Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 6/2019

Open Access 13.02.2019 | Original Paper

Recognising autism: a latent transition analysis of parental reports of child autistic spectrum disorder ‘red flag’ traits before and after age 3

verfasst von: Amanda Spikol, Donal McAteer, Jamie Murphy

Erschienen in: Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology | Ausgabe 6/2019

Abstract

Purpose

It has been proposed that parents should be educated about child autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) ‘red flag’ traits to help professionals identify and address concerning behaviours as early as possible. This study aimed to empirically demonstrate that established/recognised ‘red flag’ traits in the first 3 years of life would reliably predict ASD risk severity in later childhood, associated with established ASD risk correlates and mirroring functioning diagnostic categories.

Methods

Using retrospective parental report data from the Mental Health of Children and Young People in Great Britain survey (N = 7977), latent class analysis (LCA) and a quasi -latent transition analysis were used to (1) identify profiles of variation in parent reports of child ‘red flag’ traits before and after age 3 and (2) model transitions in risk from 3 years and below to ≥ 3 years, respectively, per the ‘optimal outcome’ model.

Results

Three distinct classes, each characterised by variation in parent ‘red flag’ trait reporting were identified for the ‘≤ 3 years of age’ and the ‘≥ 3 years of age’ data. Both LCA class profiles comprised groups of children characterised by low, medium and high ASD risk. Dose–response effects for a number of recognised ASD correlates across the low, moderate and high risk ‘≥ 3 years of age’ classes seemed to validate older classes in terms of ASD relevance. Over 54% of children characterised by the highest levels of ASD ‘red flag’ trait probability at 3 years and below (2% of sample), also populated the high-risk class evidenced in the ‘≥ 3 years of age’ LCA.

Conclusions

Retrospective parental reports of child ASD ‘red flag’ traits ≤ 3 years of age were reliable indicators of ASD risk in later childhood.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a lifelong neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by difficulties in social interaction, communication, and repetitive behaviours [1]. It is commonly described as a childhood disorder, as symptoms often first become apparent during early development. Its symptoms are diverse and behaviours associated with ASD vary in expression among those with the disorder, which can often make diagnosis difficult [2]. Recently, diagnostic categories for variants of autism, e.g. pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS) and Asperger’s syndrome (AS), have been reclassified as ASD in the DSM-5 [1], (see [35] for re-diagnosis review). ASD has been recognised as a more meaningful and sufficiently inclusive construct to describe the variations in behaviour, functioning, and presentation that commonly characterise the phenotype [68]. However, there is evidence that individuals with these variant diagnoses differ significantly from more severe ASD in intelligence [911], verbal ability [12], and overall cognitive profile [13, 14], including discreet differences in brain morphology [15]. The diagnostic definitions used in assessment have changed despite evidence of differences in symptomology between PDD-NOS/AS and ASD [16] with these differences in behavioural trait severity evident in early life and showing trait stability [17].
During the first years of a child’s life, parents and caretakers are most likely to be the first to observe, evaluate and interpret a child’s behaviour. They are also often the first to seek professional advice when a child’s behaviour seems ‘odd’ or ‘unusual’ [18, 19]. While developmental variation in the population is to be expected, a cluster of behaviours or missed milestones (‘red flags’) can signal the presence of a potential underlying problem and may be indicative of disordered behaviour [20]. Parental ‘prediction’ of ASD diagnosis via problematic behaviours has been shown to be reliable, both in prospective and longitudinal studies [21, 22] and in studies utilising retrospective population data [23, 24].
It has been proposed, therefore, that assessment should take place as soon as developmental issues become apparent [25, 26]. The average ASD diagnosis age in the US is 4 years [27] though diagnosis can be made as early as 2 years [28] with predictive diagnostic tools existing for those as young as 12 months [29]. Early intervention strategies have been shown to be advantageous in capitalising on toddlers’ developmental plasticity [30] and have shown significant benefits in adaptive behaviour, language, and overall functioning [3133].
While critics of early diagnosis cite ‘normal’ slow development and ASD false-positive diagnoses [34] the benefits of early diagnosis and intervention have been strongly advocated. Initial diagnoses are routinely revisited and show early behavioural symptoms to be consistent with ASD outcomes [3537]. Early intervention has been shown to be beneficial for the development of cognitive, social, and communication skills [38] and because further refinement of these skills comes with growth and experience, an early ASD diagnosis does not always predict diagnostic outcomes or level of deficit in later childhood [36]. A favourable or ‘optimal outcome’ (OO) of improvement in functioning to the point the child loses the ASD diagnosis is not a recent concept [39]. Recent research has highlighted that OO children still show deficits in social relationships and some developmental issues that affect social functioning [40, 41] but can function normally with typically developing children. Higher initial functioning in combination with early intervention seems to be the strongest predictor of OO [4244] and while trait expression and behaviour improve out of the clinical range, physical brain activity is closer to an ASD population [45].
The current study, therefore, utilised retrospective data with the aim to explore the variation in parent reports of child ‘red flag’ traits, before and after age 3, in the general population, to test (1) whether a spectrum of presentation in behavioural (‘red flag’) traits could be identified in both early and later childhood while displaying the variance of previous autism diagnostic categories, (2) whether severity in ‘red flag’ traits ≥ 3 years would be associated with established ASD risk correlates and (3) whether severity in ‘red flag’ traits ≤ 3 years of age would meaningfully correlate with severity in later childhood or an ‘optimal outcome’. It was predicted that distinct profiles of ‘red flag’ traits would emerge for both time periods and that these profiles would reflect variation in ASD risk mirroring the PDD-NOS/AS/HFA variant diagnoses. It was also predicted that parental reporting of ‘red flag’ traits would be a reliable indicator of ASD risk and that this would be demonstrated via dose–response associations with established ASD risk correlates. Finally, given that extant evidence supports the ‘parental concern’ model, with concerns raised in specific developmental domains between the ages of 1 and 3 years correlating with ASD diagnosis and later diagnostic outcomes [4650] it was hypothesised that the most severe ‘red flag’ trait profiles, retrospectively reported by parents, at ≤ 3 years of age, would be associated with the severity of ‘red flag’ trait reporting in later childhood in keeping with previous ‘optimal outcome’ findings.

Method

Sample

Data for this study were sourced from the national survey, Mental Health of Children and Young People in Great Britain (GB), 2004 [51], collected by the Office of National Statistics (ONS; all analyses were performed in accordance with ONS ethical and data handling regulations); the survey was a thorough census of medical, emotional, and social health of young people in GB. A sample was obtained from the Department for Work and Pensions’ Child Benefit Centre (CBC) based on Child Benefit records and from this, a sample of postal areas in England, Scotland, and Wales, and ultimately a random sample of addresses was selected, excluding any household where the CBC had an ‘action’ open such as child death or CBC involvement, describing these cases as ‘sensitive’. This census was multiphasic, conducted in 1999 and 2004. As the chosen measure did not appear in the 1999 phase, this study utilises only the 2004 data.
The resultant sample of 12,294 households was contacted via post by the CBC with survey details/opt-out instructions with 1085 (9%) opting out, 631 (5%) having moved, 82 (1%) being found ineligible, and 1798 (15%) not approached for an interview. Of the 10,496 approached for an interview, 2183 (21%) refused and 313 (3%) could not be contacted, leaving a sample of 7977 households where an ONS representative conducted interviews with a parent. The final interview sample (N = 7977) was 52% male (4111) with a mean age of 10.54 (SD 3.40) and range of 4–17.

