Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry 1/2014

Open Access 01.12.2014 | Research article

Neuroticism developmental courses - implications for depression, anxiety and everyday emotional experience; a prospective study from adolescence to young adulthood

verfasst von: Maren Aldinger, Malte Stopsack, Ines Ulrich, Katja Appel, Eva Reinelt, Sebastian Wolff, Hans Jörgen Grabe, Simone Lang, Sven Barnow

Erschienen in: BMC Psychiatry | Ausgabe 1/2014

Abstract

Background

Neuroticism is frequently discussed as a risk factor for psychopathology. According to the maturity principle, neuroticism decreases over the course of life, but not uniformly across individuals. However, the implications of differences in personality maturation on mental health have not been well studied so far. Hence, we hypothesized that different forms of neuroticism development from adolescence to young adulthood are associated with differences in depression, anxiety and everyday emotional experience at the age of 25.

Methods

A sample of 266 adolescents from the general population was examined three times over ten years (age at T0: 15, T1: 20 and T2: 25) using questionnaires, interviews and ecological momentary assessment (EMA). At all measurement points, neuroticism was assessed with the NEO inventory. At T2, diagnoses of major depression and anxiety disorders were captured with a structured clinical interview (M-CIDI). Phone-based EMA was used to assess emotional experience and affective instability over a two-week period at T2.

Results

The best fitting model was a latent class growth analysis with two groups of neuroticism development. Most individuals (n = 205) showed moderate values whereas 61 participants were clustered into a group with elevated neuroticism levels. In both groups neuroticism significantly changed during the ten year period with a peak at the age of 20. Individuals with a higher absolute level were at 14-fold increased risk for depression and 7-fold risk for anxiety disorders at the age of 25. In EMA, increased negative affect and arousal as well as decreased positive emotions were found in this high group.

Conclusions

Other than expected, personality did not mature in our sample. However, there was a significant change of neuroticism values from adolescence to young adulthood. Further, over 20% of our participants showed a neuroticism development which was associated with adverse outcomes such as negatively toned emotional experience and a heightened risk to suffer from depressive and anxiety disorders in young adulthood. These high-risk persons need to be identified early to provide interventions supporting continuous personality maturation.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​s12888-014-0210-2) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Competing interests

All authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Abkürzungen
BIC
Bayesian information criterion
BLRT
Bootstrapped parametric likelihood ratio tests
EMA
Ecological momentary assessment
HA
Harm avoidance
LCGA
Latent class growth analysis
LMR
Lo-Mendell-Rubin-test
MSSD
Mean squared successive differences
N
Neuroticism
OR
Odds ratio
SHIP
Study of health in Pomerania

Background

Personality traits are frequently discussed as risk factors for various psychopathological complaints [1],[2]. Especially, neuroticism is often examined in the context of psychopathology [3],[4]. Individuals scoring high on this personality dimension can be characterized as worried, emotionally unstable, overly reactive or nervous [5]. Particularly, the association between high neuroticism and internalizing disorders like depression [6],[7] or anxiety disorders [8],[9] is well established.
Originally, personality traits were described to reflect genetically determined and relatively stable interindividual differences e.g., [10],[11]. However, by now there is increasing evidence that personality changes in all periods of life (for reviews see [12],[13]) with great developmental steps during adolescence/young adulthood and again in old age [14]-[16]. For instance, Littlefield, Sher, and Wood [17] found mean-level decreases of neuroticism in young adulthood. Similar results were obtained by Specht, Egloff, and Schmukle [16]. They reported that emotional stability, which is often used as synonym for low neuroticism, rises during a four year interval in different age groups in the general population. Such developmental changes of personality are summarized in the maturity principle[13],[18],[19]. This principle states that in most people personality matures over time. Regarding neuroticism, a decrease of neuroticism is expected. Importantly, Caspi et al. [13] emphasize that this maturation process is not uniform across individuals. Instead the authors argue that not all individuals achieve a mature personality or at least not at the same time. This should in turn be associated with differences in outcomes such as mental health or well-being, for example [13].
Thus, the maturity principle emphasizes the possibility that individuals do not mature at the same pace. Therefore, individual courses of personality maturation and their impact on mental health need to be focused on [20]. Nevertheless, only few studies examined personality changes by differentiating developmental courses (e.g., [21],[22]). For instance, three developmental groups were identified by Robins, Fraley, Roberts, and Trzesniewski [23]: in 23% of participants neuroticism levels decreased over a 4-year-interval, in 4% they increased and in 73% neuroticism remained stable. However, in this study the implications of group membership on possible outcomes like psychopathology or well-being were not examined.
Johnson, Hicks, McGue, and Iacono [24] focused on the temperament factor harm avoidance (HA), which is strongly associated with neuroticism [25], in a female twin sample. In their study, they found four different forms of development from age 14 to age 24: Three groups showed increasing HA-values, but differed in absolute level, whereas in the fourth group a decrease of HA was found. In addition, these developmental groups differed significantly regarding the prevalence of antisocial behaviour or substance dependence at the age of 24. Mroczek and Spiro [26] even found different mortality rates dependent on neuroticism level and course in middle-aged to old men. Men who scored high on this trait at baseline and who increased over a period of 18 years were less likely to survive.
Of course, this review of studies is not exhaustive. Nevertheless it gives first evidence for the serious consequences of missed personality maturation, albeit studies longitudinally relating changes in neuroticism to psychopathology are still rare. It must be noted, that, except for Johnson et al. [24] all above mentioned studies assessed psychopathology on a subsyndromal level using self-reports. In contrast, the influence of personality maturation on diagnoses of psychiatric disorders, as measurable with structured clinical interviews, has been neglected so far.
In addition, self-reports often generate global indices of impairments in general and are prone to retrospective bias [27]. It remains unclear how different forms of development influence everyday life. Such shortcomings could be overcome by ecological momentary assessment (EMA); [28]. This approach allows capturing real-time information while individuals go about their normal lives. As alterations in affectivity can be found in most mental disorders [29], the assessment of emotional experience using EMA could provide valuable additional information. With this method, emotions can be recorded in the moment they are experienced without being subject to recollection bias or other systematic distortions [30]. Further, affect dynamics such as instability or variability can be examined aside from mean levels [31],[32]. Due to its high ecological validity and enhanced flexibility compared to traditional assessment methods, EMA has gained increased application in the context of mood and affective components of mental disorders [33],[34].
When it comes to neuroticism and emotional experience, EMA is also increasing in importance [35]-[40]. For instance, Miller, Vachon, and Lynam [41] contacted undergraduate students via palm computers eight times a day over one week. In doing so, they found questionnaire-based neuroticism to be positively associated with mean negative affect and negative affect instability in daily measures. Similar results were obtained in other studies in which the authors reported more frequent, more intense and longer lasting unpleasant affect in EMA in association with neuroticism [42]-[44]. In several studies, Suls et al. [45] found that individuals with high neuroticism values strongly respond to daily problems – a pattern which they call the neurotic cascade. Further, in one study that assessed neuroticism at multiple measurement points, these values were aggregated over time for further analyses [46]. The authors reported lower positive affect and increased negative affect variability in individuals with high neuroticism values. To sum up, EMA methods are applied more and more frequently in research on the association between neuroticism and affect in everyday life. However, none of the above mentioned studies examined the longitudinal relationship between neuroticism and affectivity in everyday life in a representative sample taking changes in personality into account.
In line with the existing literature, we hypothesized that neuroticism values change from adolescence to young adulthood. In particular, we assumed that these changes in neuroticism are not uniform across individuals. Instead, groups of different courses should be identifiable. As all but one study examined outcomes of personality maturation using self-reports, in our study we aimed at describing the implications of group membership more precisely. Therefore, a multimethod approach was chosen: first, we examined whether belonging to a specific developmental group is associated with different degrees of psychopathology assessed by structured clinical interviews and self-ratings. Further, we tested the influences of group membership on emotions in everyday life using EMA.

Methods

Participants

The sample was drawn from the population-based Greifswald family study [47],[48], a subpopulation of the Study of Health in Pomerania, Germany (SHIP; John et al., [49]). In SHIP, 4308 people aged 20 to 79 were randomly selected between March 1997 and May 2000, proportional to the population size of each community, and stratified by age and gender. From this sample, 527 families who lived in a household with at least one offspring between the ages of 11 and 18 years were invited to take part in the family study. 141 families could not be located or did not answer our phone calls and letters. Further, 71 families refused to participate, resulting in a final sample of 315 families with whom assessments of parents and offspring (n = 381, mean age 15.1, SD = 2.3) were conducted (T0).
Parents and offspring were again investigated about five years later between 2005 and 2008 (T1): 87.7% of offspring (n = 334, mean age 19.6, SD = 2.4) took part in this follow-up. Since May 2011 offspring were examined for a third time (T2). Data of this second follow-up are available from 85.0% (n = 284) of T1 participants. 23 former participants were not available via post sendings or telephone calls because they moved away. 25 individuals were contacted but refused to participate and two persons died between T1 and T2. Individuals who took part in all assessments did not differ from those who dropped out after T0 regarding sex (χ 2 = 2.37, p = .146), age (F = 2.05, p = .153), neuroticism (F = 0.73, p = .395; operationalized as harm avoidance in children younger than 16 years: F = 0.10, p = .747) and psychopathology (F = 0.16, p = .690) at T0. In 18 participants, at least one relevant questionnaire or interview was missing completely. These individuals were excluded from our analyses, resulting in a final sample of 266 young adults (56.4% female, mean age 24.9, SD = 2.3). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants after the study has been fully explained. The study was approved by the local ethics committee of the Ruprecht-Karls-University Heidelberg, Germany.

