; r = −0.49
VSS
3.35 (±0.14)
2.63 (±0.19)
3.43 (±0.16)
F 2, 73 = 6.50
PWS vs. WS
P = 0.003
P = 0.004; d = 0.84; r = 0.39
η P 2 = 0.15
PWS vs. TD
P = 0.75; d = −0.09; r = −0.05
WS vs. TD
P = 0.004; d = −0.87; r = −0.40
VOS
2.79 (±0.15)
2.71 (±0.13)
2.89 (±0.13)
F 2, 73 = 0.48
 
P = 0.62
η P 2 = 0.013
VMI, visuo-motor integration; VSS, visuo-spatial short-term memory; VOS, visuo-object short-term memory.

Experimental procedure

Each participant sat in front of a computer touch screen (distance 60 cm). In both conditions, the experimenter acting as the actor (FF) sat near the participant. An 8 × 8 black matrix appeared on the touch screen. The participant was asked to find a hidden sequence of correct squares prepared in advance by the experimenters. The sequence was composed of ten adjacent spatial positions in the matrix, which formed a snake-like pattern (Figure 1).
To explain the task to each participant, the experimenter used the same Italian verbal instructions because all participants were native Italian speakers. Below is the translation of the verbal instructions provided to all participants: ‘You have to find a snake formed by ten squares. When you touch a correct square belonging to snake body it will be turned gray and you will hear a sound; conversely, if you touch a wrong square not belonging to the snake, it will be turned red. In this case, you have to find a new gray square. You have to re-start each time you find a new correct square. After finding the whole snake, you have to re-touch it three times without making lighted red squares.’ The participants started touching a gray square, which was the first element of the sequence representing the snake body and was always lit up. In the search for the second correct square, the participants had to touch one of the four squares bordering the gray square by moving in the matrix vertically or horizontally, but never diagonally. Each touched square (correct or wrong) was lit up for 500 ms and then lighted off again; thus, no trace of the touched sequence remained on the screen.
In learning the sequence by trial and error (learning by doing), the participants tried to find the correct sequence immediately after the verbal instructions. Conversely, in learning the sequence by observation, after the verbal instructions the participants observed the actor while she (FF) detected a ten-item sequence by trial and error (observational training). The actor performed the task by always making the same errors in the same positions, so that all participants observed the same pattern of correct and wrong touches. No more than 2 min after the end of the observational training, the participants were required to reproduce the correct sequence (the snake).

Parameters

Regardless of whether learning took place by observation or by doing, the two tasks involved three phases: the detection phase (DP) that ended once the participants found the tenth correct position, the exercise phase (EP) in which they had to repeat the ten-item sequence until their performance was error-free, and the automatization phase (AP) that ended when the correct sequence was repeated three consecutive times without errors.
The parameters measured were as follows: DP errors, calculated as the number of incorrect items touched in detecting the ten correct positions; EP repetitions, calculated as the number of replications needed to reach the error-free performance; and AP times (in ms), calculated as the time spent carrying out each of the three repetitions of the sequence. Considering DP and EP together, we calculated perseverations, consecutive errors touching the same square or a fixed sequence of squares; sequence errors, touching a correct square at the wrong moment (for example, touching E7 before F7); side-by-side errors, touching the squares bordering the correct sequence (for example, E8); illogical errors, touching any other square (for example, B5); and, exclusively in the observational learning task, imitative errors, touching the squares deliberately wrongly touched by the actor during the observational training (for example, F4) (Figure 1).
The error analysis allowed a multi-faceted characterization of the performance. Specifically, sequence and side-by-side errors allowed analysis of mnesic, planning, and inhibitory abilities, and cognitive flexibility. Illogical errors permitted analysis of adherence to the experimental setting and understanding the task instructions. Finally, imitative errors provided information on the tendency to adhere to the behavior of the social model (actor) and hyperimitate it, because the observational learning did not merely involve copying an action but required that the observer transformed the observation into an action as similar as possible to the model in terms of the goal (detecting the snake) to be reached. The hyperimitative tendency is faithfully copying both necessary and unnecessary actions made by the actor. Besides a reduced understanding of the rules of the task, hyperimitation may reflect a social process linked to the individual’s motivation to affiliate with the demonstrator or to closely conform to perceived norms [36,37]. Therefore, the analysis of the imitative errors is important to facet the features of the learning by observation.

Condition 1: learning by doing followed by learning by observation

Twelve PWS, 12 WS, and 14 TD participants (Table 1) detected a sequence by doing (trial and error task, TE1), and after 10 min from task end, they observed the experimenter detect a different sequence (observational training). After 2 min, participants were required to reproduce the observed sequence (observational task, OBS2). There was no fixed time limit for executing the task.
Although the two sequences to be used as TE and OBS sequences had two different forms, their degree of difficulty did not differ because both sequences had the same number of squares (10) and corners (2). To confirm this assumption, a pilot study was conducted. Six TD children [four males] of MA 6.04 ± 0.2 years detected the two different sequences by doing; the presentation order was randomized among participants. DP errors made in detecting TE (\( \overline{x} \) = 24.83 ± 2.57) and OBS (\( \overline{x} \) = 20.83 ± 2.19) sequences, evaluated using Wilcoxon’s test, were not significantly different (Z = 1.21, P = 0.22).

Condition 2: learning by observation followed by learning by doing

Twelve PWS, 12 WS, and 14 TD participants (Table 1) observed the experimenter detect a sequence (OBS1) and then reproduced it. After 10 min from task end, they detected a different sequence by doing (TE2). The difference between the two conditions was that participants reproduced a sequence learned by observation after (Condition 1) or before (Condition 2) the detection of a different sequence by doing. This protocol encompassing the use of both tasks (OBS and TE) in each condition allowed analysis of the performances of the same participants in the two types of learning. To exclude any practice effect, inevitably present in the second tasks and potentially affecting performances, Conditions 1 and 2 (with the only change being the order of presentation) were needed.
No significant differences in CA, MA, and IQ (always P > 0.1) among participants performing Conditions 1 and 2 were found (Table 1).