Measures

A general development questionnaire was created by the ONS for this survey, containing a parent/caregiver directed autism sub-questionnaire of 43 items. Led by five binary-answer anchor questions pertaining to the parent/caregiver’s child before the age of 3, each concerned an area of potential autistic behaviour:
1.
Was there anything that seriously worried you or anyone else about the way his/her speech developed?
 
2.
Was there anything that seriously worried you or anyone else about how s/he got on with other people?
 
3.
Was there anything that seriously worried you or anyone else about the way his/her pretend or make-believe play developed?
 
4.
Was there anything that seriously worried you or anyone else about any odd rituals or unusual habits that were very hard to interrupt?
 
5.
Was there anything that seriously worried you or anyone else about his/her ability to learn and do new things—such as puzzles or helping get dressed?
 
Following was a binary-answer gatekeeper question ‘Have the things that seriously worried you or someone else now cleared up completely?’ A positive answer ended the autism section while a negative answer led to the remaining autism questions. Ten questions were selected from the body of this questionnaire; two questions to approximate each of the five behaviours detailed in the anchor questions (see “Appendix 1”) acting as broad early ‘red flag’ behavioural markers [37].

Additional data

The Mental Health of Children and Young People in Great Britain, 2004 [51] survey also involved a full health questionnaire, including ICD-10 criteria of mental and physical health issues. Four known comorbid conditions for autism were chosen as covariates for analysis; epilepsy, learning difficulties, poor coordination, and any anxiety disorder. These were scored as binary variables indicating either presence or absence of each condition. In addition, an ICD-10 diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder was also used as a validator in support of the latent factor typified by ‘red flag’ behaviours being autistic behavioural traits.

Analytic strategy

LTA is a longitudinal modelling technique used to examine whether individuals transition between latent classes over time. LTA consists of two components; a measurement model and an autoregressive model [52, 53]. In LTA, the measurement model (i.e. LCA) describes the structure of the latent classes at the various time points. The autoregressive model (i.e. Markov model) examines individual-level transitions between these classes over time [52, 53]. For a much more detailed description of LTA and its applications in social and behavioural sciences see Nylund [54]. LTA was conducted in the following steps.

Step 1: determine the best measurement model

To determine the best measurement model, a series of LCAs were specified and tested separately at the two time points using available binary red flag ASD trait variables as observed indicators. The first LCA was used to determine the number and qualities of sub-types of autistic trait variation (red flags) based on endorsement of each of the five behavioural anchor questions (≤ 3 years of age) from the general development questionnaire devised by the ONS. These five items were binary and treated as categorical. Three latent class models were tested (a 2-through a four-class latent class model). The second LCA was used to determine the number and qualities of sub-types of autistic trait variation based on endorsement of each of the ten behavioural questions from the body of the questionnaire relating to red flag traits ≥ 3 years of age. These items were treated as categorical. Three latent class models were tested (a 2 through to a four-class latent class model).
Models were compared using a range of common fit statistics. The Akaike information criterion (AIC) [55], the Bayesian information criterion (BIC) [56], and the sample size-adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion (ssaBIC) [57] were used to compare model fit, with lower values indicative of better fit. The Lo–Mendel–Rubin likelihood ratio test (LMR-LRT) is used to compare a solution with k number of classes with a solution with k − 1 classes [58]. A non-significant p value indicates that the model with k − 1 classes provides a better fit [58]. Model fit was also assessed using the entropy criterion [59]. This statistic determines how accurately individuals were assigned to their classes based on the posterior probabilities [59]. Entropy values range from 0 to 1, with higher values reflecting more accurate classification [59]. To ensure that the models converged on global rather than local solutions, 100 random sets of starting values and 50 final stage optimizations were used.
It remains debated whether the LMR-LRT or BIC is more useful when it comes to determining the optimal number of classes in an LCA [54]. A number of simulation studies suggest that the BIC is highly effective at identifying the correct underlying class structure, while the LMR-LRT can occasionally extract too many classes when the sample size is large (N > 1000) [54, 60]. Given that the sample size was relatively large in the present study, the BIC was considered a more reliable indicator of the optimal class solution.

Step 2: validate classes using ASD diagnosis and clinical correlates

Multinomial logistic regression was used to test whether the red flag trait classes/typologies at time one were meaningful in relation to ASD. Associations between class membership and an ASD diagnosis variable and four common clinical correlates of ASD was conducted.

Step 3: specify latent transition model

In this step, the LTA model is specified, producing a matrix of latent transition probabilities. This model affords the opportunity to classify individuals as ‘movers’ (i.e. those who transition from one class to a different class over time) or ‘stayers’ (i.e. those who remain in the same class across time) [52, 53]. Mover–stayer models more accurately describe transitions between classes, as transition probabilities are estimated for ‘movers’ only [52, 53].
All analyses included the first-stage sampling were made weighted variables to account for non-equal probabilities of selection. This standardising technique adds an additional ‘weight’ to under-represented sub-populations that may not be accurately represented due to missing data and was used rather than excluding cases listwise. Analyses were conducted using Mplus 4 [61].

Results

Table 1 (section a) shows the fit indices for the first latent class analysis (≤ 3 years of age). The three-class solution was the model of best fit; the likelihood ratio Chi square was non-significant, the AIC was lower for the three-class solution than for the two-class solution, and the Lo–Mendell–Rubin’s LRT showed that the four-class solution was not significantly better than the three-class solution. The entropy value (0.83) also showed a meaningful classification of cases.
Table 1
Fit indices for the latent class analyses
 
