Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Sleep and Biological Rhythms 3/2017

Open Access 01.07.2017 | Original Article

Effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive and physical performance in university students

verfasst von: Yusuf Patrick, Alice Lee, Oishik Raha, Kavya Pillai, Shubham Gupta, Sonika Sethi, Felicite Mukeshimana, Lothaire Gerard, Mohammad U. Moghal, Sohag N. Saleh, Susan F. Smith, Mary J. Morrell, James Moss

Erschienen in: Sleep and Biological Rhythms | Ausgabe 3/2017

Abstract

Sleep deprivation is common among university students, and has been associated with poor academic performance and physical dysfunction. However, current literature has a narrow focus in regard to domains tested, this study aimed to investigate the effects of a night of sleep deprivation on cognitive and physical performance in students. A randomized controlled crossover study was carried out with 64 participants [58% male (n = 37); 22 ± 4 years old (mean ± SD)]. Participants were randomized into two conditions: normal sleep or one night sleep deprivation. Sleep deprivation was monitored using an online time-stamped questionnaire at 45 min intervals, completed in the participants’ homes. The outcomes were cognitive: working memory (Simon game© derivative), executive function (Stroop test); and physical: reaction time (ruler drop testing), lung function (spirometry), rate of perceived exertion, heart rate, and blood pressure during submaximal cardiopulmonary exercise testing. Data were analysed using paired two-tailed T tests and MANOVA. Reaction time and systolic blood pressure post-exercise were significantly increased following sleep deprivation (mean ± SD change: reaction time: 0.15 ± 0.04 s, p = 0.003; systolic BP: 6 ± 17 mmHg, p = 0.012). No significant differences were found in other variables. Reaction time and vascular response to exercise were significantly affected by sleep deprivation in university students, whilst other cognitive and cardiopulmonary measures showed no significant changes. These findings indicate that acute sleep deprivation can have an impact on physical but not cognitive ability in young healthy university students. Further research is needed to identify mechanisms of change and the impact of longer term sleep deprivation in this population.

Introduction

Sleep deprivation is common amongst university students whom live in a culture that promotes reduced sleep, due to the burden of academic work and social pursuits. The reasons for poor sleep hygiene include alcohol and caffeine intake, stimulants, and technology, which prevent students achieving sufficient sleep time and quality [1]. A cross-sectional survey found that 71% of students did not achieve the recommended 8 h of sleep, with 60% classified as poor sleepers [2]. An average of 5.7 h sleep has been reported for students studying architecture, and sleepless nights due to academic work throughout the night—defined by the Oxford English Dictionary as an all-nighter—occurred, on average, 2.7 days a month [3].
While many studies have investigated the effects of acute sleep deprivation, few focus on university students, despite the prevalence and impact of sleep deprivation in this population [4, 5]. Such studies often have a narrow focus on disease states, limiting their ability to provide a holistic assessment of physical, emotional and cognitive wellbeing [46]. The importance of physical and cognitive function is especially appreciable in the student population, 52% of whom play sport at least once a week. Moreover, students rate sleep problems second only to stress in relation to negative impact on academic performance [7]. The effect of acute sleep deprivation on physical performance has been well documented with negligible effects on intense periods of exercise, whilst endurance task performance suffers due to decreased motivation [8, 9].
The effect of sleep deprivation on cognitive performance has also been documented previously with a correlation between sleep quality and grade point average in first year university students [10]. Moreover, sleep deprivation has been shown to have a detrimental effect on certain aspects of working memory, such as filtering efficiency, whilst Stroop test scores show degradation; however, this has been evidenced to be due to deficits in reaction time rather than processing skills [5, 1117]. Taken together, these data suggest that sleep deprivation may have a limited effect on cognitive ability in university students.
This study aimed to determine whether a night of sleep deprivation, equivalent to an “all-nighter”, would have a negative impact on the motor and cognitive performance of students, specifically focusing on reaction time, executive function, working memory, and cardiopulmonary function.

Materials and methods

Study design, participants, and recruitment

This was a randomized, controlled crossover study, which took place from June to September 2015. Exclusion criteria were: (1) any medication or medical history that would make participation in the study, in particular the sleep deprivation and exercise test, unsafe, or inappropriate; (2) mental incapacity to provide informed consent, or (3) recent (within 6 months) participation in a research trial. Participants were recruited via direct approach and posters on campus, social media, and a National Heart and Lung Institute newsletter. Participants travel expenses were reimbursed and all participants were offered the opportunity to be entered into a prize draw. Participants were told that the study involved testing parameters following sleep deprivation, but no information was given regarding the anticipated results. All participants gave written informed consent, and the study was approved by Medical Education Ethics Committee (Imperial College London, 23/4/15, MEEC1415-24).
Participants were randomized to either the sleep deprivation or a normal night’s sleep first, using a random number sequence. Twenty-four hours prior to the morning assessment, participants were instructed to refrain from consuming alcohol and caffeinated drinks as well as abstaining from exercise, smoking, and nicotine patches. Those having a normal night’s sleep where asked to report how much they had slept. The sleep deprivation arm were required to fill out a form every 45 min to confirm that they were still awake. This form was checked the following morning. More than two unexplained missed form completions resulted in disqualification from the study. The crossover condition and assessment were undertaken within 3–12 days of one another. Testing occurred between 09:00 to 13:00, with participants being allowed flexible timings; however, all follow-up testing aimed to take place within 1 h of initial session time. The outcome was to measure the change, if any, which occurred between the cognitive and physical performance of participants undergoing sleep deprivation.
Before testing began, height and weight were recorded and participants were asked to fill out a fitness questionnaire. The results of this questionnaire and participant sex were used to estimate the appropriate Monark Ergomedic 828e resistance for each participant, (male: 2.0 kp = unfit, 2.5 kp = fit, 3.0 kp = athlete; female: 1.5 kp = unfit, 2.0 kp = fit, 2.5 kp = athlete). A second questionnaire enquired about recent (within 24 h) intake of food, caffeine, alcohol, and nicotine, and any physical exercise was also completed.
All participants were provided with standardized descriptions of tests and given the opportunity to habituate with procedures.