Cognitive mapping abilities

In all participants, we evaluated the cognitive map, which was the spatial mental representation in which information about the relative locations of the squares was coded to connect them in the global sequence [38,39]. To this aim, at the end of each task (OBS or TE), every participant drew the arrangement of the just-reproduced sequence on an 8 × 8 matrix sketched on a paper sheet, in which only the starting point was indicated (Additional file 3). Each participant drew two sequences, one learned by observation and the other one by doing. We evaluated the positions of every square and considered error any marked square outside of the just-reproduced sequence. Three categories of errors were considered: no error, one error, and more than one error.

Statistical analyses

The data were first tested for normality (Shapiro-Wilk’s test) and homoscedasticity (Levene’s test) and then compared by using two-, three-, or four-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) followed by post hoc multiple comparisons by using Newman-Keuls’s test. The two-way ANOVAs were performed by applying the mixed model for independent variables (PWS, WS, and TD groups) and repeated measures (type of error: illogical, sequence, side-by-side, and imitative). Three-way ANOVAs (group × condition × task; group (PWS, WS, TD); condition (1, 2); task (OBS, TE)) were performed on DP errors, EP repetitions, and perseverations. A four-way ANOVA was performed on AP times by applying the mixed model for independent variables (group (PWS, WS, TD); condition (1, 2); task (OBS, TE)) and repeated measures (times (1, 2, 3) spent carrying out each of the three repetitions of the sequence). Error categories of mapping abilities were analyzed by the χ 2 test. Data of the pilot study were analyzed by using nonparametric analysis (Wilcoxon’s test). Analyses were performed by Statistica 8.0, and the significance level was established at P < 0.05. Since in the present study a number of analyses was run, controlling for the alpha inflation was needed. We controlled the proportion of type I errors among all rejected null hypotheses by setting the false discovery rate (FDR) to 0.05. The FDR was estimated through the procedure described in [40]. In our results, the 0.05 level of significance corresponded to an FDR < 0.05. The complete statistical analyses are reported as Additional file 4: Table S4 and Additional file 5: Table S5.

Results

Learning tasks

In TE1, unlike WS participants, PWS participants did not differ from TD children in DP errors they performed in detecting the sequence by doing (Figure 2A). Conversely, in comparison with TD and WS participants, PWS participants performed a number of DP errors significantly higher in OBS1 but not significantly different in OBS2 and TE2 tasks (Figure 2A), as revealed by post hoc comparisons on the second-order interaction of the three-way ANOVA (group × condition × task) (F (2, 70) = 5.13, P = 0.0083, η P 2 = 0.13).
As for EP repetitions, while WS participants needed a significantly higher number in comparison to TD participants, PWS and TD participants did not differ as revealed by post hoc comparisons made on the group effect (F (2, 70) = 3.36, P = 0.040, η P 2 = 0.09) of the three-way ANOVA (group × condition × task) (Figure 2B). Even the analysis of perseverations revealed no significant difference among PWS and TD participants. Conversely, in TE1, WS individuals performed a number of perseverations significantly higher than PWS and TD participants, as revealed by post hoc comparisons on the second-order interaction (F (2, 70) = 3.18, P = 0.048, η P 2 = 0.08) of the three-way ANOVA (group × condition × task) (Figure 2C).
A similar pattern was found in the analysis of the three AP times. PWS participants exhibited AP times significantly lower than WS individuals, but not significantly different from those of TD children, as revealed by post hoc comparisons on the group effect (F (2, 70) = 8.26, P = 0.0006, η P 2 = 0.19) of the four-way ANOVA (group × condition × task × time) (Figure 2D). All participants exhibited significantly reduced times as the task proceeded (F (2, 140) = 33.67, P < 0.000001, η P 2 = 0.32), indicating a progressive automatization of the task.

Analysis of error

In OBS1, PWS individuals exhibited a number of sequence errors higher than TD children and interestingly higher than WS participants, as revealed by post hoc comparisons made on the significant interaction (F (6, 105) = 2.93, P = 0.011, η P 2 = 0.14) of the two-way ANOVA (group × type of error). The PWS individuals exhibited also a number of side-by-side errors higher than TD children. PWS, WS, and TD participants did not differ in the number of illogical and imitative errors (Figures 3 and 4). The analysis of error in the remaining TE1, OBS2, and TE2 tasks revealed no significant difference among the groups, even if significant differences among errors were found (always P < 0.000001) (Figures 3 and 4). Also interactions were not significant.

Cognitive mapping abilities

No significant difference among groups and among error categories was found in any sequence (always P > 0.3), a clear index of similar cognitive mapping abilities in all groups.