LRχ2 (df) p
AIC
BIC
SSABIC
LRT (p)
Entropy
A (≤ 3 years)
 2 class
229.81 (20) < 0.01
16,831.08
16,907.72
16,872.77
2557.63 (< 0.01)
0.91
 3 class
50.10 (14) < 0.01
16,661.71
16,780.16
16,726.14
178.06 (< 0.01)
0.83
 4 class
16.41 (8) 0.03
16,639.72
16,799.98
16,726.89
33.36 (0.02)
0.85
 5 class
1.32 (2) 0.51
16,636.51
16,838.58
16,746.42
14.93 (0.01)
0.85
B (≥ 3 years)
 2 class
888.92 (981) 0.98
8950.98
9047.81
8981.12
510.67 (< 0.01)
0.67
 3 class
800.25 (970) 1.0
8880.38
9027.92
8926.31
91.34 (0.08)
0.72
 4 class
744.87 (959) 1.0
8849.06
9047.32
8910.78
52.59 (0.33)
0.67
 5 class
716.87 (951) 1.0
8839.72
9088.69
8917.22
30.91 (0.51)
0.73
\(LR{\chi ^2}\) likelihood ratio Chi square, AIC Akaike information criterion, BIC Bayesian information criterion, SSABIC sample size adjusted BIC, LRT Lo–Mendell–Rubin’s adjusted likelihood ratio test
The three-class model, shown in Fig. 1, revealed a ‘High endorsement class’ comprised of 1.9% of the population where the probability of ‘red flag’ trait endorsement was > 70% for all five traits. A larger ‘Moderate endorsement class’ also emerged, representing 10.8% of the population and was characterised by moderate endorsement probabilities of language, social, and developmental problem behaviours. A large ‘Low endorsement class’ (baseline class) comprised of 87.3% of the population was characterised by extremely low endorsement probabilities (< 10%) across all ‘red flag’ traits.
A second LCA was carried out to identify distinct groups characterised by red flag traits after the age of 3 using 10 questions from the body of the questionnaire (see “Appendix 1”); two exemplifying each of the five behaviours described by the anchor questions. Another three-class solution emerged. Table 1 (section B) shows the fit indices for the second latent class analysis. The three-class solution was the model of best fit; the AIC was lower for the three-class solution than for the two-class and the Lo–Mendell–Rubin’s LRT showed that the four-class solution was not significantly better than the three-class solution. The entropy value (0.72) showed a meaningful classification of cases. Probability estimates for class membership in both LCAs were robust.
The three-class model for the ‘≥ 3 years of age’ data, shown in Fig. 2, revealed a small moderate presentation class; 16.7% of the sample characterised by varied endorsement of the five ‘red flag’ behaviour categories. A larger high presentation class emerged, where 31.5% of the sample were characterised by high endorsement probabilities of most items. A larger low presentation class was also evident, 51.8% of the sample which was characterised mainly by moderate endorsement probabilities relating to pretend/play and ritual/habit traits, with a higher endorsement of language issues.
A multinomial logistic regression was conducted to test whether severity in ‘red flag’ traits ≤ 3 years would be associated with established ASD risk correlates (see Table 2). The ASD risk variables showed significant odds ratios across the high and moderate classes when compared to the low class. Moderate presentation was described by highly significant odds ratios of epilepsy, learning difficulties, poor coordination, and anxiety and by significant odds ratios of ASD diagnosis. High presentation showed very highly significant odds ratios of epilepsy, learning difficulties, poor coordination, and ASD diagnosis and highly significant odds ratios of anxiety.
Table 2
ASD risk for high, moderate, and low presentations at ≤ 3 years of age
 
Odds ratio (95% confidence intervals)
High
Moderate
Epilepsy (absence)
36.72*** (8.96–150.58)
4.94*** (2.02–12.08)
Learning D (absence)
70.36*** (33.63–147.22)
8.23*** (5.87–11.52)
Coordination (absence)
26.55*** (11.36–62.08)
14.37*** (7.41–27.87)
Anxiety (absence)
3.47** (1.36–8.88)
3.40*** (2.11–5.48)
ASD diagnosis (absence)
954*** (57.61–15,830.94)
20.73* (1.12–382.38)
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001
A latent transition analysis (LTA) was used to identify transitions in class membership from early to later childhood (see Tables 3, 4).
Table 3
Average latent class probabilities for most likely latent class membership
 
High (%)
Moderate (%)
Low (%)
A (≤ 3 years)
 High
88.4
11.6
0
 Moderate
3.6
81.7
14.7
 Low
0
5.3
94.7
B (≥ 3 years)
 High
87.1
5.9
7
 Moderate
11.7
80.8
7.5
 Low
4.6
5.3
90.1
Table 4
Latent transition probabilities from ≤ 3 years of age to > 3 years of age
≤ 3 years of age
> 3 years of age
High
Moderate
Low
High
0.823
0.151
0.026
Moderate
0.331
0.226
0.442
Low
0.113
0.145
0.742
Members of the ‘≤ age 3’ high class were more likely to remain in the high class ≥ age 3 (82.3%) than transition to the moderate (15.1%) or low (2.6%) classes. The moderate class showed a mixed effect, with 22.6% remaining moderate, 33.1% transitioning ‘up’ to high and 44.2% transitioning ‘down’ to low. The low class were more likely to remain low (74.2%) than to transition into either the moderate (14.5%) or high (11.3%) classes. When examined in the context of a move-or-stay model (see Table 5), the majority of the overall sample (89.9%) fell into the stayer category; beginning and remaining low. Movers showed a ‘downward’ transitional trend, with 72.7% of this sample category transitioning from high to moderate or moderate to low.
Table 5
Count and relative percent of mover–stayer patterns
 
≤ 3 years of age
> 3 years of age
n
% within movers/stayers (%)
% of total sample (%)
Movers
High
Moderate
23
3.9
< 1
High
Low
1
< 1
< 1
Moderate
High
95
15.9
1.2
Moderate
Low
411
68.8
5.2
Low
High
30
5
< 1
Low
Moderate
37
6.2
< 1
Stayers
High
High
133
1.8
1.7
Moderate
Moderate
65
< 1
< 1
Low
Low
7054
97.3
89.9

