Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Current Addiction Reports 2/2017

Open Access 01.06.2017 | Food Addiction (A Meule, Section Editor)

Food Addiction Beliefs Amongst the Lay Public: What Are the Consequences for Eating Behaviour?

verfasst von: Helen K. Ruddock, Charlotte A. Hardman

Erschienen in: Current Addiction Reports | Ausgabe 2/2017

Abstract

Purpose of Review

The current paper reviews recent research on perceptions of food addiction in the lay public. It also examines the potential consequences of such beliefs for eating behaviour.

Recent Findings

Surveys suggest that, within community samples, the majority of individuals believe that certain foods are addictive, and that food addiction causes obesity. Further, many people believe themselves to be ‘food addicts’, and these individuals demonstrate increased patterns of aberrant eating. However, there is also initial experimental evidence to suggest that believing oneself to be a food addict leads to short-term food restriction.

Summary

To reconcile these findings, a self-perpetuating relationship between food addiction beliefs and aberrant eating is proposed. Specifically, in the short term, food addiction beliefs may encourage individuals to avoid certain foods. However, attempts at restriction may eventually lead to increased cravings and disinhibition, thus reinforcing perceptions of oneself as a food addict. These possibilities merit scrutiny in future research.
Hinweise
This article is part of the Topical Collection on Food Addiction

Introduction

Scientific interest in the potential ‘addictive’ properties of certain foods dates back to the late nineteenth century [1•]. In 1956, Theron Randolph introduced the term ‘food addiction’ suggesting that certain foods produce a “common pattern of symptoms descriptively similar to those of other addictive processes” ([2], p. 221). Since then, much research has focused upon elucidating the biological and behavioural similarities between the effects of drugs and food (e.g. [3, 4]). In line with this, the Yale Food Addiction Scale (YFAS; 5, 6) provides a method of quantifying and ‘diagnosing’ food addiction using the clinical criterion on the DSM IV/5 for substance dependence.
However, within the scientific community, there exists substantial debate surrounding the validity of the food addiction concept. For example, Ziauddeen et al. [7] discuss the limited applicability of the DSM IV/5 substance-dependence criteria to the assessment of eating behaviour, and others point out important differences between the effects of drugs and food [8]. Nonetheless, while scientific opinion of the food addiction concept has been extensively reviewed, there has been relatively little consideration of the way in which food addiction is conceptualised and understood by members of the lay public. This is important because people’s beliefs about food addiction and weight gain may have far-reaching consequences on dietary behaviours, weight-related stigma, and support for obesity treatments [9•, 10]. Therefore, the focus of the current review was to provide an overview of recent research which has explored the lay public’s perceptions of food addiction and to consider whether these beliefs may have helpful or counterproductive effects on eating behaviour. Finally, we propose a self-perpetuating relationship between food addiction beliefs and longer-term patterns of dietary behaviour and provide suggestions for future research.

The Popularity of the Food Addiction Concept

The concept of food addiction is widely endorsed amongst members of the lay public. This is reflected by the plethora of books, magazine articles, and self-help groups all dedicated to ‘curing’ people of their addiction to food. In one survey, as many as 86% of an Australian and American sample believed that certain foods are addictive, and over two thirds reported being aware of scientific evidence for comparable neurobiological effects of food and drugs [11]. Similarly, a survey of over 5000 members of a North American health organisation found that 66% believed that certain foods are addictive, and these beliefs were most prevalent amongst those with increased weight or binge eating tendencies [12]. In particular, foods that are high in fat, salt, and sugar are believed to yield the greatest addictive potential. Indeed, Lee et al. [11] found that 75% of respondents perceived sugar to be as addictive as cocaine, and foods such as chocolate, pizza, and ice cream are amongst the most commonly reported ‘problematic’ foods [13•, 14]. Furthermore, an addiction to these foods is widely believed to underlie many cases of obesity [11], and in a survey of 1000 Americans, food addiction was one of the most frequently endorsed explanations for the increasing rates of obesity in Western society [15].

Self-Perceived Food Addiction

As well as more general support for the food addiction concept, there is evidence to suggest that many people believe themselves to be addicted to food. Indeed, self-perceived food addiction was reported in over half of participants from a university student sample [16] and in 27 to 42% of participants from community samples in the UK [17, 18••]. Moreover, in a survey conducted with users of a self-help website for overweight adolescents, two thirds believed themselves to be addicted to at least one food [19].
Notably, the prevalence of self-perceived food addiction amongst the general population is somewhat higher than the number of people who fulfil the YFAS diagnostic criterion for food addiction (typically 5–16%) [6, 17, 20]. In particular, in a recent study, we found that only 12% of self-perceived food addicts met this criterion [21•]. This may reflect fundamental differences between the lay public’s conceptualisation of food addiction and that proposed by clinical models of substance-dependence upon which the YFAS is based. To address this possibility, it is necessary to consider the way in which food addiction is commonly conceptualised within the lay public.

