Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics 1/2019

Open Access 01.12.2019 | Research article

Motor and cognitive functioning in children treated for idiopathic clubfoot at the age of 3 years

verfasst von: Julia Dillmann, Gudrun Schwarzer, Christian-Dominik Peterlein

Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2019

Abstract

Background

Several studies have investigated motor and cognitive skills in infants as well as gross motor abilities in schoolchildren treated for congenital idiopathic clubfoot, mostly indicating specific impairments in those children. However, until now, little is known about the motor and cognitive abilities of preschool children treated for idiopathic clubfoot. Thus, it was the aim of this study to examine gross motor, fine motor and cognitive skills of 3-year-old-children treated for idiopathic clubfoot.

Method

We tested gross motor, fine motor and cognitive functioning of 10 children treated for idiopathic clubfoot and 10 typically developing children at the age of 40 months (SD = 1) with the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development.

Results

The children treated for idiopathic clubfoot showed a slight delay in gross motor development. In particular, they demonstrated difficulties in tiptoeing, walking upstairs and walking downstairs. Moreover, we found some slight deficits in cognitive development, particularly in visual-spatial memory.

Discussion

Children treated for idiopathic clubfoot appear to have an increased risk of gross motor and spatial cognitive deficits. Orthopedic pediatrics should incorporate measures of gross motor functioning, for example tiptoeing, in their orthopedic setting. Moreover, future studies are needed to clarify whether the observed deficits persist through childhood. If so, some kind of a motor training for children with idiopathic clubfoot might be required.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Background

Congenital idiopathic clubfoot, an isolated birth defect with multifactorial etiology [14] has a prevalence in Central Europe and North America of 1–2 in 1000 newborns [5, 6]. For approximately 50 %, idiopathic clubfoot is bilateral and males are affected more frequently than females [6, 7]. Typically, the foot is rotated and characterized by midfoot cavus, forefoot adductus and hindfoot equinovarus. Mostly, idiopathic clubfoot is corrected using the Ponseti method [8, 9], a predominantly conservative treatment with little operative interventions, good functional results and a relapse rate that is comparable to that of other treatments [1013]. With the goal of sufficient foot mobility, treatment should start as soon as possible after birth. The Ponseti method consists of weekly manipulation and casting, percutaneous Achilles tenotomy (90% of patients) and foot abduction bracing [8, 9]. Previous studies in infants treated for idiopathic clubfoot revealed some gross motor impairments, especially in the second part of the first year of life, particularly in crawling and walking [1417]. Additionally, there seems to be some slight deficits in specific cognitive tasks, including problem solving and spatial cognition in clubfoot infants [14], that parallels the corresponding link between motor and cognitive development in healthy infants [1821] and infants with locomotor delay due to spina bifida [22]. Moreover, studies in older children treated for idiopathic clubfoot demonstrated an increased risk of motor activity limitations [23], gross motor deficits and asymmetrics [24] as well as a slightly decreased walking capacity [25]. In contrast, another study did not find any impaired athletic abilities in school-age children after satisfactory treatment of congenital clubfoot [26]. However, no studies have been made on motor and cognitive abilities in children treated for idiopathic clubfoot at the age of 3 years. Previous studies primary focused on motor development of infants or school-age children with mixed results regarding the long-term recovery from the Ponseti-treatment during infancy. Hence, the aims of this study were to investigate both motor and cognitive abilities among 3-year-old children treated for idiopathic clubfoot with the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, third edition [27].
Based on previous studies [1417], we expected that children treated for idiopathic clubfoot would be delayed in gross motor development but we did not expect to find any impact on fine motor development. According to a previous study [23], we did not expect any gross motor differences between children with unilateral and children with bilateral clubfoot. Following previous studies with children treated for spina bifida [22, 28], we expected that children treated for idiopathic clubfoot could show some deficits in spatial cognitive functioning.

Method

Ethics statement

The present study was conducted in full accordance with the Research Ethics Guidelines of the German Psychological Society (DGPs). The Office of Research Ethics at the University of Giessen approved the experimental procedure and the informed consent protocol. Moreover, all parents provided written informed consent prior to the first testing of their children.