Materials and procedure

An overview over all constructs and their assessment at each measurement point can be seen in Table 1.
Table 1
Constructs, measures and measurement mode for the three measurement points
 
Measure
Mode
Transformation
Neuroticism
   
T0
   
• age < 16
harm avoidance scale of the J-TCI
self-report
scale 1 to 5
• age ≥ 16
NEO-FFI
self-report
-
T1
NEO-PI-R (only corresponding FFI-Items)
self-report
-
T2
NEO-FFI
self-report
-
Subsyndromal psychopathology
   
T0
YSR total score
self-report
scale 1 to 5
T1
SCL-90-R: GSI
self-report
scale 1 to 5
T2
BSI: GSI
self-report
scale 1 to 5
Diagnoses of depression and anxiety disorders
   
T0 lifetime
   
• age < 16
children version of the DIPS
structured clinical interview
• age ≥ 16
DIA-X
standardized clinical interview
T1 lifetime
DIA-X
standardized clinical interview
T2 current & lifetime
DIA-X
standardized clinical interview
Emotions in everyday life
   
T2
ecological momentary assessment
phone-based self-report
Notes. J-TCI: Junior Temperament and Character Inventory; NEO-FFI: NEO Five Factor Inventory; NEO-PI-R: NEO Personality Inventory Revised; YSR: Youth Self Report; SCL-90-R: Symptom Checklist 90 Revised; GSI: Global Severity Index; BSI: Brief Symptom Inventory; DIPS: Diagnostic Interview for Mental Disorders.

Assessment of neuroticism

At all points of measurement, neuroticism was assessed with versions of the NEO personality inventory [49]. The NEO measures the Big-Five personality traits extraversion, neuroticism, openness, agreeableness and conscientiousness on a 5-point likert-type scale. The versions solely differ in their item number: whereas the NEO-Five-Factor-Inventory (T0 & T2; NEO-FFI; [50]) consists of 60 items, the NEO-Personality-Inventory-Revised (T1; NEO-PI-R; [51]) has 240 items. We only included the corresponding NEO-FFI-items from the NEO-PI-R in our analyses. Validity [52] and reliability of the NEO-FFI were found to be satisfying (Cronbach’s α T0: 0.716, T1: 0.870, T2: 0.868).
As the NEO is not applicable in children younger than 16 years [53], we used the harm avoidance subscale of the Junior Temperament and Character Inventory J-TCI; [54] in younger participants at T0 instead. This is an adapted version of Cloninger’s Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI); [55]. The harm avoidance scale comprises the subscales anticipatory worry, fear of uncertainty, shyness, and fatigability. Cronbach’s α in our sample was 0.775. Studies examining personality with multiple questionnaires found harm avoidance and neuroticism to be highly correlated and to compose a common dimension in factor analysis [56],[57]. Thus, Aluja and Blanch [25] concluded that both scales measure equivalent constructs. In our study, individuals older than 16 years answered both the NEO and the TCI at T0 and harm avoidance and neuroticism were highly correlated (r = .614, p = .000). To enhance comparability between measurements, we transformed the J-TCI harm avoidance scale into the NEO 1 to 5 answering mode.

Assessment of psychopathology

Diagnoses of depression and anxiety disorders

At all measurement points, diagnoses of depressive and anxiety disorders were assessed with the standardized Munich-Composite International Diagnostic Interview (DIA-X/M-CIDI); [58] in individuals older than 15 years. All interviews were conducted by trained clinical psychologists either in person or via telephone if a participant was living too far away. Unfortunately, we were not able to tape our interviews. Hence, inter-rater-reliability of our diagnostic interviews could not be calculated. However, according to the developers of the DIA-X, inter-rater reliability of this interview is high (κ = .81 - 1.0) and validity according to comparison with clinical diagnoses is at least satisfying (κ = .39 - .82) [59]. As the DIA-X is not applicable in children younger than 16, at T0 the child version of the Diagnostic Interview for Mental Disorders (DIPS); [60] was used. The DIPS is a structured clinical interview with satisfying to good psychometric properties [60].

General psychopathological complaints

At first assessment, the German version of the Youth Self Report (YSR); [61],[62] was used for examination of general psychopathological complaints. The YSR is a self-report instrument and consists of 112 items assessing behavioural and emotional problems on eight scales in adolescents aged 11 to 18. A general psychopathology score was calculated from 101 items. In our study, reliability was excellent (Cronbach’s α = 0.921).
At T1, general psychopathology was measured with the German version of the Symptom Checklist-Revised (SCL-90-R); [63],[64] and at T2 with its short form, the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI); [65], respectively. Both are self-rating inventories with nine scales assessing different symptoms during the last seven days and were found to be comparable [66]. Reliability and validity were found to be excellent for both, the SCL-90-R and the BSI (T1 SCL-90-R: Cronbach’s α = 0.965; T2 BSI: Cronbach’s α = 0.963) [67],[68]. To assess general psychopathology, the Global Severity Index (GSI); [63] was calculated for both questionnaires. In order to enhance comparability with neuroticism and interpretability of these different measurements, all instruments were transformed into a 1 to 5 response format.

Emotional experience in everyday life

An ecological momentary approach was used to gather information regarding emotional experience in everyday life at T2. Computer-based phone calls were made with the SmartQ/DialQ software package (© Telesage Inc.), and recorded questions were red out by a staff member. Participants were called on their cell phones three times a day, every second day during a two week period. If the call was not answered, two additional trials were made 30, respectively 60, minutes later. Besides other questions, we asked the participants how they felt in the current moment. First, the emotional state was examined in general by indicating current valence (from good to bad) and arousal (from relaxed to tense). Answers were given on likert-type scales ranging from 0 to 6 by pressing the according number on the keyboard. Second, we asked more specifically for the experience of eight different emotions (happiness, sadness, disgust, anxiety, anger, interest, shame, boredom) again using scales from 0 to 6. Higher values indicated stronger momentary experience of this particular emotion. To date, methodology in EMA studies is manifold, and standardized questions and instruments are missing so far [33]. Nevertheless, as mentioned above, these designs are meant to diminish recall biases and increase ecological validity compared to self-report questionnaires. Further, there is some literature reporting good reliability and validity of EMA in clinical psychology research [69],[70].

Data analyses

When it comes to modelling longitudinal growth data, various approaches can be used [71]. In this study we examined two different models, namely latent class growth analysis (LCGA) and growth mixture modelling (GMM). Both models were conducted with neuroticism at the three measurement points using Mplus version 6 [72]. As an extension to conventional latent growth models, LCGA and GMM allow to identify latent groups with different developmental trajectories. Individuals are grouped based on latent growth factors, namely intercept (initial status) and slope. In our models the factor loading for the slope growth factor on T2 was freely estimated. Due to our relatively large age range as well as differences in neuroticism assessment depending on age at T0, age was included as a covariate. Further, the error variances of T1 and T2 neuroticism were set to be equal because at these assessments the same instrument was used as opposed to T0. LCGA is a specific form of GMM in which trajectories within a class are defined to be homogenous, i.e., the variance of the slope factor is fixed to zero within groups (see Figure 1). In contrast, in GMM the variance of the slope factor is freely estimated. Thus, the slope factor can covariate with other variables such as the intercept, (for example for a detailed description of LCGA and GMM see [73]). In Mplus, a variety of indices is provided to evaluate model fit. In this study, the best group solution was identified on the basis of the following criteria [74]: the Bayesian information criterion (BIC, lowest values considered best), the Lo-Mendell-Rubin-test (LMR); [75] and bootstrapped parametric likelihood ratio tests (BLRT); [76]. LMR and BLRT were applied to test whether a solution with k + 1 groups fits the data significantly better than the solution with k groups. Further, relative entropy should be at least 0.8 as with a value of 1.0 indicating perfect classification [77]. However, there is no binding criterion to decide the number of trajectory classes. Instead, a variety of factors like theoretical considerations, interpretability or replicability among others should be considered [73].
Second, repeated measures analysis of variances with time as within-subjects factor and group membership as between-subjects factor was performed for general psychopathology for the three measurement points. In addition, survival analyses were run to examine the courses of lifetime diagnoses of depression and anxiety disorders over the ten year period. Further, we conducted logistic regression analyses to examine odds ratios (OR) for the T2 diagnoses of current depressive and anxiety disorders depending on developmental group and controlled for depression and anxiety symptoms at T0 (as measured with the YSR).
Third, data from EMA were aggregated into a mean experience score for valence, arousal and each specific emotion. Further, exploratory factor analysis with oblimin rotation was conducted with the specific emotion scores. In addition, mean squared successive differences (MSSD) within a day were calculated as a marker for emotional instability for a detailed description of the MSSD see [78],[79]. MSSDs were averaged over the assessment days for each participant and weighted by the emotion level, as there is evidence that absolute level and affect dynamics are interrelated [41]. Finally, a multivariate analysis of variances (MANOVA) was performed to identify group differences regarding valence, arousal, emotional factor values and emotional instability.

