Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Community Mental Health Journal 3/2022

Open Access 26.05.2021 | Original Paper

The Important Role of Stereotypes in the relation between Mental Health Literacy and Stigmatization of Depression and Psychosis in the Community

verfasst von: Carolin M. Doll, Chantal Michel, Linda T. Betz, Benno G. Schimmelmann, Frauke Schultze-Lutter

Erschienen in: Community Mental Health Journal | Ausgabe 3/2022

Abstract

Increased mental health literacy (MHL) has not reduced stigmatization of people with mental disorder. Thus, we examined the role of stereotypes in the interplay of MHL (correct labelling, causal explanations) and the wish for social distance (WSD) from people with depressive and psychotic symptoms in a community sample of 1526 German-speaking participants in the Swiss ‘Bern Epidemiological At-Risk’ study (age 16–40 years; response rate: 60.1%). Following the presentation of an unlabelled case vignette of depression or psychosis, MHL, stereotypes and WSD were assessed in a questionnaire survey. Their interrelations were studied using structural equation modelling. MHL was not directly linked to WSD, only the psychosocial causal model was directly negatively associated with WSD. Perceived dangerousness particularly increased WSD, this was increased by a biogenetic causal model and decreased by a psychosocial causal model. Awareness-campaigns that, next to biological causes, emphasize psychosocial causes of mental disorders might better reduce stigmatization.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10597-021-00842-5.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Approximately every 4th European adult experiences a mental illness each year (World Health Organization, 2019). In doing so, patients suffer not only from their symptoms and related disabilities, but also experience stigmatization in the community (Alonso et al., 2009; Tsang, 2003; Wahl, 1999) and, similarly, in mental health care facilities (Nyblade et al., 2019; Schulze, 2007). Stigmatization is defined in the World Health Report 2001 as “a mark of shame, disgrace or disapproval which results in an individual being rejected, discriminated against, and excluded from participating in a number of different areas of society” (World Health Organization, 2001, p. 4). Stigma is commonly divided into public stigma, self-stigma and personal stigma. While public stigma is defined as negative stereotypes and prejudice toward people with mental illness held by the community, personal stigma is defined by the individual’s own stereotypes and prejudice (Eisenberg et al., 2009; Griffiths et al., 2004). Based on these definitions, self-stigma occurs when a patient identifies him-/herself with the stigmatized group; thus causing shame, social withdrawal and demoralisation (Corrigan et al., 2009; Corrigan & Shapiro, 2010). Personal stigma, among others, can be measured as the wish for social distance (WSD), i.e., the wish to avoid a specific group, such as people with a mental disorder (Jorm & Oh, 2009).
Many anti-stigma campaigns (Brijnath et al., 2016; Crisp et al., 2004; Henderson et al., 2013; Larkings & Brown, 2018; Reavley et al., 2005) were based on the intuitive assumption that improved Mental Health Literacy (MHL) would reduce discrimination and stigmatization of people with mental disorders (Angermeyer et al., 2009; Hanisch et al., 2016; Hinshaw & Stier, 2008). MHL is defined as the knowledge about symptoms, causes, treatment, and prevention of mental disorders. This includes effective self-help strategies for mild mental problems and first-aid skills to help others (Jorm, 2012). Thus, MHL is frequently considered an important target in campaigns to improve help-seeking for mental problems (Henderson et al., 2013). MHL can vary depending on the mental disorder, for example people were more likely to correctly label symptoms of depression rather than symptoms of schizophrenia (Furnham et al., 2009; Jorm et al., 1997).
However, although MHL and hypothetical help-seeking intentions have steadily increased in the community (Angermeyer & Matschinger, 2005; Angermeyer et al., 2009; Deacon, 2013; Chamberlain et al., 2012; Goldney & Fisher, 2008; Goldney et al., 2005; Jorm et al., 2006; Schomerus et al., 2012), delays in or lack of active help-seeking and stigmatization of people with mental disorder continue to be a serious problem (Angermeyer et al., 2009; Angermeyer et al., 2004; Henderson et al., 2013; Schnyder et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2007).
One reason suggested for this lack of improvement in active help-seeking and attitudes towards people with mental disorders was an unintended consequence of increasing MHL. As part of MHL-improving campaigns, biological factors have frequently been emphasized as a cause of mental illness, in particular depression or schizophrenia (Pescosolido et al., 2010; Schomerus et al., 2012). Some studies also found that people were more likely to attribute schizophrenia to biological causal factors rather than depression (Angermeyer et al., 2015; Dietrich et al., 2004; Von Lersner et al., 2019). While the resulting higher endorsement of a biological model decreased the perception of psychiatric patients as responsible and blameworthy for their problems (Kvaale et al., 2013; Lebowitz & Appelbaum, 2017), it also increased prognostic pessimism, and the perceived unpredictability and dangerousness, i.e., negative stereotypes (Haslam, 2015). Thus, the related decrease in empathy towards psychiatric patients, and the increase in self-blame and personal distress likely intensified the WSD (Haslam, 2015; Kvaale et al., 2013; Larkings & Brown, 2018; Lebowitz, 2019; Rüsch et al., 2010; Von Lersner et al., 2019), thus not reducing but possibly even increasing stigmatization, especially in the case of psychosis (Larkings & Brown, 2018; Read et al., 2006; Schnyder et al., 2018).
Commonly, these various associations between MHL, personal stigma and stereotypes were studied selectively in separate regression analyses (Angermeyer et al., 2009; Norman et al., 2008; Pescosolido et al., 2010). These studies revealed various, partly contradictory associations between MHL and personal stigma that are summarized in Fig. 1. In particular, the role of biogenetic causal models was ambiguous, as, it was associated with good MHL that is related to lower WSD (Angermeyer, Matschinger, et al., 2013; Angermeyer, Millier, et al., 2013; Schomerus et al., 2012; Von Lersner et al., 2019) and, conversely, was related to both more negative stereotypes (Haslam, 2015; Kvaale et al., 2013) and stronger WSD (Haslam, 2015; Kvaale et al., 2013; Larkings & Brown, 2018; Lebowitz, 2019; Rüsch et al., 2010; Von Lersner et al., 2019).
To resolve such inconsistencies, complex models are needed, such as path analyses or structural equation modelling (SEM) that simultaneously consider complex interrelations of several factors. Yet, these were rarely conducted in this area of research and, if so, with regard to other variables; such as cultural collectivism, professional help-seeking beliefs, help-seeking intentions, healthcare utilisation, emotional reactions to people with mental illness (Altweck et al., 2015; Schnyder et al., 2018; Schomerus et al., 2014; Von Lersner et al., 2019), or in small non-representative or selected samples (Clement et al., 2015; Lanfredi et al., 2019; Trani et al., 2016; Von Lersner et al., 2019).
As SEM models have the advantage to show direct and indirect effects, we examined the interplay between MHL and stereotypes, with respect to personal stigmatization in terms of WSD, in the context of schizophrenia and depression, in a large representative Swiss community sample using a SEM approach (Fig. 1). Better understanding of this complex interplay will help to improve future anti-stigma campaigns, by avoiding potential unintended negative consequences.