Participants

A total of 20 children participated in our study. All tested children were born at term and from middle class families. Ten children (1 female, 9 males), treated for congenital idiopathic clubfoot (6 unilateral, 4 bilateral), were matched concerning age and gender with 10 typically developing children. We had to exclude two additional clubfoot children from the study because they were born prior to the 37th week of pregnancy. Additionally, we had to exclude two clubfoot children because of missed appointments. The mean age of the clubfoot group was 40 months (range: 39–41, SD = 1). Those children had no further impairments and were recruited from the Department of Orthopaedics at the university hospital of Giessen and Marburg, Germany, where the clubfeet were treated according to the Ponseti method. The treatment of all participants with idiopathic clubfoot was the same, including weekly manipulation and casting (mean number of casts M = 5.44, SD = 1.59), an Achilles tendon tenotomy and foot abduction bracing. All patients had to wear the foot abduction orthosis full time during their first months of life, gradually reduced to only nighttime. At the age of 3 years, none of the patients had any relapse and all of them used braces at night. We randomly chose for every child treated for idiopathic clubfoot a typically developing child, matching gender and age, from the address lists of the department of Developmental Psychology at the University of Giessen, Germany. The typically developing children came to be on the address list of our department because they have already participated in an earlier study and agreed to be contacted again. Originally, we got the addresses from the city administration. The mean age of the control group was 40 months (range: 39–42, SD = 1).

Measures

We measured gross motor, fine motor and cognitive skills of all children with a standardized and objective measuring instrument, the German version of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, third edition [27]. The Bayley-III-Scales are reliable, valid and applied measurement tools for assessing early development both in clinical practice and research settings (Bayley, 2006). The Bayley-III-Scales are based on direct child interaction, and the experimenter assess the performance of the child immediately during the testing. The items of the cognitive subscale measure sensorimotor development, exploration and manipulation, object relatedness, concept formation, memory and other aspects of cognitive processing. The items of the fine motor subscale quantify prehension, perceptual-motor integration, motor planning and motor speed. The items of the gross motor subscale assess static positioning; dynamic movement, including locomotion and coordination; balance; and motor planning [27]. We derived scaled scores from the subtests’ total raw scores. The Bayley-III scaled scores represent a child’s performance relative to same-age peers and range from 1 to 19, with a mean (M) of 10 and a standard deviation (SD) of 3 [27].

Procedure

Written informed consent was obtained from all parents in advance. The two experimenters were non-blinded with regard to the clubfoot diagnosis of the children. However, they were carefully trained and supervised by experienced researchers regarding the administration and interpretation of the Bayley-III-Scales. This should ensure that they were able to run and to analyse the Bayley-III-Scales objectively, regardless of the children’s diagnosis. One assessor tested the children twice within 1 week, individually at home or in a laboratory at the university. Each testing session took approximately 1 h and was videotaped by another experimenter, giving the possibility for reanalyzing off-line. We measured gross motor and cognitive skills at the first testing session and fine motor skills at the second testing session. According to the manual [27], each subscale test started with an appropriate start item that corresponded with the chronological age of the child and continued until the child failed five consecutive items. If a child failed one of the first three items, the items corresponding with the previous age group were presented. Each participating child received a small toy as gift after the first and second test session as well as a certificate of participation after the second testing session.

Data analysis

Initially, we converted the raw scores into scaled scores using the tables in the Bayley manual [27]. The normality of our data sets was assessed by the Kolmogorov–Smirnoff test of normality. Fine motor and cognitive data were normal distributed (ps > .05); however, not the gross motor data of the control group (p < .05). Hence, we conducted independent samples t-tests to analyze the fine motor and cognitive performances and a Mann–Whitney-U-test to analyze the gross motor performances of both groups. We further compared the performance of both groups on each item of the Bayley Scales individually with Fisher’s exact tests to uncover whether group differences exist on specific tasks. We chose Fisher’s exact test because of our small sample size. We used SPSS 22.0 [29] for all statistical analyses and G*Power 3.1 [30] for calculating effect sizes (Cohens d) and statistical power (1-ß).

Results

Gross motor development

As can be seen from Fig. 1, the 3-years-old children treated for idiopathic clubfoot (M = 8.33, SD = 1.23) showed significantly lower gross motor performance than the typically-developing children (M = 11.40, SD = 2.32) at the same age (Mann–Whitney-U = 6.00, p = .001, d = 1.655, 1-ß = 0.965). Moreover, we did not find any significant differences between children with unilateral and children with bilateral clubfoot (t (7) = .171, p = .869).
Notably, as can be seen from Fig. 2, children treated for idiopathic clubfoot had difficulties with tiptoeing (p = .033, Fisher’s exact test), walking upstairs (p = .005, Fisher’s exact test, d = 1.883), and walking downstairs (p = .033, Fisher’s exact test).

