Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 4/2017

Open Access 28.09.2016 | Original Contribution

Psychopathic personality traits in 5 year old twins: the importance of genetic and shared environmental influences

verfasst von: Catherine Tuvblad, Kostas A. Fanti, Henrik Andershed, Olivier F. Colins, Henrik Larsson

Erschienen in: European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry | Ausgabe 4/2017

Abstract

There is limited research on the genetic and environmental bases of psychopathic personality traits in children. In this study, psychopathic personality traits were assessed in a total of 1189 5-year-old boys and girls drawn from the Preschool Twin Study in Sweden. Psychopathic personality traits were assessed with the Child Problematic Traits Inventory, a teacher-report measure of psychopathic personality traits in children ranging from 3 to 12 years old. Univariate results showed that genetic influences accounted for 57, 25, and 74 % of the variance in the grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional, and impulsive–need for stimulation dimensions, while the shared environment accounted for 17, 48 and 9 % (n.s.) in grandiose–deceitful and callous–unemotional, impulsive–need for stimulation dimensions, respectively. No sex differences were found in the genetic and environmental variance components. The non-shared environment accounted for the remaining 26, 27 and 17 % of the variance, respectively. The three dimensions of psychopathic personality were moderately correlated (0.54–0.66) and these correlations were primarily mediated by genetic and shared environmental factors. In contrast to research conducted with adolescent and adult twins, we found that both genetic and shared environmental factors influenced psychopathic personality traits in early childhood. These findings indicate that etiological models of psychopathic personality traits would benefit by taking developmental stages and processes into consideration.

Introduction

Psychopathy is a multifaceted syndrome often described as a constellation of affective (e.g., lack of remorse or guilt, shallow affect, callous/lack of empathy), interpersonal (e.g., glib/superficial charm, grandiose self-worth, manipulative), behavioral and antisocial (e.g., poor behavior control, impulsive, inability to accept responsibility for one’s actions) features [17, 38]. Psychopathy is related to a range of negative and dysfunctional outcomes including substance use, criminal behavior, psychopathology (e.g., borderline personality disorder) [21, 40, 42, 44, 56], and social maladjustment such as lower educational performance, unemployment and poor social relationships [4, 19, 44, 71]. The prevalence of psychopathy has been estimated to be between 0.6 and 4 % in the general population, with a higher proportion of males to females [65, 75]. Despite the low prevalence, these individuals are believed to account for a large portion of all serious crimes and their recidivism rate is higher than for other offenders [8, 38, 56]. Furthermore, psychopathy is considered to be a neurodevelopmental disorder rooted early in life [22, 33, 35, 36, 41, 52]. Identifying individuals with psychopathic personality traits early in development is, therefore, crucial for intervention efforts, especially since these traits have been linked to early engagement in criminal activities (e.g., [39, 58, 62]). To date, there has been very limited research on the genetic and environmental etiology of psychopathic personality traits in young children. This study aimed to bridge this research gap by investigating the extent of genetic influences on these traits in boys and girls and to investigate whether shared environmental influences also play a significant role.
Recent twin studies report that heritable factors have a moderate to high influence, non-shared environmental factors have a small to moderate influence, and shared environmental factors have little or no importance explaining the variance in psychopathic personality (for reviews: [66, 72]). This has been found among adolescents (e.g., [5, 9, 25, 30, 47, 48, 64, 69, 70]), as well as among adults (e.g., [5, 10, 11, 44]). However, a more mixed pattern has been found across the few studies that have included children (i.e., participants 12 years of age or younger; see Table 1 for a summary). Please also note that the majority of the studies summarized in Table 1 have only examined the affective (or callous–unemotional) traits. For example, an early study by Viding et al. [76] using DeFries–Fulker extreme analysis showed that the heritability of callous–unemotional traits was 67 % in a sample of 7-year-old twins. Furthermore, Bezdjian et al. used the Child Psychopathy Scale [51] to assess psychopathic personality traits in a set of 9–10 year old twins. The affective–interpersonal factor was primarily influenced by genetic factors with slight sex differences (boys 64 %; girls 49 %), with the non-shared environment contributing 36 % in boys and 44 % in girls. Similarly, the impulsive–antisocial factor was primarily influenced by genetic factors (boys 46 %; girls 58 %), with the non-shared environment contributing 53 % in boys and 37 % in girls. There were little and non-significant influence from the shared environment [6]. Ficks et al. included children as young as 4 years in their study (age range 4.4–17.8 years, age-corrected analysis) and the antisocial process screening device [31] was used to assess psychopathic personality traits. For callous–unemotional traits, genetic influences explained 49 % of the variance, shared environment 19 % and the non-shared environment 32 %; for narcissism, genetic influences explained 63 % and the non-shared environment 37 %; and for impulsivity, additive and non-additive genetic influences contributed 61 % in boys and 74 % in girls, with the non-shared environment contributing the remaining variance.
Table 1
Genetic (A), shared environmental (C) (or dominant, D), and non-shared environmental (E) estimates for psychopathic personality traits in children (i.e., participants 12 years of age or younger)—a summary from previous twin studies
References
Sample (N)
Psychopathy measure
Informant
Age (years)
Psychopathy dimension
A
C/(D)
E
Bezdjian et al. [6]
RFABa (1219 twins)
Child Psychopathy Scale (CPS) [50]
Caregiver rated (>90 % biological Mothers)
9–10
Affective–interpersonal factor (boys)
0.64 (0.49 to 0.72)
0 (0.00 to 0.11)
0.36 (0.28–0.47)
Affective–interpersonal factor (girls)
0.49 (0.21 to 0.65)
0.06 (0.00 to 0.30)
0.44 (0.35–0.56)
Impulsive–antisocial factor (boys)
0.46 (0.22 to 0.58)
0.01 (0.00 to 0.195)
0.53 (0.41–0.66)
Impulsive–antisocial factor (girls)
0.58 (0.25 to 0.70)
0.04 (0.00 to 0.34)
0.37 (0.29–0.48)
Frick and Hare [31]
Georgia Twin Studyc (885 twin pairs)
Antisocial process screening device [31]
Mother rated
4.4–17.8
Callous–unemotional
0.49d
0.19
0.32
Narcissism
0.63
 
0.37
Impulsivity (boys)
0.21
0.40
0.39
Impulsivity (girls)
0.26
0.48
0.26
Viding et al. [76]
TEDSb (612 + 234 + 210 twins)
Three antisocial process screening device [31] and four Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire [37] items were used to assess callous–unemotional traits
Teacher rated
7
Elevated levels of callous–unemotional traits
0.67e (0.47 to 0.87)
0.06 (−0.23 to 0.35)
 
Elevated levels of antisocial behavior and callous–unemotional traits
0.81 (0.50 to 1.12)
0.05 (0.00 to 0.72)
 
Elevated levels of antisocial behavior w/o callous–unemotional traits
0.30 (−0.10 to 0.70)
0.34 (−0.40 to 1.08)
 
Larsson et al. [49]
TEDSb (352 + 234 twins)
All items from callous–unemotional dimension of the antisocial process screening device [31] and two items from the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire [37] were used to assess callous–unemotional traits
Teacher rated
7
Callous–unemotional traits and elevated levels of antisocial behavior
0.68e (0.42 to 0.95)
0.00 (−0.82 to 0.18)
 
Callous–unemotional traits w/o elevated levels of antisocial behavior
0.80 (0.51 to 1.03)
0.00 (−77 to 0.23)
 