Discussion

The current study aimed at analyzing learning by observation and learning by doing in PWS in comparison with WS and TD individuals. With the exception of the imitative competencies, both visuo-motor learning tasks required attentive and mnesic functions, sequencing abilities, planning, response inhibition, cognitive flexibility, good knowledge and anticipatory expectation of effects related to actions, goal-directed actions, and motor imagery allowing recombination of novel actions with novel effects [29-31,41]. The main result of the present study showed a specific PWS deficit in learning by observation. The observational training did not help PWS individuals to detect and encode information, such as rules of the task, correct moves, and goals they had to reach. PWS individuals were impaired in reproducing the previously observed visuo-motor sequence when the observational task was proposed at first (OBS1), while they were as efficient as the TD children in detecting a sequence by trial and error (TE1 and TE2) and in reproducing the previously observed sequence when proposed as a second task (OBS2). The learning pattern of PWS was the reverse of that of WS individuals who were severely impaired in detecting the visuo-motor sequence in TE1 and as efficient as TD children in OBS1.
The deficit in learning by observation found in PWS individuals may be related to the impairment in social functioning described in this population [17]. Studies on the social deficits present in PWS have focused on either high frequencies of stubbornness, disobedience, and obsessive/compulsive and ritualistic behaviors or solitary behaviors, social withdrawal, and poor peer relations [42-44]. Even the frequent temper outbursts of PWS individuals are attributed to their impaired capacity to understand the motivations of others in the social milieu [17]. The poor performance of PWS individuals in OBS1 suggests a specific incapacity to use the information provided by the actor. PWS individuals display difficulty in recognizing and interpreting social cues and situations on the Social Attribution Task that measures the specific abilities necessary for interpreting social information [17]. It has been suggested that in PWS individuals the few attributions of feeling in social relationships indicate their deficit in empathy and ‘theory of mind.’ Such a deficit may determine impairment in interpersonal processes that are crucial for developing social abilities and understanding another’s thoughts and perspectives [12,45]. In a functional neuroimaging study, the typical difficulties in interacting with peers and understanding social environment displayed by PWS individuals are related to the perfusion abnormalities of the anterior cingulum and the cingulate gyrus found in these individuals [46]. Conclusively, the impaired PWS performance in OBS1 can be at least partially attributed to their difficulties in processing social information. The specular learning pattern of WS individuals (impaired TE1 and efficient OBS1) was coherent with their spared social abilities [47]. The PWS good performances in OBS2 do not contradict such an interpretation, because any second task (OBS2 and TE2) allowed overcoming the specific deficits of the clinical populations, by taking advantage of the previous experience (practice effect).
Once detected, the visuo-motor sequence had to be repeated until the error-free performance (exercise phase). The exercise phase mainly required working memory, memory load to form and maintain the trace of the correct sequence, long-term memory, and attentional demands to monitor its correct execution. Therefore, the efficient EP performance of PWS but not WS participants indicates a sparing of these abilities. Such a result complements the indication that the visuo-spatial domain is a strength point of PWS individuals [9,48,49]. Actually, the already-described PWS competence in solving spatial tasks, as for example jigsaw puzzles [50], may represent an advantage in performing the exercise phases. Also, the competent cognitive mapping abilities we found in PWS individuals point to this direction. The specular findings obtained by PWS and WS participants in EPs are related to the respective cognitive profiles. Indeed, the visuo-spatial domain is a strength point in PWS and conversely a strong weakness in WS. The WS deficits in spatial working and long-term memory [10,51-55] heavily impaired performances in all EPs. Finally, the PWS performances harmonize with the good capacity of spatial learning and localizatory memory shown by an animal model deficient of Necdin, a candidate gene in PWS etiology [56].
As for the kind of errors, all participants made an analogously low number of illogical errors, indicating that they all similarly managed the task fundamentals. Despite the specific deficit in observational learning of PWS participants, no difference in imitative errors was found among the groups. This result indicates that the imitative PWS deficit was not accompanied by a tendency to hyperimitate. The hyperimitation may be considered a tendency to affiliate or establish, maintain, and enhance relationships with the other. It may be linked to an ingratiating behavior that enhances the conformity with others [57]. Consistently, more empathic individuals and people scoring high in measures of social motivation tend to imitate [58,59]. Interestingly, PWS individuals are often hostile, with social withdrawal, put less emphasis on managing their social image, and exhibit scarce social motivation. Thus, the reduced number of imitative errors performed by PWS individuals is consistent with the social interpretation of their deficits in learning by observation. Given that people learn a lot through social interactions, the role of social motivation in the observational learning and whether a reduced social motivation may lead to impaired learning are interesting issues requiring future studies aimed to address which ways may boost learning.
In OBS1, PWS participants in comparison with TD children made more sequence and side-by-side errors when a change of direction was required. Errors in stopping the easier ‘keep-straight’ response and performing the more demanding ‘turn-left’ response resulted in the PWS participants’ difficulty suppressing a previously correct but then inappropriate response. Not by chance, correctly responding requires executive control processes based on frontal function, as response inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and attentional shifting [60,61], which are already indicated to be impaired in PWS [11,62-65]. Only a few studies have investigated brain abnormalities in PWS individuals; however, it is suggested that their executive dysfunction may be associated with fronto-parietal abnormalities [65-67]. The current findings can be nicely related to those obtained in an animal PWS model with a defect in the imprinting center, in which impaired abilities related to frontal abnormalities have been described in a five-choice serial reaction time task [68,69].
In the automatization phases, while WS participants displayed slowed down automatization times, PWS and TD participants showed similar times that progressively declined as the task was repeated. Specifically, the automatization phase required automatization of sequential visuo-motor productions to increase the efficiency and speed of the response and to achieve the highest levels of performance [70]. Automatizing skills are mainly linked to the functions of subcortical structures, such as the cerebellum and basal ganglia, and to their bidirectional interconnections with cortical structures [71-73]. Therefore, the efficient automatization in PWS indicates the preserved functionality of these brain networks. Similarly, the impaired WS automatization is consistent with brain abnormalities characterized by remarkable hypoplasia of the basal ganglia and the disproportionate enlargement of the cerebellum [74-77].
The performances of PWS individuals improved dramatically in OBS2, indicating the beneficial practice effect on the ability to learn by observation. Notably, the production of actions has a strong impact on action memory, so producing actions helps remember them [78]. Thus, actively produced actions influence the accessibility of memories by enhancing both the content and strength of the memory representation [79]. In this study, others’ actions appear to be linked to self-performed actions, as if agentive experience were functioning as a catalyst for action observation [80,81].
It should be emphasized that PWS individuals have language difficulties [16] that could impair their comprehension of verbal task instructions. However, the efficient performances of PWS individuals in the TE1 task (explained by means of exactly the same verbal instructions) indicated that their poor performances in OBS1 were not caused by a failure to understand the verbal instructions. If that were the case, both first tasks (OBS1 and TE1) would have been compromised.
Finally, a critical point in interpreting our results rests on our choice to adopt a between-group design, which meant that the conclusions were based on the performance of two different groups of PWS individuals. Although this design has some limitations in respect to the within-group design, we retained that a between-group design was adapted to address the differences between learning modalities. In fact, submitting people to various visuo-motor learning tasks inevitably implies a practice effect (learning effect), being difficult to propose them on different occasions, to render them different enough not to expect a change resulting from repeated testing, and to present them fully counterbalanced.
The present results could have important implications for developing interventions aimed at improving learning. In school, teaching is generally based on first showing how a task should be executed and then allowing for actual performance. The present data indicate that a useful way to improve learning in PWS individuals could be to use the ‘trick’ of first allowing them to actually perform a task and then eventually showing them how to refine the task that they have just experienced.