Fine motor development

As seen from Fig. 1, the total fine motor performance of the children treated for idiopathic clubfoot (M = 11.00, SD = 1.85) was comparable (t (17) = − 1.935; p = .071, d = 0.884; 1-ß = 0.555) to those of the typically developing children (M = 12.30, SD = 0.95). Moreover, we did not find any significant differences concerning the performance on individual fine motor items (all ps > .05, Fisher’s exact test).

Cognitive development

As seen in Fig. 1, clubfoot children (M = 9.80, SD = 1.32) showed significantly lower total cognitive performance (t (18) = − 2.305; p = .033, d = 1.04) than the typically-developing children (M = 10.90, SD = 0.74), especially in visual-spatial memory (p = .023, Fisher’s exact test). As can be seen from Fig. 2, children treated for idiopathic clubfoot were less able to select object pairs correctly, a task quantifying visual-spatial-memory performance. However, on all other cognitive items, the two groups achieved comparable results.

Discussion

This is, to our knowledge, the first study investigating motor and cognitive abilities of 40-months old children treated for idiopathic clubfoot. The current study yielded several important results and implications:
First, 3-years old children treated for idiopathic clubfoot appear to have an increased risk of gross motor limitations. In particular, we found deficits in tiptoeing, walking upstairs and walking downstairs without support. Indeed, the children treated for idiopathic clubfoot were able to walk upstairs/downstairs, however, they did not manage to perform the special requirement for these tasks, for example needing to hold on to the handrail. Hence, orthopedic pediatrics should incorporate measures of gross motor functioning, for example tiptoeing, in their orthopedic setting to uncover children with a need for further diagnostic and therapy. Moreover, consistent with previous findings [23], children with unilateral clubfoot showed the same gross motor results as children with bilateral clubfoot.
Second, as expected, the present results demonstrated age-appropriate fine motor performances in 40-months old children with idiopathic clubfoot. Our findings confirmed previous findings in infants treated for idiopathic clubfoot [14, 31].
Third, we found some slight cognitive impairments in children treated for idiopathic clubfoot. These children had difficulties selecting object pairs correctly, a task quantifying visual-spatial-memory performance. Particularly, the children were allowed to view 10 s a display of six cards (three object pairs: tops, flowers and cars). Then, the cards were turned over and the children had to identify the correct pairs of cards for the first two objects (tops and flowers) [27]. Notably, the children treated for idiopathic clubfoot had difficulties selecting these object-pairs correctly. However, on all other cognitive items, the two groups achieved comparable results. Hence, it seems that gross motor development does not have a strong impact on cognitive development per se, but it seems to facilitate the development of specific cognitive skills, for example visual spatial memory performance. These results confirmed the findings of current studies in typically developing children [1821], spina bifida children [22] and infants treated for idiopathic clubfoot [14]. Overall, it seems that gross motor abilities can influence specific spatial cognitive skills, emphasizing the need for a motor intervention program for children with strong motor disabilities.
The major strengths of this study were definitely the testing of both, motor and cognitive abilities in children treated for idiopathic clubfoot as well as in typically developing children. One former study did not test a group of healthy control children, instead they simply compared the values of children treated for idiopathic clubfoot with norm-based values [23]. The major limitation of the present study is definitely the small sample size, only 20 children, 10 typically developing and 10 treated for idiopathic clubfoot, participated in this study. However, despite the relatively small sample size, the analyses revealed significant differences with large effect sizes and high statistical power [32], indicating considerable differences between the two groups. Other obvious limitations of the current study are the non-blinding of the experimenter and the matching procedure: We matched the children of the two groups only concerning age and gender. We were not able to match other attributes such as weight or height. Another limitation was that the two experimenters were not blinded with regard to the diagnosis of the children. However, the Bayley III-Scales are objective, reliable, valid and applied measurement tools for assessing early motor and cognitive development both in clinical practice and research settings [27]. Therefore, we consider it unlikely that the data were considerably influenced by subjective factors of the experimenters.
Initially, future studies should investigate motor and cognitive abilities of children treated for idiopathic clubfoot with a larger sample to verify our results. Moreover, previous studies have shown that school-aged children with motor difficulties due to spina bifida or overweight demonstrated impaired spatial cognitive skills [28, 33]. Hence, future studies in older children treated for idiopathic clubfoot are needed to examine whether the observed gross motor and spatial cognitive deficits persist through childhood. If so, some kind of motor intervention program might be required. Finally, previous studies revealed that motor competence seems to be important for school children’s self-esteem [34, 35]. Hence, future studies with school-aged children treated for idiopathic clubfoot should measure self-esteem additionally.