How Is Food Addiction Defined by the Lay Public?

Amongst members of the lay public, food addiction appears to be associated with a number of core behavioural features. For example, in a sample of low-income women, Malika et al. [22] found that food addiction was characterised by a tendency to always have food available and to go out of one’s way to obtain food (e.g. shopping at odd hours in the night). Food addiction was considered distinct and more severe than food cravings; all addictive foods could be craved, but not all craved foods were thought to be addictive. Furthermore, the presence (or absence) of certain behaviours appears to be used to draw conclusions about one’s own ‘food addiction’ status. In a previous study, Hetherington and Macdiarmid [23] asked a group of self-perceived ‘chocolate addicts’ to indicate why they perceived themselves to be addicted. The majority of participants referred to an inability to resist chocolate. More specifically, they felt unable to moderate their chocolate consumption and also felt unable to stop eating chocolate once they had started.
We have recently extended these findings by providing insight into the eating-related cognitions and behaviours which are associated with a self-perceived addiction to food in general (i.e. not just chocolate) [18••]. In this study, participants from a community sample completed a brief online questionnaire in which they were asked whether or not they perceived themselves to be ‘food addicts’ and to provide the reasons behind their response. Participants’ reasons were analysed thematically, and we identified six core behaviours that were commonly attributed to an addiction to food. These included (1) reward-driven eating (i.e. eating for the rewarding aspects of food or to alleviate negative emotional states, as opposed to physiological hunger), (2) a functional or psychological preoccupation with food (i.e. a lot of time spent thinking about food, shopping, cooking, planning meals, etc.), (3) a perceived lack of self-control around food (i.e. an inability to resist tempting foods or stick to a strict diet plan), (4) frequent food cravings, (5) increased weight or an unhealthy diet, and (6) a problem with a specific type of food, particularly foods that were high in fat, salt, and sugar. Importantly, the themes identified did not differ between self-perceived food addicts and nonaddicts. Instead, self-perceived food addicts and nonaddicts reported opposite behaviours. For example, while self-perceived food addicts said that they “think about food all of the time”, nonaddicts reported “little interest in food”. Furthermore, the themes generated did not differ as a function of weight status or age, although females were more likely than males to attribute food addiction to a tendency to engage in reward-driven eating. Taken together, these findings suggest that food addiction is perceived to be identifiable through a core set of behaviours, and that these causal attributions are mostly consistent throughout the lay community.
These perceptions of food addiction share some notable similarities with the clinical criterion for substance dependence [5, 6]. For example, the experience of food cravings reflects a ‘persistent desire’ (in the DSM IV) and ‘craving or strong desire/urge to use a substance’ (in the DSM 5). Similarly, the tendency to consume an unhealthy diet is analogous to the DSM criterion in which ‘use continues despite knowledge of adverse consequences’. However, in contrast to the clinical criterion for substance dependence, few participants in our study referred to symptoms of tolerance (i.e. requiring increasing amounts of food over time) and withdrawal, and no participants made causal attributions to giving up ‘important social, occupational, or recreational activities’ as a result of their eating [18••]. Furthermore, participants did not associate food addiction with ‘significant distress’ or an ‘impairment to daily functioning’. These differences may underlie the aforementioned discrepancy between the widespread prevalence of self-perceived food addiction and the relatively few number of people that fulfil the YFAS criterion [16, 17, 21•].

Eating Behaviour in Self-Perceived Food Addicts

While the majority of self-perceived food addicts do not fulfil an existing clinical criterion for food addiction (i.e. the YFAS), there is emerging evidence to suggest that these individuals may have increased aberrant patterns of eating. For example, one study revealed that self-perceived food addicts scored higher on various measures of pathological eating compared to self-perceived nonaddicts [16], and we found an increased prevalence of self-perceived food addiction in those with increased BMI [18••]. Furthermore, several experimental studies have revealed increased desire for chocolate and greater ad libitum chocolate consumption in self-perceived chocolate addicts relative to chocolate nonaddicts [2426].
Building upon these findings, we recently explored food reward and intake in self-perceived food addicts and nonaddicts [21•]. Food reward was assessed using an operant response task and by asking participants how much they would be ‘willing to pay’ for and the strength of their current ‘desire to eat’, a portion of food. These measures were completed for two high-fat foods and two low-fat foods when participants were hungry and again when they were satiated. Participants were then given ad libitum access to a high-fat and a low-fat food. Despite demonstrating no elevated levels of hunger or ‘liking’ for the test foods, self-perceived food addicts showed increased food reward (as indicated by ‘desire to eat’ ratings) for both the high- and low- fat foods, when they were hungry and satiated, compared to self-perceived nonaddicts. Further, self-perceived food addicts consumed more calories during the taste task. In particular, and consistent with beliefs about the addictiveness of certain foods (e.g. 14), self-perceived food addicts consumed more calories from the high-fat food, but not the low-fat food. These findings suggest that self-perceived food addicts have problematic patterns of eating that may go undetected by an existing measure of addictive eating (i.e. the YFAS). Notably, increased calorie intake in self-perceived food addicts was explained by increased dietary disinhibition and diminished dietary restraint (as indicated by a variety of eating-trait questionnaires). This is consistent with dual-process theories of overeating and addiction which suggest that the overconsumption of food and drugs is driven by increased appetitive motivation and a diminished ability for self-control [27, 28].