Discussion

The first analysis revealed three profiles characterised by behaviours that would strongly reflect ‘red flag’ risk at an age where initial assessment is common. The scale questions narrowed in focus, helping to present a better picture of these behaviours, for example, going from ‘speech development’ to correct application of speech to the situation and appropriate phrase use. The differences in functioning between the classes may have demonstrated varied expression of ASD in accordance with older diagnostic categories, however, it is important to note that they were only approximations of ASD ‘red flags’. Variation in the latent class profiles after the age of 3, however, showed levels of expression in specific traits that were consistent with variation in ASD presentation [62]. The Low Presentation profile, largest at 51.8% of the subsample, was described by in/formal language use problems and some repetitive play and obsession behaviours but low instances of the other traits. These issues were enough for parents to report when asked, but may not have constituted an actual clinical threshold for ASD, as such traits vary naturally in the population [8]. The line between ‘personality quirk’ and ‘symptom’ is often quite thin [63], especially for the atypical functioning of individuals formerly categorised under PDD-NOS [16]. The Low Presentation profile represents a group that varies from a baseline population (‘no ongoing concerns’ or minimal trait expression) to just over the cusp of an older clinical designation. The moderate presentation profile, smallest at 16.7%, was typified by all-cause developmental problems, repetitive play, and in/formal language use problems in keeping with the presentation of AS. This profile also showed a 100% endorsement of eye contact issues, one of the hallmarks of ASD [64, 65], though it was low in other domains. The high presentation profile, 31.5% of the subsample, showed overall higher endorsements of more behaviours at levels which could be interpreted as past the clinical threshold for disorder and fitting the perception of ‘classical’ autism.
The LTA exposed a rough estimation of ‘red flag’ behaviour before the age of 3 and the second analysis focused on a more nuanced picture of these behaviours after the age of 3. A quasi-latent transitional analysis between those two ‘time points’ described transition between the latent classes, representing changes in symptom severity and occurrence over time. Those with membership in the ‘≤ age 3’ high class had the highest probability of remaining high ≥ age 3 while the greatest percentage of the sample population were those classified as low ≤ age 3 who remained so after. Those classified as moderate showed the greatest variance of transition ≥ age 3 as would be expected from this class, having the lowest overall classification probabilities in both LCAs. These results showed a clear relationship between initial ‘red flag’ behaviour severity (ASD risk) and behaviour persistence. Wolff et al. [66] observed this effect in the persistence of repetitive behaviour in toddlers over time, with the most severe behaviour persisting in an ASD sample. It is important to note, however that possible interventions, family/school socialisation, and general development [36] may have accounted for the downward trend in symptom severity over time but detailed intervention/treatment data was not available (see limitations below).
In seeking external validation for the hypothesis of the latent factor being ASD risk, the resultant high and moderate profiles were examined against the low category as a baseline in terms of four known ASD comorbidities (learning difficulties, poor coordination, any anxiety disorder, and epilepsy) and instances of ICD-10 ASD diagnosis in the sample. Odds ratios were highly significant as empirical predictors of ASD risk and confirmation that it was the latent factor. ASD diagnosis odds were 954 times higher than baseline for the high class and 20.73 times higher for the moderate class, indicating that ‘red flag’ behaviour traits are a predictor of ASD risk. Higher odds ratios were found in the High class than the moderate class for all items, further confirming a difference in severity in accordance with the hypothesis of multiple levels of severity and functioning aligning with older variant diagnoses. Epilepsy rates are important to note here as the relationship between ASD and epilepsy is well established with research indicating a potential genetic relationship [6770].
These results support the hypothesis of parental perception of ‘red flag’ behaviours before the age of 3 being predictive of ASD risk and severity. Analysis revealed distinct behavioural profiles with variance in expression of ASD conducive with variant diagnostic categories, validated by the presence of known comorbidities and gender rates. The data from this survey are from 2004 and the sample’s mean age was 10.51 (SD 3.39), indicating that most children from the sample were below the age of 3 in the mid to late 1990s. Research into ASD as a developmental disorder increased during the 1970s–1980s, challenging the notion of autism resulting from ‘refrigerator mothers’ [71]. The 1990s saw a wider awareness of variation in ASD and the concept of a spectrum of behaviours/diagnoses but the true tipping point of media awareness (and perhaps over-awareness) would not come until the early 2000’s [72]. The survey asked about ‘red flag’ behaviours worrying parents at that time, meaning before widespread ASD awareness and media coverage of the ‘autism epidemic’, removing an element of potential contamination in parental perception.
There is a solid foundation of research supporting the stability of ASD behaviours in early diagnosis ≤ age 3 to later reassessment ≥ age 4 in both general population [73, 74] and high risk samples [75, 76]. That stability does not contradict the OO model of improvement through early intervention programs and socialisation based on severity of initial symptomology. The trend for functioning-based improvement [40, 42] was replicated in this study as severe individuals tending to stay severe and moderate-to-low severity individuals tended to improve.
The evidence presented should be evaluated with the study’s limitations in mind.
The ASD section of the general development questionnaire designed by the ONS was in nonclinical language, was not a measure intended for clinical diagnosis, and was not a pre-existing, tested psychometric. The latent classes in the first LCA had a large catchment due to the general ‘red flag’ ASD terminology used; intended for parents rather than clinicians. The wording, “Was there anything that seriously worried you or anyone else about the way his/her speech developed?” failed to differentiate between language use and speech disorder. “Odd rituals or unusual habits that were very hard to interrupt” could have described the repetitive behaviours of ASD but could also have described the ritualistic learning behaviour common in toddlers [77, 78]. Issues of ‘learning development’ could have referred to any developmental delay or learning disability; even the traditional ‘late bloomer’ who goes on to develop with no impairment [79, 80]. While this may have seemed like a limitation to the validity of the study, the non-clinical language was actually considered a strength of the analysis, in that it described issues in a way that might be considered consistent with parental disclosures of concern to GPs/paediatricians. No health professionals were involved in the interviews and questionnaire responses were only parental perception/interpretation of the child’s behaviour. No data were collected concerning any treatment the children may have received for their issues after the age of 3, any effects that treatment may have had, or the presence of other developmental disorders. In addition, this parental-report data was susceptible to hindsight bias as no prospective screening was performed to isolate an ASD risk sample. Hindsight bias appears in diagnosis amongst clinicians trained to use diagnostic criteria and conduct assessments [81, 82] so its effect cannot be understated in non-clinical individuals. As there was little diagnostic outcome information available for the sample, the model of change in ASD behaviours over time could only be described as a trend model; further studies would benefit from a longer longitudinal design. In addition, the LTA performed was only a quasi-LTA, as the data used for each time period differed, and the items in the second model attempted only to approximate those in the first.
Parents’ perception of early ‘red flag’ behaviours certainly seem to act as valid predictors of future ASD risk while reinforcing the variation in expression of ASD that had been previously described by variant diagnosis. While many missed milestones or developmental delays can often clear up in time, some behavioural symptomology may persist and signify developmental disorder. Meaningful transition from disorder to a typical development population is the hoped-for OO model depending on the initial level of functioning. Further studies of large general population data with a longitudinal design, featuring clinical measures and clinician involvement could help create a clearer picture of ASD variation and transition in the population. Such parental report data would be valuable assets in the development and testing of future diagnostic tools.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

Neuer Inhalt

Print-Titel

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Weitere Produktempfehlungen anzeigen
Anhänge