Conclusions

This study showed that 40-months old children treated for idiopathic clubfoot appear to have an increased risk of specific gross motor and spatial cognitive impairments. Our results indicate that gross motor difficulties can affect spatial cognitive skills in children treated for idiopathic clubfoot. Therefore, orthopedic pediatrics should incorporate measures of gross motor functioning, for example tiptoeing, in their orthopedic setting to uncover children with a need for further diagnostic and therapy.

Acknowledgments

We wish to give special thanks to the children and parents who participated in this study.
The present study was conducted in full accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki of the World Medical Association (WMA) and the Research Ethics Guidelines of the German Psychological Society (DGPs). The Office of Research Ethics at the University of Giessen approved the experimental procedure and the informed consent protocol. Moreover, all parents provided written informed consent prior to the first testing of their children.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Lochmiller C, Johnston D, Scott A, Risman M, Hecht JT. Genetic epidemiology study of idiopathic talipes equinovarus. Am J Med Genet A. 1998;79:90–6.CrossRef Lochmiller C, Johnston D, Scott A, Risman M, Hecht JT. Genetic epidemiology study of idiopathic talipes equinovarus. Am J Med Genet A. 1998;79:90–6.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Honein MA, Paulozzi LJ, Moore CA. Family history, maternal smoking, and clubfoot: an indication of a gene-environment interaction. Am J Epidemiol. 2000;152:658–65.CrossRef Honein MA, Paulozzi LJ, Moore CA. Family history, maternal smoking, and clubfoot: an indication of a gene-environment interaction. Am J Epidemiol. 2000;152:658–65.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Dobbs MB, Gurnett CA. Update on clubfoot: etiology and treatment. Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2009;467:1146.CrossRef Dobbs MB, Gurnett CA. Update on clubfoot: etiology and treatment. Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2009;467:1146.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Dodwell E, Risoe P, Wright J. Factors associated with increased risk of clubfoot: a norwegian national cohort analysis. J Pediatr Orthop. 2015;35:e104–9.CrossRef Dodwell E, Risoe P, Wright J. Factors associated with increased risk of clubfoot: a norwegian national cohort analysis. J Pediatr Orthop. 2015;35:e104–9.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Wynne-Davies R. Family studies and the cause of congenital club foot. Bone Jt J. 1964;46:445–63.CrossRef Wynne-Davies R. Family studies and the cause of congenital club foot. Bone Jt J. 1964;46:445–63.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Wallander H, Hovelius L, Michaelsson K. Incidence of congenital clubfoot in Sweden. Acta Orthop. 2006;77:847–52.CrossRef Wallander H, Hovelius L, Michaelsson K. Incidence of congenital clubfoot in Sweden. Acta Orthop. 2006;77:847–52.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Zionts LE, Jew MH, Ebramzadeh E, Sangiorgio SN. The influence of sex and laterality on clubfoot severity. J Pediatr Orthop. 2017;37:e129–33.CrossRef Zionts LE, Jew MH, Ebramzadeh E, Sangiorgio SN. The influence of sex and laterality on clubfoot severity. J Pediatr Orthop. 2017;37:e129–33.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Ponseti IV. Congenital clubfoot: fundamentals of treatment. New York: Oxford University Press; 1996. Ponseti IV. Congenital clubfoot: fundamentals of treatment. New York: Oxford University Press; 1996.
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Ponseti IV, Smoley EN. The classic: congenital club foot: the results of treatment. Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2009;467:1133–45.CrossRef Ponseti IV, Smoley EN. The classic: congenital club foot: the results of treatment. Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2009;467:1133–45.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Eberhardt O, Peterlein CD, Fernandez FF, Wirth T. Mid-term results of idiopathic clubfeet treated with the Ponseti method. Z Orthop Unfall. 2012;150:190–7.CrossRef Eberhardt O, Peterlein CD, Fernandez FF, Wirth T. Mid-term results of idiopathic clubfeet treated with the Ponseti method. Z Orthop Unfall. 2012;150:190–7.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Halanski MA, Davison JE, Huang J-C, Walker CG, Walsh SJ, Crawford HA. Ponseti method compared with surgical treatment of clubfoot: a prospective comparison. JBJS. 2010;92:270–8.CrossRef Halanski MA, Davison JE, Huang J-C, Walker CG, Walsh SJ, Crawford HA. Ponseti method compared with surgical treatment of clubfoot: a prospective comparison. JBJS. 2010;92:270–8.