Viding et al. [80]
TEDSb (140 probands in 88 twin pairs, and 174 probands in 144 twin pairs)
All items from callous–unemotional dimension of the antisocial process screening device [31] and two items from the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire [37] were used to assess callous–unemotional traits
Teacher rated
9
Antisocial behavior and callous–unemotional traits
0.75e (0.45 to 1.06)
0.00 (−1.63 to 1.27)
 
Antisocial behavior only
0.53 (0.13 to 0.92)
0.00 (−0.83 to 0.64)
 
Antisocial behavior, hyperactivity and callous–unemotional traits
0.71 (0.24 to 1.18)
0.00 (−1.66 to 1.36)
 
Antisocial behavior and hyperactivity
0.36 (−0.14 to 0.86)
0.00 (−0.76 to 0.71)
 
Humayun et al. [43]
TEDSb (627 + 119 twins)
Three antisocial process screening device [31] and four Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire [37] items were used to assess callous–unemotional traits
Teacher rated
7
Elevated levels of callous–unemotional
0.75e (0.58 to 0.92)
−0.02 (−1.22 to 2.38)
 
Elevated levels of callous–unemotional and Anxiety
0.66 (0.33 to 0.95)
0.05 (−1.62 to 2.28)
 
Fontaine et al. [29]
TEDSc (9462)
Three Antisocial Process Screening Device [31] and four Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire [37] items were used to assess callous–unemotional traits
Teacher rated
7, 9, 12
Stable high (boys) (girls)
0.78 (0.42 to 0.88)
0.01 (0.00 to 0.35)
0.21 (0.12–0.34)
0.00 (0.00 to 0.57)
0.75 (0.35 to 0.90)
0.25 (0.07–0.48)
Increasing (boys) (girls)
0.58 (0.12 to 0.72)
0.03 (0.00 to 0.41)
0.39 (0.28–0.53)
0.26 (0.00 to 0.70)
0.47 (0.08 to 0.74)
0.27 (0.16–0.43)
Decreasing (boys) (girls)
0.61 (0.35 to 0.72)
0.02 (0.00 to 0.23)
0.37 (0.28–0.49)
0.54 (0.23 to 0.85)
0.26 (0.00 to 0.53)
0.20 (0.13–0.29)
Stable low (boys + girls)
0.68 (0.52 to 0.81)
0.08 (0.00 to 0.21)
0.24 (0.19–0.30)
aThe University of Southern California Risk Factors for Antisocial Behavior (RFAB) twin study. Other papers using the same sample and measure but not necessarily examining the influence of genetic and environmental factors on psychopathic personality traits are not included in Table 1. For example, a study examining the genetic and environmental overlap between psychopathic traits and aggression at age 9–10 [7], a study examining the relationship between psychopathic traits and autonomic responses during the countdown task at age 9–10 [74], a study examining the relationship between psychopathic traits and anticipatory fear skin conductance responses at age 9–10 [73], a study examining the relationship between psychopathic traits and skin conductance orienting response at age 9–10 [45]
bTwins Early Development Study. Other papers using the same sample and measure but not necessarily examining the influence of genetic and environmental factors on psychopathic personality traits are not included in Table 1. For example, a study examining the genetic and environmental overlap between callous–unemotional traits and conduct problems at age 7 [79], a study examining the genetic and environmental overlap between callous–unemotional traits at age 7 and autistic traits at age 8 [57], a study examining the relationship among negative parental discipline, conduct problems and callous–unemotional traits at age 7 and 12 [78], a study examining the relationship among dimensions of psychopathy (callous–unemotional traits, narcissism, impulsivity) and cognitive abilities at age 9 [26], a study using growth mixture modeling to identify four trajectories of callous–unemotional traits (stable high, increasing, decreasing, and stable low) at ages 7, 9, 12 [28], a study examining the relationship between peer victimization and trajectories of callous–unemotional traits (i.e., stable high, increasing, decreasing and stable low) at ages 7, 9, 12 [27]. See also reviews: a review on callous–unemotional traits, including twin studies examining its genetic and environmental etiology [81], a review on callous–unemotional traits and antisocial behavior [77]
cGeorgia Twin Study. Other papers using the same sample and measure and are not included in Table 1 include for example a study examining the factor structure of the antisocial process screening device [20]
d95 % confidence intervals for the ACE estimates nor standard errors were not reported
eEstimates of group heritability and group shared environment were calculated using the DeFries–Fulker extreme analysis regression model [18]
In sum, studies examining the genetic and environmental etiology of psychopathic personality traits in young children are scarce. The few studies that do exist have produced diverse findings showing a moderate to strong heritability; the role of the shared environment on these traits is mixed, and whether or not the genetic and environmental estimates vary across sex is unclear. Using data from a population-based sample of Swedish 5-year-old twins this study had the following goals: (1) to examine the genetic and environmental etiology of the three psychopathic personality dimensions—grandiose–deceitful, a callous–unemotional, and impulsive–need for stimulation-assessed with the Child Problematic Traits Inventory (CPTI; [13]), which was designed to be used specifically among young children; (2) to examine whether the genetic and environmental etiology of the three psychopathic personality dimensions is comparable in boys and girls; and (3) to examine how much of the phenotypic correlation among these dimensions that are accounted for by genetic and environmental influences.

Method

Participants and procedure

This study used data from the Preschool Twin Study in Sweden (PETSS) project. The overall aim of PETSS was to examine how genetic and environmental factors in early childhood contribute to cognition, emotional regulation and behavioral problems. Parents of all twins born in Sweden between January, 2004 and May, 2005 were identified through the Swedish population-based medical birth register and contacted 1 month prior to their twins’ 5th birthday. Thus, all children in PETSS were 5 years old at study start. Questionnaires were mailed to parents and pre-school teachers of 1261 twin pairs (n = 2522 children). Non-responders were approached with up to three reminders. Parents were approached separately, resulting in 828 (65 %) responses from the mothers, and 698 (55 %) responses from the fathers. Mother and/or father ratings were available from 879 twin pairs (n = 1758 children). The teacher-rated questionnaires had a response rate of 54 % 686 twin pairs (n = 1372 children). The PETSS project was evaluated and approved by an ethics committee at the Karolinska Institute (#2007-1034). For more information on study protocol and procedures: http://​ki.​se/​meb/​petss, and [12].
The main focus of this study was to examine genetic and environmental influences on psychopathic personality traits assessed with the CPTI [13]. The CPTI was completed only by teachers and data were available from a total of n = 1189 children (591 boys; 598 girls), Table 2.
Typically in Sweden, children start pre-school (day care) around or soon after their first birthday. They remain in pre-school for a period of approximately 4 years. They go to Kindergarten in the fall of the year that they turn 6 years. The PETSS questionnaire was mailed out and completed by participating pre-school teachers close to the twins’ fifth birthday. Of the participating teachers, 3 % reported that they had known the twins less than 6 months, 12 % reported that they had known the twins between 6 and 12 months, 11 % reported that they had known the twins between 13 and 18 months, 12 % reported that they had known the twins between 19 and 24 months, and 62 % reported that they had known the twins more than 24 months.         
Table 2
Descriptive statistics and twin correlations for the psychopathic personality dimensions grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional and impulsive–need for stimulation at age 5, teacher ratings
 
Boys
Girls
 
Grandiose–deceitful (mean, SD)
1.41, 0.52 n = 591
1.33, 0.49 n = 598
t (1187) 3.04, p = 0.0025
Callous–unemotional (mean, SD)
1.60, 0.65 n = 591
1.38, 0.51 n = 598
t (1187) 6.61, p < 0.001
Impulsive–need for stimulation (mean, SD)
2.11, 0.72 n = 591
1.91, 0.66 n = 598
t (1187) 5.17, p < 0.001
 