Appendix 1

Q1. (Pretend/Play) “Some children spend a lot of their play time repeating the same action over and over again, for example spinning the wheels on a toy car, turning taps or light switches on and off, or opening and shutting doors. Has this ever been true of CHILD?”
Q2. (Pretend/Play) “Children are sometimes very interested in unusual aspects of toys or other things. For example, rather than playing with a toy, they may spend their time sniffing it, or running their fingers over its surface, or listening to any noise or vibration that it makes. Has this ever been true of CHILD?”
Q3. (Ritual/Habit) “Some ^Children/teenagers enjoy putting a lot of time into collecting things, or get a lot of pleasure out of focusing on just one topic, such as sport, cars or a particular pop group. In everyday language, we often say that these ^Children/teenagers are ’obsessed’ by their interest, but this is not an unpleasant obsession—this is something they like and usually enjoy talking about. Has CHILD had any long-lasting obsessions of this sort?”
Q4. (Language) “Some ^Children/teenagers have trouble adjusting their language to suit different social occasions. For example, they may speak too casually to a teacher or too formally to other children. Does CHILD change the way ^s/he speaks according to whether it is a formal or informal situation?”
Q5. (Social) “When we’re talking with someone face-to-face, eye contact is very important. It generally makes us feel uneasy, or as if there’s something wrong, if the other person makes too little eye contact, or too much, or makes it at the wrong time. Has CHILD ever been through a phase of making too little or too much eye contact, or making it in the wrong sort of way?”
Q6. (Social) “^Children/Teenagers respond in different ways to other people’s emotions. For example, if their mother is upset because she has cut her finger badly with a knife, children can be sympathetic, or not pay much attention, or respond in unusual ways such as laughing. What would CHILD typically do in this sort of situation?”
Q7. (Language) “Another way in which children repeat themselves is by using the same catch-phrase or cliché over and over again. For example, almost every sentence may begin ’If you want my opinion...’ or ’Logically speaking...’ Occasionally the phrase is appropriate, but it is used far more than is really needed. Has CHILD ever filled ^his/her speech with a lot of these fairly empty catch-phrases or clichés?”
Q8. (Ritual/Habit) “Some children enjoy routines and want things to be the same every day. For example, they may want to eat the same food off the same plate while sitting in the same chair every single day. Or there may be very fixed routines for dressing or undressing. Has CHILD ever had strong or unusual routines that ^s/he has insisted on because ^s/he enjoyed doing it that way?”
Q9. (Development) “Some preschool children go through a phase of flapping or waving their hands or arms up and down if they are excited or upset. Some continue doing this for years. Since CHILD has been going to school, has ^s/he tended to flap ^his/her arms when excited or upset?”
Q10. (Development) “You have answered a lot of questions about CHILD’s pattern of development - focusing particularly on language, play, routines and ^his/her ability to get along with other people. Are you concerned at present about any of these aspects of CHILD’s development?”
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing, ArlingtonCrossRef American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing, ArlingtonCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Chawarska K, Paul R, Klin A, Hannigan S, Dichtel L, Volkmar F (2007) Parental recognition of developmental problems in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 37(1):62–72PubMedCrossRef Chawarska K, Paul R, Klin A, Hannigan S, Dichtel L, Volkmar F (2007) Parental recognition of developmental problems in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 37(1):62–72PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat McPartland J, Reichow B, Volkmar F (2012) Sensitivity and specificity of proposed DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 51(4):368–383PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef McPartland J, Reichow B, Volkmar F (2012) Sensitivity and specificity of proposed DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 51(4):368–383PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Blumberg S, Zablotsky B, Avila R, Colpe L, Pringle B, Kogan M (2016) Diagnosis lost: differences between children who had and who currently have an autism spectrum disorder diagnosis. Autism 20(7):783–795PubMedCrossRef Blumberg S, Zablotsky B, Avila R, Colpe L, Pringle B, Kogan M (2016) Diagnosis lost: differences between children who had and who currently have an autism spectrum disorder diagnosis. Autism 20(7):783–795PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Constantino J, Todd R (2003) Autistic traits in the general population: a twin study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 60(5):524–530PubMedCrossRef Constantino J, Todd R (2003) Autistic traits in the general population: a twin study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 60(5):524–530PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Constantino J, Todd R (2005) Intergenerational transmission of subthreshold autistic traits in the general population. Biol Psychiatry 57(6):655–660PubMedCrossRef Constantino J, Todd R (2005) Intergenerational transmission of subthreshold autistic traits in the general population. Biol Psychiatry 57(6):655–660PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Lundström S, Chang Z, Råstam M, Gillberg C, Larsson H et al (2012) Autism spectrum disorders and autistic-like traits: similar etiology in the extreme end and the normal variation. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69(1):46–52PubMedCrossRef Lundström S, Chang Z, Råstam M, Gillberg C, Larsson H et al (2012) Autism spectrum disorders and autistic-like traits: similar etiology in the extreme end and the normal variation. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69(1):46–52PubMedCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Ghaziuddin M, Mountain-Kimchi K (2004) Defining the intellectual profile of Asperger syndrome: comparison with high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord 34(3):279–284PubMedCrossRef Ghaziuddin M, Mountain-Kimchi K (2004) Defining the intellectual profile of Asperger syndrome: comparison with high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord 34(3):279–284PubMedCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Munson J, Dawson G, Sterling L, Beauchaine T, Zhou A, Koehler E et al (2008) Evidence for latent classes of IQ in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Am J Ment Retard 113(6):439–452PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Munson J, Dawson G, Sterling L, Beauchaine T, Zhou A, Koehler E et al (2008) Evidence for latent classes of IQ in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Am J Ment Retard 113(6):439–452PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Chiang H-M, Tsai L, Cheung Y, Brown A, Li H (2013) A meta-analysis of differences in IQ profiles between individuals with Asperger’s disorder and highfunctioning autism. J Autism Dev Disorders 44(7):1577–1596CrossRef Chiang H-M, Tsai L, Cheung Y, Brown A, Li H (2013) A meta-analysis of differences in IQ profiles between individuals with Asperger’s disorder and highfunctioning autism. J Autism Dev Disorders 44(7):1577–1596CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Koyama T, Tachimori H, Osada H, Takeda T, Kurita H (2007) Cognitive and symptom profiles in Asperger’s syndrome and high-functioning autism. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 61:99–104PubMedCrossRef Koyama T, Tachimori H, Osada H, Takeda T, Kurita H (2007) Cognitive and symptom profiles in Asperger’s syndrome and high-functioning autism. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 61:99–104PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Ozonoff S, Rogers S, Pennington B (1991) Asperger’s syndrome: evidence of an empirical distinction from high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 32(7):1107–1122PubMedCrossRef Ozonoff S, Rogers S, Pennington B (1991) Asperger’s syndrome: evidence of an empirical distinction from high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 32(7):1107–1122PubMedCrossRef
14.
15.
Zurück zum Zitat McAlonan G, Suckling J, Wong N, Cheung V, Lienenkaemper N, Cheung C et al (2008) Distinct patterns of grey matter abnormality in high-functioning autism and Asperger’s syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 49(12):1287–1295PubMedCrossRef McAlonan G, Suckling J, Wong N, Cheung V, Lienenkaemper N, Cheung C et al (2008) Distinct patterns of grey matter abnormality in high-functioning autism and Asperger’s syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 49(12):1287–1295PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing, Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing, Washington, DC