Cognitive function tests

The working memory mobile application was derived from the SIMON© game, an appropriate test for working memory span [18]. It involved repeating a random sequence of colors and sounds. As each level progressed, another random color-sound combination was added to the previous sequence. This test was repeated three times.
Standard stroop charts were used: (1) monochrome (reading black text); (2) conflicting color (reading words with a mismatched color); (3) color blocks (articulating the color of colored blocks), and (4) conflicting words (articulating the color of mismatched words) [19]. Four versions of each test were created, so that no participant used the same chart twice. Time taken to complete each chart and the number of mistakes were recorded providing a measure of selective attention, automatic responses, inhibition, and control of executive functions [2022].

Physical function tests

Participants performed two concordant volume-time spirometry traces, in adherence to standard guidelines [23].
Participants underwent submaximal 8 min cardiopulmonary exercise testing (CPET) using a cycle ergometer target cadence 50 ± 5; this intensity of exercise test was chosen to replicate more closely students’ daily activities (as opposed to maximal exercise testing). Three electrocardiogram (ECG) electrodes were attached, and non-invasive blood pressure (BP) measurements and rating of perceived exertion (RPE) were also recorded throughout the exercise test (Fig. 1). Measurement of RPE has repeatedly been shown to have a strong correlation with the intensity of exercise being performed, independently from other factors [2426].

Reaction time

The ruler drop test was used to assess average reaction speed, a simple and inexpensive test compared to computerized assessments, with comparable reliability [2730]. Participants carried out three practice runs to eliminate a learning effect [27].

Statistical analyses

Statistical tests were carried out using IBM SPSS Statistics (V 22.0), Armonk, NY, USA. Due to the novel measures used in this study, a preliminary study was performed to estimate appropriate sample size. The study consisted of eight participants representative of the target population with a target sample size determined of 51 at a power level of 80%; therefore, a target recruitment of 70 was set with an estimated 25% attrition rate. Initially, descriptive statistics were obtained for all variables. Data were inspected for normality using histograms. Parametric data were reported as mean ± standard deviation, and paired two-tailed T tests were carried out to assess the difference between normal night sleep and sleep deprived arms. The tests were carried out with alpha significance level p ≤ 0.05, and 95% confidence intervals were calculated. Levene’s test (homogeneity of variances) was used to assess the differences in variance of samples. Differences in rating of perceived exertion and heart rate during exercise were assessed using multivariate analysis of variance test.

Results

Participant characteristics

The study sample consisted of 64 Imperial College London students, 57 (89%) of which completed the study (Fig. 2). The characteristics of the participants are summarised in Table 1; upon study entry, participants reported typically sleeping between 5 and 9 h per night, with 94.7% reporting these hours as ‘typical’ and 98.2% reporting sleeping through until morning without waking on most nights. 58% of participants were male and 69% were undergraduates. The participants’ mean age was 22 ± 4 years. Participants in the sleep deprivation arm filled in the online form on average every 49 ± 21 min throughout the night. Participants in the normal night’s sleep arm reported sleeping on average 7.2 ± 1.0 h. The mean difference in time of day that testing occurred (between condition 1 and condition 2) was 32 ± 15 min with no significant difference between testing times (p = 0.220).
Table 1
Participant characteristics (n = 57)
Characteristic
Normal night sleep (n = 57)
Sleep deprived (n = 57)
Age (years)
22 ± 4
Weight (kg)
67 ± 14
Height (m)
1.7 ± 0
Body mass index (kg/m2)
23 ± 4
Normal sleep duration (h)
7.4 ± 1.0
Disrupted normal sleep [n (%)]
1 (1.8%)
Sleep-related characteristics
 Quality of sleep (/10)
7.48 ± 1.99
0.67 ± 1.64
 Current mood (/10)
7.21 ± 1.69
3.51 ± 2.42
 Rating of adequacy of sleep  (/10)
7.40 ± 2.02
0.69 ± 1.73
Data are reported as mean ± standard deviation. Sleep-related characteristics were obtained using a questionnaire with a 10-point scale, ranging from 0 (lowest quality sleep, poor mood, and inadequate sleep) to 10 (best quality sleep, good mood, and fully adequate sleep)