Method

Study Design and Procedure

Our study was conducted as an add-on study to the ‘Bern Epidemiological At-Risk’ (BEAR) study between June 2011 and June 2015; and participation in the BEAR study was the main eligibility criterion of the add-on study, sufficient language skills in German the only other (Schnyder et al., 2018; Schultze-Lutter et al., 2014; Schultze-Lutter, Michel, et al., 2018; Schultze-Lutter, Schmidt, et al., 2018). Within the BEAR study, 2683 participants, randomly drawn from the population register of the Canton Bern, Switzerland, were recruited for a telephone interview (response rate: 63.4%; Supplementary material eFigure 1) (Schultze-Lutter et al., 2018; Schultze-Lutter, Schmidt, et al., 2018). Because the BEAR study had focussed on the assessment of the prevalence and clinical relevance of clinical high-risk of psychosis criteria and symptoms, inclusion criteria restricted the age range to between 16 and 40 years, i.e., age range with the highest incidence of first-episode psychosis, and excluded people with a past or present psychosis (Schultze-Lutter et al., 2018a, 2018b). Further inclusion criteria were main residency in the Canton Bern, an identified working telephone number, and availability during the recruitment period. Exclusion criteria included insufficient language skills in German, English, French or Spanish (Schultze-Lutter et al., 2018a, 2018b). Compared to the Canton statistics of 16-to-40-year-olds, the BEAR sample was well representative (Schultze-Lutter et al., 2018a, 2018b).
At the conclusion of the telephone interview, 2539 participants in the BEAR study with sufficient knowledge of German (eligibility rate: 94.6%) were asked to participate in the separate add-on study on MHL and attitudes towards people with mental illness. Of these, 2215 participants agreed to additionally participate in the add-on survey (82.4%) and were mailed the questionnaires (details in Schnyder et al., 2018). After a maximum of three reminder calls, 1526 participants returned the questionnaire. Thus, according to the definitions of the American Association for Public Opinion Research (American Association for Public Opinion Research, 2016), both response and cooperation rates of the add-on study were 60.1%, and the overall refusal/non-responder rate was 39.9%. The minor differences between responders and non-responders/refusers to the add-on study were of small effect size at most (Supplementary material eTable1).
Separate verbal informed consent was obtained and recorded from all subjects prior to assessments in both the BEAR study and the add-on study. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008, and were approved by the ethical committee of the University of Bern (No. 172/09). Furthermore, all authors certify responsibility.

Assessments

Demographic and clinical information was assessed as part of the telephone interview (Schultze-Lutter, Michel, et al., 2018; Schultze-Lutter, Schmidt, et al., 2018). For the assessment of MHL (incl. labelling and causal explanations), attitudes towards and perceived characteristics of people with a mental disorder (stereotypes), we used the well-established German questionnaires of Angermeyer and colleagues (Angermeyer & Matschinger, 1996, 1999; Angermeyer et al. 2001; van Brakel, 2006; Link et al., 2004). The questions on causal explanations resulted from a review of the literature at the time and were further refined by first studies using open questions (Angermeyer & Matschinger, 1996; Angermeyer et al. 2001). Stereotypes were assessed according to the scale “stereotype agreement” from the German version of the “Self-Stigma in Mental Illness Scale” (Corrigan et al., 2006; Rüsch et al., 2006) that was slightly modified in the wording of the instruction. The questionnaire starts with an unlabelled case vignette (Angermeyer et al., 2009; Angermeyer et al., 2001, 2004). The two alternatively presented vignettes describe a hypothetical acquaintance with symptoms fulfilling DSM-III-R criteria for either schizophrenia or depression (Supplementary material eText1); their validity was established by blinded rating of five mental health professionals (Angermeyer et al., 2001). The participants were randomly presented either the psychosis vignette or the depression vignette, and, within an open question, first asked to briefly state what they think the person in the vignette is suffering from. These descriptions were subsequently dichotomized as correct when the description included correct name of the disorder or of its constituting symptoms; all other descriptions were labelled as incorrect (Angermeyer & Matschinger, 1999; see Supplementary material eTable 2 for coding examples). Correct labelling was equalled to good overall MHL. Questions regarding 18 possible causal explanations for the behaviour described in the vignette (Table 2) were presented using a five-point Likert scale from 0 = ‘certainly not a cause’ to 4 = ‘certainly a cause’. Furthermore, for the assessment of stereotypes, the participants were presented nine characteristics (Table 3) to be rated according to the degree that they apply to the person described in the vignette on a five-point Likert scale from 0 = ‘definitely not true’ to 4 = ‘definitely true’.
Stigmatization in terms of WSD was assessed according to the Social Distance Scale (Link et al., 1987), self-rating the participant’s willingness to socially interact in seven different situations with the person described in the vignette on a five-point Likert scale from 0 = ‘definitely willing’ to 4 = ‘definitely not willing’ (Angermeyer & Matschinger, 1996). Higher sum scores indicate stronger WSD.

Statistical Analyses

First, principal component analyses (PCA) with Varimax rotation and pairwise complete observations to deal with missing values were conducted separately on the 18 items on causal explanations, and on the nine items on stereotypes. Resulting factors were examined for their construct validity in terms of the composite reliability using “lavaan” (Rosseel, 2012). Composite reliability is favored over Cronbach’s α when the requirement of t-equivalence (i.e., all items measure the same true value) is violated (Danner, 2015), e.g., in factors composed of items measuring different aspects of a latent construct. However, calculation of the composite reliability requires at least four items per factor (Danner, 2015). Thus, despite the reported biases and limitations of the α coefficient (Cho & Kim, 2015; Revelle & Zinbarg, 2009; Sijtsma, 2009; Sijtsma & van der Ark, 2015), we also calculated Cronbach’s α, which, however, can only be regarded an estimation of the lower boundary of reliability as it tends to underestimate the reliability in factors with few items as well as when t-equivalence is not given (Danner, 2015). For the ordinal nature of items, we used Spearman’s correlation coefficient to construct the correlation matrix. The Kaiser–Meyer Olkin (KMO) measure was used to check the sampling adequacy for the analyses.
Next, we conducted a SEM, which included all associations reported in previous studies (Fig. 1). Missing items (0.03%) were accounted for by using the estimator ‘full information maximum likelihood’ (FIML; Kline, 2011). Based on the results of the PCA and of previous studies (Angermeyer et al., 2004; Schnyder et al., 2018), we defined five latent variables for causal explanations (‘biogenetic’, ‘psychosocial stress’, ‘childhood adversity’, ‘substance abuse’, and ‘constitution/personality’) and two for stereotypes (‘perceived dangerousness’ and ‘perceived dependency’). The variables ‘group’ (depression or schizophrenia vignette), ‘correct labelling’ as a general measure of MHL and ‘WSD’ were modelled as observed binary variables. The pathways from ‘group’ and ‘correct labelling’ via causal explanations and stereotypes to WSD with all possible associations between latent and observed variables were modelled (Fig. 1).
To control for the reported effect of type of mental disorder (Angermeyer et al., 2014; Angermeyer, Matschinger, et al., 2013; Angermeyer, Millier, et al., 2013; McCann et al., 2018; Norman et al., 2012; Sevensson & Hansson, 2016), we included the variable ‘vignette’ in our analysis. Furthermore, for the reported sex differences in stigmatization and MHL (Dey et al., 2020; Hadjimina & Furnham, 2017), we analysed a model with the control variable ‘sex’ (see Supplementary material eFigure 3). However, due to our age restriction to 16- to 40-year-olds, the reported age effect in older people of age 65 + was unlikely to work in our younger sample; thus, we did not include age as a control variable (Mackenzie et al., 2019).
In order to test for mediation effects in the final model, we used “lavaan” (Rosseel, 2012). Thereby, we labelled potential variables in the regression as parameters, so that we could use these parameters to create mediation pathways within the model. The statistical analyses were conducted in SPSS 25.0 and in the R language for statistical computing using the packages “lavaan” (Rosseel, 2012) and “psych” (Revelle, 2018). Throughout, we considered a level of significance of α < 0.05.