Food Addiction as a Self-Serving Attribution

Psychosocial theories consider the role of psychological and societal influences in the formation of food addiction attitudes and beliefs. From this perspective, it has been suggested that, rather than reflecting addictive patterns of eating per se, self-perceived food addiction may reflect attempts to minimise perceptions of blame and guilt associated with overeating. Specifically, Rogers and Smit [29] propose that labelling oneself a ‘food addict’ may arise following failed attempts to restrict one’s intake of highly palatable but unhealthy (‘naughty but nice’) foods. By insinuating that such lapses in self-control are the result of a physiological inevitability, Rogers and Smit [29] suggest that the concept of food addiction may help to alleviate feelings of guilt and personal responsibility. This theory is consistent with the core principles of ‘attribution theory’ which posits the tendency for individuals to make attributions for negative outcomes which minimise the role of internal and/or controllable factors (e.g. personal choice) and maximise the role of external and/or uncontrollable factors (e.g. environmental or biological factors). Based on this theory, Davies [30] proposes that the concept of ‘addiction’ minimises perceptions of blame by portraying the drug user or overeater as a ‘helpless victim of disease’. Indeed, there is evidence to suggest that addiction-based explanations may be effective in reducing obesity-related blame [31], although this has not been consistently demonstrated [32].
In a recent study [33], we tested this theory by examining whether those who were led to feel guilty following eating would be particularly likely to identify as food addicts and to attribute their eating to the foods’ addictive properties. Feelings of guilt following eating were manipulated by leading participants to believe that they had consumed more than (high-guilt condition), less than (low-guilt condition), or roughly the same (control condition) amount of high-calorie foods than previous bogus participants and relative to their own estimated intake. Participants were then asked to indicate the extent to which they perceived themselves to be ‘food addicts’ and to rank ten ‘reasons for eating’ from most to least influential. In particular, we were interested in the rank assigned to a ‘foods were addictive’ attribution. It was predicted that participants in the high-guilt condition would be more likely to label themselves ‘food addicts’ and would assign a lower rank (indicative of being more influential) to the ‘foods were addictive’ attribution, compared to those in low-guilt and control conditions. Contrary to expectation, there was no effect of condition (i.e. high-guilt vs. low-guilt vs. control) on food addiction attributions. However, across the whole sample of participants, those with higher levels of eating-related guilt assigned a lower rank (indicative of being more influential) to the ‘foods were addictive’ reason for eating. Importantly, the rank assigned to this attribution was unrelated to actual or perceived calorie intake. Thus, while these findings do not fully support the concept of food addiction as an ‘attribution’, they do suggest that beliefs about the addictive potential of foods may be more closely related to feelings of guilt than to actual calorie intake. More research is needed to directly test this hypothesis.