MZ
DZ
MZ
DZ
OSDZ
Grandiose–deceitful
0.79*
0.25*
0.75*
0.44*
0.50*
Callous–unemotional
0.72*
0.66*
0.66*
0.58*
0.54*
Impulsive–need for stimulation
0.82*
0.25*
0.80*
0.34*
0.54*
MZ monozygotic, DZ dizygotic, OSDZ opposite sex
p < 0.05, transformed data

Measures

Psychopathic personality traits were assessed with the Child Problematic Traits Inventory (CPTI) [13, 14, 16, 63]. The CPTI has the following response format: 1 = ‘Does not apply at all’; 2 = ‘Does not apply well’; 3 = ‘Applies fairly well’; and 4 = ‘Applies very well’. Respondents were instructed to rate each child on how he/she usually and typically behaves rather than based on how he or she behaves at the moment. CPTI contains 28 items which have been found to load on three factors, and this three factor structure has been identified in PETSS across boys and girls [15], as well as in another Swedish sample across boys and girls and across age [13]. The grandiose–deceitful factor score includes eight items (e.g., lies often to avoid problems; seems to see himself/herself as superior compared to others), the callous–unemotional factor score includes 10 items (e.g., seldom expresses sympathy for others; usually does not seem to share others’ joy and sorrow), and the impulsive–need for stimulation factor score includes ten-items (e.g., likes change and that things happen all the time; often has difficulties with awaiting his/her turn). The CPTI three factor scores showed excellent internal consistency (all Cronbach’s alphas in the present study >0.89). In terms of external validity, the three factors exhibited positive and significant correlations with teacher and parent rated variables of interest in PETSS, including conduct problems, attention–deficit/hyperactivity (ADHD) symptoms, aggression, and fearlessness [15]. All three scores were log-transformed to approximate a normal distribution.

Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics and correlations

Descriptive statistics, including means and standard deviations, were first computed for three psychopathic personality dimensions: grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional, and impulsive–need for stimulation, as well as their phenotypic correlations.

Twin modeling

In the twin design, data from monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins are used to decompose the variance in a measured trait to genetic and environmental components. MZ twins share their common environment and they are assumed to share 100 % of their genes. DZ twins also share their common environment and they are assumed to share about 50 % of their genes. By comparing the resemblance between MZ and DZ twins, the variance of a measured trait can be divided into additive genetic factors (A), shared environmental factors (C), and non-shared environmental factors (E). Shared environmental factors refer to non-genetic influences that contribute to similarity within pairs of twins, whereas non-shared environmental factors refer to experiences that make siblings dissimilar [55].
To get a first indication of the underlying sources of variance in grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional, and impulsive–need for stimulation dimensions, comparisons were made among twin correlations (Twin-1–Twin-2 correlations). A DZ correlation approximately half the value of the MZ correlation would indicate the presence of additive genetic effects, whereas a DZ correlation more than half an MZ correlation indicates the presence of both genetic and shared environmental effects [55].

Univariate modeling

Univariate models were fit to grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional, and impulsive–need for stimulation separately to estimate the relative contributions of genetic factors (A), shared environmental factors (C), and non-shared environmental factors (E, plus error). To test for sex differences in the variance components, a model in which the genetic and environmental effects were allowed to differ between boys and girls were compared against a model in which the estimates were constrained to be equal. A saturated model, which estimates the variances, covariances, and means were first fit and used as a baseline model to which all subsequent models were compared.

Bivariate modeling

A bivariate Cholesky decomposition was fit to estimate how much of the phenotypic correlation that is due to genetic and environmental influences between grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional and impulsive–need for stimulation, also referred to as bivariate heritability, bivariate shared environment and bivariate non-shared environment. These estimates are proportions and range from 0 to 1. They provide information regarding the extent to which the phenotypic correlation between two traits is mediated by genetic and/or environmental factors.
All genetic models were fit with the structural equation program Mx [54]. The goodness of fit was compared through the difference in the Chi-square statistic (χ 2), where a significant χ 2 indicates that the model with less number of parameters fits the data worse. Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) [1] and Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) [60] were also used to determine fit, where increasingly negative values correspond to increasingly better fitting models.

Results

Descriptive statistics and correlations

There were significant mean differences between boys and girls, with boys having higher mean values for grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional, and impulsive–need for stimulation. The pattern of the twin correlations indicates that genetic and shared environmental influences are important for the three psychopathic personality dimensions, Table 2.

Univariate genetic analysis

Univariate model-fitting results for grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional, and impulsive–need for stimulation are displayed in Table 3. A low DZ twin correlation (in boys for grandiose–deceitful; boys and girls for impulsive–need for stimulation, Table 2) may be due to non-additive genetic effects, such as epistasis or dominance [54]. A model estimating additive genetic (A) effects, non-additive genetic (D) effects and non-shared environmental (E) effects was, therefore, first tested. However, the full ACE model (Model 2 in Table 3) was found to fit better than the ADE model (grandiose–deceitful: AIC 768.632, BIC −2210.855; impulsive–need for stimulation: AIC 694.695, BIC −2247.824).
Table 3
Univariate genetic results and parameter estimates for the psychopathic personality dimensions grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional and impulsive–need for stimulation at age 5, teacher ratings
Model #
Overall fit
Parameter estimates
−2log
df
AIC
BIC
χ 2-diff
Δdf
p
A
C
E
Grandiose–deceitful
 1. Saturated model
3109.314
1172
765.314
−2194.920
      
 2. ACE boys ≠ girls
3116.395
1181
754.395
−2220.173
7.081
9
0.629
   
 3. ACE boys = girls
3120.357
1184
752.357
−2227.789
11.043
12
0.525
0.57 (0.39–0.75)
0.17 (0.001–0.32)
0.26 (0.21–0.33)
Callous–unemotional
 1. Saturated model
3018.635
1172
674.635
−2240.259
      
 2. ACE boys ≠ girls
3023.728
1181
661.728
−2266.506
5.093
9
0.826
   
 3. ACE boys = girls
3028.018
1184
660.018
−2273.959
9.383
12
0.670
0.25 (0.10–0.40)
0.48 (0.35–0.60)
0.27 (0.22–0.33)
Impulsive–need for stimulation
 1. Saturated model
3035.396
1172
691.396
−2231.879
      
 2. ACE boys ≠ girls
3049.638
1181
687.638
−2253.552
14.242
9
0.114
   
 3. ACE boys = girls
3050.872
1184
682.872
−2262.532
15.476
12
0.216
0.74 (0.59–0.86)
0.09 (0.00–0.23)
0.17 (0.14–0.21)
AIC Akaike’s Information Criterion, BIC Bayesian Information Criterion, χ 2 -diff difference in log-likelihoods between models, df degrees of freedom, A additive genetic, C shared environment, E non-shared environment
The full ACE model described the data better than the baseline saturated model (Model 2, grandiose–deceitful: χ 2 = 7.081, df = 9, p = 0.629, callous–unemotional: χ 2 = 5.093, df = 9, p = 0.826, impulsive–need for stimulation: χ 2 = 14.242, df = 9, p = 0.114); Model 2 also had smaller AIC and BIC. A model constraining genetic and environmental components to be equal in boys and girls provided a better fit than the full ACE model (Model 3, grandiose–deceitful: χ 2 = 3.962, df = 3, p = 0.266, callous–unemotional: χ 2 = 4.29, df = 3, p = 0.232, impulsive–need for stimulation: χ 2 = 1.234, df = 3, p = 0.745). Genetic influences accounted for 57, 25, and 74 % of the phenotypic variance for grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional and impulsive–need for stimulation, respectively; shared environmental factors accounted for 17, 48, and 9 % (n.s.), and non-shared environmental factors (including error) accounted for the remaining variance, 26, 27, and 17 %, respectively.