Conclusions

The present study compared two learning mechanisms, learning by observation involving social processing and learning by doing involving direct experience, with all the other variables (for example, motor and cognitive complexity) being equal. A specific PWS deficit in learning by observation was found. Specifically, in comparison to WS and TD groups, PWS individuals were impaired in reproducing the previously observed visuo-motor sequence when the observational task was proposed first (OBS1), while they were as efficient as the TD children in detecting a sequence by trial and error (TE1 and TE2) and in reproducing the previously observed sequence when proposed as a second task (OBS2). We propose that the observational learning deficit in PWS individuals may be rooted, at least partially, in their incapacity to understand and/or use social information. As emphasized by Dimitropoulos et al. [16], there is increasing acknowledgement of social difficulties of PWS individuals above and beyond what is thought to be experienced by a person with a similar level of intellectual impairment. The characterization of their behavioral and cognitive phenotype allows for more targeted interventions aimed at stimulating and improving learning performances.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the individuals with PWS and WS and their parents for making this study possible. We acknowledge the competent and kind help of Professor Fabio Ferlazzo for the statistical indications.
This research was funded by the Fondation Jérôme Lejeune to F. Foti.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

FF, DM, AC, SV, LP, and LM designed the research. FF, EO, CR, and SS performed the research. FF, EO, CR, and LP analyzed the data. FF, DM, LP, and LM wrote the paper. All authors read, revised, and approved the final manuscript.
Anhänge