Results

Descriptive statistics regarding demographic variables as well as neuroticism, psychopathology and everyday emotional experience can be seen in Table 2.
Table 2
Descriptive statistics regarding demographics, neuroticism, psychopathology, prevalences of depression and anxiety disorders and emotional experience (n = 266)
 
%
n
T 2 demographics
  
education
  
university degree
17.6
47
A-Levels
41.0
109
secondary school diploma
36.5
97
others
4.9
13
living in partnership
64.3
171
having children
12.8
34
T 2 current diagnosis
  
depression
9.8
26
anxiety
4.5
12
 
M
SD
Neuroticism
  
T0
2.26
(0.69)
T1
2.77
(0.58)
T2
2.55
(0.64)
General psychopathology
  
T0
1.42
(0.25)
T1
1.39
(0.35)
T2
1.32
(0.40)
T 2 everyday emotional experience a
  
valenceb
1.89
(0.96)
arousalc
1.81
(0.87)
Notes. an = 222 due to missing values in ecological momentary assessment; b scaled from feeling good (0) to feeling bad (6); cscaled from being relaxed (0) to being tense (6).
Using LCGA our models converged and fit indices for different class solutions can be seen in Table 3. In GMM, a non-significant negative residual variance (estimate -0.151, p = 0.07) of the slope factor occurred in the two group solution. This pattern did not change after modification of starting values and thus may indicate that there is no substantial variance of the slope factor within groups. Thus, for further examination we decided to go with the LCGA in which the variance of the slope factor is fixed to zero as this seemed to be a more proper model. In LCGA, differences in BIC were not wide, but it was the lowest for a three group solution (see Table 3). However, in this model entropy was slightly lower than 0.8 and the LMRT did not reach significance, indicating deficits in classification. Further, in this solution one class consisted of less than 10% of our sample which limits our confidence regarding the replicability of these results. As entropy was good in the two-group solution and LMR as well as BLRT were also significant in this model, we chose two trajectory classes for further analysesa. Estimated path coefficients for the model with two classes can be seen in Figure 1.
Table 3
Fit indices for latent class growth analysis with neuroticism values at the three measurement points
Number of groups
BICa
Entropy
Lo-Mendell-Rubin likelihood ratio test
Bootstrapped parametric likelihood ratio test
pb
pb
2
1430.303
0.833
.0005
.0000
3
1413.441
0.750
.1119
.0000
4
1417.614
0.687
.0655
.0000
Notes. aBayesian Information Criterion; btesting if a model with k groups fits the data better than the model with k-1 groups; group sizes: 2 class solution: n1 = 205, n2 = 61; 3 class solution: n1 = 162, n2 = 23, n3 = 81; 4 class solution: n1 = 48, n2 = 113, n3 = 98, n4 = 7.
In both groups, neuroticism significantly changed from T0 to T2 with a peak at T1. However, the groups differed regarding absolute neuroticism levels. The majority of participants (77.1%; mean age T0 14.75, SD = 2.20; mean age T1 19.20, SD = 2.26; mean age T2 24.73, SD = 2.31; 51.0% female) showed a pattern of moderate neuroticism values. Thus, this group was labelled “neuroticism (N) moderate” (mean intercept 2.084, SE = 0.362, p ≤ .001; mean slope 1.380, SE = 0.309, p ≤ .001). Individuals clustered into the second group (22.9%; mean age T0 15.75, SD 2.04; mean age T1 19.89, SD 1.76, mean age T2 25.26, SD 1.96; 77.0% female) showed higher neuroticism levels (mean intercept 2.844, SE = 0.428, p ≤. 001) as well as slightly greater change over ten years (mean slope 1.666, SE = 0.377, p ≤. 001). This group was named “N high”. Neuroticism means for the trajectory groups are visualized in Figure 2.
Repeated measures analysis of variances revealed a significant main effect for trajectory group (F = 132.01, p ≤. 001, effect size partial eta squared (ƞP 2) = .33) as well as a significant interaction effect of group x time (F = 31.59, p ≤ .001, ƞP 2 = .11) on general psychopathology. The main effect for time did not yield significance (F = 0.21, p = .644, ƞP 2 = .001). Across the three assessments, the group “N high” showed higher psychopathological burden than individuals with stable moderate neuroticism values (see Figure 2). Further, in individuals with high neuroticism, psychopathology slightly increased in our ten year period whereas it decreased in the “N moderate” group.
The results of survival analyses regarding the lifetime prevalences of depressive and anxiety disorders for groups can be seen in the morbidity curves in Figure 3. Curves differed significantly between groups (depression χ 2 = 41.44, df = 1, p ≤ .001; anxiety χ 2 = 28.84, df = 1, p ≤ .001) with elevated prevalences in the “N high” group. The gap between groups widened with increasing age. The estimated course shows that at an age of 28 or older nearly every person in the “N high” group suffered from depression or anxiety disorders at least once during their lives. Further, logistic regression analyses predicting current diagnoses at T2 revealed a 14-fold increased risk for depressive disorders in the “N high” compared to the “N moderate” group (β = 2.64, SE = 0.52, p ≤ .001, OR 14.00, confidence interval (CI) 5.08 - 38.34) controlled for internalizing symptoms at T0 (regression without trajectory group: β = 0.08, SE = 0.04, p = .035; regression with trajectory group: β = -0.02, SE = 0.05, p = .669). Regarding anxiety disorders a 7-fold risk was found for this high group (β = 1.92, SE = 0.74, p ≤. 01, OR 6.84, confidence interval (CI) 1.61 - 29.07; coefficients for the control variable internalizing symptoms at T0: without trajectory group: β = 0.17, SE = 0.05, p ≤ .001; regression with trajectory group: β = 0.11, SE = 0.05, p = .035).
In the next step, we focused on information about emotional experience in everyday life as assessed by EMA. Completion rate was 88% with a mean of 18 answered calls. Data were analysed if at least 50% of calls were answered resulting in a sample of 208 individuals (“N moderate”: 162; “N high”: 46). A MANOVA revealed significant group differences in the global emotional indices valence (F = 17.54, p ≤. 001, ƞP 2 = .08) and arousal (F = 15.57, p ≤ .001, ƞP 2 = .07). Individuals with a neuroticism course on a moderate level felt better and were more relaxed during a two week period than individuals whose neuroticism values were higher (see Figure 4).
Further, exploratory factor analysis was conducted with ratings of specific emotions. Here, a two factor solution emerged. The first factor consisted of ratings for sadness, disgust, anxiety, anger, shame and boredom, and accounted for 57% of variance. This factor was labelled “negative affect”. Happiness and interest ratings constituted a second factor which accounted for 20% of variance and was named “positive affect”. As can also be seen in Figure 3, groups differed significantly in negative (F = 10.71, p ≤ .001, ƞP 2 = .05) and in positive affect (F = 10.39, p ≤ .001, ƞP 2 = .05). Individuals with high neuroticism values experienced more negative and less positive affect in everyday life than the “N moderate” group. According to the results of the factor analysis, the MSSD was calculated separately for positive and negative affect. However, groups did not differ regarding emotional instability, neither in positive (F = 0.17, p = .682, ƞP 2 = .001) nor in negative affect (F = 0.01, p = .929, ƞP 2 = .000).