Results

Sample Characteristics

A similar number of questionnaires with a psychosis (n = 784) and with a depression vignette (n = 742) was returned (\(\chi_{(1)}^{2} = 1.1.56\), p = 0.282). Slightly less males than females returned the questionnaire (Table 1). The average age of participants was 31 years; most of them were Swiss, unmarried, and normally employed, and had a short cycle tertiary education or Master degree (Table 1). Every 8th participant had met criteria for a current non-psychotic axis I disorder in the telephone interview (Table 1), this number going down to every 14th participant (n = 108; 7.1%) when excluding specific phobias. Almost half of the sample reported a 1st- or 2nd-degree family member with suspected or diagnosed mental disorder—mostly with an affective, rarely a psychotic disorder (Table 1). Clinical and sociodemographic variables did not differ significantly between responders of the two vignettes (Table 1).
Table 1
Sample characteristics of the responders to the add-on study (N = 1526) according to the case vignette of the questionnaire
 
Depression
(n = 742)
Psychosis
(n = 784)
Total sample
(N = 1526)
Statistics
U/χ2(df); Pearson’s r/Cramer’s V
Sex, n (%) male
353 (47.8)
365 (46.7)
718 (47.2)
\(\chi_{(1)}^{2}\)  = 0.162, p = 0.687, V = 0.010
Age: median (mean ± SD)
33.84 (31.10 ± 7.3)
33.91 (31.53 ± 7.22)
33.86 (31.32 ± 7.27)
U = 277 609, p = 0.200, r = − 0.030
Nationality, n (%) Swiss
706 (95.5)
749 (95.9)
1455 (95.7)
\(\chi_{(1)}^{2}\) = 0.126, p = 0.723, V = 0.009
Highest educational level (ISCED 2011)a, n (%)
   
\(\chi_{(6)}^{2}\) = 7.452, p = 0.281, V = 0.070
 Primary education (1)
0
0
0
 
 Lower secondary education (2)
24 (3.2)
18 (2.3)
42 (2.8)
 
 Higher secondary education (3)
13 (1.8)
13 (1.7)
26 (1.7)
 
 Post-secondary non-tertiary education (4)
4 (0.5)
9 (1.2)
13 (0.9)
 
 Short cycle tertiary education (5)
405 (54.8)
390 (49.9)
795 (52.1)
 
 Master’s or equivalent level (7)
247 (33.4)
301 (38.5)
548 (35.9)
 
 Doctoral or equivalent level (8)
12 (1.6)
12 (1.5)
24 (1.6)
 
Employment, n (%)
   
\(\chi_{(3)}^{2}\) = 2.698, p = 0.441, V = 0.042
 Unemployed
16 (2.2)
9 (1.2)
25 (1.6)
 
 Protected employment
1 (0.1)
2 (0.3)
3 (0.2)
 
 Temporarily/self-employed
9 (1.2)
9 (1.2)
18 (1.2)
 
 Normal employment, in school/training
713 (96.5)
761 (97.4)
1474 (97.0)
 
Marital status, n (%)
   
\(\chi_{(2)}^{2}\) = 0.259, p = 0.998, V = 0.013
 Unmarried
393 (53.2)
406 (52.0)
799 (52.6)
 
 Married or registered partnership
320 (43.3)
348 (44.6)
668 (43.9)
 
 Separated/Divorced/Widowed
25 (3.4)
26 (3.3)
51 (3.3)
 
Current non-psychotic axis-I disorderb, n (%)
97 (13.1)
95 (12.2)
192 (12.6)
\(\chi_{(1)}^{2}\) = 0.318, p = 0.573, V = 0.014
Family member with a mental disorder, n (%)
    
 Affective disorder
185 (25.1)
216 (27.7)
401 (26.4)
\(\chi_{(1)}^{2}\) = 1.238, p = 0.266, V = 0.029
 Psychotic disorder
23 (3.1)
25 (3.2)
48 (3.2)
\(\chi_{(1)}^{2}\) = 0.007, p = 0.993. V = 0.002
aAccording to International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) (UNESCO Institute for Statistics, 2012)
bAcording to Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview

Factors of Causal Explanations and Stereotypes

The KMO measure indicated excellent or “meritorious” (Kaiser, 1974) sampling adequacy for the analyses (KMO = 0.79 and KMO = 0.78), and all KMO values for individual items were > 0.65 in the first and > 0.57 in the second PCA, and therewith above the threshold for acceptability of 0.5 (Schneeweiss & Mathes, 1995). Bartlett’s test of sphericity (\(\chi_{(153)}^{2} = 6073.26\), p < 0.001 and \(\chi_{(36)}^{2} = 3723.98\), p < 0.001) indicated that correlations between items were sufficiently large for PCA (Schneeweiss & Mathes, 1995). In the PCA of the 18 causal explanations items, five independent factors (‘psychosocial stress’, ‘childhood adversity’, ‘biogenetic’, ‘substance abuse’ and ‘constitution/personality’) had an eigenvalue over Kaiser’s criterion of 1 and explained 55% of the variance (Table 2). In the second PCA, two independent factors (‘perceived dangerousness’, ‘perceived dangerousness’) had an eigenvalue over Kaiser’s criterion of 1 and explained 53% of the variance (Table 3). With regard to the internal consistency of the factors, the two largest factors, ‘psychosocial stress’ and ‘perceived dangerousness’, had satisfactory to good composite reliability values above 0.70 (Hair et al., 2019). The two other factors of four items each, ‘constitution/personality’ and ‘childhood adversity’, were well or almost acceptable (Hair et al., 2019; see Table 2). Cronbach’s α, for the factors with less than four items, indicated lower estimations within the range of “poor” and “questionable” reliability for ‘substance abuse’ and ‘biogenetic’, and just within the “unacceptable” range for ‘perceived dependency’ (see Tables 2 and 3).
Table 2
Results of the principal component analysis (PCA) of 18 questions regarding 18 possible causal explanations for the behaviour described in the vignette (n = 1526), and the internal consistency of the factors (composite reliability and Cronbach’s α)
Items
Factor 1:
Psychosocial stress
Factor 2:
Substance abuse
Factor 3:
Constitution/personality
Factor 4:
Childhood adversity
Factor 5:
Biogenetics
Work-related stress
0.80
    