The Consequences of Food Addiction Beliefs

Psychosocial perspectives on food addiction also consider the consequences of food addiction beliefs on eating behaviour [9•]. This is of potential concern as biological explanations for obesity may lead people to believe that their weight is uncontrollable [34] and consequently undermine their efforts to lose weight. Indeed, believing that one is in control of one’s behaviour (i.e. self-control beliefs) has been found to be important in the initiation of health behaviours [35], and those with increased self-control beliefs are less likely to snack on sugary foods [36] and are more likely to act upon healthy eating intentions [37] and lose weight [38].
Research has also demonstrated a deleterious effect of biological explanations for obesity on dietary behaviour. Across a series of studies, Hoyt et al. [39] found that participants who read an article which explained that ‘obesity is a disease’, made higher calorie food choices compared to those who read a control article or one which stated that ‘obesity is not a disease’. In particular, this effect was observed in overweight and obese participants, but not in healthy weight participants, and was mediated by the effect of the disease message on decreased levels of body dissatisfaction. A similar finding was observed by Dar-Nimrod et al. [40] who found that participants who read a genetic explanation for obesity perceived weight to be less controllable and consumed more calories in a subsequent taste task, compared to those who read a psychosocial explanation for obesity or a control article.
In contrast, however, a recent study uncovered no effect of a ‘food addiction is real’ message, relative to a ‘food addiction is a myth’ message, on subsequent calorie intake [17]. This may be due to that fact that, unlike disease- or genetic-based explanations for obesity, food addiction beliefs do not necessarily imply a lack of control over weight. Indeed, Lee et al. [41] found that, despite strong endorsement of the food addiction concept (by 86% of the sample), 55% of those surveyed believed that overeating and weight gain are within personal control. Furthermore, following a survey of 570 American adults, DePierre et al. [42••] found that food addiction was perceived to be more of a behavioural choice compared to other addictions, such as alcoholism.
Taken together, these findings suggest that food addiction messages may not elicit the same negative effects on self-control beliefs and eating behaviour as more general biological explanations of obesity. On the contrary, there is initial evidence to suggest that perceiving oneself to be a food addict may actually be helpful for those attempting to reduce their overeating tendencies. For example, members of the self-help group, Overeaters Anonymous, are encouraged to view their overeating as an ‘addiction’ and to avoid exposing themselves to their ‘problem’ foods. Qualitative reports suggest that this perspective helps to alleviate members’ feelings of eating-related guilt and shame and promotes a sense of personal responsibility for members’ own recovery [43, 44].
Our recent empirical study also suggests that believing oneself to be a food addict may have initial helpful effects on eating behaviour [45••]. Over two studies, participants were led to believe that they had scored either high, low, or average on an ostensible measure of food addiction. Following this, a measure of dietary concern was taken, and participants were given ad libitum access to high-fat foods during a taste task. The amount of time participants spent tasting and rating the foods was covertly recorded. Results revealed that participants who were led to believe they had scored high on food addiction consumed fewer calories during the taste task compared to those who were in low and average conditions. Further analyses revealed that this was due to increased levels of dietary concern in the high-food addiction condition following the food addiction feedback and a subsequent reduction in the amount of time these participants spent tasting the foods. Although conducted in a laboratory setting, these findings have potential real-world implications. For example, it is possible that perceiving oneself to be a food addict may encourage individuals to avoid exposing themselves to tempting situations, such as the supermarket confectionary aisle or the buffet table at a party.
However, before drawing definitive conclusions regarding the consequences of food addiction beliefs, it is necessary to consider their longer-term impact. More specifically, while food addiction beliefs may encourage individuals to restrict their food intake in the short term, previous research suggests that attempts to restrict food intake over longer time periods can exacerbate cravings and promote disinhibited eating [46, 47]. Indeed, our previous findings suggest that self-perceived food addiction is associated with increased, rather than decreased, levels of dietary disinhibition and BMI [18••, 21•].
In order to reconcile these disparate findings, we propose a self-perpetuating relationship between food addiction beliefs and food intake (Fig. 1). Specifically, it is proposed that, over the short term, perceiving oneself to be a food addict elicits attempts to avoid particular ‘problem’ foods. In turn, and consistent with previous evidence [46, 47], it is thought that attempts at dietary restriction will strengthen cravings and eventually lead to the over consumption of the forbidden foods. Finally, these increased cravings and failed attempts at dietary restriction may reinforce perceptions of oneself as a ‘food addict’, thus resulting in further attempts at dietary restriction. Future research is required to explore this potential self-perpetuating relationship between food addiction beliefs and high-calorie food consumption. In particular, it would be informative to extend our previous findings [45••] by examining the longer-term effects of manipulating food addiction beliefs on intentions to restrict food intake and the effects of this on food cravings and subsequent consumption. Furthermore, research should examine the possibility that labelling oneself a food addict may help to alleviate feelings of guilt following regular and repeated episodes of overeating.

Conclusion

The concept of food addiction is widely endorsed throughout the lay public, and many people attribute their food cravings and overeating tendencies to an ‘addiction’ to foods that are high in fat, salt, and sugar. While the majority of these individuals do not meet an existing substance-based definition of food addiction, they nonetheless demonstrate increased food reward and calorie intake within the lab. Further, these behaviours appear to be driven by increased appetitive motivation and diminished self-control around food, consistent with dual-process models of overeating and addiction. As such, self-perceived food addicts appear to represent a population that is at particular risk of overeating and weight gain and may therefore benefit from early dietary interventions which aim to increase food-related self-control and minimise temptation for high-calorie foods.
It is necessary for future research to reconcile findings of aberrant eating behaviour in self-perceived food addicts, with those which suggest that food addiction beliefs may have short-term helpful consequences for eating behaviour. In particular, given the popularity of the food addiction concept throughout the lay community and the promotion of food addiction messages within weight management groups, examining the longer-term consequences of food addiction beliefs is an especially important avenue for future research.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

Helen K. Ruddock declares that she has no conflicts of interest. Charlotte A. Hardman receives research funding from the American Beverage Association and personal fees from the International Sweeteners Association.

Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all participants who took part in research conducted by the authors.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Psychiatrie, den Premium-Inhalten der psychiatrischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat • Meule A. Back by popular demand: a narrative review on the history of food addiction research. Yale J Biol Med. 2015;88:295–302. Describes the history of food addiction research and highlights the ways in which the term ‘food addiction’ has been reconceptualised since it was first introduced within the scientific literature. PubMedPubMedCentral • Meule A. Back by popular demand: a narrative review on the history of food addiction research. Yale J Biol Med. 2015;88:295–302. Describes the history of food addiction research and highlights the ways in which the term ‘food addiction’ has been reconceptualised since it was first introduced within the scientific literature. PubMedPubMedCentral
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Randolph TG. The descriptive features of food addiction. Addictive eating and drinking. Q J Stud Alcohol. 1956;17:198–224.PubMed Randolph TG. The descriptive features of food addiction. Addictive eating and drinking. Q J Stud Alcohol. 1956;17:198–224.PubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Davis C, Curtis C, Levitan RD, Carter JC, Kaplan AS, Kennedy JL. Evidence that “food addiction” is a valid phenotype of obesity. Appetite. 2011;57(3):711–7.CrossRefPubMed Davis C, Curtis C, Levitan RD, Carter JC, Kaplan AS, Kennedy JL. Evidence that “food addiction” is a valid phenotype of obesity. Appetite. 2011;57(3):711–7.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat DiLeone RJ, Taylor JR, Picciotto MR. The drive to eat: comparisons and distinctions between mechanisms of food reward and drug addiction. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(10):1330–5.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral DiLeone RJ, Taylor JR, Picciotto MR. The drive to eat: comparisons and distinctions between mechanisms of food reward and drug addiction. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(10):1330–5.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Preliminary validation of the Yale Food Addiction Scale. Appetite. 2009;52(2):430–6.CrossRefPubMed Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Preliminary validation of the Yale Food Addiction Scale. Appetite. 2009;52(2):430–6.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Development of the Yale Food Addiction Scale Version 2.0. Psychol Addict Behav. 2016;30(1):113–21.CrossRefPubMed Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Development of the Yale Food Addiction Scale Version 2.0. Psychol Addict Behav. 2016;30(1):113–21.CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Ziauddeen H, Farooqi IS, Fletcher PC. Obesity and the brain: how convincing is the addiction model? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13:279–86.CrossRefPubMed Ziauddeen H, Farooqi IS, Fletcher PC. Obesity and the brain: how convincing is the addiction model? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13:279–86.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Hebebrand J, Albayrak Ö, Adan R, Antel J, Dieguez C, de Jong J, et al. “Eating addiction”, rather than “food addiction”, better captures addictive-like eating behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;47:295–306.CrossRefPubMed Hebebrand J, Albayrak Ö, Adan R, Antel J, Dieguez C, de Jong J, et al. “Eating addiction”, rather than “food addiction”, better captures addictive-like eating behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;47:295–306.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat • Carter A, Hendrikse J, Lee N, Yücel M, Verdejo-Garcia A, Andrews Z, et al. The neurobiology of “food addiction” and its implications for obesity treatment and policy. Annu Rev Nutr. 2016;36:105–28. Reviews the evidence that overeating may be driven by addictive processes and examines the potential clinical and public policy implications of the food addiction concept. CrossRefPubMed • Carter A, Hendrikse J, Lee N, Yücel M, Verdejo-Garcia A, Andrews Z, et al. The neurobiology of “food addiction” and its implications for obesity treatment and policy. Annu Rev Nutr. 2016;36:105–28. Reviews the evidence that overeating may be driven by addictive processes and examines the potential clinical and public policy implications of the food addiction concept. CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat McFerran B, Mukhopadhyay A. Lay theories of obesity predict actual body mass. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(8):1428–36.CrossRefPubMed McFerran B, Mukhopadhyay A. Lay theories of obesity predict actual body mass. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(8):1428–36.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Lee NM, Lucke J, Hall WD, Meurk C, Boyle FM, Carter A. Public views on food addiction and obesity: implications for policy and treatment. PLoS One. 2013;8(9). Lee NM, Lucke J, Hall WD, Meurk C, Boyle FM, Carter A. Public views on food addiction and obesity: implications for policy and treatment. PLoS One. 2013;8(9).