Discussion

In this study we longitudinally examined differential developmental courses of neuroticism from adolescence to young adulthood, and their association to psychopathology and emotional experience in a general population sample. In particular, various levels of psychopathology were assessed using self-reports, structured clinical interviews, and an ecological momentary assessment approach.
As hypothesized, neuroticism course was not uniform across individuals in our general population sample. Instead, over a period of ten years, two different forms of neuroticism development were revealed. In both groups, neuroticism was not stable, but changed from adolescence to young adulthood as indicated by the significant slope factors. Interestingly, the shape of the neuroticism course was similar in both groups with a peak around the age of 20. In contrast, courses differed regarding absolute neuroticism level. The majority of individuals showed neuroticism values on a moderate absolute level. However, there was also a group with higher absolute neuroticism levels. This higher pattern was associated with an elevated level of psychopathology from adolescence to young adulthood. Further, individuals who stood out from the masses by being more timid, nervous and emotionally unstable were at 14-fold increased risk for developing depressive and at 7-fold risk for anxiety disorders compared to persons with moderate neuroticism levels. These results are in line with studies associating lower levels of neuroticism with positive outcomes such as life satisfaction [80] or subjective well-being [81].
According to the maturity principle [13], a decrease of neuroticism values would have been expected in the majority of individuals. This pattern could not be observed in our data. Instead, neuroticism increased from 15 to 20 and decreased afterwards in both groups. This pattern might be explained by the model of Ormel et al. [82]. Here, the authors provide evidence that personality development bases on two factors: on the one hand, there is an individually fixed set point. On the other hand, there are experience-dependent alterations in personality. Hence, it can be assumed that in line with personality models some individuals do have a higher neuroticism set point than others [5]. At the same time, the age of 20 reflects an important developmental step associated with experiences that potentially lead to an increase in neuroticism values. It is easily imaginable that moving out, finding a job and perhaps starting an own family can fuel fears, worries and negative emotions. Perhaps, five years later individuals become more settled which is reflected in a decrease of neuroticism values back to the respective set point. This assumption is in line with findings showing a decrease in neuroticism between 20 and 40 [14]. In fact, lots of studies reporting decreases in neuroticism assess individuals older than 18 years [23],[83]. Hence, it seems plausible that hypothesized personality maturation is just about to start in our sample. Of course, this must be clarified in future studies which should also account for potentially different paces of such maturation processes.
In parallel to the neuroticism course, morbidity rates of anxiety and depressive disorders strongly rose from T0 to T1 but the increase slowed down from T1 to T2, particularly in the “N high” group. This is in line with other studies showing increases in depressive symptoms starting in the ages between 12 and 14 [84],[85]. Hence, our data might suggest that in parallel with increasing neuroticism values from T0 to T1, depressive and anxiety symptoms reach the threshold of diagnoses at the age of 20, particularly in individuals with a higher absolute level of neuroticism. However, it could be argued, that these associations result from the conceptual overlap of neuroticism and psychopathology measures [86]. Yet, there is evidence suggesting that content overlap is not the main explanation for associations between neuroticism and depression/anxiety [9]. Instead, neuroticism seems to reflect more than depressive and anxious symptoms, as a general neuroticism factor including all of its facets is a better predictor for depression and anxiety than the disorder-specific subscales [9]. Further, general psychopathology measures also include externalizing symptomatology and thus are supposed to be sufficiently distinct from neuroticism. Nevertheless, it is possible that the strength of the association is a little overestimated. Hence, in line with Nicholls et al. [87], we decided not to exclude overlapping items but to include a variety of outcome assessment methods to account for potential conceptual overlap.
Further, it could be assumed that retrospective recall of symptoms and personality is biased by current psychopathology and mood [88],[89]. Therefore, we additionally used EMA to assess implications of neuroticism developmental groups. This method minimizes recall biases and other systematic distortions, as individuals spontaneously indicate their current emotional experience at multiple random assessment points. However, this method was only applicable at T2, so recall biases at T0 and T1 cannot be ruled out.
Using EMA at T2, we found high neuroticism course from adolescence to adulthood to be associated with increased negative affect and arousal at the age of 25. In addition, levels of positive affect were reduced. This is of particular importance, as there is evidence that negative emotions in everyday life are associated with various adverse outcomes, such as an increased vulnerability for depression [90], smoking relapse [91], or binge eating [92], for instance. Further, Wichers et al. [93] found positive emotions in everyday life to buffer the disadvantageous effects of stress on depression development. Hence, the high neuroticism group is affected in two ways: first, by its increased negative affectivity, and second, through the lack of possibly protecting positive emotions. In sum, it can be assumed that alterations in emotional experience constitute a mechanism relating neuroticism development to psychopathology. This idea needs clarification in future research.
Interestingly, no group differences emerged regarding emotional instability. This is in contrast to other studies reporting significant associations between neuroticism and affect instability [41],[46],[94]. However, these studies did not test the influence of longitudinal neuroticism courses on emotional instability, but assessed or averaged concurrent neuroticism levels instead. Another methodological explanation for these inconsistent findings is provided in a recent study of Koval, Pe, Meers, and Kuppens [31]. They argue that overlap in conceptualizations (variability, instability, inertia) and measures (SD, MSSD, autocorrelation) of affect dynamics account for inconsistencies in results (in their case regarding depression). Thus, it would be interesting to test whether neuroticism courses differentially influence diverse measures of affect dynamics in future studies.
Our results have to be interpreted in the light of several limitations. Although data were collected longitudinally, causal statements cannot be made. Whereas in our argumentation the developmental course of neuroticism is interpreted as risk factor for different negative outcomes, it is also plausible that differences in mental stress influence personality (for review see [95]). For instance, evidence is inconsistent regarding depression: whereas the vulnerability hypothesis states that personality constitutes a risk factor for depressive disorders [96],[97] the scar hypothesis arguments that an episode of depression leads to alterations in personality [98]. In our study we found evidence for the vulnerability hypothesis, as a neuroticism course with high absolute levels led to an increased risk of depression in adulthood. However, it would be promising to examine whether previous depressive episodes influenced personality development in a future study.
Further, it is also imaginable that third factors like a family history of mental illness [99], treatment experiences [100], significant life events [101] or traumas [102] influence the associations between neuroticism course and mental health. Hence, such mechanisms should be considered in further research. In addition, the concurrent assessment of personality and psychopathology might lead to mood-state distortions [103]. Hence, for the future it might be promising to assess personality and psychopathology at different time points controlling for current mood-state.
Moreover, in our EMA-design we did not capture the context in which emotions were experienced. However, there is increasing evidence emphasizing the importance of context-specific information on emotions [104],[105]. Therefore including a few questions on the activities, stressors and interactions partners in everyday situations might help to get a more detailed insight in the emotionality of individuals at risk for depression and anxiety disorders. However, EMA research is still at its very beginning and our results give a first idea of the association between trait affectivity and affective experience in everyday life.
Further, in this study we focused on the personality trait neuroticism, as this trait is frequently examined in the context of internalizing psychopathology [106],[107]. Of course, the development of other traits such as extraversion or impulsivity would also be interesting as these traits are discussed as risk factors of mental disorders, too [8],[108]. In addition, future research should examine the association between the development of trait combinations in terms of personality profiles and psychopathology.
Methodologically, varying assessment methods for neuroticism and general psychopathology were used at the different measurement points. This adaption was inevitable due to age-specific application of the questionnaires. However, we included age as a covariate in our models to account for possible assessment effects. Still, method-specific biases cannot completely be ruled out. Thus, overcoming measurement problems is a major challenge for future research on personality development in the transition from adolescence to adulthood.
Nevertheless, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study differentiating courses of neuroticism development in this phase of life and longitudinally linking them to different forms and severities of psychological impairment. Therefore, a multimethod approach with self-reports, interview data and ecological momentary assessment was used. Further, our data were collected in both sexes in the general population instead of patient samples or college students, thus enhancing the generalizability of our findings.

Conclusions

This study highlights that neuroticism changes in the transition from adolescence to young adulthood. However, personality maturation as indicated by a decrease of neuroticism could not be observed. Instead, neuroticism peaked at the age of 20. Interestingly, this form of development was similar across individuals. However, the absolute neuroticism level strongly differed between two groups. Over 20% of our participants showed elevated neuroticism levels over all assessments which were associated with adverse outcomes such as negatively toned emotional experience, increased general psychopathology over ten years, and a heightened risk to suffer from depression and anxiety disorders in young adulthood. Thus, these high-risk persons need to be identified early to be able to provide individually suited interventions to support continuous personality maturation. At the same time, the assessment of possible negative outcomes needs to be refined in order to detect specific patterns increasing the risk for mental disorders, such as increased experience of negative emotions in everyday life.

Endnote

aWe also tested a model including a dummy-coded control variable, indicating whether J-TCI or NEO was used to assess neuroticism at T0 instead of age (results not shown). Here, a similar 2-class-solution was obtained and further results were comparable. As it was age-dependent whether the J-TCI or the NEO was used, age and the control variable were highly interrelated (r = 0.862, p ≤ .001). Hence, we decided to display the age-controlled model only.

Authors’ contributions

MA made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the study, acquisition, statistical analyses and interpretation of the data (particularly EMA), and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MS made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the study as well as to statistical analyses and revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. IU made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the study, and revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. KA made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the study, had the lead in diagnostics and revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. ER made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the study, and revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. SW made substantial contributions to the acquisition of data, and revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. HJG made substantial contributions to the acquisition of data, and revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. SL revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. SB made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the study, the analysis and interpretation of the data, and revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgments

The Greifswald family study was supported by the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, BA-2172/2-1, BA2172.8-1.GR1912.7-1). Further, we acknowledge financial support by the German Research Foundation and the Ruprecht-Karls-University Heidelberg within the funding program Open Access Publishing.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Competing interests

All authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Anhänge

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D: Linking “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2010, 136: 768-821.PubMed Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D: Linking “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2010, 136: 768-821.PubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Barnow S, Spitzer C, Grabe HJ, Kessler C, Freyberger HJ: Individual characteristics, familial experience, and psychopathology in children of mothers with borderline personality disorder. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 2006, 45: 965-972. Barnow S, Spitzer C, Grabe HJ, Kessler C, Freyberger HJ: Individual characteristics, familial experience, and psychopathology in children of mothers with borderline personality disorder. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 2006, 45: 965-972.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Sandahl C, Lindberg S, Bergman H: The relation between drinking habits and neuroticism and weak ego among male and female alcoholic patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1987, 75: 500-508.PubMed Sandahl C, Lindberg S, Bergman H: The relation between drinking habits and neuroticism and weak ego among male and female alcoholic patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1987, 75: 500-508.PubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Cervera S, Lahortiga F, Martínez-González MA, Gual P, de Irala-Estévez J, Alonso Y: Neuroticism and low self-esteem as risk factors for incident eating disorders in a prospective cohort study. Int J Eat Disord. 2003, 33: 271-280.PubMed Cervera S, Lahortiga F, Martínez-González MA, Gual P, de Irala-Estévez J, Alonso Y: Neuroticism and low self-esteem as risk factors for incident eating disorders in a prospective cohort study. Int J Eat Disord. 2003, 33: 271-280.PubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Eysenck HJ, Eysenck MW: Personality and individual differences. 1985, Plenum Press, New York Eysenck HJ, Eysenck MW: Personality and individual differences. 1985, Plenum Press, New York
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Dunkley DM, Sanislow CA, Grilo CM, McGlashan TH: Self-criticism versus neuroticism in predicting depression and psychosocial impairment for 4 years in a clinical sample. Compr Psychiatry. 2009, 50: 335-346.PubMed Dunkley DM, Sanislow CA, Grilo CM, McGlashan TH: Self-criticism versus neuroticism in predicting depression and psychosocial impairment for 4 years in a clinical sample. Compr Psychiatry. 2009, 50: 335-346.PubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Kendler KS, Gatz M, Gardner CO, Pedersen NL: Personality and major depression: a Swedish longitudinal, population-based twin study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006, 63: 1113-1120.PubMed Kendler KS, Gatz M, Gardner CO, Pedersen NL: Personality and major depression: a Swedish longitudinal, population-based twin study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006, 63: 1113-1120.PubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Jylhä P, Isometsä E: The relationship of neuroticism and extraversion to symptoms of anxiety and depression in the general population. Depress Anxiety. 2006, 23: 281-289.PubMed Jylhä P, Isometsä E: The relationship of neuroticism and extraversion to symptoms of anxiety and depression in the general population. Depress Anxiety. 2006, 23: 281-289.PubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Uliaszek AA, Hauner KK, Zinbarg RE, Craske MG, Mineka S, Griffith JW, Rose RD: An examination of content overlap and disorder-specific predictions in the Associations of Neuroticism with Anxiety and Depression. J Res Pers. 2009, 43: 785-794.PubMedPubMedCentral Uliaszek AA, Hauner KK, Zinbarg RE, Craske MG, Mineka S, Griffith JW, Rose RD: An examination of content overlap and disorder-specific predictions in the Associations of Neuroticism with Anxiety and Depression. J Res Pers. 2009, 43: 785-794.PubMedPubMedCentral
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Eysenck HJ: The Structure of Human Personality. 1953, Methuen, London Eysenck HJ: The Structure of Human Personality. 1953, Methuen, London
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR: A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993, 50: 975-990.PubMed Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR: A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993, 50: 975-990.PubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Roberts BW, DelVecchio WF: The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2000, 126: 3-25.PubMed Roberts BW, DelVecchio WF: The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2000, 126: 3-25.PubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Caspi A, Roberts BW, Shiner RL: Personality development: stability and change. Annu Rev Psychol. 2005, 56: 453-484.PubMed Caspi A, Roberts BW, Shiner RL: Personality development: stability and change. Annu Rev Psychol. 2005, 56: 453-484.PubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W: Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2006, 132: 1-25.PubMed Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W: Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2006, 132: 1-25.PubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Terracciano A, McCrae RR, Costa PT: Intra-individual change in personality stability and age. J Res Pers. 2010, 44: 31-37.PubMedPubMedCentral Terracciano A, McCrae RR, Costa PT: Intra-individual change in personality stability and age. J Res Pers. 2010, 44: 31-37.PubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Specht J, Egloff B, Schmukle SC: Stability and change of personality across the life course: the impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the big five. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011, 101: 862-882.PubMed Specht J, Egloff B, Schmukle SC: Stability and change of personality across the life course: the impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the big five. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011, 101: 862-882.PubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Littlefield AK, Sher KJ, Wood PK: Is “maturing out” of problematic alcohol involvement related to personality change?. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009, 118: 360-374.PubMedPubMedCentral Littlefield AK, Sher KJ, Wood PK: Is “maturing out” of problematic alcohol involvement related to personality change?. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009, 118: 360-374.PubMedPubMedCentral
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Roberts BW, Robins RW, Caspi A, Trzesniewski KH: Personality Trait Development in Adulthood. Handbook of the Life Course. Edited by: Mortimer J, Shanahan M. 2003, Kluwer, New York Roberts BW, Robins RW, Caspi A, Trzesniewski KH: Personality Trait Development in Adulthood. Handbook of the Life Course. Edited by: Mortimer J, Shanahan M. 2003, Kluwer, New York
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Roberts BW, Wood D: Personality Development in the Context of the neo-Socioanalytical Model of Personality. Handbook of Personality Development. Edited by: Mroczek D, Little TD. 2006, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, New Jersey Roberts BW, Wood D: Personality Development in the Context of the neo-Socioanalytical Model of Personality. Handbook of Personality Development. Edited by: Mroczek D, Little TD. 2006, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, New Jersey
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Klimstra TA, Luyckx K, Hale WW, Goossens L, Meeus WHJ: Longitudinal associations between personality profile stability and adjustment in college students: distinguishing among overall stability, distinctive stability, and within-time normativeness. J Pers. 2010, 78: 1163-1184.PubMed Klimstra TA, Luyckx K, Hale WW, Goossens L, Meeus WHJ: Longitudinal associations between personality profile stability and adjustment in college students: distinguishing among overall stability, distinctive stability, and within-time normativeness. J Pers. 2010, 78: 1163-1184.PubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat De Bolle M, Beyers W, De Clercq B, De Fruyt F: General personality and psychopathology in referred and nonreferred children and adolescents: an investigation of continuity, pathoplasty, and complication models. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012, 121: 958-970.PubMed De Bolle M, Beyers W, De Clercq B, De Fruyt F: General personality and psychopathology in referred and nonreferred children and adolescents: an investigation of continuity, pathoplasty, and complication models. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012, 121: 958-970.PubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Robins RW, Fraley RC, Roberts BW, Trzesniewski KH: A longitudinal study of personality change in young adulthood. J Pers. 2001, 69: 617-640.PubMed Robins RW, Fraley RC, Roberts BW, Trzesniewski KH: A longitudinal study of personality change in young adulthood. J Pers. 2001, 69: 617-640.PubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson W, Hicks BM, McGue M, Iacono WG: Most of the girls are alright, but some aren’t: personality trajectory groups from ages 14 to 24 and some associations with outcomes. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007, 93: 266-284.PubMed Johnson W, Hicks BM, McGue M, Iacono WG: Most of the girls are alright, but some aren’t: personality trajectory groups from ages 14 to 24 and some associations with outcomes. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007, 93: 266-284.PubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Aluja A, Blanch A: The five and seven factors personality models: differences and similitude between the TCI-R, NEO-FFI-R and ZKPQ-50-CC. Span J Psychol. 2011, 14: 659-666.PubMed Aluja A, Blanch A: The five and seven factors personality models: differences and similitude between the TCI-R, NEO-FFI-R and ZKPQ-50-CC. Span J Psychol. 2011, 14: 659-666.PubMed
26.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Ben-Zeev D, Young MA, Madsen JW: Retrospective recall of affect in clinically depressed individuals and controls. Cognition Emotion. 2009, 23: 1021-1040. Ben-Zeev D, Young MA, Madsen JW: Retrospective recall of affect in clinically depressed individuals and controls. Cognition Emotion. 2009, 23: 1021-1040.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Stone AA, Shiffman S: Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in behavorial medicine. Ann Behav Med. 1994, 16: 199-202. Stone AA, Shiffman S: Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in behavorial medicine. Ann Behav Med. 1994, 16: 199-202.
29.
Zurück zum Zitat W: International Classification of Disorders, ICD-10. 1991, Huber, Bern W: International Classification of Disorders, ICD-10. 1991, Huber, Bern
