Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Cognitive Therapy and Research 3/2011

01.06.2011 | Original Article

Selective Attention and Health Anxiety: Ill-Health Stimuli are Distracting for Everyone

verfasst von: Cassandra Shields, Karen Murphy

Erschienen in: Cognitive Therapy and Research | Ausgabe 3/2011

Einloggen, um Zugang zu erhalten

Abstract

Psychological theories of anxiety are increasingly referring to information processing paradigms in order to understand the cognitive processes which underlie these disorders. Numerous studies of anxiety have demonstrated an attentional bias towards anxiety relevant stimuli. In addition, there is consistent evidence that it is more difficult to process absent than present information. Prior research has suggested that both these processing biases contribute to the maintenance of health anxiety. The present study investigated differences in attentional processing between participants high and low in health anxiety, using two visual search tasks. The visual search tasks used either letters (domain free stimuli), or words (anxiety-related stimuli). Both low and high health anxious participants demonstrated poorer performance for target absent than target present trials. In addition, all participants showed an attentional bias towards ill-health words over good-health, negative, positive, or neutral words. These results suggest that concern about health is relevant to all people regardless of level of health anxiety.
Anhänge
Nur mit Berechtigung zugänglich
Fußnoten
1
A Repeated Measures ANOVA revealed the same pattern of results for both the letter and the word visual search tasks, when the WI or the IAS were used individually, and when the measures were combined to categorize individuals into high and low health anxious groups.
 
2
Power calculations were conducted using the Effect Size Generator element of ClinTools Version 4.1 (Devilly 2007). As the methodology for assessing attentional biases in health anxiety within this study is relatively unique, details entered into ClinTools were based upon three research articles investigating information processing and anxiety. Specifically, Rassin et al. (2008) who reported a medium effect (r = .29) of health anxiety during a visual search task with non-health specific stimuli. When converted to Cohen’s d using ClinTools (Devilly, 2007) r = .29 is equivalent to d = .61. Owens et al. (2004) Stroop paradigm utilising health specific words among high and low health anxious individuals, revealing a large effect (d = .97); and Rinck et al. (2003) who observed a medium to large effect (d = .73) between generalised anxiety patients and controls during a visual search task involving anxiety-related words. Results suggested an adequate sample size of approximately 28 participants per group to achieve power of .8 in a two-tailed analysis with an expected medium to large effect size. Therefore, the current group sample sizes were deemed sufficient to produce suitable power for the analyses.
 
3
In total 6, 12, or 18 items were presented in the word and letter task, for target present and absent trials. This allowed the same number of items to be displayed within the present and absent trials, preventing participants from using counting to respond to target, present versus absent (e.g., 6 items = absent, 7 items = present).
 
4
Regression coefficients were not calculated for the error scores (ms) due to insufficient data (i.e., few errors within some conditions).
 
5
The authors would like to acknowledge the input from one of the reviewers suggesting this further analysis.
 
6
Originally, ANCOVAs were applied to the data sets, with scores from the STAI-S, STAI-T, and BDI as covariates. However, the assumption of linear relationships between the covariates at each level of the independent variables were violated for error data, in the absent condition, for both letter and word tasks. In order to run the same analyses for both the RT and error data to check for speed-accuracy trade-offs, the data were investigated using mixed factorial ANOVA. Initial analyses revealed, results were not influenced by speed-accuracy trade-offs and the pattern of results produced using both errors and mean RT per condition including display size, was similar to that reported using the regression based approach.
 