Bivariate genetic analysis

A bivariate Cholesky decomposition was next fit to data. The Cholesky decomposition fit the data better than a saturated model (χ 2 = 76.143, df = 69, p = 0.260). Similar to the univariate analyses, the genetic and environmental variance components could be constrained to be equal in boys and girls (χ 2 = 21.157, df = 18, p = 0.272). The phenotypic correlations were moderate to high across the three psychopathic personality dimensions (Table 3). The phenotypic correlations between grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional, and impulsive–need for stimulation were primarily accounted for by genetic and shared environmental influences (Table 4).
Table 4
Proportion of the phenotypic correlations between grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional, and impulsive–need for stimulation accounted for by genetic (A), shared environmental (C) and non-shared environmental (E) factors
 
Phenotypic correlation
bivh2 (95 % CI)
bivc2 (95 % CI)
bive2 (95 % CI)
Grandiose–deceitful/callous–unemotional
0.66
0.25 (0.05–0.43)
0.52 (0.36–0.67)
0.23 (0.17–0.32)
Grandiose–deceitful/impulsive–need for stimulation
0.61
0.56 (0.40–0.72)
0.26 (0.12–0.40)
0.18 (0.12–0.24)
Callous–unemotional/impulsive–need for stimulation
0.54
0.39 (0.20–0.59)
0.44 (0.26–0.60)
0.17 (0.11–0.24)
bivh 2 bivariate heritability, bivc 2 bivariate shared environment, bive 2 bivariate non-shared environment

Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the genetic and environmental sources among three psychopathic personality dimensions, grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional and impulsive–need for stimulation in a community sample of 5-year-old children assessed by teachers. There are three main points of interest for discussion in this study. First, familial influences (i.e., genetic and/or shared environment) explained the majority of variance in grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional and impulsive–need for stimulation. Second, no sex differences were found in the genetic and environmental variance components. Third, the proportions of the phenotypic correlations among these dimensions were mainly mediated by genetic and shared environmental influences.
Similar to Ficks et al. [25], our univariate analyses indicated that genetic and shared environmental influences primarily explained the variance in the callous–unemotional dimension, and that a large genetic influence was important for impulsive–need for stimulation. We also found that genetic and shared environmental influences explained the variances in grandiose–deceitful, whereas Ficks et al. found that mainly genetic influences were important for narcissism. This discrepancy in findings between our study and Ficks et al. could partly be explained by methodological differences in that we were using the CPTI rated by teachers and they were using the antisocial process screening device rated by mothers. As genetic influences on psychopathic personality traits may vary across the ways in which these traits are measured, in terms of both informant and instrument used [69], more research examining the genetic and environmental etiology of these traits in early childhood is warranted. Also, Ficks et al. [25] age-corrected their data (age range 4.4–17.8 years), whereas we used a sample of 5-year-old twins.
Further, callous–unemotional traits have previously received attention [25, 32, 41], and recently a callous–unemotional-based specifier for the diagnosis of conduct disorder has been added in the fifth edition of the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders [2]. Our finding of a moderate genetic (25 %) influence and higher shared environmental (48 %) influence on the callous–unemotional dimension is in sharp contrast to findings by Viding et al. [76] who found a high heritability (67 %) for antisocial behavior in the presence of callous–unemotional traits as reported by teachers in a sample of 7 year old twins, and no influence from the shared environment. Thus, we found that both genetic and shared environmental factors contributed to callous–unemotional traits at age five. Shared environmental risk factors may include family related factors (e.g., neglect, prenatal stressors) or contextual factors in the surrounding community [53, 68]. Our finding agree with prior work linking environmental factors to callous–unemotional traits and studies suggesting that interventions focusing on environmental stimuli may be effective in reducing callous–unemotional traits (for a review: [34]). The moderate genetic influence in our sample for callous–unemotional traits might also be related to heterogeneity within these traits, with subgroups showing differences in behavioral and physiological measures of anxiety and fear reactivity (e.g., [23, 24]).
The three dimensions grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional and impulsive–need for stimulation were all moderately correlated. The proportions of these phenotypic correlations were mainly accounted for by genetic and shared environmental influences. Again, these findings provide support for the importance of both genetic and shared environmental influences in psychopathic personality traits in young children.
The significant shared environmental influences in particular for callous–unemotional traits identified in our sample of 5-year-old twins are of great importance. Typically, a pattern of decreasing shared environment and a concomitant increase in heritability over the course of development is found; this has been reported for several phenotypes including personality traits, cognitive abilities, and aggression [59]. It will be interesting to follow the twins included in this study across development to see if a similar pattern will emerge for the shared environment on psychopathic personality traits. Then again, the bulk of literature on psychopathy has shown little or no influence of the shared environment (e.g., [66, 72]); however, the majority of previous research has been conducted on adolescent or adult twin samples. Of note, twin studies typically have low power to detect shared environmental influences relative to genetic influences. Shared environmental influences can also be confounded with for example the effects of assortative mating or passive gene-environment correlation (rGE) [46]. Thus, part of the shared environment we found could be explained by the fact that the same teacher was rating both twins in a pair [3]. In our case, 97 % of the participating twin pairs went to the same pre-school class, and 85 % were rated by the same teacher. Studies of children typically rely on parent or teacher reports; it is, therefore, possible that the shared environment found in these studies and in our study is partly an artifact of rater bias. This suggests that future studies are needed examining how genetic and environmental factors influence psychopathic personality traits in children, and it will be interesting to see if they can replicate our finding of a shared environmental component.
We also found higher mean values for psychopathic personality traits in boys than girls across all three psychopathic personality dimensions, indicating that these traits are somewhat more prevalent among boys than girls. Higher mean values for psychopathic personality traits have also been found among males than females across incarcerated and community samples [75]. However, no differences in the magnitude in genetic and environmental variance components were found across boys and girls and the variance components could be constrained to be equal. This finding is in contrast to Bezdjian et al. [7], who found significant sex differences across 9–10 year old boys and girls, with the affective–interpersonal factor showing higher heritability in boys and the impulsive–antisocial factor showing higher heritability in girls. Similarly, Ficks et al. [25] found sex differences in the Impulsivity dimension, with a higher heritability in girls. Our findings suggest that despite sex differences on a mean level, the underlying genetic and environmental etiology of these traits appears to be similar for both boys and girls. This would in turn indicate that there are specific circumstances (biological) or experiences (social, environmental) that may lead to greater expression of psychopathic personality traits in boys. Future research needs to determine which specific factors that contributes to the sex difference in prevalence.

Limitations

A few limitations in this study must be considered when interpreting these findings. First, we examined the genetic and environmental influences on psychopathic personality traits in a community sample of young twins. Our results may not be generalizable to children in clinical settings. We only had one time point, future research need to investigate how genetic and environmental factors influence change in these traits from early childhood through adolescence and whether the shared environment that we found will decrease across development. There are several assumptions related to the classical twin design [59], for example, the heritability estimate is time and population specific. A more detailed discussion of these and other assumption in the twin design in relation to psychopathology can be found elsewhere [67].