Exercise testing

Fifty-four (95%) percent of participants completed both exercise tests. Table 2 shows key variables from the exercise test. There were no significant differences between the normal sleep and sleep deprived arms, except for systolic blood pressure post-exercise (135 ± 12 vs. 140 ± 17 mmHg; p = 0.012). There is no significant difference between the changes in mean heart rate and RPE when compared using a MANOVA test p = 0.723 and p = 0.559, respectively (see Fig. 3).
Table 2
Participant characteristics during CPET
Test
Normal night sleep (n = 54)
Sleep deprived (n = 54)
Difference
p value
Heart rate (bpm)
 Max heart rate
149 ± 22
146 ± 20
3 ± 14
0.079
Blood pressure (mmHg)
 Systolic at rest
116 ± 10
115 ± 12
1 ± 12
0.733
 Mean arterial pressure at rest
88 ± 7
86 ± 9
2 ± 7
0.123
 Systolic post-exercise
135 ± 12
140 ± 17
−6 ± 17
0.012*
 Mean arterial pressure post-exercise
94 ± 7
94 ± 11
0 ± 11
0.812
 Systolic post-recovery
120 ± 10
122 ± 15
−1 ± 13
0.429
 Mean arterial pressure post-recovery
89 ± 7
89 ± 10
0 ± 9
0.908
Data are reported as mean ± standard deviation. Data analysed using paired two-tailed T test
*Indicates significant result at an alpha of 5% (p ≤ 0.05)

Cognitive and physical testing

Table 3 compares the results of the cognitive tests and additional physical tests under sleep deprivation and normal sleep test conditions. There was no significant difference observed across all tests except reaction time, with a significantly higher average (p = 0.03) among individuals who were sleep deprived.
Table 3
Participant characteristics for cognitive tests
Test
Normal night sleep (n = 57)
Sleep deprived (n = 57)
Difference
p value
Memory
 Mean sequence length
10 ± 4
10 ± 3
0 ± 3
0.307
Stroop
 Monochrome
  Time (s)
37 ± 6
38 ± 6
−1 ± 1
0.185
  Errors (n)
1 ± 1
2 ± 1
0 ± 0
0.268
 Conflicting colors
  Time (s)
40 ± 7
41 ± 8
−1 ± 1
0.123
  Errors (n)
1 ± 1
1 ± 1
0 ± 0
0.768
Color block
  Time (s)
53 ± 10
52 ± 10
0 ± 0
0.437
  Errors (n)
1 ± 2
1 ± 1
0 ± 0
0.409
Conflicting words
  Time (s)
73 ± 17
72 ± 16
1 ± 1
0.469
  Errors (n)
2 ± 3
2 ± 3
0 ± 0
0.866
Reaction time
 Mean (s)
0.18 ± 0.04
0.19 ± 0.03
−0.15 ± 0.04
0.030*
Spirometry
 FEV1 (L)
3.63 ± 0.99
3.67 ± 0.95
−0.03 ± 0.25
0.303
 FVC (L)
4.33 ± 1.19
4.36 ± 1.22
−0.03 ± 0.19
0.321
 FEV1/FVC
0.85 ± 0.09
0.85 ± 0.08
−0.01 ± 0.06
0.511
 PEFR (L/min)
467 ± 134
468 ± 137
−1 ± 79
0.930
Data are reported as mean ± standard deviation. Data analysed using paired two-tailed T test
*Indicates significant result at an alpha of 5% (p ≤ 0.05)
The results of the first visit and second visit were compared irrespective of condition for the cognitive tests and reaction time. There was a significant difference for the monochrome, color blocks, and conflicting words Stroop tests (p = 0.001, p = 0.000, and p = 0.011, respectively); no significant difference was found in other variables.

Discussion

The main findings of this study were that sleep deprivation resulted in a significant increase in reaction time and post-exercise systolic BP in university students after one night of sleep deprivation, compared to a normal night’s sleep.

Cognitive

No significant differences were found in the cognitive tests, suggesting that one night of sleep deprivation has minimal effect on a student’s cognitive capacity.
Working memory and executive function both heavily rely on the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and salience network. This network has been shown reduced activation post-acute sleep deprivation [3135]. Interestingly, a comprehensive review has noted that particular tests show no significant difference, such as the digit span test. This test is most similar to the assessment of memory used in our study [35, 36]. Another study found that both partial and total sleep deprivation had no effect on visual working memory, but that total sleep deprivation had a significant effect on filtering efficiency [37]. As the Simon© game tests visual working memory, our results suggest that the effect of sleep deprivation may not be as widespread in university students, with components of memory being preserved [31].
The stroop test showed no significant increase in time taken to complete, or the number of mistakes made in each set. Acute sleep deprivation has been demonstrated to have no effect on the principal processes of interference or facilitation with performance influenced by increased reaction time [15]. Previously, the stroop test has been found to show practice effects [20]. This was evident in the present study with significant improvements made on the second visit, irrespective of participant condition. While it is difficult to interpret the reason for this result, it has previously been demonstrated that with increased age, there is an increase in brain activity in response to acute sleep deprivation [38]. Therefore, the young student population may be more effective at dealing with acute sleep deprivation.
Working memory and executive function are important with regard to university students, since they are linked to the understanding of complex concepts. Indeed, the previous research has shown a stronger correlation between attainment and working memory than with Intelligence Quotient [39]. Our study indicates that acute sleep deprivation was not detrimental to students’ cognitive ability; however, a review of other cognitive variables is necessary [35].