Too high self-expectation
0.71
    
Problems or sorrows in family
0.68
    
Daily hustles
0.67
    
Severe or very stressful life event
0.57
    
An unconscious conflict
0.50
    
Medication or drug abuse
 
0.81
   
Alcohol abuse
 
0.79
   
Weak will
  
0.74
  
Weak constitution
  
0.68
  
Immoral lifestyle
  
0.59
  
God’s will
  
0.42
  
Grown up in a broken home
   
0.80
 
Lack of parental affection
   
0.76
 
Little support others
   
0.43
 
Spoiling or over-protective parents
   
0.57
 
Heredity
    
0.75
Brain disease
    
0.55
Eigenvalue
2.88
1.89
1.87
1.81
1.50
Composite reliabilitya
0.77
0.59
0.67
Cronbach’s α
0.70
0.60
0.60
0.64
0.55
Only factor loading > 0.40 are displayed in descending order per factor (causal explanations). The instruction this item is as follows “Now, we would like to know your opinion about the cause of problems like the one described above. For your answers, a 5-point response scale is provided. Please, tick for every possible cause to what extent this might be the cause of such a problem.” Rating for each characteristic is done on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “is certainly one of the causes” to “is certainly not a cause”
aIn empirical research, values between 0.60 and 0.70 are considered “acceptable,” values between 0.70 and 0.90 range from “satisfactory” to “good” (Hair et al., 2015)
Table 3
Results of the principal component analysis (PCA) of 9 questions regarding the characteristics of the person described in the vignette (n = 1526), and the internal consistency of the factors (composite reliability and Cronbach’s α)
Items
Factor 1:
Dangerous/unpredictable
Factor 2:
Dependent/needy
Dangerous
0.78
 
Lacking self-control
0.76
 
Frightening
0.75
 
Unpredictable
0.74
 
Aggressive
0.70
 
Strange
0.67
 
Dependent on others
 
0.82
Helpless
 
0.73
Needy
 
0.49
Eigenvalue
3.27
1.50
Composite reliabilitya
0.83
Cronbach’s α
0.83
0.47
Only factor loading > 0.40 are displayed in descending order per factor (stereotype). The instruction this item is as follows “Now we would like to get to know what characteristics you think apply to this person. Please, tick with each characteristic of the list to what extent it applies or not.” Rating for each characteristic is done on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “certainly applies” to “certainly not applies”
aIn empirical research, values between 0.60 and 0.70 are considered “acceptable,” values between 0.70 and 0.90 range from “satisfactory” to “good” (Hair et al., 2015)

Association Between Correct Labelling, Type of Vignette, Causal Explanations and Personal Attributions on Stigmatizing