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Wilson GT, Perrin NA, Rosselli F, Striegel-Moore RH, Debar LL, Kraemer HC. Beliefs about eating and eating disorders. Eat Behav. 2009;10(3):157–60.CrossRefPubMed Wilson GT, Perrin NA, Rosselli F, Striegel-Moore RH, Debar LL, Kraemer HC. Beliefs about eating and eating disorders. Eat Behav. 2009;10(3):157–60.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat • Moran A, Musicus A, Soo J, Gearhardt AN, Gollust SE, Roberto CA. Believing that certain foods are addictive is associated with support for obesity-related public policies. Prev Med. 2016;90:39–46. Believing that certain foods are ‘addictive’ was associated with support for obesity-related policies CrossRefPubMed • Moran A, Musicus A, Soo J, Gearhardt AN, Gollust SE, Roberto CA. Believing that certain foods are addictive is associated with support for obesity-related public policies. Prev Med. 2016;90:39–46. Believing that certain foods are ‘addictive’ was associated with support for obesity-related policies CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Schulte EM, Avena NM, Gearhardt AN. Which foods may be addictive? The roles of processing, fat content, and glycemic load. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0117959.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Schulte EM, Avena NM, Gearhardt AN. Which foods may be addictive? The roles of processing, fat content, and glycemic load. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0117959.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Barry CL, Brescoll VL, Brownell KD, Schlesinger M. Obesity metaphors: how beliefs about the causes of obesity affect support for public policy. Milbank Q. 2009;87(1):7–47.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Barry CL, Brescoll VL, Brownell KD, Schlesinger M. Obesity metaphors: how beliefs about the causes of obesity affect support for public policy. Milbank Q. 2009;87(1):7–47.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Meadows A, Higgs S. I think, therefore I am? Characteristics of a non-clinical population of self-perceived food addicts Appetite. 2013;71:482. Meadows A, Higgs S. I think, therefore I am? Characteristics of a non-clinical population of self-perceived food addicts Appetite. 2013;71:482.
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Hardman CA, Rogers PJ, Dallas R, Scott J, Ruddock HK, Robinson E. “Food addiction is real”. The effects of exposure to this message on self-diagnosed food addiction and eating behaviour. Appetite. 2015;91:179–84.CrossRefPubMed Hardman CA, Rogers PJ, Dallas R, Scott J, Ruddock HK, Robinson E. “Food addiction is real”. The effects of exposure to this message on self-diagnosed food addiction and eating behaviour. Appetite. 2015;91:179–84.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat •• Ruddock HK, Dickson JM, Field M, Hardman CA. Eating to live or living to eat? Exploring the causal attributions of self-perceived food addiction. Appetite. 2015;95:262–8. Amongst members of the lay public, food addiction was associated with several core behavioural features. CrossRefPubMed •• Ruddock HK, Dickson JM, Field M, Hardman CA. Eating to live or living to eat? Exploring the causal attributions of self-perceived food addiction. Appetite. 2015;95:262–8. Amongst members of the lay public, food addiction was associated with several core behavioural features. CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Pretlow RA. Addiction to highly pleasurable food as a cause of the childhood obesity epidemic: a qualitative Internet study. Eat Disord. 2011;19(4):295–307.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pretlow RA. Addiction to highly pleasurable food as a cause of the childhood obesity epidemic: a qualitative Internet study. Eat Disord. 2011;19(4):295–307.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Pedram P, Wadden D, Amini P, Gulliver W, Randell E, Cahill F, et al. Food addiction: its prevalence and significant association with obesity in the general population. PLoS One. 2013;8(9):e74832.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pedram P, Wadden D, Amini P, Gulliver W, Randell E, Cahill F, et al. Food addiction: its prevalence and significant association with obesity in the general population. PLoS One. 2013;8(9):e74832.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
21.
Zurück zum Zitat • Ruddock HK, Field M, Hardman CA. Exploring food reward and calorie intake in self-perceived food addicts. Appetite. 2016; doi:10.1016/j.appet.2016.12.003. Participants who believed that they are ‘food addicts’ demonstrated increased food reward and consumed more calories from a high-fat food, compared to those who did not perceive themselves as food addicts. PubMed • Ruddock HK, Field M, Hardman CA. Exploring food reward and calorie intake in self-perceived food addicts. Appetite. 2016; doi:10.​1016/​j.​appet.​2016.​12.​003. Participants who believed that they are ‘food addicts’ demonstrated increased food reward and consumed more calories from a high-fat food, compared to those who did not perceive themselves as food addicts. PubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Malika NM, Hayman LW, Miller AL, Lee HJ, Lumeng JC. Low-income women’s conceptualizations of food craving and food addiction. Eat Behav. 2015;18:25–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Malika NM, Hayman LW, Miller AL, Lee HJ, Lumeng JC. Low-income women’s conceptualizations of food craving and food addiction. Eat Behav. 2015;18:25–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Hetherington MM, Macdiarmid JI. “Chocolate addiction”: a preliminary study of its description and its relationship to problem eating. Appetite. 