Physical

This study found an increase in reaction time after sleep deprivation, which has previously been well described with student subjects [40]. The underlying physiology of this effect is localised to the anterior cingulate cortex, middle prefrontal gyrus, and inferior parietal lobes which have been shown to be hypoactivated in acute sleep deprivation. This finding is pertinent to students, in particular the large proportion of them (52%) who take part in sport at least once a week [7, 31, 41]. Of greater concern, the previous research has demonstrated that tired students are more likely to drive dangerously, another activity requiring prompt reactions. In one study of 1039 students, 16% reported falling asleep while driving [42]. Another found that an ‘all-nighter’, had a comparable effect on driving performance to a blood alcohol concentration of 0.1% which is above the UK Drink Driving limit [43]. Reaction time has also been linked with cognitive processing speed via mental chronometry, indicating that the effect of slowed reaction time may not be wholly in the realm of physical exertion, but also a pseudo measurement of IQ [44].
RPE is commonly described as involving aspects of both metabolism and CNS activity [45, 46]. There was no significant change in RPE in the present study, although mean RPE after sleep deprivation was higher than after a night of sleep. This contradicts much of the pre-existing literature, which suggests that sleep deprivation is associated with a significantly increased RPE but little or no change in physiological parameters [35, 47].
This study found a reduction in HR post-exercise after acute sleep deprivation, although not significantly. A previous study has shown following two nights of sleep deprivation, and HR was significantly lower, during, and after an exercise test, compared to two nights of sleep. This was speculated to be due to ACTH concentration, which was lowest on the second day of sleep deprivation [48]. This is the same period over which exercise testing was carried out in our study, and could explain the effect on HR found.
According to the present study, acute sleep deprivation had no effect on resting blood pressure, but caused an increase in systolic blood pressure post-exercise. Thus, the effect on blood pressure from this study was unclear, a finding which has been reflected in the literature [49, 50]. Sleep deprivation has previously been demonstrated to stimulate sympathetic activity and neuroendocrine response to stressor stimuli [47]. Therefore, continuous periods of sleep deprivation, e.g., before a project deadline, may lead to students developing hypertension or having inappropriate response to intense exercise.
Spirometry was used to assess lung function parameters including FVC, FEV1, and the FEV1/FVC ratio. Spirometry is commonly used in clinical practice for monitoring baseline lung function. Analyses showed no statistically significant difference when comparing the sleep deprived values against the values for the normal night’s sleep, a finding supported by the literature [51, 52].

Limitations

Several limitations need to be considered when interpreting the findings of this study. First, participants carried out their night of sleep deprivation in an environment of their choice rather than a supervised environment. Therefore, the study design was reliant on self-reported sleep deprivation and form completion, which may mean that some students had more sleep than others on the sleep deprivation night. Whilst this reduces the generalisability of our results in more diverse samples, the results are indicative of the effects of acute sleep deprivation on students in higher education. Alternate equipment and a larger test selection would have given a wider holistic prospective on the impact of sleep deprivation in university students; however, the interventions used in the study were chosen to maximise participant familiarity and minimise testing time.

Conclusion

This study found that acute sleep deprivation has a significant effect on postexercise blood pressure and reaction time in students. These changes are likely due to neuroendocrine changes and downregulation in salience and motor areas of the brain. Most notably reduced reaction times will impact competitive sports, and can pose a danger to safety critical actions such as driving. However cognitive and neurophysical impairment in other functions was not as widespread as previously thought. Overall, this study found that an “all-nighter” does not affect a student’s cognitive ability, whilst physical performance is significantly affected.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a School of Medicine Education Innovations Award and the NIHR Respiratory Biomedical Research Unit at the Royal Brompton and Harefield NHS Foundation Trust, Imperial College London. Special thanks to Mr Matthew Barrett and Mr Andrew Horn for their technical assistance.

Author contributions

All authors contributed equally to the writing and revision of this manuscript. Data collection and analysis was carried out by LG, SG, AL, FM, YP, KP, OR, SS, and JM.

Compliance with ethical standards

All participants gave written informed consent, and the study was approved by Medical Education Ethics Committee (Imperial College London, 23/4/15, MEEC1415-24).