In the initial SEM that included all 5 causal models, the factors ‘childhood adversity’ (R2 = 0.01) and ‘substance abuse’ (R2 = 0.02) missed crucial thresholds for good model fit because of their low R-square (see Supplementary material eFigure 2). Thus, they were removed, and only three causal models (‘psychosocial stress’, ‘biogenetic’, ‘constitution/personality’) were taken into the final SEM (Fig. 2, Supplementary material eTable 3).
For the final SEM, the fit indices RMSEA and its 90% confidence intervals, and SRMR were in line with recommended values (≤ 0.06, not containing 0.08, and ≤ 0.08, resp.) suggesting good model fit to data (Hooper et al., 2011) (Fig. 2). The PNFI value was 0.705. Yet, the χ2-statistic became significant. The CFI was below the recommended value of ≥ 0.95 (Hooper et al. 2008; Kline, 2011), suggesting possibly insufficient fit. However, two severe problems limit the use of the χ2-statistic: (1) severe violations of the assumption of multivariate normality may result in model rejections even of properly specified models; and (2) being essentially a statistical significance test, it is sensitive to sample size and nearly always rejects the model in large samples like ours (Hooper et al., 2008). A problem of the CFI is its assumption of all latent variables being uncorrelated/independent; thus, it is less reliable in models like ours that violate this assumption, as demonstrated by the significant intercorrelations of factors (Supplementary material eTable 4) (Hooper et al., 2008).
Regarding the other fit indices, RMSEA, which measures how well the model, including unknown but optimally chosen parameter estimates, would fit the sample’s covariance matrix, is increasingly “regarded as one of the most informative fit indices” (Hooper et al., 2008, p. 54). Despite its sensitivity to the number of estimated parameters in the model and, relatedly, its favouring of parsimonious models, this important fit index indicated good fit of our model even in the face of its complex, non-parsimonious nature. Furthermore, the SRMR also indicated good fit. Because Hu and Bentler’s ‘2-index presentation strategy’ suggests that a model should be regarded as well fitting, if both RMSEA and SRMR indicate acceptable fit (Hooper et al., 2008), we considered the fit of our model (Fig. 2) acceptable overall.
In line with earlier reports (Fig. 1), the type of vignette was associated with WSD, whereby the psychosis vignette was related to stronger WSD (Fig. 2). Additionally, the type of vignette was significantly related to correct labelling, all causal models and ‘perceived dangerousness’ but not ‘perceived dependency’. In doing so, the psychosis vignette increased the likelihood to endorse a biogenetic model and to perceive the illustrated person as dangerous, while the depression vignette was more likely correctly labelled, and explained by psychosocial stress or constitution/personality-related causes. Unexpectedly, the path from correct labelling as a general measure of good MHL to WSD was not significant, and neither were those between correct labelling and stereotypes. In order to examine an indirect effect of correct labelling on stereotypes via causal models, we tested the pathway “correct labelling—biogenetic—perceived dangerousness” that, however also remained insignificant (p = 0.219). Thus, correct labelling had neither direct nor indirect effects on WSD in our model. Yet, again in line with earlier reports, correct labelling increased endorsement of a biogenetic and non-endorsement of a psychosocial stress or constitution/personality-related causal model. As expected, endorsement of a biogenetic model was related to more perceived dangerousness that intensified WSD. However, contrary to the literature, the biogenetic model was not directly related to WSD. Furthermore, it was also not indirectly related to WSD via perceived dangerousness, as this indirect effect also remained insignificant (p = 0.154). Contrary to the biogenetic model, the constitution/personality-related causal model was negatively associated with correct labelling, and its endorsement not only increased perceived dangerousness but also perceived dependency. Like the biogenetic model, the constitution/personality-related model was also not indirectly related to WSD via perceived dangerousness, as this indirect effect on WSD was also not significant (p = 0.756). Of all causal models, the psychosocial stress model was the only one directly and negatively related to WSD. This positive effect of psychosocial stress-related causal models also worked via reducing the likelihood of perceiving patients as dangerous (p < 0.001) (Fig. 2).
With regard to the impact of sex (see Supplementary material eFigure 3), compared to men, women labelled the vignette correctly more often, were more likely to relate the described disorder to a biogenetic or psychosocial stress model, and were less likely to assume constitution/personality as cause for the mental disorder, and to regard the person in the vignette as ‘perceived dependency’. Despite these significant sex differences in MHL in particular, no significant sex differences revealed for perceived dangerousness or WSD; and the associations of the overall model (Fig. 2) were replicated.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to examine the associations of MHL, stereotypes and stigma in one complex model. While most of its paths had earlier been described, our model sheds light on some of the apparently conflicting results in the literature, in particular the association of good MHL with low WSD as a measure of stigmatization on the one hand (Angermeyer et al., 2009; Hanisch et al., 2016; Hinshaw & Stier, 2008) and, on the other hand, the association of good MHL with endorsement of a biogenetic model (Pescocolido et al., 2010; Schomerus et al., 2012) that, in turn, is associated with a stronger WSD (Haslam, 2015; Kvaale et al., 2013; Larkings & Brown, 2018; Lebowitz, 2019; Rüsch et al., 2010; Von Lersner et al., 2019). Our model now indicates that this apparent contradiction may have resulted from (incorrectly) assuming a direct link between MHL and WSD when important mediators such as stereotypes had not been considered.
In our model, MHL had little effect on WSD as especially evidenced by the non-significant direct paths to WSD from correct labelling and endorsement of a biogenetic causal model. Rather, the symptomatology of the mental illness (i.e., the case vignettes) and the perceived characteristics of the affected person (i.e., the stereotypes) were determinants of WSD.
Anti-stigma and awareness campaigns to improve help-seeking for mental problems and mental health first aid trainings have commonly focused on improving MHL (Angermeyer et al., 2009; Brijnath et al., 2016; Corrigan, 2016; Crisp et al., 2004; Hanisch et al., 2016; Henderson et al., 2013; Hinshaw & Stier, 2008; Larkings & Brown, 2018; Morgan et al., 2018; Sartorius & Schulze, 2005). Yet, although MHL has steadily increased in the community, in particular in terms of increased endorsement of biogenetic causal models, delays in or lack of help-seeking, and stigmatization of people with mental disorder, have improved less- or sometimes even worsened, thus, remaining a serious problem (Angermeyer et al., 2009; Angermeyer, Matschinger, et al., 2013; Angermeyer, Millier, et al., 2013; Corrigan, 2016; Deacon, 2013; Henderson et al., 2013; Larkings & Brown, 2018; Pescosolido et al., 2010; Schomerus et al., 2012). Accordingly, an earlier SEM analysis of our data already demonstrated negative effects of biogenetic and also constitution/personality-related causal models on active help-seeking via a dangerous stereotype and WSD (Schnyder et al., 2018). Yet, this SEM had not studied any direct effect on WSD other than that of the dangerous stereotype (Schnyder et al., 2018). Our study now further supports the unintended effect of improved MHL in terms of correct labelling of the vignette and, relatedly, endorsement of a biogenetic causal model that increased the perception of patients with mental disorder as dangerous and unpredictable. This is in accordance with previous reports of a positive association between endorsement of a biogenetic causal explanation, and the perception of people with a mental disorder as more dangerous (Kvaale et al., 2013; Larkings & Brown, 2018; Pescosolido et al., 2010; Read, 2007; Read & Harré, 2001; Schnyder et al., 2018).
To control for differences related to symptomatology, the vignette was included as a control variable in the model. As in other studies, in the schizophrenia vignette group a biogenetic causal explanation was more likely compared to the depression vignette group (Angermeyer et al., 2015; Dietrich et al., 2004; Von Lersner et al., 2019). The depression vignette group, in turn, more often endorsed a psychosocial explanation. Overall, participants expressed a higher WSD to the person depicted in the psychosis vignette compared to the person in the depression vignette. Thus, in light of the missing direct effect of correct labelling on WSD and with regard to the classic debate of whether behaviours/symptoms or label formed the basis of stigma, our results indicate a major role of symptoms not label (Pescosolido, 2013).
Interestingly, correct labelling was significantly related only to causal explanations but not to stereotypes, having likely a weaker effect on both stereotypes and WSD, compared to symptoms. Hence, our results support earlier findings of psychiatric terminology, i.e., correct labels, not having a direct impact on attitudes toward mental illness (Mann & Himelein, 2004). They also support earlier reports that (illustrated) symptoms play a significant role, with psychotic symptoms being more stigmatized than depressive symptoms (Mann & Himelein, 2004; Norman et al., 2008). Earlier, it was suggested that stigmatization is the worst when the disorder is severe, unfamiliar and, most importantly, socially debilitating, because lay people would focus on visible aspects of social disability (Gaebel et al., 2006). And indeed, the illustrated psychosocial disability was worst in the psychosis vignette compared to the depression vignette (see Supplementary material eText1). This indicates a necessity to prevent development of severe symptoms and psychosocial functional impairment in order to avoid stigmatization, thus reinforcing the view of the WHO that effective prevention of mental disorders can “change the way mental disorders are looked upon by society” (World Health Organization, 2004, p. 3).
Aside from a less severe symptomatology, the only factor with a potential to reduce WSD was endorsement of a psychosocial causal model. This became more apparent in the depression vignette and incorrect labelling of the vignette, mostly of the psychosis vignette (Angermeyer et al., 2009). Furthermore, endorsing a psychosocial causal model reduced perceived dangerousness that increased WSD. Taken together, our findings support critique on awareness campaigns that primarily convey a medical, biological etiological model of mental disorders (Lebowitz, 2019; Longdon & Read, 2017; Schomerus et al., 2014). This critique was based on the “substantial evidence that campaigns based on the "medical model" (such as the "mental illness is an illness like any other" approach) are not only ineffective, but can actually compound the problem” (Longdon & Read, 2017, p. 24). Supporting earlier recommendations, our results support calls for a stronger role of psychosocial explanatory models in MHL-supporting campaigns, in order to promote more positive and tolerant attitudes towards, and inclusion of psychiatric patients (Longdon & Read, 2017; Pescosolido et al., 2010). However, endorsing a biogenetic model was reported to increase help-seeking intentions and support of psychopharmacological and psychotherapeutic treatments (Arboleda-Flórez & Stuart, 2012; Lebowitz & Appelbaum, 2017; Pescosolido et al., 2010), possibly via an underlying fear, reflecting a desire for protection against people with mental illness (Schnyder et al., 2018; Speerforck et al., 2017). Thus, awareness campaigns with a stronger focus on psychosocial causes might bring about the unintended consequence of supporting non-professional help-seeking recommendations (Altweck et al., 2015). In order to escape this vicious circle of unintended consequences, future studies of awareness campaigns should further examine the reported differential effects of biological “brain disease” and genetic “heredity” causal models in relation to different disorders, and their defining and accompanying symptoms, in order to find the most advantageous balance between psychosocial and biogenetic causal models, to optimize beneficial effects on both stigmatization and help-seeking (Speerforck et al., 2014). Such research that cross-sectionally and, importantly, longitudinally examines the interplay of different aspects of MHL and attitudes towards people with mental disorders on both stigmatization and help-seeking using appropriate measures (such as SEM or network analyses) is clearly needed, and might solve the dilemma between the potential stigma-increasing effect of biogenetic models, and the potential help-seeking-reducing effect of psychosocial models.