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Trull TJ, Ebner-Priemer UW: Using experience sampling methods/ecological momentary assessment (ESM/EMA) in clinical assessment and clinical research: introduction to the special section. Psychol Assess. 2009, 21: 457-462.PubMedPubMedCentral Trull TJ, Ebner-Priemer UW: Using experience sampling methods/ecological momentary assessment (ESM/EMA) in clinical assessment and clinical research: introduction to the special section. Psychol Assess. 2009, 21: 457-462.PubMedPubMedCentral
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Koval P, Pe ML, Meers K, Kuppens P: Affect dynamics in relation to depressive symptoms: variable, unstable or inert?. Emotion. 2013, 13: 1132-1141.PubMed Koval P, Pe ML, Meers K, Kuppens P: Affect dynamics in relation to depressive symptoms: variable, unstable or inert?. Emotion. 2013, 13: 1132-1141.PubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Thompson RJ, Mata J, Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Gotlib IH: The everyday emotional experience of adults with major depressive disorder: examining emotional instability, inertia, and reactivity. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012, 121: 819-829.PubMedPubMedCentral Thompson RJ, Mata J, Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Gotlib IH: The everyday emotional experience of adults with major depressive disorder: examining emotional instability, inertia, and reactivity. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012, 121: 819-829.PubMedPubMedCentral
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Ebner-Priemer UW, Trull TJ: Ecological momentary assessment of mood disorders and mood dysregulation. Psychol Assess. 2009, 21: 463-475.PubMed Ebner-Priemer UW, Trull TJ: Ecological momentary assessment of mood disorders and mood dysregulation. Psychol Assess. 2009, 21: 463-475.PubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Telford C, McCarthy-Jones S, Corcoran R, Rowse G: Experience sampling methodology studies of depression: the state of the art. Psychol Med. 2012, 42: 1119-1129.PubMed Telford C, McCarthy-Jones S, Corcoran R, Rowse G: Experience sampling methodology studies of depression: the state of the art. Psychol Med. 2012, 42: 1119-1129.PubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Tong EM: Personality influences in appraisal-emotion relationships: the role of neuroticism. J Pers. 2010, 78: 393-417.PubMed Tong EM: Personality influences in appraisal-emotion relationships: the role of neuroticism. J Pers. 2010, 78: 393-417.PubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Ilies R, Judge TA: Understanding the dynamic relationships among personality, mood and job satisfaction: a field experience sampling study. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2002, 89: 1119-1139. Ilies R, Judge TA: Understanding the dynamic relationships among personality, mood and job satisfaction: a field experience sampling study. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2002, 89: 1119-1139.
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Bolger N, Schilling EA: Personality and the problems of everyday life: the role of neuroticism in exposure and reactivity to daily stressors. J Pers. 1991, 59: 355-386.PubMed Bolger N, Schilling EA: Personality and the problems of everyday life: the role of neuroticism in exposure and reactivity to daily stressors. J Pers. 1991, 59: 355-386.PubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Wichers M, Peeters F, Geschwind N, Jacobs N, Simons CJP, Derom C, Thiery E, Delespaul PH, van Os J: Unveiling patterns of affective responses in daily life may improve outcome prediction in depression: a momentary assessment study. J Affect Disord. 2010, 124: 191-195.PubMed Wichers M, Peeters F, Geschwind N, Jacobs N, Simons CJP, Derom C, Thiery E, Delespaul PH, van Os J: Unveiling patterns of affective responses in daily life may improve outcome prediction in depression: a momentary assessment study. J Affect Disord. 2010, 124: 191-195.PubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Wichers M, Peeters F, Rutten BPF, Jacobs N, Derom C, Thiery E, Delespaul P, van Os J: A time-lagged momentary assessment study on daily life physical activity and affect. Health Psychol. 2012, 31: 135-144.PubMed Wichers M, Peeters F, Rutten BPF, Jacobs N, Derom C, Thiery E, Delespaul P, van Os J: A time-lagged momentary assessment study on daily life physical activity and affect. Health Psychol. 2012, 31: 135-144.PubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Zautra AJ, Affleck GG, Tennen H, Reich JW, Davis MC: Dynamic approaches to emotions and stress in everyday life: Bolger and Zuckerman reloaded with positive as well as negative affects. J Pers. 2005, 73: 1511-1538.PubMedPubMedCentral Zautra AJ, Affleck GG, Tennen H, Reich JW, Davis MC: Dynamic approaches to emotions and stress in everyday life: Bolger and Zuckerman reloaded with positive as well as negative affects. J Pers. 2005, 73: 1511-1538.PubMedPubMedCentral
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller DJ, Vachon DD, Lynam DR: Neuroticism, negative affect, and negative affect instability: establishing convergent and discriminant validity using ecological momentary assessment. Pers Individ Differ. 2009, 47: 873-877. Miller DJ, Vachon DD, Lynam DR: Neuroticism, negative affect, and negative affect instability: establishing convergent and discriminant validity using ecological momentary assessment. Pers Individ Differ. 2009, 47: 873-877.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Schimmack U: Affect measurement in experience sampling research. J Happiness. 2003, 4: 79-106. Schimmack U: Affect measurement in experience sampling research. J Happiness. 2003, 4: 79-106.
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Howell RT, Rodzon KS: An exploration of personality–affect relations in daily life: determining the support for the affect-level and affect-reactivity views. Pers Individ Differ. 2011, 51: 797-801. Howell RT, Rodzon KS: An exploration of personality–affect relations in daily life: determining the support for the affect-level and affect-reactivity views. Pers Individ Differ. 2011, 51: 797-801.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat David JP, Green PJ, Martin R, Suls J: Differential roles of neuroticism, extraversion, and event desirability for mood in daily life: an integrative model of top-down and bottom-up influences. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997, 73: 149-159.PubMed David JP, Green PJ, Martin R, Suls J: Differential roles of neuroticism, extraversion, and event desirability for mood in daily life: an integrative model of top-down and bottom-up influences. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997, 73: 149-159.PubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Suls J, Martin R: The daily life of the garden-variety neurotic: reactivity, stressor exposure, mood spillover, and maladaptive coping. J Pers. 2005, 73: 1485-1510.PubMed Suls J, Martin R: The daily life of the garden-variety neurotic: reactivity, stressor exposure, mood spillover, and maladaptive coping. J Pers. 2005, 73: 1485-1510.PubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacobs N, van Os J, Derom C, Thiery E, Delespaul P, Wichers M: Neuroticism explained? From a non-informative vulnerability marker to informative person-context interactions in the realm of daily life. Br J Clin Psychol. 2011, 50: 19-32.PubMed Jacobs N, van Os J, Derom C, Thiery E, Delespaul P, Wichers M: Neuroticism explained? From a non-informative vulnerability marker to informative person-context interactions in the realm of daily life. Br J Clin Psychol. 2011, 50: 19-32.PubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Barnow S, Aldinger M, Arens EA, Ulrich I, Spitzer C, Grabe HJ, Stopsack M: Maternal transmission of borderline personality disorder symptoms in the community-based Greifswald family study. J Pers Disord. 2012, 27: 806-819.PubMed Barnow S, Aldinger M, Arens EA, Ulrich I, Spitzer C, Grabe HJ, Stopsack M: Maternal transmission of borderline personality disorder symptoms in the community-based Greifswald family study. J Pers Disord. 2012, 27: 806-819.PubMed
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Barnow S, Ulrich I, Grabe HJ, Freyberger HJ, Spitzer C: The influence of parental drinking behaviour and antisocial personality disorder on adolescent behavioural problems: results of the Greifswalder family study. Alcohol Alcohol. 2007, 42: 623-628.PubMed Barnow S, Ulrich I, Grabe HJ, Freyberger HJ, Spitzer C: The influence of parental drinking behaviour and antisocial personality disorder on adolescent behavioural problems: results of the Greifswalder family study. Alcohol Alcohol. 2007, 42: 623-628.PubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Costa PTJ, McCrae RR: Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO-Five-Factor-Inventory (NEO-FFI): professional manual. 1992, Psychological Assessment Ressources, Odessa Costa PTJ, McCrae RR: Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO-Five-Factor-Inventory (NEO-FFI): professional manual. 1992, Psychological Assessment Ressources, Odessa
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Borkenau P, Ostendorf F: NEO-Fünf-Faktoren Inventar (NEO-FFI). 1993, Hogrefe Verlag, Göttingen Borkenau P, Ostendorf F: NEO-Fünf-Faktoren Inventar (NEO-FFI). 1993, Hogrefe Verlag, Göttingen
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Ostendorf F, Angleitner A: NEO-Persönlichkeitsinventar nach Costa und McCrae, Revidierte Fassung (NEO-PI-R). Manual. 2003, Hogrefe, Göttingen Ostendorf F, Angleitner A: NEO-Persönlichkeitsinventar nach Costa und McCrae, Revidierte Fassung (NEO-PI-R). Manual. 2003, Hogrefe, Göttingen
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Lüdtke O, Trautwein U, Nagy G, Köller O: Eine Validierungsstudie zum NEO-FFI in einer Stichprobe junger Erwachsener: effekte des Itemformats, faktorielle Validität und Zusammenhänge mit Schulleistungsindikatoren. Diagnostica. 2004, 50: 134-144. Lüdtke O, Trautwein U, Nagy G, Köller O: Eine Validierungsstudie zum NEO-FFI in einer Stichprobe junger Erwachsener: effekte des Itemformats, faktorielle Validität und Zusammenhänge mit Schulleistungsindikatoren. Diagnostica. 2004, 50: 134-144.
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Roth M: Überprüfung der Anwendbarkeit des NEO-Fünf-Faktoren Inventars (NEO-FFI) bei Jugendlichen im Alter zwischen 14 und 16 Jahren. Diagnostica. 2002, 48: 59-67. Roth M: Überprüfung der Anwendbarkeit des NEO-Fünf-Faktoren Inventars (NEO-FFI) bei Jugendlichen im Alter zwischen 14 und 16 Jahren. Diagnostica. 2002, 48: 59-67.
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Luby JL, Svrakic DM, McCallum K, Przybeck TR, Cloninger CR: The junior temperament and character inventory: preliminary validation of a child self-report measure. Psychol Rep. 1999, 84: 1127-1138.PubMed Luby JL, Svrakic DM, McCallum K, Przybeck TR, Cloninger CR: The junior temperament and character inventory: preliminary validation of a child self-report measure. Psychol Rep. 1999, 84: 1127-1138.PubMed