Additional files

Additional file 1: Table S1. Statistical comparisons of performances of new WS and old WS. To verify the stability of WS participants’ data between the sample employed in the previously published study [30] and the present one, we compared the performances displayed by the 16 WS of the present study (new WS) and those by the 20 WS of the previously published study [30] (old WS) on the four main parameters by means of ANOVAs.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Bittel DC, Kibiryeva N, Butler MG. Expression of 4 genes between chromosome 15 breakpoints 1 and 2 and behavioral outcomes in Prader–Willi syndrome. Pediatrics. 2006;118:E1276–83.CrossRefPubMed Bittel DC, Kibiryeva N, Butler MG. Expression of 4 genes between chromosome 15 breakpoints 1 and 2 and behavioral outcomes in Prader–Willi syndrome. Pediatrics. 2006;118:E1276–83.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Whittington JE, Holland AJ, Webb T, Butler J, Clarke D, Boer H. Population prevalence and estimated birth incidence and mortality rate for people with Prader–Willi syndrome in one UK Health Region. J Med Genet. 2001;38:792–8.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed Whittington JE, Holland AJ, Webb T, Butler J, Clarke D, Boer H. Population prevalence and estimated birth incidence and mortality rate for people with Prader–Willi syndrome in one UK Health Region. J Med Genet. 2001;38:792–8.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Ho AY, Dimitropoulos A. Clinical management of behavioural characteristics of Prader–Willi syndrome. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2010;6:107–18.PubMedCentralPubMed Ho AY, Dimitropoulos A. Clinical management of behavioural characteristics of Prader–Willi syndrome. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2010;6:107–18.PubMedCentralPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Tunnicliffe P, Woodcock K, Bull L, Oliver C, Penhallow J. Temper outbursts in Prader-Willi syndrome: causes, behavioural and emotional sequence and responses by carers. J Intellect Disabil Rese. 2014;58:134–50.CrossRef Tunnicliffe P, Woodcock K, Bull L, Oliver C, Penhallow J. Temper outbursts in Prader-Willi syndrome: causes, behavioural and emotional sequence and responses by carers. J Intellect Disabil Rese. 2014;58:134–50.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Veltman MWM, Craig EE, Bolton PF. Autism spectrum disorders in Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes: a systematic review. Psychiatr Genet. 2005;15:243–54.CrossRefPubMed Veltman MWM, Craig EE, Bolton PF. Autism spectrum disorders in Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes: a systematic review. Psychiatr Genet. 2005;15:243–54.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Whittington J, Holland A, Webb T, Butler J, Clarke D, Boer H. Cognitive abilities and genotype in a population-based sample of people with Prader–Willi syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2004;48:172–87.CrossRefPubMed Whittington J, Holland A, Webb T, Butler J, Clarke D, Boer H. Cognitive abilities and genotype in a population-based sample of people with Prader–Willi syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2004;48:172–87.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Bertella L, Girelli L, Grugni G, Marchi S, Molinari E, Semenza C. Mathematical skills in Prader-Willi syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2005;49:159–69.CrossRefPubMed Bertella L, Girelli L, Grugni G, Marchi S, Molinari E, Semenza C. Mathematical skills in Prader-Willi syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2005;49:159–69.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Copet P, Jauregi J, Laurier V, Ehlinger V, Arnaud C, Cobo AM, et al. Cognitive profile in a large French cohort of adults with Prader-Willi syndrome: differences between genotypes. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2010;54:204–15.CrossRefPubMed Copet P, Jauregi J, Laurier V, Ehlinger V, Arnaud C, Cobo AM, et al. Cognitive profile in a large French cohort of adults with Prader-Willi syndrome: differences between genotypes. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2010;54:204–15.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Dykens E. Are jigsaw puzzle skills ‘spared’ in persons with Prader-Willi syndrome? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002;43:343–52.CrossRefPubMed Dykens E. Are jigsaw puzzle skills ‘spared’ in persons with Prader-Willi syndrome? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002;43:343–52.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Foti F, Menghini D, Petrosini L, Valerio G, Crinò A, Vicari S, et al. Spatial competences in Prader-Willi syndrome: a radial arm maze study. Behav Genet. 2011;41:445–56.CrossRefPubMed Foti F, Menghini D, Petrosini L, Valerio G, Crinò A, Vicari S, et al. Spatial competences in Prader-Willi syndrome: a radial arm maze study. Behav Genet. 2011;41:445–56.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Jauregi J, Arias C, Vegas O, Alen F, Martinez S, Copet P, et al. A neuropsychological assessment of frontal cognitive functions in Prader–Willi syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2007;51:350–65.CrossRefPubMed Jauregi J, Arias C, Vegas O, Alen F, Martinez S, Copet P, et al. A neuropsychological assessment of frontal cognitive functions in Prader–Willi syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2007;51:350–65.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Lo ST, Siemensma E, Collin P, Hokken-Koelega A. Impaired theory of mind and symptoms of autism spectrum disorder in children with Prader-Willi syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34:2764–73.CrossRefPubMed Lo ST, Siemensma E, Collin P, Hokken-Koelega A. Impaired theory of mind and symptoms of autism spectrum disorder in children with Prader-Willi syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34:2764–73.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat State MW, Dykens EM. Genetics of childhood disorders: XV. Prader-Willi syndrome: genes, brain, and behaviour. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39:797–800.CrossRefPubMed State MW, Dykens EM. Genetics of childhood disorders: XV. Prader-Willi syndrome: genes, brain, and behaviour. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39:797–800.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Woodcock KA, Oliver C, Humphreys GW. Task-switching deficits and repetitive behaviour in genetic neurodevelopmental disorders: data from children with Prader-Willi syndrome chromosome 15 q11-q13 deletion and boys with Fragile X syndrome. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2009;26:172–94.CrossRefPubMed Woodcock KA, Oliver C, Humphreys GW. Task-switching deficits and repetitive behaviour in genetic neurodevelopmental disorders: data from children with Prader-Willi syndrome chromosome 15 q11-q13 deletion and boys with Fragile X syndrome. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2009;26:172–94.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Woodcock KA, Humphreys GW, Oliver C. Dorsal and ventral stream mediated visual processing in genetic subtypes of Prader-Willi syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2367–73.CrossRefPubMed Woodcock KA, Humphreys GW, Oliver C. Dorsal and ventral stream mediated visual processing in genetic subtypes of Prader-Willi syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2367–73.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Dimitropoulos A, Ferranti A, Lemler M. Expressive and receptive language in Prader-Willi syndrome: report on genetic subtype differences. J Commun Disord. 2013;46:193–201.CrossRefPubMed Dimitropoulos A, Ferranti A, Lemler M. Expressive and receptive language in Prader-Willi syndrome: report on genetic subtype differences. J Commun Disord. 2013;46:193–201.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Koenig K, Klin A, Schultz R. Deficits in social attribution ability in Prader-Willi syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34:573–82.CrossRefPubMed Koenig K, Klin A, Schultz R. Deficits in social attribution ability in Prader-Willi syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34:573–82.CrossRefPubMed
18.