Strengths and Limitations

Our study has several strengths and limitations. Among the strengths are clearly the large sample size and the use of SEM as a means to simultaneously consider a multitude of direct and indirect associations.
Among the limitations is a CFI below the recommended value of ≥ 0.95 (Hooper et al. 2008; Kline, 2011), which indicates a possibly insufficient fit. However, the CFI is sensitive to sample size and nearly always rejects the model in large samples like ours (Hooper et al., 2008). Other fit indices (RMSEA and SRMR), however, indicated a good model fit according to Hu and Bentler’s ‘2-index presentation strategy’ (Hooper et al., 2008) This strategy suggests that a model should be regarded as well fitting, if RMSEA and SRMR indicate acceptable fit—like they do in our model. Another limitation of the study might be the partly low internal consistency of the factors that, however, seems mostly related to methodological factors, such as inclusion of few items and/or the fact that included items frequently reflect different aspects of an underlying construct, i.e., are not t-equivalent (Danner, 2015). Thus, while the composite reliability had certainly been the method of choice, in factors with less than three items, only Cronbach’s α could be calculated as an estimate of the lower boundary of reliability. Thus, the true reliability of the factors that all seem clinically plausible is likely higher and in no case unacceptable. Another limitation is the restriction of the sample to German-speaking people of mainly Middle-European background aged 16 to 40. As cultural and also age-related differences with respect to MHL and stigmatization were reported, conclusions drawn from our results may primarily be relevant to European health care systems and to young adults (Altweck et al., 2015; Angermeyer et al., 2016; Nersessova et al., 2019; Pescosolido, 2013; Von Lersner et al., 2019). Yet, the replication of several well-known findings in a complex modelling set-up might also provide evidence for the generalizability of these associations across the Western culture. And the examined age-group represents that of the highest incidence of mental disorders (Pedersen et al., 2014), and, consequently, the age group in which WSD might have its most adverse effects on first help-seeking for mental disorder.
Furthermore, a number of potential moderators on WSD were not considered in our model, e.g., (level of) familiarity with mental illness (Angermeyer et al., 2004; Kasow & Weisskirch, 2010), personal values (Norman et al., 2008), and perceived social norms (Norman et al., 2008). In regards to familiarity—either by own illness or mental illness of a family member, friend or colleague, we had only assessed family history of first- and second-degree biological relatives and only current mental disorders of the participant. Thus, we would have missed familiarity by mental disorders of other well-known people and by own past mental disorder. To avoid introducing a systematic assessment bias, we therefore refrained from including a latent variable ‘familiarity’. A limitation that our study shares with most other interview- or questionnaire-based studies on this topic is the possibility of systematic response biases such as social desirability. Yet, the results are well in line with earlier reported single association between the examined variables, and thus, point towards only minor response biases. Further, in line with the criteria employed by Sastre-Rus et al. (2019), the results indicate that the questionnaires can be assumed to have a strong level of evidence for good quality assessment, supporting earlier notions of them as good-quality instruments to assess attitudes and beliefs about mental illness (Link et al., 2004; van Brakel, 2006).
Overall, our results challenge the view that an improved MHL will unequivocally reduce stigmatization and discrimination of people with mental disorder, especially when biological models are emphasized. Rather, preventive approaches that reduce symptom exacerbation, in combination with education about psychosocial causes, as well as the causes and interplay of symptoms in public campaigns (Schultze-Lutter, Michel, et al., 2018; Schultze-Lutter, Schmidt, et al., 2018), might reduce stigmatizing stereotypes best while still facilitating help-seeking.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