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Laaveg SJ, Ponseti IV. Long-term results of treatment of congenital club foot. JBJS. 1980;62:23–31.CrossRef Laaveg SJ, Ponseti IV. Long-term results of treatment of congenital club foot. JBJS. 1980;62:23–31.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Morcuende JA, Dolan LA, Dietz FR, Ponseti IV. Radical reduction in the rate of extensive corrective surgery for clubfoot using the Ponseti method. Pediatrics. 2004;113:376–80.CrossRef Morcuende JA, Dolan LA, Dietz FR, Ponseti IV. Radical reduction in the rate of extensive corrective surgery for clubfoot using the Ponseti method. Pediatrics. 2004;113:376–80.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Dillmann J, Peterlein CD, Schwarzer G. A longitudinal study of motor and cognitive development in infants with congenital idiopathic clubfoot. J Motor Learn Dev. 2018;6:S24–43.CrossRef Dillmann J, Peterlein CD, Schwarzer G. A longitudinal study of motor and cognitive development in infants with congenital idiopathic clubfoot. J Motor Learn Dev. 2018;6:S24–43.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Zionts LE, Packer DF, Cooper S, Ebramzadeh E, Sangiorgio S. Walking age of infants with idiopathic clubfoot treated using the Ponseti method. JBJS. 2014;96:e164.CrossRef Zionts LE, Packer DF, Cooper S, Ebramzadeh E, Sangiorgio S. Walking age of infants with idiopathic clubfoot treated using the Ponseti method. JBJS. 2014;96:e164.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Sala DA, Chu A, Lehman WB, van Bosse HJP. Achievement of gross motor milestones in children with idiopathic clubfoot treated with the Ponseti method. J Pediatr Orthop. 2013;33:55–8.CrossRef Sala DA, Chu A, Lehman WB, van Bosse HJP. Achievement of gross motor milestones in children with idiopathic clubfoot treated with the Ponseti method. J Pediatr Orthop. 2013;33:55–8.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Garcia NL, McMulkin ML, Tompkins BJ, Caskey PM, Mader SL, Baird GO. Gross motor development in babies with treated idiopathic clubfoot. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2011;23:347–52.CrossRef Garcia NL, McMulkin ML, Tompkins BJ, Caskey PM, Mader SL, Baird GO. Gross motor development in babies with treated idiopathic clubfoot. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2011;23:347–52.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Bai DL, Bertenthal BI. Locomotor status and the development of spatial search skills. Child Dev. 1992;63:215–26.CrossRef Bai DL, Bertenthal BI. Locomotor status and the development of spatial search skills. Child Dev. 1992;63:215–26.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Berger SE. Locomotor expertise predicts infants’ perseverative errors. Dev Psychol. 2010;46:326.CrossRef Berger SE. Locomotor expertise predicts infants’ perseverative errors. Dev Psychol. 2010;46:326.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Clearfield MW. The role of crawling and walking experience in infant spatial memory. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;89:214–41.CrossRef Clearfield MW. The role of crawling and walking experience in infant spatial memory. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;89:214–41.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Kermoian R, Campos JJ. Locomotor experience: a facilitator of spatial cognitive development. Child Dev. 1988;59:908–17.CrossRef Kermoian R, Campos JJ. Locomotor experience: a facilitator of spatial cognitive development. Child Dev. 1988;59:908–17.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Campos JJ, Anderson DI, Telzrow R. Locomotor experience influences the spatial cognitive development of infants with spina bifida. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie. 2009;41:181–8.CrossRef Campos JJ, Anderson DI, Telzrow R. Locomotor experience influences the spatial cognitive development of infants with spina bifida. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie. 2009;41:181–8.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Andriesse H, Westbom L, Hägglund G. Motor ability in children treated for idiopathic clubfoot. A controlled pilot study. BMC Pediatr. 2009;9:78.CrossRef Andriesse H, Westbom L, Hägglund G. Motor ability in children treated for idiopathic clubfoot. A controlled pilot study. BMC Pediatr. 2009;9:78.CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Lööf E, Andriesse H, André M, Böhm S, Iversen MD, Broström EW. Gross motor skills in children with idiopathic clubfoot and the association between gross motor skills, foot involvement, gait, and foot motion. J Pediatr Orthop. 