Conflict of interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Innere Medizin

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Innere Medizin erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Innere Medizin, den Premium-Inhalten der internistischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten internistischen Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Neurologie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes, den Premium-Inhalten der neurologischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Neurologie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Literatur
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Lund HG, Reider BD, Whiting AB, Prichard JR. Sleep patterns and predictors of disturbed sleep in a large population of college students. J Adolesc Health. 2010 Feb;46(2):124–32.CrossRefPubMed Lund HG, Reider BD, Whiting AB, Prichard JR. Sleep patterns and predictors of disturbed sleep in a large population of college students. J Adolesc Health. 2010 Feb;46(2):124–32.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Bachman L, Bachman C. Student perceptions of academic workload in architectural education. J Archit Plan Res. 2006;23(4):271–304. Bachman L, Bachman C. Student perceptions of academic workload in architectural education. J Archit Plan Res. 2006;23(4):271–304.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Perez-Olmos I, Ibanez-Pinilla M. Night shifts, sleep deprivation, and attention performance in medical students. Int J Med Educ. 2014;5:56–62.CrossRef Perez-Olmos I, Ibanez-Pinilla M. Night shifts, sleep deprivation, and attention performance in medical students. Int J Med Educ. 2014;5:56–62.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Tantawy AO, Tallawy HNE, Farghaly HR, Farghaly WM, Hussein AS. Impact of nocturnal sleep deprivation on declarative memory retrieval in students at an orphanage: a psychoneuroradiological study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2013;9:403–8.PubMedPubMedCentral Tantawy AO, Tallawy HNE, Farghaly HR, Farghaly WM, Hussein AS. Impact of nocturnal sleep deprivation on declarative memory retrieval in students at an orphanage: a psychoneuroradiological study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2013;9:403–8.PubMedPubMedCentral
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Hodge AB, Snyder AC, Fernandez AL, Boan AD, Malek AM, Sistino JJ. The effect of acute sleep deprivation and fatigue in cardiovascular perfusion students: a mixed methods study. J Extra Corpor Technol. 2012;44(3):116–25.PubMedPubMedCentral Hodge AB, Snyder AC, Fernandez AL, Boan AD, Malek AM, Sistino JJ. The effect of acute sleep deprivation and fatigue in cardiovascular perfusion students: a mixed methods study. J Extra Corpor Technol. 2012;44(3):116–25.PubMedPubMedCentral
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Fullagar HHK, Skorski S, Duffield R, Hammes D, Coutts AJ, Meyer T. Sleep and athletic performance: the effects of sleep loss on exercise performance, and physiological and cognitive responses to exercise. Sports Med Auckl NZ. 2015;45(2):161–86.CrossRef Fullagar HHK, Skorski S, Duffield R, Hammes D, Coutts AJ, Meyer T. Sleep and athletic performance: the effects of sleep loss on exercise performance, and physiological and cognitive responses to exercise. Sports Med Auckl NZ. 2015;45(2):161–86.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Thun E, Bjorvatn B, Flo E, Harris A, Pallesen S. Sleep, circadian rhythms, athletic performance. Sleep Med Rev. 2015;23:1–9.CrossRefPubMed Thun E, Bjorvatn B, Flo E, Harris A, Pallesen S. Sleep, circadian rhythms, athletic performance. Sleep Med Rev. 2015;23:1–9.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Baert S, Omey E, Verhaest D, Vermeir A. Mister Sandman, Bring Me Good Marks! On the Relationship between Sleep Quality and Academic Achievement [Internet]. Rochester, NY: Social Science Research Network; 2014 Jun [cited 2016 Jan 17]. Report No.: ID 2450423. Available from: http://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=2450423. Baert S, Omey E, Verhaest D, Vermeir A. Mister Sandman, Bring Me Good Marks! On the Relationship between Sleep Quality and Academic Achievement [Internet]. Rochester, NY: Social Science Research Network; 2014 Jun [cited 2016 Jan 17]. Report No.: ID 2450423. Available from: http://​papers.​ssrn.​com/​abstract=​2450423.
11.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Brien MJ, O’Toole RV, Newell MZ, Lydecker AD, Nascone J, Sciadini M, et al. Does sleep deprivation impair orthopaedic surgeons’ cognitive and psychomotor performance? J Bone Joint Surg Am. 20127;94(21):1975–81.CrossRefPubMed O’Brien MJ, O’Toole RV, Newell MZ, Lydecker AD, Nascone J, Sciadini M, et al. Does sleep deprivation impair orthopaedic surgeons’ cognitive and psychomotor performance? J Bone Joint Surg Am. 20127;94(21):1975–81.CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Xie M, Yan J, He C, Yang L, Tan G, Li C, et al. Short-term sleep deprivation impairs spatial working memory and modulates expression levels of ionotropic glutamate receptor subunits in hippocampus. Behav Brain Res. 2015;286:64–70.CrossRefPubMed Xie M, Yan J, He C, Yang L, Tan G, Li C, et al. Short-term sleep deprivation impairs spatial working memory and modulates expression levels of ionotropic glutamate receptor subunits in hippocampus. Behav Brain Res. 2015;286:64–70.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Lei Y, Shao Y, Wang L, Zhai T, Zou F, Ye E, et al. Large-scale brain network coupling predicts total sleep deprivation effects on cognitive capacity. PloS One. 2015;10(7):e0133959.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lei Y, Shao Y, Wang L, Zhai T, Zou F, Ye E, et al. Large-scale brain network coupling predicts total sleep deprivation effects on cognitive capacity. PloS One. 2015;10(7):e0133959.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Gevers W, Deliens G, Hoffmann S, Notebaert W, Peigneux P. Sleep deprivation selectively disrupts top-down adaptation to cognitive conflict in the Stroop test. J Sleep Res. 2015;24(6):666–72.CrossRefPubMed Gevers W, Deliens G, Hoffmann S, Notebaert W, Peigneux P. Sleep deprivation selectively disrupts top-down adaptation to cognitive conflict in the Stroop test. J Sleep Res. 2015;24(6):666–72.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Cain SW, Silva EJ, Chang A-M, Ronda JM, Duffy JF. One night of sleep deprivation affects reaction time, but not interference or facilitation in a Stroop task. Brain Cogn. 2011;76(1):37–42.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cain SW, Silva EJ, Chang A-M, Ronda JM, Duffy JF. One night of sleep deprivation affects reaction time, but not interference or facilitation in a Stroop task. Brain Cogn. 2011;76(1):37–42.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim HJ, Kim JH, Park K-D, Choi K-G, Lee HW. A survey of sleep deprivation patterns and their effects on cognitive functions of residents and interns in Korea. Sleep Med. 2011;12(4):390–6.CrossRefPubMed Kim HJ, Kim JH, Park K-D, Choi K-G, Lee HW. A survey of sleep deprivation patterns and their effects on cognitive functions of residents and interns in Korea. Sleep Med. 2011;12(4):390–6.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Bratzke D, Steinborn MB, Rolke B, Ulrich R. Effects of sleep loss and circadian rhythm on executive inhibitory control in the Stroop and Simon tasks. Chronobiol Int. 2012;29(1):55–61.CrossRefPubMed Bratzke D, Steinborn MB, Rolke B, Ulrich R. Effects of sleep loss and circadian rhythm on executive inhibitory control in the Stroop and Simon tasks. Chronobiol Int. 2012;29(1):55–61.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Gendle MH, Ransom, Michael R. Use of the electronic game SIMON as a measure of working memory span in college age adults. J Behav Neurosci Res. 2006;4. Gendle MH, Ransom, Michael R. Use of the electronic game SIMON as a measure of working memory span in college age adults. J Behav Neurosci Res. 2006;4.