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Dimitropoulos A, Schultz RT. Autistic-like symptomatology in Prader-Willi syndrome: a review of recent findings. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2007;9:159–64.CrossRefPubMed Dimitropoulos A, Schultz RT. Autistic-like symptomatology in Prader-Willi syndrome: a review of recent findings. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2007;9:159–64.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Clarke DJ, Boer H, Chung MC, Sturmey P, Webb T. Maladaptive behavior in Prader–Willi syndrome in adult life. J Intellect Disabil Res. 1996;40:159–65.CrossRefPubMed Clarke DJ, Boer H, Chung MC, Sturmey P, Webb T. Maladaptive behavior in Prader–Willi syndrome in adult life. J Intellect Disabil Res. 1996;40:159–65.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Lieshout CF, de Meyer RE, Curfs L, Koot HM, Fryns JP. Problem behaviors and personality of children and adolescents with Prader–Willi syndrome. J Pediatr Psychol. 1998;23:111–20.CrossRefPubMed Van Lieshout CF, de Meyer RE, Curfs L, Koot HM, Fryns JP. Problem behaviors and personality of children and adolescents with Prader–Willi syndrome. J Pediatr Psychol. 1998;23:111–20.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Dykens EM, Kasari C. Maladaptive behavior in children with Prader–Willi syndrome, down syndrome, and nonspecific mental retardation. Am J Ment Retard. 1997;102:228–37.CrossRefPubMed Dykens EM, Kasari C. Maladaptive behavior in children with Prader–Willi syndrome, down syndrome, and nonspecific mental retardation. Am J Ment Retard. 1997;102:228–37.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Bandura A. Social Learning Theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall; 1977. Bandura A. Social Learning Theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall; 1977.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Bird G, Heyes C. Effector-dependent learning by observation of a finger movement sequence. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2005;31:262–75.CrossRefPubMed Bird G, Heyes C. Effector-dependent learning by observation of a finger movement sequence. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2005;31:262–75.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Kokkinakki T, Kugiumutzakis G. Basic aspects of vocal imitation in infant–parent interaction during the first 6 months. J Reprod Infant Psychol. 2000;18:173–87.CrossRef Kokkinakki T, Kugiumutzakis G. Basic aspects of vocal imitation in infant–parent interaction during the first 6 months. J Reprod Infant Psychol. 2000;18:173–87.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Meltzoff AN, Decety J. What imitation tells us about social cognition: a rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2003;358:491–500.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed Meltzoff AN, Decety J. What imitation tells us about social cognition: a rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2003;358:491–500.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Foti F, Mazzone L, Menghini D, De Peppo L, Federico F, Postorino V, et al. Learning by observation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Psychol Med. 2014;17:1–11.CrossRef Foti F, Mazzone L, Menghini D, De Peppo L, Federico F, Postorino V, et al. Learning by observation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Psychol Med. 2014;17:1–11.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Foti F, Menghini D, Mandolesi L, Federico F, Vicari S, Petrosini L. Learning by observation: insights from Williams syndrome. PLoS One. 2013;8:e53782.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed Foti F, Menghini D, Mandolesi L, Federico F, Vicari S, Petrosini L. Learning by observation: insights from Williams syndrome. PLoS One. 2013;8:e53782.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Menghini D, Vicari S, Mandolesi L, Petrosini L. Is learning by observation impaired in children with dyslexia? Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:1996–2003.CrossRefPubMed Menghini D, Vicari S, Mandolesi L, Petrosini L. Is learning by observation impaired in children with dyslexia? Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:1996–2003.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Jarrold C, Brock J. To match or not to match? Methodological issues in autism-related research. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34:81–6.CrossRefPubMed Jarrold C, Brock J. To match or not to match? Methodological issues in autism-related research. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34:81–6.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Roid GH, Miller LJ. Leiter - R, Leiter International Performance Scale–Revised. Firenze: Giunti O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali; 2002. Roid GH, Miller LJ. Leiter - R, Leiter International Performance Scale–Revised. Firenze: Giunti O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali; 2002.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Beery KE, Buktenica NA. The Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test Of Visual-Motor Integration With Additional Tests of Visual Perception And Motor Coordination [Italian translation by C. Preda]. Florence: O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali; 2000. Beery KE, Buktenica NA. The Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test Of Visual-Motor Integration With Additional Tests of Visual Perception And Motor Coordination [Italian translation by C. Preda]. Florence: O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali; 2000.
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Vicari S. In: Giunti O.S., editor. PROMEA: Prove di memoria e apprendimento. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali; 2007. Vicari S. In: Giunti O.S., editor. PROMEA: Prove di memoria e apprendimento. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali; 2007.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Kenward B, Karlsson M, Persson J. Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning. Proc Biol Sci. 2011;278:1239–46.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed Kenward B, Karlsson M, Persson J. Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning. Proc Biol Sci. 2011;278:1239–46.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Over H, Carpenter M. Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behaviour. J Comp Psychol. 2012;126:182–92.CrossRefPubMed Over H, Carpenter M. Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behaviour. J Comp Psychol. 2012;126:182–92.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Palermo L, Foti F, Ferlazzo F, Guariglia C, Petrosini L. I find my way in a maze but not in my own territory! Navigational processing in developmental topographical disorientation. Neuropsychology. 2014;28:135–46.CrossRefPubMed Palermo L, Foti F, Ferlazzo F, Guariglia C, Petrosini L. I find my way in a maze but not in my own territory! Navigational processing in developmental topographical disorientation. Neuropsychology. 2014;28:135–46.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Storey JD, Tibshirani RJ. Statistical significance for genomewide studies. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;2003(100):9440–5.CrossRef Storey JD, Tibshirani RJ. Statistical significance for genomewide studies. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;2003(100):9440–5.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Torriero S, Oliveri M, Koch G, Caltagirone C, Petrosini L. The what and how of observational learning. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:1656–63.CrossRefPubMed Torriero S, Oliveri M, Koch G, Caltagirone C, Petrosini L. The what and how of observational learning. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:1656–63.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Dykens EM, Cassidy SB. Correlates of maladaptive behavior in children and adults with Prader-Willi syndrome. Am J Med Genet. 1995;60:546–9.CrossRefPubMed Dykens EM, Cassidy SB. Correlates of maladaptive behavior in children and adults with Prader-Willi syndrome. Am J Med Genet. 1995;60:546–9.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Greenswag LR. Adults with Prader-Willi syndrome: a survey of 232 cases. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1987;29:145–52.CrossRefPubMed Greenswag LR. Adults with Prader-Willi syndrome: a survey of 232 cases. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1987;29:145–52.CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Stein DJ, Keating J, Zar HJ, Hollander E. A survey of the phenomenology and pharmacotherapy of compulsive and impulsive-aggressive symptoms in Prader-Willi syndrome. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;6:23–9.CrossRefPubMed Stein DJ, Keating J, Zar HJ, Hollander E. A survey of the phenomenology and pharmacotherapy of compulsive and impulsive-aggressive symptoms in Prader-Willi syndrome. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;6:23–9.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Tager-Flusberg H, Sullivan K. A componential view of theory of mind: evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition. 2000;76:59–90.CrossRefPubMed Tager-Flusberg H, Sullivan K. A componential view of theory of mind: evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition. 2000;76:59–90.CrossRefPubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Mantoulan C, Payoux P, Diene G, Glattard M, Rogé B, Molinas C, et al. PET scan perfusion imaging in the Prader-Willi syndrome: new insights into the psychiatric and social disturbances. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2011;31:275–82.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed Mantoulan C, Payoux P, Diene G, Glattard M, Rogé B, Molinas C, et al. PET scan perfusion imaging in the Prader-Willi syndrome: new insights into the psychiatric and social disturbances. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2011;31:275–82.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Sparaci L, Stefanini S, Marotta L, Vicari S, Rizzolatti G. Understanding motor acts and motor intentions in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:1639–49.CrossRefPubMed Sparaci L, Stefanini S, Marotta L, Vicari S, Rizzolatti G. Understanding motor acts and motor intentions in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:1639–49.CrossRefPubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Curfs LM, Wiegers AM, Sommers JR, Borghgraef M, Fryns JP. Strengths and weaknesses in the cognitive profile of youngsters with Prader-Willi syndrome. Clin Genet. 1991;40:430–4.CrossRefPubMed Curfs LM, Wiegers AM, Sommers JR, Borghgraef M, Fryns JP. Strengths and weaknesses in the cognitive profile of youngsters with Prader-Willi syndrome. Clin Genet. 1991;40:430–4.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Dykens E, Hodapp R, Walsh K, Nash L. Profiles, correlates, and trajectories of intelligence in Prader-Willi syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1992;31:1125–30.CrossRefPubMed Dykens E, Hodapp R, Walsh K, Nash L. Profiles, correlates, and trajectories of intelligence in Prader-Willi syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1992;31:1125–30.CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Verdine BN, Troseth GL, Hodapp RM, Dykens EM. Strategies and correlates of jigsaw puzzle and visuospatial performance by persons with Prader-Willi syndrome. Am J of Ment Retard. 2008;113:343–55.CrossRef Verdine BN, Troseth GL, Hodapp RM, Dykens EM. Strategies and correlates of jigsaw puzzle and visuospatial performance by persons with Prader-Willi syndrome. Am J of Ment Retard. 2008;113:343–55.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Atkinson J, Braddick O, Anker S, Curran W, Andrew R, Wattam-Bell J, et al. Neurobiological models of visuospatial cognition in children with Williams syndrome: measures of dorsal-stream and frontal function. Dev Neuropsychol. 2003;23:139–72.CrossRefPubMed Atkinson J, Braddick O, Anker S, Curran W, Andrew R, Wattam-Bell J, et al. Neurobiological models of visuospatial cognition in children with Williams syndrome: measures of dorsal-stream and frontal function. Dev Neuropsychol. 2003;23:139–72.CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Foti F, Petrosini L, Cutuli D, Menghini D, Chiarotti F, Vicari S, et al. Explorative function in Williams syndrome analyzed through a large-scale task with multiple rewards. Res Dev Disabil. 2011;32:972–85.CrossRefPubMed Foti F, Petrosini L, Cutuli D, Menghini D, Chiarotti F, Vicari S, et al. Explorative function in Williams syndrome analyzed through a large-scale task with multiple rewards. Res Dev Disabil. 2011;32:972–85.CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Mandolesi L, Addona F, Foti F, Menghini D, Petrosini L, Vicari S. Spatial competences in Williams syndrome: a radial arm maze study. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2009;27:205–13.CrossRefPubMed Mandolesi L, Addona F, Foti F, Menghini D, Petrosini L, Vicari S. Spatial competences in Williams syndrome: a radial arm maze study. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2009;27:205–13.CrossRefPubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA. Visual and spatial long-term memory: differential pattern of impairments in Williams and Down syndromes. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2005;47:305–11.CrossRefPubMed Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA. Visual and spatial long-term memory: differential pattern of impairments in Williams and Down syndromes. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2005;47:305–11.CrossRefPubMed
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA. Evidence from two genetic syndromes for the independence of spatial and visual working memory. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48:126–31.CrossRefPubMed Vicari S, Bellucci S, Carlesimo GA. Evidence from two genetic syndromes for the independence of spatial and visual working memory. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48:126–31.CrossRefPubMed
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Muscatelli F, Abrous DN, Massacrier A, Boccaccio I, Le Moal M, Cau P, et al. Disruption of the mouse Necdin gene results in hypothalamic and behavioural alterations reminiscent of the human Prader-Willi syndrome. Hum Mol Genet. 2000;9:3101–10.CrossRefPubMed Muscatelli F, Abrous DN, Massacrier A, Boccaccio I, Le Moal M, Cau P, et al. Disruption of the mouse Necdin gene results in hypothalamic and behavioural alterations reminiscent of the human Prader-Willi syndrome. Hum Mol Genet. 2000;9:3101–10.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Lakin JL, Jefferis VE, Cheng CM, Chartrand TL. The chameleon effect as social glue: evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. J Nonverbal Behav. 2003;27:145–62.CrossRef Lakin JL, Jefferis VE, Cheng CM, Chartrand TL. The chameleon effect as social glue: evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. J Nonverbal Behav. 2003;27:145–62.CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Chrtrand TL, Bargh JA. The chameleon effect: the perception–behavior link and social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1999;76:893–910.CrossRef Chrtrand TL, Bargh JA. The chameleon effect: the perception–behavior link and social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1999;76:893–910.CrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Lain JL, Chartrand TL. Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychol Sci. 2003;14:334–9.CrossRef Lain JL, Chartrand TL. Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychol Sci. 2003;14:334–9.CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Chambers CD, Bellgrove MA, Gould IC, English T, Garavan H, McNaught E, et al. Dissociable mechanisms of cognitive control in prefrontal and premotor cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98:3638–47.CrossRefPubMed Chambers CD, Bellgrove MA, Gould IC, English T, Garavan H, McNaught E, et al. Dissociable mechanisms of cognitive control in prefrontal and premotor cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98:3638–47.CrossRefPubMed
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Swick D, Ashley V, Turken U. Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks. Neuroimage. 2011;56:1655–65.CrossRefPubMed Swick D, Ashley V, Turken U. Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks. Neuroimage. 2011;56:1655–65.CrossRefPubMed
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Ogura K, Shinohara M, Ohno K, Mori E. Frontal behavioural syndromes in Prader-Willi syndrome. Brain Dev. 2008;30:469–76.CrossRefPubMed Ogura K, Shinohara M, Ohno K, Mori E. Frontal behavioural syndromes in Prader-Willi syndrome. Brain Dev. 2008;30:469–76.CrossRefPubMed
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Stauder JE, Boer H, Gerits RH, Tummers A, Whittington J, Curfs LM. Differences in behavioural phenotype between parental deletion and maternal uniparental disomy in Prader-Willi syndrome: an ERP study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2005;116:1464–70.CrossRefPubMed Stauder JE, Boer H, Gerits RH, Tummers A, Whittington J, Curfs LM. Differences in behavioural phenotype between parental deletion and maternal uniparental disomy in Prader-Willi syndrome: an ERP study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2005;116:1464–70.CrossRefPubMed