Conclusions

In contrast to research conducted with adolescent and adult twins, we found that both genetic and shared environmental influences are of importance for psychopathy personality traits in childhood. The phenotypic correlations between three dimensions of psychopathic personality grandiose–deceitful, callous–unemotional, and impulsive–need for stimulation were primarily accounted for by common genetic and common shared environmental influences. This highlights the importance of considering all three dimensions of psychopathic personality simultaneously in clinical work as well as in future research, see also [13, 61]. These findings further indicate that etiological models of psychopathic personality should take developmental stages and processes into consideration. This evidence is important for prevention efforts, suggesting that preventions designed to reduce the development of psychopathic personality traits can be successful if administered during the preschool developmental period.

Acknowledgments

We thank the twins, their families and teachers for their participation.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

Henrik Larsson has served as a speaker for Eli Lilly and has received a research grant from Shire; both outside the submitted work. Author Catherine Tuvblad declares that she has no conflict of interest. Author Kostas A. Fanti declares that he has no conflict of interest. Author Henrik Andershed declares that he has no conflict of interest. Author Olivier F. Colins declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Funding

We acknowledge financial support from the Swedish Research Council (2014-3831).

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Psychiatrie, den Premium-Inhalten der psychiatrischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Literatur
1.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, vol 5. American Psychiatric Publishing, ArlingtonCrossRef American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, vol 5. American Psychiatric Publishing, ArlingtonCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Baker LA, Jacobson KC, Raine A, Lozano DI, Bezdjian S (2007) Genetic and environmental bases of childhood antisocial behavior: a multi-informant twin study. J Abnorm Psychol 116(2):219–235PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Baker LA, Jacobson KC, Raine A, Lozano DI, Bezdjian S (2007) Genetic and environmental bases of childhood antisocial behavior: a multi-informant twin study. J Abnorm Psychol 116(2):219–235PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Beaver KM, da Silva Costa C, Poersch AP, Freddi MC, Stelmach MC, Connolly EJ, Schwartz JA (2014) Psychopathic personality traits and their influence on parenting quality: results from a nationally representative sample of Americans. Psychiatr Q 85(4):497–511PubMedCrossRef Beaver KM, da Silva Costa C, Poersch AP, Freddi MC, Stelmach MC, Connolly EJ, Schwartz JA (2014) Psychopathic personality traits and their influence on parenting quality: results from a nationally representative sample of Americans. Psychiatr Q 85(4):497–511PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Beaver, K. M., Vaughn, M. G., and Delisi, M. (2013). Nonshared environmental effects on adulthood psychopathic personality traits: results from a monozygotic twin difference scores analysis. Psychiatr Q (Epub ahead of print) Beaver, K. M., Vaughn, M. G., and Delisi, M. (2013). Nonshared environmental effects on adulthood psychopathic personality traits: results from a monozygotic twin difference scores analysis. Psychiatr Q (Epub ahead of print)
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Bezdjian S, Raine A, Baker LA, Lynam D (2011) Psychopathic personality in children: genetic and environmental contribution. Psychol Med 41(3):589–600PubMedCrossRef Bezdjian S, Raine A, Baker LA, Lynam D (2011) Psychopathic personality in children: genetic and environmental contribution. Psychol Med 41(3):589–600PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Bezdjian S, Tuvblad C, Raine A, Baker LA (2011) The genetic and environmental covariation among psychopathic personality traits, and reactive and proactive aggression in childhood. Child Dev 82(4):1267–1281PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bezdjian S, Tuvblad C, Raine A, Baker LA (2011) The genetic and environmental covariation among psychopathic personality traits, and reactive and proactive aggression in childhood. Child Dev 82(4):1267–1281PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Blair JR, Mitchell DA, Blair K (2007) The psychopath, emotion and the brain. Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Oxford Blair JR, Mitchell DA, Blair K (2007) The psychopath, emotion and the brain. Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Oxford
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Blonigen DM, Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG (2005) Psychopathic personality traits: heritability and genetic overlap with internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Psychol Med 35:637–648PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Blonigen DM, Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG (2005) Psychopathic personality traits: heritability and genetic overlap with internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Psychol Med 35:637–648PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Blonigen DM, Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG (2006) Continuity and change in psychopathic traits as measured via normal-range personality: a longitudinal-biometric study. J Abnorm Psychol 115(1):85–95PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Blonigen DM, Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG (2006) Continuity and change in psychopathic traits as measured via normal-range personality: a longitudinal-biometric study. J Abnorm Psychol 115(1):85–95PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Brook M, Panizzon MS, Kosson DS, Sullivan EA, Lyons MJ, Franz CE, Kremen WS (2010) Psychopathic personality traits in middle-aged male twins: a behavior genetic investigation. J Personal Disord 24(4):473–486CrossRef Brook M, Panizzon MS, Kosson DS, Sullivan EA, Lyons MJ, Franz CE, Kremen WS (2010) Psychopathic personality traits in middle-aged male twins: a behavior genetic investigation. J Personal Disord 24(4):473–486CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Burt SA, Larsson H, Lichtenstein P, Klump KL (2012) Additional evidence against shared environmental contributions to attention–deficit/hyperactivity problems. Behav Genet 42(5):711–721PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Burt SA, Larsson H, Lichtenstein P, Klump KL (2012) Additional evidence against shared environmental contributions to attention–deficit/hyperactivity problems. Behav Genet 42(5):711–721PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Colins OF, Andershed H, Frogner L, Lopez-Romero L, Veen V, Andershed AK (2014) A new measure to assess psychopathic personality in children: the Child Problematic Traits Inventory. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 36(1):4–21PubMedCrossRef Colins OF, Andershed H, Frogner L, Lopez-Romero L, Veen V, Andershed AK (2014) A new measure to assess psychopathic personality in children: the Child Problematic Traits Inventory. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 36(1):4–21PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Colins OF, Fanti K, Larsson H, Anckarsäter H (2016) Psychopathic traits in early childhood: further validation of the Child Problematic Traits Inventory. Assessment:1–16 (epub ahead of print) Colins OF, Fanti K, Larsson H, Anckarsäter H (2016) Psychopathic traits in early childhood: further validation of the Child Problematic Traits Inventory. Assessment:1–16 (epub ahead of print)
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Colins OF, Kostas AF, Larsson H, Andershed H (2015) Psychopathic traits in early childhood: further validation of the Child Problematic Traits Inventory (in preparation) Colins OF, Kostas AF, Larsson H, Andershed H (2015) Psychopathic traits in early childhood: further validation of the Child Problematic Traits Inventory (in preparation)
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Colins OF, Veen V, Venstra M, Frogner L, Andershed H (2016) The Child Problematic Traits Inventory in a Dutch General Population sample of 3- to 7-year-old children. Eur J Psychol Assess. doi:10.1027/1015-5759/a000347 Colins OF, Veen V, Venstra M, Frogner L, Andershed H (2016) The Child Problematic Traits Inventory in a Dutch General Population sample of 3- to 7-year-old children. Eur J Psychol Assess. doi:10.​1027/​1015-5759/​a000347