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Grant Aram Killian. Stroop Color and Word Test. Test Critiques. Grant Aram Killian. Stroop Color and Word Test. Test Critiques.
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Davidson DJ, Zacks RT, Williams CC. Stroop interference, practice, and aging. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2003 Jun;10(2):85–98.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Davidson DJ, Zacks RT, Williams CC. Stroop interference, practice, and aging. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2003 Jun;10(2):85–98.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Ghimire N, Paudel BH, Khadka R, Singh PN. Reaction time in Stroop test in Nepalese medical students. J Clin Diagn Res JCDR. 2014;8(9):BC14–16.PubMed Ghimire N, Paudel BH, Khadka R, Singh PN. Reaction time in Stroop test in Nepalese medical students. J Clin Diagn Res JCDR. 2014;8(9):BC14–16.PubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Khng KH, Lee K. The relationship between Stroop and stop-signal measures of inhibition in adolescents: influences from variations in context and measure estimation. PloS One. 2014;9(7):e101356.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Khng KH, Lee K. The relationship between Stroop and stop-signal measures of inhibition in adolescents: influences from variations in context and measure estimation. PloS One. 2014;9(7):e101356.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Scherr J, Wolfarth B, Christle JW, Pressler A, Wagenpfeil S, Halle M. Associations between Borg’s rating of perceived exertion and physiological measures of exercise intensity. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2013 Jan;113(1):147–55.CrossRefPubMed Scherr J, Wolfarth B, Christle JW, Pressler A, Wagenpfeil S, Halle M. Associations between Borg’s rating of perceived exertion and physiological measures of exercise intensity. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2013 Jan;113(1):147–55.CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Borg G, Ljunggren G, Ceci R. The increase of perceived exertion, aches and pain in the legs, heart rate and blood lactate during exercise on a bicycle ergometer. Eur J Appl Physiol. 1985;54(4):343–9.CrossRef Borg G, Ljunggren G, Ceci R. The increase of perceived exertion, aches and pain in the legs, heart rate and blood lactate during exercise on a bicycle ergometer. Eur J Appl Physiol. 1985;54(4):343–9.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Borg G, Hassmén P, Lagerström M. Perceived exertion related to heart rate and blood lactate during arm and leg exercise. Eur J Appl Physiol. 1987;56(6):679–85.CrossRef Borg G, Hassmén P, Lagerström M. Perceived exertion related to heart rate and blood lactate during arm and leg exercise. Eur J Appl Physiol. 1987;56(6):679–85.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Del Rossi G, Malaguti A, Del Rossi S. Practice effects associated with repeated assessment of a clinical test of reaction time. J Athl Train. 2014 Jun;49(3):356–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Del Rossi G, Malaguti A, Del Rossi S. Practice effects associated with repeated assessment of a clinical test of reaction time. J Athl Train. 2014 Jun;49(3):356–9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Eckner JT, Kutcher JS, Richardson JK. Effect of concussion on clinically measured reaction time in 9 NCAA division I collegiate athletes: a preliminary study. PM R. 2011;3(3):212–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Eckner JT, Kutcher JS, Richardson JK. Effect of concussion on clinically measured reaction time in 9 NCAA division I collegiate athletes: a preliminary study. PM R. 2011;3(3):212–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Eckner JT, Kutcher JS, Richardson JK. Pilot evaluation of a novel clinical test of reaction time in national collegiate athletic association division I football players. J Athl Train. 2010;45(4):327–32.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Eckner JT, Kutcher JS, Richardson JK. Pilot evaluation of a novel clinical test of reaction time in national collegiate athletic association division I football players. J Athl Train. 2010;45(4):327–32.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Eckner JT, Kutcher JS, Richardson JK. Between-seasons test-retest reliability of clinically measured reaction time in National Collegiate Athletic Association Division I athletes. J Athl Train. 2011;46(4):409–14.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Eckner JT, Kutcher JS, Richardson JK. Between-seasons test-retest reliability of clinically measured reaction time in National Collegiate Athletic Association Division I athletes. J Athl Train. 2011;46(4):409–14.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Ma N, Dinges DF, Basner M, Rao H. How acute total sleep loss affects the attending brain: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Sleep. 2015;38(2):233–40.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Ma N, Dinges DF, Basner M, Rao H. How acute total sleep loss affects the attending brain: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Sleep. 2015;38(2):233–40.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Widmaier EP, Raff H, Strang KT. Vander’s Human Physiology. McGraw-Hill Education; 2014. p. 707 Widmaier EP, Raff H, Strang KT. Vander’s Human Physiology. McGraw-Hill Education; 2014. p. 707
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Mars RB, Grol MJ. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, working memory, and prospective coding for action. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2007;27(8):1801–2.CrossRef Mars RB, Grol MJ. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, working memory, and prospective coding for action. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2007;27(8):1801–2.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Nee DE, Wager TD, Jonides J. Interference resolution: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7(1):1–17.CrossRefPubMed Nee DE, Wager TD, Jonides J. Interference resolution: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7(1):1–17.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Alhola P, Polo-Kantola P. Sleep deprivation: impact on cognitive performance. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2007;3(5):553–67.PubMedPubMedCentral Alhola P, Polo-Kantola P. Sleep deprivation: impact on cognitive performance. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2007;3(5):553–67.PubMedPubMedCentral