Drs Michel, Schimmelmann, Schultze-Lutter, MSc Doll, and MSc Betz have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study. Dr. Schimmelmann reports and received honoraria and is on the speakers’ board of Takeda (Shire) and Infectopharm.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study was approved by the ethical committee of the University of Bern (No. 172/09).
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Psychiatrie, den Premium-Inhalten der psychiatrischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat American Association for Public Opinion Research. (2016). Standard Definitions: Final Dispositions of Case Codes and Outcome Rates for Surveys. 9 edition. AAPOR. American Association for Public Opinion Research. (2016). Standard Definitions: Final Dispositions of Case Codes and Outcome Rates for Surveys. 9 edition. AAPOR.
Zurück zum Zitat Angermeyer, M. C., Daubmann, A., Wegscheider, K., Mnich, E., Schomerus, G., & Knesebeck, O. V. (2015). The relationship between biogenetic attributions and desire for social distance from persons with schizophrenia and major depression revisited. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 24(4), 335–341.PubMedCrossRef Angermeyer, M. C., Daubmann, A., Wegscheider, K., Mnich, E., Schomerus, G., & Knesebeck, O. V. (2015). The relationship between biogenetic attributions and desire for social distance from persons with schizophrenia and major depression revisited. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 24(4), 335–341.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Angermeyer, M. C., & Matschinger, H. (1996). The effect of personal experience with mental illness on the attitude towards individuals suffering from mental disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 31(6), 321–326.PubMedCrossRef Angermeyer, M. C., & Matschinger, H. (1996). The effect of personal experience with mental illness on the attitude towards individuals suffering from mental disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 31(6), 321–326.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Angermeyer, M. C., Matschinger, H., & Corrigan, P. W. (2004). Familiarity with mental illness and social distance from people with schizophrenia and major depression: Testing a model using data from a representative population survey. Schizophrenia Research, 69(2–3), 175–182.PubMedCrossRef Angermeyer, M. C., Matschinger, H., & Corrigan, P. W. (2004). Familiarity with mental illness and social distance from people with schizophrenia and major depression: Testing a model using data from a representative population survey. Schizophrenia Research, 69(2–3), 175–182.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Angermeyer, M. C., Matschinger, H., & Riedel-Heller, S. G. (2001). What to do about mental disorder–help-seeking recommendations of the lay public. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 103(3), 220–225.PubMedCrossRef Angermeyer, M. C., Matschinger, H., & Riedel-Heller, S. G. (2001). What to do about mental disorder–help-seeking recommendations of the lay public. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 103(3), 220–225.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Angermeyer, M. C., Matschinger, H., & Schomerus, G. (2013a). Attitudes towards psychiatric treatment and people with mental illness: Changes over two decades. British Journal of Psychiatry, 203(2), 146–151.CrossRef Angermeyer, M. C., Matschinger, H., & Schomerus, G. (2013a). Attitudes towards psychiatric treatment and people with mental illness: Changes over two decades. British Journal of Psychiatry, 203(2), 146–151.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Angermeyer, M. C., Millier, A., Rémuzat, C., Refaï, T., & Toumi, M. (2013b). Attitudes and beliefs of the French public about schizophrenia and major depression: Results from a vignette-based population survey. BMC Psychiatry, 13, 313.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Angermeyer, M. C., Millier, A., Rémuzat, C., Refaï, T., & Toumi, M. (2013b). Attitudes and beliefs of the French public about schizophrenia and major depression: Results from a vignette-based population survey. BMC Psychiatry, 13, 313.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Arboleda-Flórez, J., & Stuart, H. (2012). From sin to science: Fighting the stigmatization of mental illnesses. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 57(8), 457–463.PubMedCrossRef Arboleda-Flórez, J., & Stuart, H. (2012). From sin to science: Fighting the stigmatization of mental illnesses. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 57(8), 457–463.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Blanz, M. (2015). Forschungsmethoden und Statistik für die Soziale Arbeit: Grundlagen und Anwendungen. Kohlhammer. Blanz, M. (2015). Forschungsmethoden und Statistik für die Soziale Arbeit: Grundlagen und Anwendungen. Kohlhammer.
Zurück zum Zitat Brijnath, B., Protheroe, J., Mahtani, K. R., & Antoniades, J. (2016). Do web-based mental health literacy interventions Improve the mental health literacy of adult consumers? Results from a systematic review. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 18(6), e165.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Brijnath, B., Protheroe, J., Mahtani, K. R., & Antoniades, J. (2016). Do web-based mental health literacy interventions Improve the mental health literacy of adult consumers? Results from a systematic review. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 18(6), e165.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Chamberlain, P. N., Goldney, R. D., Taylor, A. W., & Eckert, K. A. (2012). Have mental health education programs influenced the mental health literacy of those with major depression and suicidal ideation? A comparison between 1998 and 2008 in South Australia. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 42(5), 525–540.PubMedCrossRef Chamberlain, P. N., Goldney, R. D., Taylor, A. W., & Eckert, K. A. (2012). Have mental health education programs influenced the mental health literacy of those with major depression and suicidal ideation? A comparison between 1998 and 2008 in South Australia. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 42(5), 525–540.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Clement, S., Williams, P., Farrelly, S., Hatch, S. L., Schauman, O., Jeffery, D., et al. (2015). Mental health-related discrimination as a predictor of low engagement with mental health services. Psychiatric Services, 66(2), 171–176.PubMedCrossRef Clement, S., Williams, P., Farrelly, S., Hatch, S. L., Schauman, O., Jeffery, D., et al. (2015). Mental health-related discrimination as a predictor of low engagement with mental health services. Psychiatric Services, 66(2), 171–176.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Corrigan, P. W. (2016). Lessons learned from unintended consequences about erasing the stigma of mental illness. World Psychiatry, 15(1), 67–73.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Corrigan, P. W. (2016). Lessons learned from unintended consequences about erasing the stigma of mental illness. World Psychiatry, 15(1), 67–73.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Crisp, A., Cowan, L., & Hart, D. (2004). The college’s anti-stigma campaign, 1998–2003. Psychiatric Bulletin, 28, 133–136.CrossRef Crisp, A., Cowan, L., & Hart, D. (2004). The college’s anti-stigma campaign, 1998–2003. Psychiatric Bulletin, 28, 133–136.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Danner, D. (2016). Reliability - The precision of a measurement. GESIS Surveys Guidelines. Mannheim, Germany: GESIS- Leibniz-Institute for the Social Sciences. Danner, D. (2016). Reliability - The precision of a measurement. GESIS Surveys Guidelines. Mannheim, Germany: GESIS- Leibniz-Institute for the Social Sciences.
Zurück zum Zitat Deacon, B. J. (2013). The biomedical model of mental disorder: A critical analysis of its validity, utility, and effects on psychotherapy research. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(7), 846–861.PubMedCrossRef Deacon, B. J. (2013). The biomedical model of mental disorder: A critical analysis of its validity, utility, and effects on psychotherapy research. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(7), 846–861.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dey, M., Paz Castro, R., Jorm, A. F., Marti, L., Schaub, M. P., & Mackinnon, A. (2020). Stigmatizing attitudes of Swiss youth towards peers with mental disorders. PLoS ONE, 15(7), e0235034.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Dey, M., Paz Castro, R., Jorm, A. F., Marti, L., Schaub, M. P., & Mackinnon, A. (2020). Stigmatizing attitudes of Swiss youth towards peers with mental disorders. PLoS ONE, 15(7), e0235034.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dietrich, S., Beck, M., Bujantugs, B., Kenzine, D., Matschinger, H., & Angermeyer, M. C. (2004). The relationship between public causal beliefs and social distance toward mentally ill people. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 38(5), 348–354; discussion 355–347. Dietrich, S., Beck, M., Bujantugs, B., Kenzine, D., Matschinger, H., & Angermeyer, M. C. (2004). The relationship between public causal beliefs and social distance toward mentally ill people. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 38(5), 348–354; discussion 355–347.
Zurück zum Zitat Furnham, A., Daoud, Y., & Swami, V. (2009). “How to spot a psychopath”. Lay theories of psychopathy. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 44(6), 464–472.PubMedCrossRef Furnham, A., Daoud, Y., & Swami, V. (2009). “How to spot a psychopath”. Lay theories of psychopathy. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 44(6), 464–472.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gaebel, W., Zäske, H., & Baumann, A. E. (2006). The relationship between mental illness severity and stigma. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica Supplement, 429, 41–45.CrossRef Gaebel, W., Zäske, H., & Baumann, A. E. (2006). The relationship between mental illness severity and stigma. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica Supplement, 429, 41–45.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Goldney, R. D., & Fisher, L. J. (2008). Have broad-based community and professional education programs influenced mental health literacy and treatment seeking of those with major depression and suicidal ideation? Suicide and Lifethreatening Behavior, 38(2), 129–142.CrossRef Goldney, R. D., & Fisher, L. J. (2008). Have broad-based community and professional education programs influenced mental health literacy and treatment seeking of those with major depression and suicidal ideation? Suicide and Lifethreatening Behavior, 38(2), 129–142.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Goldney, R. D., Fisher, L. J., Dal Grande, E., & Taylor, A. W. (2005). Changes in mental health literacy about depression: South Australia, 1998 to 2004. Medical Journal of Australia, 183(3), 134–137.CrossRef Goldney, R. D., Fisher, L. J., Dal Grande, E., & Taylor, A. W. (2005). Changes in mental health literacy about depression: South Australia, 1998 to 2004. Medical Journal of Australia, 183(3), 134–137.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hadjimina, E., & Furnham, A. (2017). Influence of age and gender on mental health literacy of anxiety disorders. Psychiatry Research, 251, 8–13.PubMedCrossRef Hadjimina, E., & Furnham, A. (2017). Influence of age and gender on mental health literacy of anxiety disorders. Psychiatry Research, 251, 8–13.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hanisch, S. E., Twomey, C. D., Szeto, A. C., Birner, U. W., Nowak, D., & Sabariego, C. (2016). The effectiveness of interventions targeting the stigma of mental illness at the workplace: a systematic review. BMC Psychiatry, 16, 1.