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Cooke DJ, Michie C (2001) Refining the construct of psychopathy: towards a hierarchical model. Psychol Assess 13(2):171–188PubMedCrossRef Cooke DJ, Michie C (2001) Refining the construct of psychopathy: towards a hierarchical model. Psychol Assess 13(2):171–188PubMedCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat DeFries JC, Fulker DF (1985) Multiple regression analysis of twin data. Behav Genet 15:467–473PubMedCrossRef DeFries JC, Fulker DF (1985) Multiple regression analysis of twin data. Behav Genet 15:467–473PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat DeLisi M, Vaughn M, Beaver KM, Wexler J, Barth AE, Fletcher JM (2011) Fledgling psychopathy in the classroom: ADHD subtypes, psychopathy, and reading comprehension in a community sample of adolescents. Youth Violence Juv Justice 9:43–58PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef DeLisi M, Vaughn M, Beaver KM, Wexler J, Barth AE, Fletcher JM (2011) Fledgling psychopathy in the classroom: ADHD subtypes, psychopathy, and reading comprehension in a community sample of adolescents. Youth Violence Juv Justice 9:43–58PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Dong L, Wu H, Waldman ID (2014) Measurement and structural invariance of the antisocial process screening device. Psychol Assess 26(2):598–608PubMedCrossRef Dong L, Wu H, Waldman ID (2014) Measurement and structural invariance of the antisocial process screening device. Psychol Assess 26(2):598–608PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Douglas KS, Vincent GM, Edens JF (2006) Risk for criminal recidivism: the role of psychopathy. In: Patrick CJ (ed) Handbook of psychopathy. Guilford Press, New York, pp 533–554 Douglas KS, Vincent GM, Edens JF (2006) Risk for criminal recidivism: the role of psychopathy. In: Patrick CJ (ed) Handbook of psychopathy. Guilford Press, New York, pp 533–554
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Fairchild G, van Goozen SH, Calder AJ, Goodyer IM (2013) Research review: evaluating and reformulating the developmental taxonomic theory of antisocial behaviour. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54(9):924–940PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Fairchild G, van Goozen SH, Calder AJ, Goodyer IM (2013) Research review: evaluating and reformulating the developmental taxonomic theory of antisocial behaviour. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54(9):924–940PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Fanti KA, Demetriou CA, Kimonis ER (2013) Variants of callous–unemotional conduct problems in a community sample of adolescents. J Youth Adolesc 42(7):964–979PubMedCrossRef Fanti KA, Demetriou CA, Kimonis ER (2013) Variants of callous–unemotional conduct problems in a community sample of adolescents. J Youth Adolesc 42(7):964–979PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Fanti KA, Panayiotou G, Lazarou C, Michael R, Georgiou G (2016) The better of two evils? Evidence that children exhibiting continuous conduct problems high or low on callous–unemotional traits score on opposite directions on physiological and behavioral measures of fear. Dev Psychopathol 28(1):185–198. doi:10.1017/S0954579415000371 PubMedCrossRef Fanti KA, Panayiotou G, Lazarou C, Michael R, Georgiou G (2016) The better of two evils? Evidence that children exhibiting continuous conduct problems high or low on callous–unemotional traits score on opposite directions on physiological and behavioral measures of fear. Dev Psychopathol 28(1):185–198. doi:10.​1017/​S095457941500037​1 PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Ficks CA, Dong L, Waldman ID (2014) Sex differences in the etiology of psychopathic traits in youth. J Abnorm Psychol 123(2):406–411PubMedCrossRef Ficks CA, Dong L, Waldman ID (2014) Sex differences in the etiology of psychopathic traits in youth. J Abnorm Psychol 123(2):406–411PubMedCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Fontaine N, Barker ED, Salekin RT, Viding E (2008) Dimensions of psychopathy and their relationships to cognitive functioning in children. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 37(3):690–696PubMedCrossRef Fontaine N, Barker ED, Salekin RT, Viding E (2008) Dimensions of psychopathy and their relationships to cognitive functioning in children. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 37(3):690–696PubMedCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Fontaine NM, Hanscombe KB, Berg MT, McCrory E, Viding E (2016) Trajectories of callous–unemotional traits in childhood predict different forms of peer victimization in adolescence. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 28(1):1–9CrossRef Fontaine NM, Hanscombe KB, Berg MT, McCrory E, Viding E (2016) Trajectories of callous–unemotional traits in childhood predict different forms of peer victimization in adolescence. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 28(1):1–9CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Fontaine NM, McCrory EJ, Boivin M, Moffitt TE, Viding E (2011) Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous–unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. J Abnorm Psychol 120(3):730–742PubMedCrossRef Fontaine NM, McCrory EJ, Boivin M, Moffitt TE, Viding E (2011) Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous–unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. J Abnorm Psychol 120(3):730–742PubMedCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Fontaine NM, Rijsdijk FV, McCrory EJ, Viding E (2010) Etiology of different developmental trajectories of callous–unemotional traits. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49(7):656–664PubMed Fontaine NM, Rijsdijk FV, McCrory EJ, Viding E (2010) Etiology of different developmental trajectories of callous–unemotional traits. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49(7):656–664PubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Forsman M, Lichtenstein P, Andershed H, Larsson H (2008) Genetic effects explain the stability of psychopathic personality from mid- to late adolescence. J Abnorm Psychol 117(3):606–617PubMedCrossRef Forsman M, Lichtenstein P, Andershed H, Larsson H (2008) Genetic effects explain the stability of psychopathic personality from mid- to late adolescence. J Abnorm Psychol 117(3):606–617PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Frick PJ, Hare RD (2001) Antisocial Process Screening Device Toronto. Multi-Health Systems, Canada Frick PJ, Hare RD (2001) Antisocial Process Screening Device Toronto. Multi-Health Systems, Canada
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Frick PJ, Moffitt TE (2010) A proposal to the DSM-V childhood disorders and the ADHD and disruptive behavior disorders work groups to include a specifier to the diagnosis of conduct disorder based on the presence of callous–unemotional traits. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC Frick PJ, Moffitt TE (2010) A proposal to the DSM-V childhood disorders and the ADHD and disruptive behavior disorders work groups to include a specifier to the diagnosis of conduct disorder based on the presence of callous–unemotional traits. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE (2014) Can callous–unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychol Bull 140(1):1–57PubMedCrossRef Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE (2014) Can callous–unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychol Bull 140(1):1–57PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Gao Y, Glenn AL, Schug RA, Yang Y, Raine A (2009) The neurobiology of psychopathy: a neurodevelopmental perspective. Can J Psychiatry 54(12):813–823PubMedCrossRef Gao Y, Glenn AL, Schug RA, Yang Y, Raine A (2009) The neurobiology of psychopathy: a neurodevelopmental perspective. Can J Psychiatry 54(12):813–823PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Gao Y, Raine A (2010) Successful and unsuccessful psychopaths: a neurobiological model. Behav Sci Law 28:194–210PubMed Gao Y, Raine A (2010) Successful and unsuccessful psychopaths: a neurobiological model. Behav Sci Law 28:194–210PubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodman R (1997) The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 38(5):581–586PubMedCrossRef Goodman R (1997) The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 38(5):581–586PubMedCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Hare RD (2003) The Hare psychopathy checklist-revised (PCL-R), 2nd edn. Multi-Health Systems, Toronto Hare RD (2003) The Hare psychopathy checklist-revised (PCL-R), 2nd edn. Multi-Health Systems, Toronto