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Quigley N, Green JF, Morgan D, Idzikowski C, King DJ. The effect of sleep deprivation on memory and psychomotor function in healthy volunteers. Hum Psychopharmacol Clin Exp. 2000;15(3):171–7.CrossRef Quigley N, Green JF, Morgan D, Idzikowski C, King DJ. The effect of sleep deprivation on memory and psychomotor function in healthy volunteers. Hum Psychopharmacol Clin Exp. 2000;15(3):171–7.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Drummond SPA, Anderson DE, Straus LD, Vogel EK, Perez VB. The effects of two types of sleep deprivation on visual working memory capacity and filtering efficiency. PloS One. 2012;7(4):e35653.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Drummond SPA, Anderson DE, Straus LD, Vogel EK, Perez VB. The effects of two types of sleep deprivation on visual working memory capacity and filtering efficiency. PloS One. 2012;7(4):e35653.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Almklov EL, Drummond SPA, Orff H, Alhassoon OM. The effects of sleep deprivation on brain functioning in older adults. Behav Sleep Med. 2015;13(4):324–45.CrossRefPubMed Almklov EL, Drummond SPA, Orff H, Alhassoon OM. The effects of sleep deprivation on brain functioning in older adults. Behav Sleep Med. 2015;13(4):324–45.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Alloway TP, Alloway RG. Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. J Exp Child Psychol. 2010;106(1):20–9.CrossRefPubMed Alloway TP, Alloway RG. Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. J Exp Child Psychol. 2010;106(1):20–9.CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Souissi M, Chtourou H, Abedelmalek S, Ghozlane IB, Sahnoun Z. The effects of caffeine ingestion on the reaction time and short-term maximal performance after 36 h of sleep deprivation. Physiol Behav. 2014;131:1–6.CrossRefPubMed Souissi M, Chtourou H, Abedelmalek S, Ghozlane IB, Sahnoun Z. The effects of caffeine ingestion on the reaction time and short-term maximal performance after 36 h of sleep deprivation. Physiol Behav. 2014;131:1–6.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Lim J, Dinges DF. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129:305–22.CrossRefPubMed Lim J, Dinges DF. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129:305–22.CrossRefPubMed
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor DJ, Bramoweth AD. Patterns and consequences of inadequate sleep in college students: substance use and motor vehicle accidents. J Adolesc Health. 2010;46(6):610–2.CrossRefPubMed Taylor DJ, Bramoweth AD. Patterns and consequences of inadequate sleep in college students: substance use and motor vehicle accidents. J Adolesc Health. 2010;46(6):610–2.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Dawson D, Reid K. Fatigue, alcohol and performance impairment. Nature. 1997;388(6639):235–235.CrossRefPubMed Dawson D, Reid K. Fatigue, alcohol and performance impairment. Nature. 1997;388(6639):235–235.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Dempsey JA, Amann M, Romer LM, Miller JD. Respiratory system determinants of peripheral fatigue and endurance performance. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008;40(3):457–61.CrossRefPubMed Dempsey JA, Amann M, Romer LM, Miller JD. Respiratory system determinants of peripheral fatigue and endurance performance. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008;40(3):457–61.CrossRefPubMed
46.
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Myles WS. Sleep deprivation, physical fatigue. and the perception of exercise intensity. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1985;17(5):580–4. Myles WS. Sleep deprivation, physical fatigue. and the perception of exercise intensity. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1985;17(5):580–4.
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Konishi M, Takahashi M, Endo N, Numao S, Takagi S, Miyashita M, et al. Effects of sleep deprivation on autonomic and endocrine functions throughout the day and on exercise tolerance in the evening. J Sports Sci. 2013;31(3):248–55.CrossRefPubMed Konishi M, Takahashi M, Endo N, Numao S, Takagi S, Miyashita M, et al. Effects of sleep deprivation on autonomic and endocrine functions throughout the day and on exercise tolerance in the evening. J Sports Sci. 2013;31(3):248–55.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Kaygisiz Z, Erkasap N, Soydan M. Cardiorespiratory responses to submaximal incremental exercise are not affected by one night’s sleep deprivation during the follicular and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 2003;47(3):279–87.PubMed Kaygisiz Z, Erkasap N, Soydan M. Cardiorespiratory responses to submaximal incremental exercise are not affected by one night’s sleep deprivation during the follicular and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 2003;47(3):279–87.PubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Keeffe M, Roberts AL, Kelleman M, Roychoudhury A, St-Onge M-P. No effects of short-term sleep restriction, in a controlled feeding setting, on lipid profiles in normal-weight adults. J Sleep Res. 2013;22(6):717–20.CrossRefPubMed O’Keeffe M, Roberts AL, Kelleman M, Roychoudhury A, St-Onge M-P. No effects of short-term sleep restriction, in a controlled feeding setting, on lipid profiles in normal-weight adults. J Sleep Res. 2013;22(6):717–20.CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Chen HI, Tang YR. Sleep loss impairs inspiratory muscle endurance. Am Rev Respir Dis. 1989;140(4):907–9.CrossRefPubMed Chen HI, Tang YR. Sleep loss impairs inspiratory muscle endurance. Am Rev Respir Dis. 1989;140(4):907–9.CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Azboy O, Kaygisiz Z. Effects of sleep deprivation on cardiorespiratory functions of the runners and volleyball players during rest and exercise. Acta Physiol Hung. 2009;96(1):29–36.CrossRefPubMed Azboy O, Kaygisiz Z. Effects of sleep deprivation on cardiorespiratory functions of the runners and volleyball players during rest and exercise. Acta Physiol Hung. 2009;96(1):29–36.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive and physical performance in university students
verfasst von
Yusuf Patrick
Alice Lee
Oishik Raha
Kavya Pillai
Shubham Gupta
Sonika Sethi
Felicite Mukeshimana
Lothaire Gerard
Mohammad U. Moghal
Sohag N. Saleh
Susan F. Smith
Mary J. Morrell
James Moss
Publikationsdatum
01.07.2017
Verlag
Springer Japan
Erschienen in
Sleep and Biological Rhythms / Ausgabe 3/2017
Print ISSN: 1446-9235
Elektronische ISSN: 1479-8425
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s41105-017-0099-5