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Walley R, Donaldson M. An investigation of executive function abilities in adults with Prader-Willi syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2005;49:613–25.CrossRefPubMed Walley R, Donaldson M. An investigation of executive function abilities in adults with Prader-Willi syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2005;49:613–25.CrossRefPubMed
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Woodcock KA, Humphreys GW, Oliver C, Hansen PC. Neural correlates of task switching in paternal 15q11-q13 deletion Prader-Willi syndrome. Brain Res. 2010;1363:128–42.CrossRefPubMed Woodcock KA, Humphreys GW, Oliver C, Hansen PC. Neural correlates of task switching in paternal 15q11-q13 deletion Prader-Willi syndrome. Brain Res. 2010;1363:128–42.CrossRefPubMed
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Ogura K, Fujii T, Abe N, Hosokai Y, Shinohara M, Takahashi S, et al. Small gray matter volume in orbitofrontal cortex in Prader-Willi syndrome: a voxel-based MRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32:1059–66.CrossRefPubMed Ogura K, Fujii T, Abe N, Hosokai Y, Shinohara M, Takahashi S, et al. Small gray matter volume in orbitofrontal cortex in Prader-Willi syndrome: a voxel-based MRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32:1059–66.CrossRefPubMed
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Dimitropoulos A, Blackford J, Walden T, Thompson T. Compulsive behavior in Prader-Willi syndrome: examining severity in early childhood. Res Dev Disabil. 2006;27:190–202.CrossRefPubMed Dimitropoulos A, Blackford J, Walden T, Thompson T. Compulsive behavior in Prader-Willi syndrome: examining severity in early childhood. Res Dev Disabil. 2006;27:190–202.CrossRefPubMed
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Relkovic D, Doe CM, Humby T, Johnstone KA, Resnick JL, Holland AJ, et al. Behavioural and cognitive abnormalities in an imprinting centre deletion mouse model for Prader-Willi syndrome. Eur J Neurosci. 2010;31:156–64.CrossRefPubMed Relkovic D, Doe CM, Humby T, Johnstone KA, Resnick JL, Holland AJ, et al. Behavioural and cognitive abnormalities in an imprinting centre deletion mouse model for Prader-Willi syndrome. Eur J Neurosci. 2010;31:156–64.CrossRefPubMed
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Relkovic D, Isles AR. Behavioural and cognitive profiles of mouse models for Prader-Willi syndrome. Brain Res Bull. 2013;92:41–8.CrossRefPubMed Relkovic D, Isles AR. Behavioural and cognitive profiles of mouse models for Prader-Willi syndrome. Brain Res Bull. 2013;92:41–8.CrossRefPubMed
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Hubert V, Beaunieux H, Chételat G, Platel H, Landeau B, Danion JM, et al. The dynamic network subserving the three phases of cognitive procedural learning. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28:1415–29.CrossRefPubMed Hubert V, Beaunieux H, Chételat G, Platel H, Landeau B, Danion JM, et al. The dynamic network subserving the three phases of cognitive procedural learning. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28:1415–29.CrossRefPubMed
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Doeller CF, King JA, Burgess N. Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:5915–20.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed Doeller CF, King JA, Burgess N. Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:5915–20.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Seidler RD, Purushotham A, Kim SG, Ugurbil K, Willingham D, Ashe J. Neural correlates of encoding and expression in implicit sequence learning. Exp Brain Res. 2005;165:114–24.CrossRefPubMed Seidler RD, Purushotham A, Kim SG, Ugurbil K, Willingham D, Ashe J. Neural correlates of encoding and expression in implicit sequence learning. Exp Brain Res. 2005;165:114–24.CrossRefPubMed
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Torriero S, Oliveri M, Koch G, Lo Gerfo E, Salerno S, Ferlazzo F, et al. Changes in cerebello-motor connectivity during procedural learning by actual execution and observation. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23:338–48.CrossRefPubMed Torriero S, Oliveri M, Koch G, Lo Gerfo E, Salerno S, Ferlazzo F, et al. Changes in cerebello-motor connectivity during procedural learning by actual execution and observation. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23:338–48.CrossRefPubMed
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Jones W, Hesselink J, Courchesne E, Duncan T, Matsuda K, Bellugi U. Cerebellar abnormalities in infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2002;44:688–94.CrossRefPubMed Jones W, Hesselink J, Courchesne E, Duncan T, Matsuda K, Bellugi U. Cerebellar abnormalities in infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2002;44:688–94.CrossRefPubMed
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Chiang MC, Reiss AL, Lee AD, Bellugi U, Galaburda AM, Korenberg JR, et al. 3D pattern of brain abnormalities in Williams syndrome visualized using tensor-based morphometry. Neuroimage. 2007;36:1096–109.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed Chiang MC, Reiss AL, Lee AD, Bellugi U, Galaburda AM, Korenberg JR, et al. 3D pattern of brain abnormalities in Williams syndrome visualized using tensor-based morphometry. Neuroimage. 2007;36:1096–109.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Menghini D, Di Paola M, Federico F, Vicari S, Petrosini L, Caltagirone C, et al. Relationship between brain abnormalities and cognitive profile in Williams syndrome. Behav Genet. 2011;41:394–402.CrossRefPubMed Menghini D, Di Paola M, Federico F, Vicari S, Petrosini L, Caltagirone C, et al. Relationship between brain abnormalities and cognitive profile in Williams syndrome. Behav Genet. 2011;41:394–402.CrossRefPubMed
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Menghini D, Di Paola M, Murri R, Costanzo F, Caltagirone C, Vicari S, et al. Cerebellar vermis abnormalities and cognitive functions in individuals with Williams syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34:2118–26.CrossRefPubMed Menghini D, Di Paola M, Murri R, Costanzo F, Caltagirone C, Vicari S, et al. Cerebellar vermis abnormalities and cognitive functions in individuals with Williams syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34:2118–26.CrossRefPubMed
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Schütz-Bosbach S, Prinz W. Perceptual resonance: action-induced modulation of perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:349–55.CrossRefPubMed Schütz-Bosbach S, Prinz W. Perceptual resonance: action-induced modulation of perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:349–55.CrossRefPubMed
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Hayne H, Barr R, Herbert J. The effect of prior practice on memory reactivation and generalization. Child Dev. 2003;74:1615–27.CrossRefPubMed Hayne H, Barr R, Herbert J. The effect of prior practice on memory reactivation and generalization. Child Dev. 2003;74:1615–27.CrossRefPubMed
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Calvo-Merino B, Grèzes J, Glaser DE, Passingham RE, Haggard P. Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Curr Biol. 2006;16:1905–10.CrossRefPubMed Calvo-Merino B, Grèzes J, Glaser DE, Passingham RE, Haggard P. Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Curr Biol. 2006;16:1905–10.CrossRefPubMed
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Hauf P. Infants’ perception and production of intentional actions. Prog Brain Res. 2007;164:285–301.CrossRefPubMed Hauf P. Infants’ perception and production of intentional actions. Prog Brain Res. 2007;164:285–301.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Learning by observation and learning by doing in Prader-Willi syndrome
verfasst von
Francesca Foti
Deny Menghini
Enzo Orlandi
Cristina Rufini
Antonino Crinò
Sabrina Spera
Stefano Vicari
Laura Petrosini
Laura Mandolesi
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2015
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders / Ausgabe 1/2015
Print ISSN: 1866-1947
Elektronische ISSN: 1866-1955
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-015-9102-0

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2015

Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 1/2015 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).