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Hemphill JF, Templeman R, Wong S, Hare RD (1998) Psychopathy and crime: recidivism and criminal careers. In: Cooke DJ, Forth AE, Hare RD (eds) Psychopathy: Theory, research and implications for society. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 375–399CrossRef Hemphill JF, Templeman R, Wong S, Hare RD (1998) Psychopathy and crime: recidivism and criminal careers. In: Cooke DJ, Forth AE, Hare RD (eds) Psychopathy: Theory, research and implications for society. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 375–399CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Hemphälä M, Hodgins S (2014) Do psychopathic traits assessed in mid-adolescence predict mental health, psychosocial, and antisocial, including criminal outcomes, over the subsequent 5 years? Can J Psychiatry 59(1):40–49PubMedPubMedCentral Hemphälä M, Hodgins S (2014) Do psychopathic traits assessed in mid-adolescence predict mental health, psychosocial, and antisocial, including criminal outcomes, over the subsequent 5 years? Can J Psychiatry 59(1):40–49PubMedPubMedCentral
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Herpers PC, Rommelse NN, Bons DM, Buitelaar JK, Scheepers FE (2012) callous–unemotional traits as a cross-disorders construct. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 47(12):2045–2064PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Herpers PC, Rommelse NN, Bons DM, Buitelaar JK, Scheepers FE (2012) callous–unemotional traits as a cross-disorders construct. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 47(12):2045–2064PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Hicks BM, Vaidyanathan U, Patrick CJ (2010) Validating female psychopathy subtypes: differences in personality, antisocial and violent behavior, substance abuse, trauma, and mental health. Personal Disord 1(1):38–57PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hicks BM, Vaidyanathan U, Patrick CJ (2010) Validating female psychopathy subtypes: differences in personality, antisocial and violent behavior, substance abuse, trauma, and mental health. Personal Disord 1(1):38–57PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Humayun S, Kahn RE, Frick PJ, Viding E (2014) Callous–unemotional traits and anxiety in a community sample of 7-year-olds. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 43(1):36–42PubMedCrossRef Humayun S, Kahn RE, Frick PJ, Viding E (2014) Callous–unemotional traits and anxiety in a community sample of 7-year-olds. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 43(1):36–42PubMedCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Hunt E, Bornovalova MA, Patrick CJ (2014) Genetic and environmental overlap between borderline personality disorder traits and psychopathy: evidence for promotive effects of factor 2 and protective effects of factor 1. Psychol Med 30:1–11 (epub ahead of print) Hunt E, Bornovalova MA, Patrick CJ (2014) Genetic and environmental overlap between borderline personality disorder traits and psychopathy: evidence for promotive effects of factor 2 and protective effects of factor 1. Psychol Med 30:1–11 (epub ahead of print)
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Isen J, Raine A, Baker LA, Dawson ME, Bezdjian S, Lozano DI (2010) Sex-specific association between psychopathic traits and electrodermal reactivity in children. J Abnorm Psychol 119(1):216–225PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Isen J, Raine A, Baker LA, Dawson ME, Bezdjian S, Lozano DI (2010) Sex-specific association between psychopathic traits and electrodermal reactivity in children. J Abnorm Psychol 119(1):216–225PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Keller MC, Medland SE, Duncan LE (2010) Are extended twin family designs worth the trouble? A comparison of the bias, precision, and accuracy of parameters estimated in four twin family models. Behav Genet 40(3):377–393PubMedCrossRef Keller MC, Medland SE, Duncan LE (2010) Are extended twin family designs worth the trouble? A comparison of the bias, precision, and accuracy of parameters estimated in four twin family models. Behav Genet 40(3):377–393PubMedCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Larsson H, Andershed H, Lichtenstein P (2006) A genetic factor explains most of the variation in the psychopathic personality. J Abnorm Psychol 115(2):221–230PubMedCrossRef Larsson H, Andershed H, Lichtenstein P (2006) A genetic factor explains most of the variation in the psychopathic personality. J Abnorm Psychol 115(2):221–230PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Larsson H, Tuvblad C, Rijsdijk FV, Andershed H, Grann M, Lichtenstein P (2007) A common genetic factor explains the association between psychopathic personality and antisocial behavior. Psychol Med 37(1):15–26PubMedCrossRef Larsson H, Tuvblad C, Rijsdijk FV, Andershed H, Grann M, Lichtenstein P (2007) A common genetic factor explains the association between psychopathic personality and antisocial behavior. Psychol Med 37(1):15–26PubMedCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Larsson H, Viding E, Plomin R (2008) Callous–unemotional traits and antisocial behavior. Crim Justice Behav 35:197–211CrossRef Larsson H, Viding E, Plomin R (2008) Callous–unemotional traits and antisocial behavior. Crim Justice Behav 35:197–211CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Lynam DR (1997) Pursuing the psychopath: capturing the fledgling psychopath in a nomological net. J Abnorm Psychol 106(3):425–438PubMedCrossRef Lynam DR (1997) Pursuing the psychopath: capturing the fledgling psychopath in a nomological net. J Abnorm Psychol 106(3):425–438PubMedCrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Lynam DR (2002) Fledgling psychopathy: a view from personality theory. Law Hum Behav 26(2):255–259PubMedCrossRef Lynam DR (2002) Fledgling psychopathy: a view from personality theory. Law Hum Behav 26(2):255–259PubMedCrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Murray J, Farrington DP (2010) Risk factors for conduct disorder and delinquency: key findings from longitudinal studies. Can J Psychiatry 55(10):633–642PubMedCrossRef Murray J, Farrington DP (2010) Risk factors for conduct disorder and delinquency: key findings from longitudinal studies. Can J Psychiatry 55(10):633–642PubMedCrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Neale MC, Boker SM, Xie G, Maes H (2003) Mx: statistical modeling, 6th edn. Department of Psychiatry, Medical College of Virginia, Richmond Neale MC, Boker SM, Xie G, Maes H (2003) Mx: statistical modeling, 6th edn. Department of Psychiatry, Medical College of Virginia, Richmond
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Neale MC, Cardon LR (1992) Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Kluwer Academic Publications, DordrechtCrossRef Neale MC, Cardon LR (1992) Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Kluwer Academic Publications, DordrechtCrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Neumann CS, Hare RD (2008) Psychopathic traits in a large community sample: links to violence, alcohol use, and intelligence. J Consult Clin Psychol 76(5):893–899PubMedCrossRef Neumann CS, Hare RD (2008) Psychopathic traits in a large community sample: links to violence, alcohol use, and intelligence. J Consult Clin Psychol 76(5):893–899PubMedCrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Nions E, Tick B, Rijsdijk F, Happé F, Plomin R, Ronald A, Viding E (2015) Examining the genetic and environmental associations between autistic social and communication deficits and psychopathic callous–unemotional traits. PLoS One 10(9):e0134331PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef O’Nions E, Tick B, Rijsdijk F, Happé F, Plomin R, Ronald A, Viding E (2015) Examining the genetic and environmental associations between autistic social and communication deficits and psychopathic callous–unemotional traits. PLoS One 10(9):e0134331PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Pechorro P, Maroco J, Gonçalves RA, Nunes C, Jesus SN (2014) Psychopathic traits and age of crime onset in male juvenile delinquents. Eur J Criminol 58(12):1412–1414 Pechorro P, Maroco J, Gonçalves RA, Nunes C, Jesus SN (2014) Psychopathic traits and age of crime onset in male juvenile delinquents. Eur J Criminol 58(12):1412–1414
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, McGuffin P (2008) Behavioral genetics, 5th edn. Worth Publisher, United States of America Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, McGuffin P (2008) Behavioral genetics, 5th edn. Worth Publisher, United States of America