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2017

Sleep and Biological Rhythms 3/2017 Zur Ausgabe

Review Article

Sleep and epilepsy

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Hirnblutung unter DOAK und VKA ähnlich bedrohlich

17.05.2024 Direkte orale Antikoagulanzien Nachrichten

Kommt es zu einer nichttraumatischen Hirnblutung, spielt es keine große Rolle, ob die Betroffenen zuvor direkt wirksame orale Antikoagulanzien oder Marcumar bekommen haben: Die Prognose ist ähnlich schlecht.

Thrombektomie auch bei großen Infarkten von Vorteil

16.05.2024 Ischämischer Schlaganfall Nachrichten

Auch ein sehr ausgedehnter ischämischer Schlaganfall scheint an sich kein Grund zu sein, von einer mechanischen Thrombektomie abzusehen. Dafür spricht die LASTE-Studie, an der Patienten und Patientinnen mit einem ASPECTS von maximal 5 beteiligt waren.

Schwindelursache: Massagepistole lässt Otholiten tanzen

14.05.2024 Benigner Lagerungsschwindel Nachrichten

Wenn jüngere Menschen über ständig rezidivierenden Lagerungsschwindel klagen, könnte eine Massagepistole der Auslöser sein. In JAMA Otolaryngology warnt ein Team vor der Anwendung hochpotenter Geräte im Bereich des Nackens.

Schützt Olivenöl vor dem Tod durch Demenz?

10.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Konsumieren Menschen täglich 7 Gramm Olivenöl, ist ihr Risiko, an einer Demenz zu sterben, um mehr als ein Viertel reduziert – und dies weitgehend unabhängig von ihrer sonstigen Ernährung. Dafür sprechen Auswertungen zweier großer US-Studien.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.