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson E, Munir K, Munafò M, Hughes M, McCormick M, Koenen K (2011) Stability of autistic traits in the general population: further evidence for a continuum of impairment. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 50(4):376–384PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Robinson E, Munir K, Munafò M, Hughes M, McCormick M, Koenen K (2011) Stability of autistic traits in the general population: further evidence for a continuum of impairment. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 50(4):376–384PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Osborne L, Reed P (2008) Parents’ perceptions of communication with professionals during the diagnosis of autism. Autism 12(3):309–324PubMedCrossRef Osborne L, Reed P (2008) Parents’ perceptions of communication with professionals during the diagnosis of autism. Autism 12(3):309–324PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Bolton P, Golding J, Emond A, Steer C (2009) Autism spectrum disorder and autistic traits in the Avon longitudinal study of parents and children: precursors and early signs. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 51(3):249–260CrossRef Bolton P, Golding J, Emond A, Steer C (2009) Autism spectrum disorder and autistic traits in the Avon longitudinal study of parents and children: precursors and early signs. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 51(3):249–260CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Beighley J, Matson J (2013) Encyclopaedia of autism spectrum disorders. Springer, New York Beighley J, Matson J (2013) Encyclopaedia of autism spectrum disorders. Springer, New York
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Hess CR, Landa RJ (2012) Predictive and concurrent validity of parent concern about young children at risk for autism. J Autism Dev Disord 42(4):575–584PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hess CR, Landa RJ (2012) Predictive and concurrent validity of parent concern about young children at risk for autism. J Autism Dev Disord 42(4):575–584PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller LE, Perkins KA, Dai YG, Fein DA (2017) Comparison of parent report and direct assessment of child skills in toddlers. Res Autism Spectr Disorders 41–42:57–65CrossRef Miller LE, Perkins KA, Dai YG, Fein DA (2017) Comparison of parent report and direct assessment of child skills in toddlers. Res Autism Spectr Disorders 41–42:57–65CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Richards M, Mossey J, Robins DL (2016) Parents’ concerns as they relate to their child’s development and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr 37(7):532–540PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Richards M, Mossey J, Robins DL (2016) Parents’ concerns as they relate to their child’s development and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr 37(7):532–540PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Zablotsky B, Colpe LJ, Pringle BA, Kogan MD, Rice C, Blumberg SJ (2017) Age of parental concern, diagnosis, and service initiation among children with autism spectrum disorder. Am J Intell Dev Disabil 122(1):49–61CrossRef Zablotsky B, Colpe LJ, Pringle BA, Kogan MD, Rice C, Blumberg SJ (2017) Age of parental concern, diagnosis, and service initiation among children with autism spectrum disorder. Am J Intell Dev Disabil 122(1):49–61CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Warren Z, McPheeters M, Sathe N, Foss-Feig J, Glasser J, Veenstra-VenderWeele J (2011) A systematic review of early intensive intervention for autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 127(5):1303–1311CrossRef Warren Z, McPheeters M, Sathe N, Foss-Feig J, Glasser J, Veenstra-VenderWeele J (2011) A systematic review of early intensive intervention for autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 127(5):1303–1311CrossRef
26.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Christensen D, Baio J, Braun K, Bilder D, Charles J, Constantino J et al (2016) Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2012. morbidity and mortality weekly report. Surveill Summ 65(3):1–23CrossRef Christensen D, Baio J, Braun K, Bilder D, Charles J, Constantino J et al (2016) Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2012. morbidity and mortality weekly report. Surveill Summ 65(3):1–23CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Lord C, Risi S, DiLavore P, Shulman C, Thurm A, Pickles A (2006) Autism from 2 to 9 years of age. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63(6):694–701PubMedCrossRef Lord C, Risi S, DiLavore P, Shulman C, Thurm A, Pickles A (2006) Autism from 2 to 9 years of age. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63(6):694–701PubMedCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Baranek GT, Watson LR, Crais E, Reznick S (2003) First-Year Inventory (FYI) 2.0. University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill Baranek GT, Watson LR, Crais E, Reznick S (2003) First-Year Inventory (FYI) 2.0. University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Makrygianni M, Reed P (2010) A meta-analytic review of the effectiveness of behavioural early intervention programs for children with autistic spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord 4(4):577–593CrossRef Makrygianni M, Reed P (2010) A meta-analytic review of the effectiveness of behavioural early intervention programs for children with autistic spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord 4(4):577–593CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat McConhachie H, Diggle T (2006) Parent implemented early intervention for young children with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review. J Eval Clin Pract 13(1):120–129CrossRef McConhachie H, Diggle T (2006) Parent implemented early intervention for young children with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review. J Eval Clin Pract 13(1):120–129CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Remington B, Hastings R, Kovshoff H, Espinosa F, Jahr E et al (2007) Early intensive behavioral intervention: outcomes for children with autism and their parents after two years. Am J Ment Retardation 112(6):418–438CrossRef Remington B, Hastings R, Kovshoff H, Espinosa F, Jahr E et al (2007) Early intensive behavioral intervention: outcomes for children with autism and their parents after two years. Am J Ment Retardation 112(6):418–438CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Dawson G, Rogers S, Munson J, Smith M, Winter J et al (2010) Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: the early start Denver model. Pediatrics 125(1):17–23CrossRef Dawson G, Rogers S, Munson J, Smith M, Winter J et al (2010) Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: the early start Denver model. Pediatrics 125(1):17–23CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Matson J, Wilkins J, González M (2008) Early identification and diagnosis in autism spectrum disorders in young children and infants: how early is too early? Res Autism Spectr Disord 2(1):75–84CrossRef Matson J, Wilkins J, González M (2008) Early identification and diagnosis in autism spectrum disorders in young children and infants: how early is too early? Res Autism Spectr Disord 2(1):75–84CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Baron-Cohen S, Cox A, Baird G, Swettenham J, Nightengale M, Morgan K et al (1996) Psychological markers in the detection of autism in infancy in a large population. Br J Psychiatry 168(2):158–163PubMedCrossRef Baron-Cohen S, Cox A, Baird G, Swettenham J, Nightengale M, Morgan K et al (1996) Psychological markers in the detection of autism in infancy in a large population. Br J Psychiatry 168(2):158–163PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Charman T, Taylor E, Drew A, Cockerill H, Brown J, Bai rd G (2004) Outcome at 7 years of children diagnosed with autism at age 2: predictive validity of assessments conducted at 2 and 3 years of age and pattern of symptom change over time. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46(5):500–513CrossRef Charman T, Taylor E, Drew A, Cockerill H, Brown J, Bai rd G (2004) Outcome at 7 years of children diagnosed with autism at age 2: predictive validity of assessments conducted at 2 and 3 years of age and pattern of symptom change over time. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46(5):500–513CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Jones E, Gliga T, Bedford R, Charman T, Johnson M (2014) Developmental pathways to autism: a review of prospective studies of infants at risk. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 39:1–33PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Jones E, Gliga T, Bedford R, Charman T, Johnson M (2014) Developmental pathways to autism: a review of prospective studies of infants at risk. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 39:1–33PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Chawarska K, Klin A, Paul R, Macari S, Volkmar F (2009) A prospective study of toddlers with ASD: short-term diagnostic and cognitive outcomes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 50(10):1235–1245PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Chawarska K, Klin A, Paul R, Macari S, Volkmar F (2009) A prospective study of toddlers with ASD: short-term diagnostic and cognitive outcomes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 50(10):1235–1245PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Lovaas OI (1987) Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. J Consult Clin Psychol 55(1):3–9PubMedCrossRef Lovaas OI (1987) Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. J Consult Clin Psychol 55(1):3–9PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Fein D, Barton M, Eigsti I-M, Kelley E, Naigles L, Schultz R et al (2013) Optimal outcome in individuals with a history of autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54(2):195–205PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Fein D, Barton M, Eigsti I-M, Kelley E, Naigles L, Schultz R et al (2013) Optimal outcome in individuals with a history of autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54(2):195–205PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Orinstein A, Suh J, Porter K, De Yoe K, Tyson K, Tryob K et al (2015) Social function and communication in optimal outcome children and adolescents with an autism history on structured test measures. J Autism Dev Disord 45(8):2443–2463PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Orinstein A, Suh J, Porter K, De Yoe K, Tyson K, Tryob K et al (2015) Social function and communication in optimal outcome children and adolescents with an autism history on structured test measures. J Autism Dev Disord 45(8):2443–2463PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Sutera S, Pandey J, Esser E, Rosenthal M, Wilson L, Barton M et al (2007) Predictors of optimal outcome in toddlers diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 37(1):98–107PubMedCrossRef Sutera S, Pandey J, Esser E, Rosenthal M, Wilson L, Barton M et al (2007) Predictors of optimal outcome in toddlers diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 37(1):98–107PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Kelley E, Naigles L, Fein D (2010) An in-depth examination of optimal outcome children with a history of autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord 4(3):526–538CrossRef Kelley E, Naigles L, Fein D (2010) An in-depth examination of optimal outcome children with a history of autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord 4(3):526–538CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Orinstein A, Helt M, Troyb E, Tyson K, Barton M, Eigsti I-M et al (2014) Intervention history of children and adolescents with high-functioning autism and optimal outcomes. J Dev Behav Pediatr 35(4):247–256PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Orinstein A, Helt M, Troyb E, Tyson K, Barton M, Eigsti I-M et al (2014) Intervention history of children and adolescents with high-functioning autism and optimal outcomes. J Dev Behav Pediatr 35(4):247–256PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Eigsti I-M, Stevens M, Schultz R, Barton M, Kelley E, Naigels L et al (2016) Language comprehension and brain function in individuals with an optimal outcome from autism. Neuroimage: Clin 10:182–191CrossRef Eigsti I-M, Stevens M, Schultz R, Barton M, Kelley E, Naigels L et al (2016) Language comprehension and brain function in individuals with an optimal outcome from autism. Neuroimage: Clin 10:182–191CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat De Giacomo A, Frombonne E (1998) Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 7(3):131–136PubMedCrossRef De Giacomo A, Frombonne E (1998) Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 7(3):131–136PubMedCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Young R, Brewer N, Pattison C (2003) Parental identification of early behavioural abnormalities in children with autistic disorder. Autism 7(2):125–143PubMedCrossRef Young R, Brewer N, Pattison C (2003) Parental identification of early behavioural abnormalities in children with autistic disorder. Autism 7(2):125–143PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Watson L, Baranek G, Crais E, Reznick J, Dykstra J et al (2007) The First Year Inventory: retrospective parent responses to a questionnaire designed to identify one-year-olds at risk for autism. J Autism Dev Disord 37(1):49–61PubMedCrossRef Watson L, Baranek G, Crais E, Reznick J, Dykstra J et al (2007) The First Year Inventory: retrospective parent responses to a questionnaire designed to identify one-year-olds at risk for autism. J Autism Dev Disord 37(1):49–61PubMedCrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Ozonoff S, Heung K, Byrd R, Hanse R, Hertz-Picciotto I (2008) The onset of autism: Patterns of symptom emergence in the first years of life. Autism Research 1(6):320–328PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ozonoff S, Heung K, Byrd R, Hanse R, Hertz-Picciotto I (2008) The onset of autism: Patterns of symptom emergence in the first years of life. Autism Research 1(6):320–328PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Ozonoff S, Young G, Steinfeld M, Hill M, Cook I et al (2009) How early do parent concerns predict later autism diagnosis? J Dev Behav Pediatr 30(5):367–375PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ozonoff S, Young G, Steinfeld M, Hill M, Cook I et al (2009) How early do parent concerns predict later autism diagnosis? J Dev Behav Pediatr 30(5):367–375PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Green H, McGinnity A, Meltzer H, Ford T, Goodman R (2005) Mental health of children and young people in Great Britain, 2004, Palgrave Macmillan, LondonCrossRef Green H, McGinnity A, Meltzer H, Ford T, Goodman R (2005) Mental health of children and young people in Great Britain, 2004, Palgrave Macmillan, LondonCrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Nylund KL (2007) Latent transition analysis: modeling extensions and an application to peer victimization. Doctoral Dissertation. University of California, Los Angeles. Nylund KL (2007) Latent transition analysis: modeling extensions and an application to peer victimization. Doctoral Dissertation. University of California, Los Angeles.
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Nylund KL, Muthén B, Nishina A, Bellmore A, Graham S (2006) Stability and instability of peer victimization during middle school: using latent transition analysis with covariates, distal outcomes, and modeling extensions. Retrieved from: http://www.statmodel.com/download/LTA_DP_FINAL.pdf. Accessed 19 Oct 2019 (unpublished manuscript) Nylund KL, Muthén B, Nishina A, Bellmore A, Graham S (2006) Stability and instability of peer victimization during middle school: using latent transition analysis with covariates, distal outcomes, and modeling extensions. Retrieved from: http://​www.​statmodel.​com/​download/​LTA_​DP_​FINAL.​pdf. Accessed 19 Oct 2019 (unpublished manuscript)
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO (2007) Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct Equ Model 14(4):535–569CrossRef Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén BO (2007) Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct Equ Model 14(4):535–569CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Akaike H (1987) Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika 52(3):317–332CrossRef Akaike H (1987) Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika 52(3):317–332CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwarz G (1978) Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann Stat 6(2):461–464CrossRef Schwarz G (1978) Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann Stat 6(2):461–464CrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Sclove SL (1987) Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika 52(3):333–343CrossRef Sclove SL (1987) Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika 52(3):333–343CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Lo Y, Mendell NR, Rubin DB (2001) Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika 88(3):767–778CrossRef Lo Y, Mendell NR, Rubin DB (2001) Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika 88(3):767–778CrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Celeux G, Soromenho G (1996) An entropy criterion for assessing the number of clusters in a mixture model. J Classif 13(2):195–212CrossRef Celeux G, Soromenho G (1996) An entropy criterion for assessing the number of clusters in a mixture model. J Classif 13(2):195–212CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Tofighi D, Enders CK (2008) Identifying the correct number of classes in growth mixture models. Advances in latent variable mixture models. Information Age Publishing Inc, Greenwich, pp 317–341 Tofighi D, Enders CK (2008) Identifying the correct number of classes in growth mixture models. Advances in latent variable mixture models. Information Age Publishing Inc, Greenwich, pp 317–341