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Raftery AE (1995) Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociol Methodol 25:111–163CrossRef Raftery AE (1995) Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociol Methodol 25:111–163CrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Salekin RT (2016) Psychopathy in childhood: toward better informing the DSM–5 and ICD-11 conduct disorder specifiers. Personal Disord Theory Res Treat 2:180–191CrossRef Salekin RT (2016) Psychopathy in childhood: toward better informing the DSM–5 and ICD-11 conduct disorder specifiers. Personal Disord Theory Res Treat 2:180–191CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith SS, Newman JP (1990) Alcohol and drug abuse/dependence disorders in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic criminal offenders. J Abnorm Psychol 99:430–439PubMedCrossRef Smith SS, Newman JP (1990) Alcohol and drug abuse/dependence disorders in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic criminal offenders. J Abnorm Psychol 99:430–439PubMedCrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Somma A, Andershed H, Borroni S, Fossati A (2016) The validity of the child problematic trait inventory in 6–12 year old Italian children: further support and issues of consistency across different sources of information and different samples. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 38(3):350–372CrossRef Somma A, Andershed H, Borroni S, Fossati A (2016) The validity of the child problematic trait inventory in 6–12 year old Italian children: further support and issues of consistency across different sources of information and different samples. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 38(3):350–372CrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Taylor A, Loney BR, Bobadilla L, Iacono WG, McGue M (2003) Genetic and environmental influences on psychopathy trait dimensions in a community sample of male twins. J Abnorm Child Psychol 31(6):633–645PubMedCrossRef Taylor A, Loney BR, Bobadilla L, Iacono WG, McGue M (2003) Genetic and environmental influences on psychopathy trait dimensions in a community sample of male twins. J Abnorm Child Psychol 31(6):633–645PubMedCrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Thompson DF, Ramos C, Willett JK (2014) Psychopathy: clinical features, developmental basis and therapeutic challenges. J Clin Pharm Ther 39(5):485–495PubMedCrossRef Thompson DF, Ramos C, Willett JK (2014) Psychopathy: clinical features, developmental basis and therapeutic challenges. J Clin Pharm Ther 39(5):485–495PubMedCrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Tuvblad C (2014) Genetic influences on antisocial behavior over the life course. In: Vaughn MG, DeLisi M (eds) Handbook of biosocial criminology. Routledge, London, pp 77–100 Tuvblad C (2014) Genetic influences on antisocial behavior over the life course. In: Vaughn MG, DeLisi M (eds) Handbook of biosocial criminology. Routledge, London, pp 77–100
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Tuvblad C, Bezdjian S, Raine A, Baker LA (2013) Psychopathic personality traits and negative parent-to-child affect: a longitudinal cross-lag twin study. J Crim Justice 41(5):331–341CrossRef Tuvblad C, Bezdjian S, Raine A, Baker LA (2013) Psychopathic personality traits and negative parent-to-child affect: a longitudinal cross-lag twin study. J Crim Justice 41(5):331–341CrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Tuvblad C, Bezdjian S, Raine A, Baker LA (2014) The heritability of psychopathic personality in 14 to 15 years old twins, multi-rater and multi-measure approach. Psychol Assess 26(3):704–716PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Tuvblad C, Bezdjian S, Raine A, Baker LA (2014) The heritability of psychopathic personality in 14 to 15 years old twins, multi-rater and multi-measure approach. Psychol Assess 26(3):704–716PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Tuvblad C, Wang P, Bezdjian S, Raine A, Baker LA (2015) Psychopathic personality development from ages 9 to 18: genes and environment. Dev Psychopathol 1:27–44 Tuvblad C, Wang P, Bezdjian S, Raine A, Baker LA (2015) Psychopathic personality development from ages 9 to 18: genes and environment. Dev Psychopathol 1:27–44
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Ullrich S, Farrington DP, Coid JW (2008) Psychopathic personality traits and life-success. Personal Individ Differ 44:1162–1171CrossRef Ullrich S, Farrington DP, Coid JW (2008) Psychopathic personality traits and life-success. Personal Individ Differ 44:1162–1171CrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Waldman ID, Rhee S (2006) Genetic and environmental influences on psychopathy and antisocial behavior. In: Patrick CJ (ed) Handbook of psychopathy. Guilford, New York, pp 205–228 Waldman ID, Rhee S (2006) Genetic and environmental influences on psychopathy and antisocial behavior. In: Patrick CJ (ed) Handbook of psychopathy. Guilford, New York, pp 205–228
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang P, Baker LA, Gao Y, Raine A, Lozano DI (2012) Psychopathic traits and physiological responses to aversive stimuli in children aged 9–11 years. J Abnorm Child Psychol 40(5):759–769PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wang P, Baker LA, Gao Y, Raine A, Lozano DI (2012) Psychopathic traits and physiological responses to aversive stimuli in children aged 9–11 years. J Abnorm Child Psychol 40(5):759–769PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Wang P, Gao Y, Isen J, Tuvblad C, Raine A, Baker LA (2015) Genetic covariance between psychopathic traits and anticipatory skin conductance responses to threat: evidence for a potential endophenotype. Dev Psychopathol 27(4 Pt 1):1313–1322PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wang P, Gao Y, Isen J, Tuvblad C, Raine A, Baker LA (2015) Genetic covariance between psychopathic traits and anticipatory skin conductance responses to threat: evidence for a potential endophenotype. Dev Psychopathol 27(4 Pt 1):1313–1322PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Verona E, Vitale J (2006) Psychopathy in women. In: Patrick CJ (ed) Handbook of psychopathy. Guilford Press, NY, pp 415–436 Verona E, Vitale J (2006) Psychopathy in women. In: Patrick CJ (ed) Handbook of psychopathy. Guilford Press, NY, pp 415–436
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Viding E, Blair JR, Moffitt TE, Plomin R (2005) Evidence of substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46(6):592–597PubMedCrossRef Viding E, Blair JR, Moffitt TE, Plomin R (2005) Evidence of substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46(6):592–597PubMedCrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Viding E, Fontaine NM, McCrory EJ (2012) Antisocial behaviour in children with and without callous–unemotional traits. J R Soc Med 105(5):195–200PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Viding E, Fontaine NM, McCrory EJ (2012) Antisocial behaviour in children with and without callous–unemotional traits. J R Soc Med 105(5):195–200PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Viding E, Fontaine NM, Oliver BR, Plomin R (2009) Negative parental discipline, conduct problems and callous–unemotional traits: monozygotic twin differences study. Br J Psychiatry 195(5):414–419PubMedCrossRef Viding E, Fontaine NM, Oliver BR, Plomin R (2009) Negative parental discipline, conduct problems and callous–unemotional traits: monozygotic twin differences study. Br J Psychiatry 195(5):414–419PubMedCrossRef
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Viding E, Frick PJ, Plomin R (2007) Aetiology of the relationship between callous–unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. Br J Psychiatry 49:33–38CrossRef Viding E, Frick PJ, Plomin R (2007) Aetiology of the relationship between callous–unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. Br J Psychiatry 49:33–38CrossRef
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Viding E, Jones AP, Frick PJ, Moffitt TE, Plomin R (2008) Heritability of antisocial behaviour at 9: do callous–unemotional traits matter? Dev Sci 11:17–22PubMedCrossRef Viding E, Jones AP, Frick PJ, Moffitt TE, Plomin R (2008) Heritability of antisocial behaviour at 9: do callous–unemotional traits matter? Dev Sci 11:17–22PubMedCrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Viding E, McCrory EJ (2012) Genetic and neurocognitive contributions to the development of psychopathy. Dev Psychopathol 24(3):969–983PubMedCrossRef Viding E, McCrory EJ (2012) Genetic and neurocognitive contributions to the development of psychopathy. Dev Psychopathol 24(3):969–983PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Psychopathic personality traits in 5 year old twins: the importance of genetic and shared environmental influences
verfasst von
Catherine Tuvblad
Kostas A. Fanti
Henrik Andershed
Olivier F. Colins
Henrik Larsson
Publikationsdatum
28.09.2016
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry / Ausgabe 4/2017
Print ISSN: 1018-8827
Elektronische ISSN: 1435-165X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-016-0899-1

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2017

European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 4/2017 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.