1993;21:233–46.CrossRefPubMed Hetherington MM, Macdiarmid JI. “Chocolate addiction”: a preliminary study of its description and its relationship to problem eating. Appetite. 1993;21:233–46.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Hetherington MM, Macdiarmid JI. Pleasure and excess: liking for and overconsumption of chocolate. Physiol Behav. 1995;57(1):27–35.CrossRefPubMed Hetherington MM, Macdiarmid JI. Pleasure and excess: liking for and overconsumption of chocolate. Physiol Behav. 1995;57(1):27–35.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Tuomisto T, Hetherington MM, Morris M-F, Tuomisto MT, Turjanmaa V, Lappalainen R. Psychological and physiological characteristics of sweet food “addiction”. Int J Eat Disord. 1999:169–75. Tuomisto T, Hetherington MM, Morris M-F, Tuomisto MT, Turjanmaa V, Lappalainen R. Psychological and physiological characteristics of sweet food “addiction”. Int J Eat Disord. 1999:169–75.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Macdiarmid JL, Hetherington MM. Mood modulation by food: an exploration of affect and cravings in “chocolate addicts”. Br J Clin Psychol. 1995;34:129–38.CrossRefPubMed Macdiarmid JL, Hetherington MM. Mood modulation by food: an exploration of affect and cravings in “chocolate addicts”. Br J Clin Psychol. 1995;34:129–38.CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Appelhans B. Neurobehavioral inhibition of reward-driven feeding: implications for dieting and obesity. Obesity. 2009;17(4):622–4.CrossRef Appelhans B. Neurobehavioral inhibition of reward-driven feeding: implications for dieting and obesity. Obesity. 2009;17(4):622–4.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Wiers RW, Bartholow BD, van den Wildenberg E, Thush C, Engels RCME, Sher KJ, et al. Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: a review and a model. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007;86(2):263–83.CrossRefPubMed Wiers RW, Bartholow BD, van den Wildenberg E, Thush C, Engels RCME, Sher KJ, et al. Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: a review and a model. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007;86(2):263–83.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Rogers PJ, Smit HJ. Food craving and food “addiction”: a critical review of the evidence from a biopsychosocial perspective. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2000;66(1):3–14.CrossRefPubMed Rogers PJ, Smit HJ. Food craving and food “addiction”: a critical review of the evidence from a biopsychosocial perspective. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2000;66(1):3–14.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Davies JB. The myth of addiction. Florence: Taylor & Francis; 2013. Davies JB. The myth of addiction. Florence: Taylor & Francis; 2013.
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Latner JD, Puhl RM, Murakami JM, O’Brien KS. Food addiction as a causal model of obesity. Effects on stigma, blame, and perceived psychopathology. Appetite. 2014;77:77–82.CrossRefPubMed Latner JD, Puhl RM, Murakami JM, O’Brien KS. Food addiction as a causal model of obesity. Effects on stigma, blame, and perceived psychopathology. Appetite. 2014;77:77–82.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat DePierre JA, Puhl RM, Luedicke J. A new stigmatized identity? Comparisons of a “food addict” label with other stigmatized health conditions. Basic Appl Soc Psych. 2013;35(1):10–21.CrossRef DePierre JA, Puhl RM, Luedicke J. A new stigmatized identity? Comparisons of a “food addict” label with other stigmatized health conditions. Basic Appl Soc Psych. 2013;35(1):10–21.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruddock HK, Hardman CA. The effect of eating-related guilt on food addiction attributions and snack choice. (Under review). Ruddock HK, Hardman CA. The effect of eating-related guilt on food addiction attributions and snack choice. (Under review).
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Pearl RL, Lebowitz MS. Beyond personal responsibility: effects of causal attributions for overweight and obesity on weight-related beliefs, stigma, and policy support. Psychol Health. 2014;29(10):1176–91.CrossRefPubMed Pearl RL, Lebowitz MS. Beyond personal responsibility: effects of causal attributions for overweight and obesity on weight-related beliefs, stigma, and policy support. Psychol Health. 2014;29(10):1176–91.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Steptoe A, Wardle J. Locus of control and health behaviour revisited: a multivariate analysis of young adults from 18 countries. Br J Psychol. 2001;92:659–72.CrossRefPubMed Steptoe A, Wardle J. Locus of control and health behaviour revisited: a multivariate analysis of young adults from 18 countries. Br J Psychol. 2001;92:659–72.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Masalu JR, Astrøm AN. Predicting intended and self-perceived sugar restriction among Tanzanian students using the theory of planned behavior. J Health Psychol. 2001;6(4):435–45.CrossRefPubMed Masalu JR, Astrøm AN. Predicting intended and self-perceived sugar restriction among Tanzanian students using the theory of planned behavior. J Health Psychol. 2001;6(4):435–45.CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Richert J, Reuter T, Wiedemann AU, Lippke S, Ziegelmann J, Schwarzer R. Differential effects of planning and self-efficacy on fruit and vegetable consumption. Appetite. 