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Muthén LK, Muthén BO (1998–2006) Mplus user’s guide, 4th edn. Muthén, Los Angeles, CA Muthén LK, Muthén BO (1998–2006) Mplus user’s guide, 4th edn. Muthén, Los Angeles, CA
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson CP, Myers SM (2007) Identification and evaluation of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 120:1183–1215CrossRefPubMed Johnson CP, Myers SM (2007) Identification and evaluation of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 120:1183–1215CrossRefPubMed
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook B, Leventhal B et al (2000) The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord 30(3):205–223PubMedCrossRef Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook B, Leventhal B et al (2000) The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord 30(3):205–223PubMedCrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Ozonoff S, Iosif A, Baguio F, Cook I, Hill M et al (2010) A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49(3):256–266PubMedPubMedCentral Ozonoff S, Iosif A, Baguio F, Cook I, Hill M et al (2010) A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49(3):256–266PubMedPubMedCentral
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolff J, Botteron K, Dager S, Elison J, Estes A et al (2014) Longitudinal patterns of repetitive behavior in toddlers with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 55(8):945–953PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wolff J, Botteron K, Dager S, Elison J, Estes A et al (2014) Longitudinal patterns of repetitive behavior in toddlers with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 55(8):945–953PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Bolton P, Carcani-Rathwell I, Hutton J, Goode S, Howlin P, Rutter M (2011) Epilepsy in autism: features and correlates. Br J Psychiatry 198(4):289–294PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bolton P, Carcani-Rathwell I, Hutton J, Goode S, Howlin P, Rutter M (2011) Epilepsy in autism: features and correlates. Br J Psychiatry 198(4):289–294PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Russ S, Larson K, Halfon N (2012) A national profile of childhood epilepsy and seizure disorder. Pediatrics 129(2):256–264PubMedCrossRef Russ S, Larson K, Halfon N (2012) A national profile of childhood epilepsy and seizure disorder. Pediatrics 129(2):256–264PubMedCrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Kohane I, McMurry A, Weber G, MacFadden D, Rappaport L et al (2012) The comorbidity burden of children and young adults with autism spectrum disorders. PLoS One 7(4):e33224PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kohane I, McMurry A, Weber G, MacFadden D, Rappaport L et al (2012) The comorbidity burden of children and young adults with autism spectrum disorders. PLoS One 7(4):e33224PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Eeghen A, Pulisifer M, Merker V, Neumeyer A, Van Eeghen E et al (2013) Understanding relationships between autism, intelligence, and epilepsy: a crossdisorder approach. Dev Med Child Neurol 55(2):146–153PubMedCrossRef Van Eeghen A, Pulisifer M, Merker V, Neumeyer A, Van Eeghen E et al (2013) Understanding relationships between autism, intelligence, and epilepsy: a crossdisorder approach. Dev Med Child Neurol 55(2):146–153PubMedCrossRef
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Langan M (2011) Parental voices and controversies in autism. Disabil Soc 26(2):193–205CrossRef Langan M (2011) Parental voices and controversies in autism. Disabil Soc 26(2):193–205CrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Verhoeff B (2013) Autism in flux: a history of the concept from Leo Kanner to DSM-5. Hist Psychiatry 24(4):442–458PubMedCrossRef Verhoeff B (2013) Autism in flux: a history of the concept from Leo Kanner to DSM-5. Hist Psychiatry 24(4):442–458PubMedCrossRef
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Woolfenden S, Sarkozy V, Ridley G, Williams K (2011) A systematic review of the diagnostic stability of autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord 6:345–354CrossRef Woolfenden S, Sarkozy V, Ridley G, Williams K (2011) A systematic review of the diagnostic stability of autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord 6:345–354CrossRef
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Guthrie W, Swineford LB, Nottke C, Wetherby AM (2013) Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: stability and change in clinical diagnosis and symptom presentation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54(5):582–590PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Guthrie W, Swineford LB, Nottke C, Wetherby AM (2013) Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: stability and change in clinical diagnosis and symptom presentation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54(5):582–590PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Charwarska K, Shic F, Macari S, Campbell DJ, Brian J, Landa R et al (2014) 18-month predictors of later outcomes in younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder: a baby siblings research consortium study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 53(12):1317–1327CrossRef Charwarska K, Shic F, Macari S, Campbell DJ, Brian J, Landa R et al (2014) 18-month predictors of later outcomes in younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder: a baby siblings research consortium study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 53(12):1317–1327CrossRef
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Ozonoff S, Young GS, Landa RJ, Brian J, Bryson S, Charman T et al (2015) Diagnostic stability in young children at risk for autism spectrum disorder: a baby siblings research consortium study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 56(9):988–998PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Ozonoff S, Young GS, Landa RJ, Brian J, Bryson S, Charman T et al (2015) Diagnostic stability in young children at risk for autism spectrum disorder: a baby siblings research consortium study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 56(9):988–998PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Evans D, Leckman J, Carter A, Reznick J, Henshaw D et al (1997) Ritual, habit and perfectionism: the prevalence and development of compulsive-like behavior in normal young children. Child Dev 68(1):58–68PubMedCrossRef Evans D, Leckman J, Carter A, Reznick J, Henshaw D et al (1997) Ritual, habit and perfectionism: the prevalence and development of compulsive-like behavior in normal young children. Child Dev 68(1):58–68PubMedCrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Zohar A, Felz L (2001) Ritualistic behavior in young children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 29(2):121–128PubMedCrossRef Zohar A, Felz L (2001) Ritualistic behavior in young children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 29(2):121–128PubMedCrossRef
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Rescorla L, Dahlsgaard K, Roberts J (2000) Late-talking toddlers: MLU and IPSyn outcomes at 3;0 and 4;0. J Child Lang 27(3):643–664PubMedCrossRef Rescorla L, Dahlsgaard K, Roberts J (2000) Late-talking toddlers: MLU and IPSyn outcomes at 3;0 and 4;0. J Child Lang 27(3):643–664PubMedCrossRef
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Simonton D (2005) Giftedness and genetics: the emergenic-epigenetic model and its implications. J Educ Gift 28(3–4):270–286CrossRef Simonton D (2005) Giftedness and genetics: the emergenic-epigenetic model and its implications. J Educ Gift 28(3–4):270–286CrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Arkes HR, Wortmann RL, Saville PD, Harkness AR (1981) Hindsight bias among physicians weighing the likelihood of diagnoses. J Appl Psychol 66(2):252–254PubMedCrossRef Arkes HR, Wortmann RL, Saville PD, Harkness AR (1981) Hindsight bias among physicians weighing the likelihood of diagnoses. J Appl Psychol 66(2):252–254PubMedCrossRef
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Graber ML, Franklin N, Gordon R (2005) Diagnostic error in internal medicine. Arch Intern Med 165:1493–1499PubMedCrossRef Graber ML, Franklin N, Gordon R (2005) Diagnostic error in internal medicine. Arch Intern Med 165:1493–1499PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Recognising autism: a latent transition analysis of parental reports of child autistic spectrum disorder ‘red flag’ traits before and after age 3
verfasst von
Amanda Spikol
Donal McAteer
Jamie Murphy
Publikationsdatum
13.02.2019
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology / Ausgabe 6/2019
Print ISSN: 0933-7954
Elektronische ISSN: 1433-9285
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-019-01664-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 6/2019

Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 6/2019 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.