7
One methodological suggestion which may have been considered to ensure priming effects in the visual search task was to block trials by distractor type (we thank a reviewer for this insightful suggestion). However if this procedural adjustment had been made it is likely that the distractor words may have lost their valence and meaning value, in turn minimising any priming effects. Researchers have previously demonstrated that repeated presentation of a word type results in habituation or a loss of valence to that word type (e.g., McNally et al. 1990).
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (Text Rev., 4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (Text Rev., 4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson, J. R., & Schunn, C. D. (2000). Implications of the ACT-R learning theory: No magic bullets. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 1–33). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Anderson, J. R., & Schunn, C. D. (2000). Implications of the ACT-R learning theory: No magic bullets. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 1–33). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Zurück zum Zitat Baños, R. M., Quero, S., & Botella, C. (2008). Detection and distraction effects for threatening information in social phobia and change after treatment. Depression and Anxiety, 25, 55–63. doi:10.1002/da.20269.PubMedCrossRef Baños, R. M., Quero, S., & Botella, C. (2008). Detection and distraction effects for threatening information in social phobia and change after treatment. Depression and Anxiety, 25, 55–63. doi:10.​1002/​da.​20269.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1.PubMedCrossRef Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24. doi:10.​1037/​0033-2909.​133.​1.​1.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Zurück zum Zitat Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Falla, S. J., & Hamilton, L. R. (1998). Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety: Manipulation of stimulus duration. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 737–753. doi:10.1080/026999398379411.CrossRef Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Falla, S. J., & Hamilton, L. R. (1998). Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety: Manipulation of stimulus duration. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 737–753. doi:10.​1080/​026999398379411.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brown, H. D., Kosslyn, S. M., Delamater, B., Fama, J., & Barsky, A. J. (1999). Perceptual and memory biases for health-related information in hypochondriacal individuals. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 47, 67–78. doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(99)00011-2.PubMedCrossRef Brown, H. D., Kosslyn, S. M., Delamater, B., Fama, J., & Barsky, A. J. (1999). Perceptual and memory biases for health-related information in hypochondriacal individuals. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 47, 67–78. doi:10.​1016/​S0022-3999(99)00011-2.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Calvo, M. G., Ramos, P., & Estevez, A. (1992). Test anxiety and comprehension efficiency: The role of prior knowledge and working memory deficits. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 5, 125–138. doi:10.1080/10615809208250492.CrossRef Calvo, M. G., Ramos, P., & Estevez, A. (1992). Test anxiety and comprehension efficiency: The role of prior knowledge and working memory deficits. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 5, 125–138. doi:10.​1080/​1061580920825049​2.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.CrossRef Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fazio, R. H., Sherman, S. J., & Herr, P. M. (1982). The feature-positive effect in the self-perception process: Does not doing matter as much as doing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 404–411. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.42.3.404.CrossRef Fazio, R. H., Sherman, S. J., & Herr, P. M. (1982). The feature-positive effect in the self-perception process: Does not doing matter as much as doing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 404–411. doi:10.​1037/​0022-3514.​42.​3.​404.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hair, J. F., Jr, Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., & Tatham, R. L. (2006). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall. Hair, J. F., Jr, Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., & Tatham, R. L. (2006). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice Hall.
Zurück zum Zitat Jellis, S., et al. (2001). Encarta thesaurus. Sydney: Pan Macmillan. Jellis, S., et al. (2001). Encarta thesaurus. Sydney: Pan Macmillan.
Zurück zum Zitat Kahneman, D., & Triesman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman & D. A. Davis (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 28–61). San Diego: Academic Press. Kahneman, D., & Triesman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman & D. A. Davis (Eds.), Varieties of attention (pp. 28–61). San Diego: Academic Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Karademas, E. C., Christopoulou, S., Dimostheni, A., & Pavlu, F. (2008). Health anxiety and cognitive interference: Evidence from the application of a modified stroop task in two studies. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1138–1150. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.007.CrossRef Karademas, E. C., Christopoulou, S., Dimostheni, A., & Pavlu, F. (2008). Health anxiety and cognitive interference: Evidence from the application of a modified stroop task in two studies. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 1138–1150. doi:10.​1016/​j.​paid.​2007.​11.​007.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kellner, R., Abbott, P., Winslow, W. W., & Pathak, D. (1987). Fears, beliefs, and attitudes in DSM-III hypochondriasis. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 20–25.PubMedCrossRef Kellner, R., Abbott, P., Winslow, W. W., & Pathak, D. (1987). Fears, beliefs, and attitudes in DSM-III hypochondriasis. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 20–25.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lees, A., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Health anxiety, anxiety sensitivity, and attentional biases for pictorial and linguistic health-threat cues. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 453–462. doi:10.1080/02699930441000184.CrossRef Lees, A., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Health anxiety, anxiety sensitivity, and attentional biases for pictorial and linguistic health-threat cues. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 453–462. doi:10.​1080/​0269993044100018​4.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Lundqvist, D. (1999). Unconscious emotion: Evolutionary perspectives, psychophysical data, and neuropsychological mechanisms. In R. L. A. L. Nadel (Ed.), The cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 296–327). New York: Oxford University Press. Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Lundqvist, D. (1999). Unconscious emotion: Evolutionary perspectives, psychophysical data, and neuropsychological mechanisms. In R. L. A. L. Nadel (Ed.), The cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 296–327). New York: Oxford University Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., Kellerman, J., & Roth, W. T. (2003). Selective attention in anxiety: Distraction and enhancement in visual search. Depression and Anxiety, 18, 18–28. doi:10.1002/da.10105.PubMedCrossRef Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., Kellerman, J., & Roth, W. T. (2003). Selective attention in anxiety: Distraction and enhancement in visual search. Depression and Anxiety, 18, 18–28. doi:10.​1002/​da.​10105.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson, J. P., Shaver, P. R., & Wrightsman, L. S. (1991). Criteria for scale selection and evaluation. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Robinson, J. P., Shaver, P. R., & Wrightsman, L. S. (1991). Criteria for scale selection and evaluation. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Rooney, K., et al. (2001). Encarta concise English dictionary. Sydney: Pan Macmillan. Rooney, K., et al. (2001). Encarta concise English dictionary. Sydney: Pan Macmillan.
Zurück zum Zitat Speckens, A. E. M., Spinhoven, P., Sloekers, P. P. A., Bolk, J. H., & van Hemert, A. M. (1996a). A validation study of the Whitely Index, the Illness Attitude Scales, and the Somatosensory Amplification Scale in general medical and general practice patients. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 40, 95–104. doi:10.1016/0022-3999(95)00561-7.PubMedCrossRef Speckens, A. E. M., Spinhoven, P., Sloekers, P. P. A., Bolk, J. H., & van Hemert, A. M. (1996a). A validation study of the Whitely Index, the Illness Attitude Scales, and the Somatosensory Amplification Scale in general medical and general practice patients. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 40, 95–104. doi:10.​1016/​0022-3999(95)00561-7.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Speckens, A. E. M., Van Hemert, A. M., Spinhoven, P., & Bolk, J. H. (1996b). The diagnostic and prognostic significance of the Whiteley Index, the Illness Attitudes Scales and the Somatosensory Amplification Scale. Psychological Medicine, 26, 1085–1090. doi:10.1017/S0033291700035418.PubMedCrossRef Speckens, A. E. M., Van Hemert, A. M., Spinhoven, P., & Bolk, J. H. (1996b). The diagnostic and prognostic significance of the Whiteley Index, the Illness Attitudes Scales and the Somatosensory Amplification Scale. Psychological Medicine, 26, 1085–1090. doi:10.​1017/​S003329170003541​8.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Spielberger, C. D., Gorsch, R. L., & Lushene, R. (1970). State-trait anxiety inventory. Palto Alto: Consulting Psychology Press. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsch, R. L., & Lushene, R. (1970). State-trait anxiety inventory. Palto Alto: Consulting Psychology Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Tipples, J., Young, A. W., Quinlan, P., Broks, P., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). Searching for threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 55, 1007–1026. doi:10.1080/02724980143000659.CrossRef Tipples, J., Young, A. W., Quinlan, P., Broks, P., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). Searching for threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 55, 1007–1026. doi:10.​1080/​0272498014300065​9.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Metadaten
Titel
Selective Attention and Health Anxiety: Ill-Health Stimuli are Distracting for Everyone
verfasst von
Cassandra Shields
Karen Murphy
Publikationsdatum
01.06.2011
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Cognitive Therapy and Research / Ausgabe 3/2011
Print ISSN: 0147-5916
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-2819
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9351-5

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2011

Cognitive Therapy and Research 3/2011 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.