2010;54(3):611–4.CrossRefPubMed Richert J, Reuter T, Wiedemann AU, Lippke S, Ziegelmann J, Schwarzer R. Differential effects of planning and self-efficacy on fruit and vegetable consumption. Appetite. 2010;54(3):611–4.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Byrne SM. Psychological aspects of weight maintenance and relapse in obesity. J Psychosom Res. 2002;53:1029–36.CrossRefPubMed Byrne SM. Psychological aspects of weight maintenance and relapse in obesity. J Psychosom Res. 2002;53:1029–36.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Hoyt CL, Burnette JL, Auster-Gussman L. “Obesity is a disease”: examining the self-regulatory impact of this public-health message. Psychol Sci. 2014;25(1):1–6. Hoyt CL, Burnette JL, Auster-Gussman L. “Obesity is a disease”: examining the self-regulatory impact of this public-health message. Psychol Sci. 2014;25(1):1–6.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Dar-Nimrod I, Cheung BY, Ruby MB, Heine SJ. Can merely learning about obesity genes affect eating behavior? Appetite. 2014;81:269–76.CrossRefPubMed Dar-Nimrod I, Cheung BY, Ruby MB, Heine SJ. Can merely learning about obesity genes affect eating behavior? Appetite. 2014;81:269–76.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Lee NM, Hall WD, Lucke J, Forlini C, Carter A. Food addiction and its impact on weight-based stigma and the treatment of obese individuals in the U.S. and Australia. Nutrients. 2014;6:5312–26.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lee NM, Hall WD, Lucke J, Forlini C, Carter A. Food addiction and its impact on weight-based stigma and the treatment of obese individuals in the U.S. and Australia. Nutrients. 2014;6:5312–26.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
42.
Zurück zum Zitat •• DePierre JA, Puhl RM, Luedicke J. Public perceptions of food addiction: a comparison with alcohol and tobacco. J Subst Use. 2014;19(1–2):1–6. Within a community sample, food addiction was perceived to be more of a behavioural choice than alcoholism. CrossRef •• DePierre JA, Puhl RM, Luedicke J. Public perceptions of food addiction: a comparison with alcohol and tobacco. J Subst Use. 2014;19(1–2):1–6. Within a community sample, food addiction was perceived to be more of a behavioural choice than alcoholism. CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Ronel N, Libman G. Eating disorders and recovery: lessons from Overeaters Anonymous. Clin Soc Work J. 2003;31(2):155–71.CrossRef Ronel N, Libman G. Eating disorders and recovery: lessons from Overeaters Anonymous. Clin Soc Work J. 2003;31(2):155–71.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Russell-Mayhew S, von Ranson KM, Masson PC. How does overeaters anonymous help its members? A qualitative analysis. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2010;18(1):33–42.CrossRefPubMed Russell-Mayhew S, von Ranson KM, Masson PC. How does overeaters anonymous help its members? A qualitative analysis. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2010;18(1):33–42.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat •• Ruddock HK, Christiansen P, Jones A, Robinson E, Field M, Hardman CA. Believing in food addiction: helpful or counterproductive for eating behavior? Obesity. 2016;24(6):1238–43. Participants who had been led to believe they had scored highly on a measure of ‘food addiction’ demonstrated increased dietary concern and consumed less calories during a subsequent taste task, compared to those who were told they had ‘low’ or ‘average’ levels of food addiction. CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral •• Ruddock HK, Christiansen P, Jones A, Robinson E, Field M, Hardman CA. Believing in food addiction: helpful or counterproductive for eating behavior? Obesity. 2016;24(6):1238–43. Participants who had been led to believe they had scored highly on a measure of ‘food addiction’ demonstrated increased dietary concern and consumed less calories during a subsequent taste task, compared to those who were told they had ‘low’ or ‘average’ levels of food addiction. CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Keeler CL, Mattes RD, Tan S-Y. Anticipatory and reactive responses to chocolate restriction in frequent chocolate consumers. Obesity. 2015;23(6):1130–5.CrossRefPubMed Keeler CL, Mattes RD, Tan S-Y. Anticipatory and reactive responses to chocolate restriction in frequent chocolate consumers. Obesity. 2015;23(6):1130–5.CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Mann T, Ward A. Forbidden fruit: does thinking about a prohibited food lead to its consumption? Int J Eat Disord. 2001;29(3):319–27.CrossRefPubMed Mann T, Ward A. Forbidden fruit: does thinking about a prohibited food lead to its consumption? Int J Eat Disord. 2001;29(3):319–27.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Food Addiction Beliefs Amongst the Lay Public: What Are the Consequences for Eating Behaviour?
verfasst von
Helen K. Ruddock
Charlotte A. Hardman
Publikationsdatum
01.06.2017
Verlag
Springer International Publishing
Erschienen in
Current Addiction Reports / Ausgabe 2/2017
Elektronische ISSN: 2196-2952
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-017-0136-0

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2017

Current Addiction Reports 2/2017 Zur Ausgabe

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.