2019;39:359–65.CrossRef Lööf E, Andriesse H, André M, Böhm S, Iversen MD, Broström EW. Gross motor skills in children with idiopathic clubfoot and the association between gross motor skills, foot involvement, gait, and foot motion. J Pediatr Orthop. 2019;39:359–65.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Lohle-Akkersdijk JJ, Rameckers EAA, Andriesse H, de Reus I, van Erve RH. Walking capacity of children with clubfeet in primary school: something to worry about? J Pediatr Orthop B. 2015;24:18–23.CrossRef Lohle-Akkersdijk JJ, Rameckers EAA, Andriesse H, de Reus I, van Erve RH. Walking capacity of children with clubfeet in primary school: something to worry about? J Pediatr Orthop B. 2015;24:18–23.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Kenmoku T, Kamegaya M, Saisu T, Ochiai N, Iwakura N, Iwase D, et al. Athletic ability of school-age children after satisfactory treatment of congenital clubfoot. J Pediatr Orthop. 2013;33:321–5.CrossRef Kenmoku T, Kamegaya M, Saisu T, Ochiai N, Iwakura N, Iwase D, et al. Athletic ability of school-age children after satisfactory treatment of congenital clubfoot. J Pediatr Orthop. 2013;33:321–5.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Bayley N. Bayley scales of infant and toddler development: Bayley-III: Harcourt assessment. San Antonio: Psych. Corporation; 2006. Bayley N. Bayley scales of infant and toddler development: Bayley-III: Harcourt assessment. San Antonio: Psych. Corporation; 2006.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Jansen-Osmann P, Wiedenbauer G, Heil M. Spatial cognition and motor development: a study of children with spina bifida. Percept Mot Skills. 2008;106:436–46.CrossRef Jansen-Osmann P, Wiedenbauer G, Heil M. Spatial cognition and motor development: a study of children with spina bifida. Percept Mot Skills. 2008;106:436–46.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Corp IBM. IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 22.0. Armonk: IBM Corp; 2013. Corp IBM. IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 22.0. Armonk: IBM Corp; 2013.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G* power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:175–91.CrossRef Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G* power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:175–91.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Dillmann J, Peterlein CD, Schwarzer G. A longitudinal study of motor and cognitive development in infants with congenital idiopathic clubfoot. J Motor Learn Dev. 2017;6(s1):1–35.CrossRef Dillmann J, Peterlein CD, Schwarzer G. A longitudinal study of motor and cognitive development in infants with congenital idiopathic clubfoot. J Motor Learn Dev. 2017;6(s1):1–35.CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. NJ: Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale; 1988. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. NJ: Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale; 1988.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Jansen P, Schmelter A, Kasten L, Heil M. Impaired mental rotation performance in overweight children. Appetite. 2011;56:766–9.CrossRef Jansen P, Schmelter A, Kasten L, Heil M. Impaired mental rotation performance in overweight children. Appetite. 2011;56:766–9.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Skinner RA, Piek JP. Psychosocial implications of poor motor coordination in children and adolescents. Hum Mov Sci. 2001;20:73–94.CrossRef Skinner RA, Piek JP. Psychosocial implications of poor motor coordination in children and adolescents. Hum Mov Sci. 2001;20:73–94.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Piek JP, Baynam GB, Barrett NC. The relationship between fine and gross motor ability, self-perceptions and self-worth in children and adolescents. Hum Mov Sci. 2006;25:65–75.CrossRef Piek JP, Baynam GB, Barrett NC. The relationship between fine and gross motor ability, self-perceptions and self-worth in children and adolescents. Hum Mov Sci. 2006;25:65–75.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Motor and cognitive functioning in children treated for idiopathic clubfoot at the age of 3 years
verfasst von
Julia Dillmann
Gudrun Schwarzer
Christian-Dominik Peterlein
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2019
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2019
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2431
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-019-1765-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2019

BMC Pediatrics 1/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.