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM, Wetzel RD: The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A Guide to its Development and use. 1994, Washington University, St. Louis Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM, Wetzel RD: The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A Guide to its Development and use. 1994, Washington University, St. Louis
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Widiger TA, Simonsen E: Alternative dimensional models of personality disorder: finding a common ground. J Pers Disord. 2005, 19: 110-130.PubMed Widiger TA, Simonsen E: Alternative dimensional models of personality disorder: finding a common ground. J Pers Disord. 2005, 19: 110-130.PubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Stepp SD, Yu L, Miller JD, Hallquist MN, Trull TJ, Pilkonis PA: Integrating competing dimensional models of personality: linking the SNAP, TCI, and NEO using item response theory. Pers Disord. 2012, 3: 107-126. Stepp SD, Yu L, Miller JD, Hallquist MN, Trull TJ, Pilkonis PA: Integrating competing dimensional models of personality: linking the SNAP, TCI, and NEO using item response theory. Pers Disord. 2012, 3: 107-126.
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Wittchen HU, Lachner G, Wunderlich U, Pfister H: Test-retest reliability of the computerized DSM-IV version of the Munich-Composite International Diagnostic Interview (M-CIDI). Soc Psych Psych Epid. 1998, 33: 568-578. Wittchen HU, Lachner G, Wunderlich U, Pfister H: Test-retest reliability of the computerized DSM-IV version of the Munich-Composite International Diagnostic Interview (M-CIDI). Soc Psych Psych Epid. 1998, 33: 568-578.
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Wittchen HU, Semmler G: Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). 1990, Weinheim, Beltz Wittchen HU, Semmler G: Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). 1990, Weinheim, Beltz
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Unnewehr S, Schneider S, Margraf J: Kinder-DIPS: Diagnostisches Interview bei Psychischen Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter. 1995, Springer, Berlin Unnewehr S, Schneider S, Margraf J: Kinder-DIPS: Diagnostisches Interview bei Psychischen Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter. 1995, Springer, Berlin
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Döpfner M, Berner W, Lehmkuhl G: Handbuch: Fragebogen für Jugendliche. Forschungsergebnisse zur Deutschen Fassung des Youth Self Report (YSR) der Child Behavior Checklist. 1994, Arbeitsgruppe Kinder-, Jugend- und Familiendiagnostik (KJFD), Köln Döpfner M, Berner W, Lehmkuhl G: Handbuch: Fragebogen für Jugendliche. Forschungsergebnisse zur Deutschen Fassung des Youth Self Report (YSR) der Child Behavior Checklist. 1994, Arbeitsgruppe Kinder-, Jugend- und Familiendiagnostik (KJFD), Köln
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Achenbach TM: Child Behaviour Checklist and Related Instruments. The use of Psychological Testing for Treatment Planning and Outcome Assessment. Edited by: Maruish ME. 1994, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, 517-549. Achenbach TM: Child Behaviour Checklist and Related Instruments. The use of Psychological Testing for Treatment Planning and Outcome Assessment. Edited by: Maruish ME. 1994, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, 517-549.
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Derogatis LR: SCL-90-R, Administration, Scoring & Procedures Manual-I for the R(Evised) Version. 1977, John Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore Derogatis LR: SCL-90-R, Administration, Scoring & Procedures Manual-I for the R(Evised) Version. 1977, John Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Franke GH: SCL-90-R: Die Symptom-Checkliste von Derogatis: Deutsche Version. 1995, Beltz Test, Göttingen Franke GH: SCL-90-R: Die Symptom-Checkliste von Derogatis: Deutsche Version. 1995, Beltz Test, Göttingen
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Derogatis LR, Melisaratos N: The brief symptom inventory: an introductory report. Psychol Med. 1983, 13: 595-605.PubMed Derogatis LR, Melisaratos N: The brief symptom inventory: an introductory report. Psychol Med. 1983, 13: 595-605.PubMed
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Prinz U, Nutzinger DO, Schulz H, Petermann F, Braukhaus C, Andreas D: The symptom-check-list-90-R (SCL-90-R) and the short versions of the SCL-90-R: psychometric analyses of inpatients with mental disorders. Phys Rehab Kur Med. 2008, 18: 337-343. Prinz U, Nutzinger DO, Schulz H, Petermann F, Braukhaus C, Andreas D: The symptom-check-list-90-R (SCL-90-R) and the short versions of the SCL-90-R: psychometric analyses of inpatients with mental disorders. Phys Rehab Kur Med. 2008, 18: 337-343.
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Hessel A, Schumacher J, Geyer M, Brähler E: Symptom-checklist SCL-90-R: validation and standardization based on a representative sample of the German population. Diagnostica. 2001, 47: 27-39. Hessel A, Schumacher J, Geyer M, Brähler E: Symptom-checklist SCL-90-R: validation and standardization based on a representative sample of the German population. Diagnostica. 2001, 47: 27-39.
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Geisheim C, Hahlweg K, Fiegenbaum W, Frank M, Schröder B, von Witzleben I: Das Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) als Instrument zur Qualitätssicherung in der Psychotherapie. Diagnostica. 2002, 48: 28-36. Geisheim C, Hahlweg K, Fiegenbaum W, Frank M, Schröder B, von Witzleben I: Das Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) als Instrument zur Qualitätssicherung in der Psychotherapie. Diagnostica. 2002, 48: 28-36.
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Moskowitz DS, Young SN: Ecological momentary assessment: what it is and why it is a method of the future in clinical psychopharmacology. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2006, 31: 13-20.PubMedPubMedCentral Moskowitz DS, Young SN: Ecological momentary assessment: what it is and why it is a method of the future in clinical psychopharmacology. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2006, 31: 13-20.PubMedPubMedCentral
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Csikszentmihalyi M, Larson R: Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1987, 175: 526-536.PubMed Csikszentmihalyi M, Larson R: Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1987, 175: 526-536.PubMed
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Feldman BJ, Masyn KE, Conger RD: New approaches to studying problem behaviors: a comparison of methods for modeling longitudinal, categorical adolescent drinking data. Dev Psychol. 2009, 45: 652-676.PubMedPubMedCentral Feldman BJ, Masyn KE, Conger RD: New approaches to studying problem behaviors: a comparison of methods for modeling longitudinal, categorical adolescent drinking data. Dev Psychol. 2009, 45: 652-676.PubMedPubMedCentral
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Muthén L, Muthén B: Mplus user’s Guide. 1998-2010, Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles Muthén L, Muthén B: Mplus user’s Guide. 1998-2010, Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Jung T, Wickrama KAS: An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Soc Pers Psychol Compass. 2008, 2: 302-317. Jung T, Wickrama KAS: An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Soc Pers Psychol Compass. 2008, 2: 302-317.
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO: Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct Equ Model. 2007, 14: 535-569. Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO: Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct Equ Model. 2007, 14: 535-569.
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Lo Y, Mendell NR, Rubin DB: Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika. 2001, 88: 767-778. Lo Y, Mendell NR, Rubin DB: Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika. 2001, 88: 767-778.
76.
Zurück zum Zitat McLachlan G: On bootstrapping the likelihood ratio test statistic for the number of components in a normal mixture. Appl Stat. 1987, 36: 318-324. McLachlan G: On bootstrapping the likelihood ratio test statistic for the number of components in a normal mixture. Appl Stat. 1987, 36: 318-324.
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Clark SL: Mixture Modeling With Behavioral Data. 2010, University of California, Los Angeles Clark SL: Mixture Modeling With Behavioral Data. 2010, University of California, Los Angeles
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Ebner-Priemer UW, Eid M, Kleindienst N, Stabenow S, Trull TJ: Analytic strategies for understanding affective (in) stability and other dynamic processes in psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009, 118: 195-202.PubMed Ebner-Priemer UW, Eid M, Kleindienst N, Stabenow S, Trull TJ: Analytic strategies for understanding affective (in) stability and other dynamic processes in psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009, 118: 195-202.PubMed
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Jahng S, Wood PK, Trull TJ: Analysis of affective instability in ecological momentary assessment: indices using successive difference and group comparison via multilevel modeling. Psychol Metod. 2008, 13: 354-375. Jahng S, Wood PK, Trull TJ: Analysis of affective instability in ecological momentary assessment: indices using successive difference and group comparison via multilevel modeling. Psychol Metod. 2008, 13: 354-375.
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Schimmack U, Oishi S, Furr RM, Funder DC: Personality and life satisfaction: a facet-level analysis. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2004, 30: 1062-1075.PubMed Schimmack U, Oishi S, Furr RM, Funder DC: Personality and life satisfaction: a facet-level analysis. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2004, 30: 1062-1075.PubMed
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Vitterso J, Nilsen F: The conceptual and relational structure of subjective well-being, neuroticism, and extraversion: once again, neuroticism is the important predictor of happiness. Soc Ind Res. 2002, 57: 89-118. Vitterso J, Nilsen F: The conceptual and relational structure of subjective well-being, neuroticism, and extraversion: once again, neuroticism is the important predictor of happiness. Soc Ind Res. 2002, 57: 89-118.
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Ormel J, Riese H, Rosmalen JGM: Interpreting neuroticism scores across the adult life course: immutable or experience-dependent set points of negative affect?. Clin Psychol Rev. 2012, 32: 71-79.PubMed Ormel J, Riese H, Rosmalen JGM: Interpreting neuroticism scores across the adult life course: immutable or experience-dependent set points of negative affect?. Clin Psychol Rev. 2012, 32: 71-79.PubMed
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Roberts BW, Caspi A, Moffitt TE: The kids are alright: growth and stability in personality development from adolescence to adulthood. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001, 81: 670-683.PubMed Roberts BW, Caspi A, Moffitt TE: The kids are alright: growth and stability in personality development from adolescence to adulthood. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001, 81: 670-683.PubMed
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Twenge JM, Nolen-Hoeksema S: Age, gender, race, socioeconomic status, and birth cohort differences on the children’s depression inventory: a meta-analysis. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002, 111: 578-588.PubMed Twenge JM, Nolen-Hoeksema S: Age, gender, race, socioeconomic status, and birth cohort differences on the children’s depression inventory: a meta-analysis. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002, 111: 578-588.PubMed
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Garber J, Keiley MK, Martin C: Developmental trajectories of adolescents’ depressive symptoms: predictors of change. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2002, 70: 79-95.PubMed Garber J, Keiley MK, Martin C: Developmental trajectories of adolescents’ depressive symptoms: predictors of change. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2002, 70: 79-95.PubMed
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Ormel J, Rosmalen J, Farmer A: Neuroticism: a non-informative marker of vulnerability to psychopathology. Soc Psych Psych Epid. 2004, 39: 906-912. Ormel J, Rosmalen J, Farmer A: Neuroticism: a non-informative marker of vulnerability to psychopathology. Soc Psych Psych Epid. 2004, 39: 906-912.
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Nicholls JG, Licht BG, Pearl RA: Some dangers of using personality questionnaires to study personality. Psychol Bull. 1982, 92: 572-580. Nicholls JG, Licht BG, Pearl RA: Some dangers of using personality questionnaires to study personality. Psychol Bull. 1982, 92: 572-580.
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Howe ML, Malone C: Mood-congruent true and false memory: effects of depression. Memory. 2011, 19: 192-201.PubMed Howe ML, Malone C: Mood-congruent true and false memory: effects of depression. Memory. 2011, 19: 192-201.PubMed
89.
Zurück zum Zitat Koster EHW, De Raedt R, Leyman L, De Lissnyder E: Mood-congruent attention and memory bias in dysphoria: exploring the coherence among information-processing biases. Behav ResTher. 2010, 48: 219-225. Koster EHW, De Raedt R, Leyman L, De Lissnyder E: Mood-congruent attention and memory bias in dysphoria: exploring the coherence among information-processing biases. Behav ResTher. 2010, 48: 219-225.
90.
Zurück zum Zitat Wichers M, Myin-Germeys I, Jacobs N, Peeters F, Kenis G, Derom C, Vlietinck R, Delespaul P, Van Os J: Genetic risk of depression and stress-induced negative affect in daily life. Br J Psych. 2007, 191: 218-223. Wichers M, Myin-Germeys I, Jacobs N, Peeters F, Kenis G, Derom C, Vlietinck R, Delespaul P, Van Os J: Genetic risk of depression and stress-induced negative affect in daily life. Br J Psych. 2007, 191: 218-223.
91.
Zurück zum Zitat Shiffman S, Balabanis MH, Gwaltney CJ, Paty JA, Gnys M, Kassel JD, Hickcox M, Paton SM: Prediction of lapse from associations between smoking and situational antecedents assessed by ecological momentary assessment. Drug Alcohol Depen. 2007, 91: 159-168. Shiffman S, Balabanis MH, Gwaltney CJ, Paty JA, Gnys M, Kassel JD, Hickcox M, Paton SM: Prediction of lapse from associations between smoking and situational antecedents assessed by ecological momentary assessment. Drug Alcohol Depen. 2007, 91: 159-168.
92.
Zurück zum Zitat Wegner KE, Smyth JM, Crosby RD, Wittrock D, Wonderlich SA, Mitchell JE: An evaluation of the relationship between mood and binge eating in the natural environment using ecological momentary assessment. Int J Eat Disord. 2002, 32: 352-361.PubMed Wegner KE, Smyth JM, Crosby RD, Wittrock D, Wonderlich SA, Mitchell JE: An evaluation of the relationship between mood and binge eating in the natural environment using ecological momentary assessment. Int J Eat Disord. 2002, 32: 352-361.PubMed
93.
Zurück zum Zitat Wichers MC, Myin-Germeys I, Jacobs N, Peeters F, Kenis G, Derom C, Vlietinck R, Delespaul P, van Os J: Evidence that moment-to-moment variation in positive emotions buffer genetic risk for depression: a momentary assessment twin study. Acta Psych Scand. 2007, 115: 451-457. Wichers MC, Myin-Germeys I, Jacobs N, Peeters F, Kenis G, Derom C, Vlietinck R, Delespaul P, van Os J: Evidence that moment-to-moment variation in positive emotions buffer genetic risk for depression: a momentary assessment twin study. Acta Psych Scand. 2007, 115: 451-457.
94.
Zurück zum Zitat Murray G, Allen NB, Trinder J: Longitudinal investigation of mood variability and the ffm: neuroticism predicts variability in extended states of positive and negative affect. Pers Individ Differ. 2002, 33: 1217-1228. Murray G, Allen NB, Trinder J: Longitudinal investigation of mood variability and the ffm: neuroticism predicts variability in extended states of positive and negative affect. Pers Individ Differ. 2002, 33: 1217-1228.
95.
Zurück zum Zitat Klein DN, Kotov R, Bufferd SJ: Personality and depression: explanatory models and review of the evidence. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2011, 7: 269-295.PubMedPubMedCentral Klein DN, Kotov R, Bufferd SJ: Personality and depression: explanatory models and review of the evidence. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2011, 7: 269-295.PubMedPubMedCentral
96.
Zurück zum Zitat Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR: Can personality assessment predict future depression? A twelve-month follow-up of 631 subjects. J Affect Disord. 2006, 92: 35-44.PubMed Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR: Can personality assessment predict future depression? A twelve-month follow-up of 631 subjects. J Affect Disord. 2006, 92: 35-44.PubMed
97.
Zurück zum Zitat Weber K, Giannakopoulos P, Bacchetta J-P, Quast S, Herrmann F, Delaloye C, Ghisletta P, De Ribaupierre A, Canuto A: Personality traits are associated with acute major depression across the age spectrum. Aging Ment Health. 2012, 16: 472-480.PubMed Weber K, Giannakopoulos P, Bacchetta J-P, Quast S, Herrmann F, Delaloye C, Ghisletta P, De Ribaupierre A, Canuto A: Personality traits are associated with acute major depression across the age spectrum. Aging Ment Health. 2012, 16: 472-480.PubMed
98.
Zurück zum Zitat Akiskal HS, Hirschfeld RM, Yerevanian BI: The relationship of personality to affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983, 40: 801-810.PubMed Akiskal HS, Hirschfeld RM, Yerevanian BI: The relationship of personality to affective disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983, 40: 801-810.PubMed
99.
Zurück zum Zitat Ulrich I, Stopsack M, Spitzer C, Grabe HJ, Freyberger HJ, Barnow S: [Familial transmission of depression: the importance of harm avoidance]. Nervenarzt. 2011, 82: 1169-1177.PubMed Ulrich I, Stopsack M, Spitzer C, Grabe HJ, Freyberger HJ, Barnow S: [Familial transmission of depression: the importance of harm avoidance]. Nervenarzt. 2011, 82: 1169-1177.PubMed
100.
Zurück zum Zitat Jain U, Blais MA, Otto MW, Hirshfeld DR, Sachs GS: Five-factor personality traits in patients with seasonal depression: treatment effects and comparisons with bipolar patients. J Affect Disord. 1999, 55: 51-54.PubMed Jain U, Blais MA, Otto MW, Hirshfeld DR, Sachs GS: Five-factor personality traits in patients with seasonal depression: treatment effects and comparisons with bipolar patients. J Affect Disord. 1999, 55: 51-54.PubMed
101.
Zurück zum Zitat Kendler KS, Kuhn J, Prescott CA: The interrelationship of neuroticism, sex, and stressful life events in the prediction of episodes of major depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2004, 161: 631-636.PubMed Kendler KS, Kuhn J, Prescott CA: The interrelationship of neuroticism, sex, and stressful life events in the prediction of episodes of major depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2004, 161: 631-636.PubMed
102.
Zurück zum Zitat Roy A: Childhood trauma and neuroticism as an adult: possible implication for the development of the common psychiatric disorders and suicidal behaviour. Psychol Med. 2002, 32: 1471-1474.PubMed Roy A: Childhood trauma and neuroticism as an adult: possible implication for the development of the common psychiatric disorders and suicidal behaviour. Psychol Med. 2002, 32: 1471-1474.PubMed
103.
Zurück zum Zitat Blaney PH: Affect and memory: a review. Psychol Bull. 1986, 99: 229-246.PubMed Blaney PH: Affect and memory: a review. Psychol Bull. 1986, 99: 229-246.PubMed
104.
Zurück zum Zitat Coifman KG, Bonanno GA: When distress does not become depression: emotion context sensitivity and adjustment to bereavement. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010, 119: 479-490.PubMed Coifman KG, Bonanno GA: When distress does not become depression: emotion context sensitivity and adjustment to bereavement. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010, 119: 479-490.PubMed
105.
Zurück zum Zitat Aldao A: The future of emotion regulation research: capturing context. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2013, 8: 155-172.PubMed Aldao A: The future of emotion regulation research: capturing context. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2013, 8: 155-172.PubMed
106.
Zurück zum Zitat Clark LA, Watson D, Mineka S: Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994, 103: 103-116.PubMed Clark LA, Watson D, Mineka S: Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994, 103: 103-116.PubMed
107.
Zurück zum Zitat Farmer A, Redman K, Harris T, Mahmood A, Sadler S, Pickering A, McGuffin P: Neuroticism, extraversion, life events and depression. The Cardiff depression study. Brit J Psych. 2002, 181: 118-122. Farmer A, Redman K, Harris T, Mahmood A, Sadler S, Pickering A, McGuffin P: Neuroticism, extraversion, life events and depression. The Cardiff depression study. Brit J Psych. 2002, 181: 118-122.
108.
Zurück zum Zitat Lejuez CW, Magidson JF, Mitchell SH, Sinha R, Stevens MC, De Wit H: Behavioral and biological indicators of impulsivity in the development of alcohol use, problems, and disorders. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2010, 34: 1334-1345.PubMedPubMedCentral Lejuez CW, Magidson JF, Mitchell SH, Sinha R, Stevens MC, De Wit H: Behavioral and biological indicators of impulsivity in the development of alcohol use, problems, and disorders. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2010, 34: 1334-1345.PubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Neuroticism developmental courses - implications for depression, anxiety and everyday emotional experience; a prospective study from adolescence to young adulthood
verfasst von
Maren Aldinger
Malte Stopsack
Ines Ulrich
Katja Appel
Eva Reinelt
Sebastian Wolff
Hans Jörgen Grabe
Simone Lang
Sven Barnow
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2014
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Psychiatry / Ausgabe 1/2014
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-244X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-014-0210-2

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2014

BMC Psychiatry 1/2014 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).