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Hanisch, S. E., Twomey, C. D., Szeto, A. C., Birner, U. W., Nowak, D., & Sabariego, C. (2016). The effectiveness of interventions targeting the stigma of mental illness at the workplace: a systematic review. BMC Psychiatry, 16, 1.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Haslam, D. (2015). Plan for the unpredictable. The Practitioner, 259(1782), 35.PubMed Haslam, D. (2015). Plan for the unpredictable. The Practitioner, 259(1782), 35.PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hair, J. F., Risher, J. J., Sarstedt, M., & Ringle, C. M. (2019). When to use and how to report the results of PLS-SEM. European Business Review, 31(1), 2–24.CrossRef Hair, J. F., Risher, J. J., Sarstedt, M., & Ringle, C. M. (2019). When to use and how to report the results of PLS-SEM. European Business Review, 31(1), 2–24.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Henderson, C., Evans-Lacko, S., & Thornicroft, G. (2013). Mental illness stigma, help seeking, and public health programs. American Journal of Public Health, 103(5), 777–780.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Henderson, C., Evans-Lacko, S., & Thornicroft, G. (2013). Mental illness stigma, help seeking, and public health programs. American Journal of Public Health, 103(5), 777–780.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hinshaw, S. P., & Stier, A. (2008). Stigma as related to mental disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychologyl, 4, 367–393.CrossRef Hinshaw, S. P., & Stier, A. (2008). Stigma as related to mental disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychologyl, 4, 367–393.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., & Mullen, M. R. (2008). Structural equation modelling: Guidelines for determining model fit. Journal of Business Research Methods, 6, 53–60. Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., & Mullen, M. R. (2008). Structural equation modelling: Guidelines for determining model fit. Journal of Business Research Methods, 6, 53–60.
Zurück zum Zitat Jorm, A. F. (2012). Mental health literacy: empowering the community to take action for better mental health. American Psychology, 67(3), 231–243.CrossRef Jorm, A. F. (2012). Mental health literacy: empowering the community to take action for better mental health. American Psychology, 67(3), 231–243.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., & Griffiths, K. M. (2006). The public’s ability to recognize mental disorders and their beliefs about treatment: Changes in Australia over 8 years. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40(1), 36–41.CrossRef Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., & Griffiths, K. M. (2006). The public’s ability to recognize mental disorders and their beliefs about treatment: Changes in Australia over 8 years. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 40(1), 36–41.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E., Jacomb, P. A., Christensen, H., Rodgers, B., & Pollitt, P. (1997). Public beliefs about causes and risk factors for depression and schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 32(3), 143–148.PubMedCrossRef Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E., Jacomb, P. A., Christensen, H., Rodgers, B., & Pollitt, P. (1997). Public beliefs about causes and risk factors for depression and schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 32(3), 143–148.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kaiser, H. F. (1974). An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika, 39, 31–36.CrossRef Kaiser, H. F. (1974). An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika, 39, 31–36.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kasow, Z. M., & Weisskirch, R. S. (2010). Differences in attributions of mental illness and social distance for portrayals of four mental disorders. Psychological Reports, 107(2), 547–552.PubMedCrossRef Kasow, Z. M., & Weisskirch, R. S. (2010). Differences in attributions of mental illness and social distance for portrayals of four mental disorders. Psychological Reports, 107(2), 547–552.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practices of structural equation modelling (3rd ed.). . The Guilford Press. Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practices of structural equation modelling (3rd ed.). . The Guilford Press.
Zurück zum Zitat Kvaale, E. P., Gottdiener, W. H., & Haslam, N. (2013). Biogenetic explanations and stigma: A meta-analytic review of associations among laypeople. Social Science & Medicine, 96, 95–103.CrossRef Kvaale, E. P., Gottdiener, W. H., & Haslam, N. (2013). Biogenetic explanations and stigma: A meta-analytic review of associations among laypeople. Social Science & Medicine, 96, 95–103.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lanfredi, M., Macis, A., Ferrari, C., Rillosi, L., Ughi, E. C., Fanetti, A., et al. (2019). Effects of education and social contact on mental health-related stigma among high-school students. Psychiatry Research, 281, 112581.PubMedCrossRef Lanfredi, M., Macis, A., Ferrari, C., Rillosi, L., Ughi, E. C., Fanetti, A., et al. (2019). Effects of education and social contact on mental health-related stigma among high-school students. Psychiatry Research, 281, 112581.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Larkings, J. S., & Brown, P. M. (2018). Do biogenetic causal beliefs reduce mental illness stigma in people with mental illness and in mental health professionals? A systematic review. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 27(3), 928–941.PubMedCrossRef Larkings, J. S., & Brown, P. M. (2018). Do biogenetic causal beliefs reduce mental illness stigma in people with mental illness and in mental health professionals? A systematic review. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 27(3), 928–941.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lebowitz, M. S. (2019). The implications of genetic and other biological explanations for thinking about mental disorders. Hastings Center Reports, 49(Suppl 1), 82–87.CrossRef Lebowitz, M. S. (2019). The implications of genetic and other biological explanations for thinking about mental disorders. Hastings Center Reports, 49(Suppl 1), 82–87.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lebowitz, M. S., & Appelbaum, P. S. (2017). Beneficial and detrimental effects of genetic explanations for addiction. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 63(8), 717–723.CrossRef Lebowitz, M. S., & Appelbaum, P. S. (2017). Beneficial and detrimental effects of genetic explanations for addiction. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 63(8), 717–723.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Link, B. G., Cullen, F. T., Frank, J., & Wozniak, J. F. (1987). The social rejection of former mental patients: understanding why labels matter. American Journal of Sociology, 92, 1461–1500.CrossRef Link, B. G., Cullen, F. T., Frank, J., & Wozniak, J. F. (1987). The social rejection of former mental patients: understanding why labels matter. American Journal of Sociology, 92, 1461–1500.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Link, B. G., Yang, L. H., Phelan, J. C., & Collins, P. Y. (2004). Measuring mental illness stigma. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30(3), 511–541.PubMedCrossRef Link, B. G., Yang, L. H., Phelan, J. C., & Collins, P. Y. (2004). Measuring mental illness stigma. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30(3), 511–541.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Longdon, E., & Read, J. (2017). “People with problems, not patients with illnesses”: Using psychosocial frameworks to reduce the stigma of psychosis. Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences, 54(1), 24–28. Longdon, E., & Read, J. (2017). “People with problems, not patients with illnesses”: Using psychosocial frameworks to reduce the stigma of psychosis. Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences, 54(1), 24–28.
Zurück zum Zitat Mann, C. E., & Himelein, M. J. (2004). Factors associated with stigmatization of persons with mental illness. Psychiatric Services, 55(2), 185–187.PubMedCrossRef Mann, C. E., & Himelein, M. J. (2004). Factors associated with stigmatization of persons with mental illness. Psychiatric Services, 55(2), 185–187.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mackenzie, C. S., Heath, P. J., Vogel, D. L., & Chekay, R. (2019). Age differences in public stigma, self-stigma, and attitudes toward seeking help: A moderated mediation model. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 75(12), 2259–2272.PubMedCrossRef Mackenzie, C. S., Heath, P. J., Vogel, D. L., & Chekay, R. (2019). Age differences in public stigma, self-stigma, and attitudes toward seeking help: A moderated mediation model. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 75(12), 2259–2272.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McCann, T. V., Savic, M., Ferguson, N., Cheetham, A., Witt, K., Emond, K., Bosley, E., Smith, K., Roberts, L., & Lubman, D. I. (2018). Recognition of, and attitudes towards, people with depression and psychosis with/without alcohol and other dru. British Medical Journal Open, 8(12), e023860. McCann, T. V., Savic, M., Ferguson, N., Cheetham, A., Witt, K., Emond, K., Bosley, E., Smith, K., Roberts, L., & Lubman, D. I. (2018). Recognition of, and attitudes towards, people with depression and psychosis with/without alcohol and other dru. British Medical Journal Open, 8(12), e023860.
Zurück zum Zitat Morgan, A. J., Ross, A., & Reavley, N. J. (2018). Systematic review and meta-analysis of Mental Health First Aid training: Effects on knowledge, stigma, and helping behaviour. PLoS ONE, 13(5), e0197102.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Morgan, A. J., Ross, A., & Reavley, N. J. (2018). Systematic review and meta-analysis of Mental Health First Aid training: Effects on knowledge, stigma, and helping behaviour. PLoS ONE, 13(5), e0197102.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nersessova, K. S., Jurcik, T., & Hulsey, T. L. (2019). Differences in beliefs and attitudes toward Depression and Schizophrenia in Russia and the United States. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 65(5), 388–398.CrossRef Nersessova, K. S., Jurcik, T., & Hulsey, T. L. (2019). Differences in beliefs and attitudes toward Depression and Schizophrenia in Russia and the United States. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 65(5), 388–398.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Norman, R. M., Sorrentino, R. M., Windell, D., & Manchanda, R. (2008). The role of perceived norms in the stigmatization of mental illness. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 43(11), 851–859.PubMedCrossRef Norman, R. M., Sorrentino, R. M., Windell, D., & Manchanda, R. (2008). The role of perceived norms in the stigmatization of mental illness. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 43(11), 851–859.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Norman, R. M., Windell, D., & Manchanda, R. (2012). Examining differences in the stigma of depression and schizophrenia. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 58(1), 69–78.CrossRef Norman, R. M., Windell, D., & Manchanda, R. (2012). Examining differences in the stigma of depression and schizophrenia. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 58(1), 69–78.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Nyblade, L., Stockton, M. A., Giger, K., Bond, V., Ekstrand, M. L., Lean, R. M., et al. (2019). Stigma in health facilities: Why it matters and how we can change it. BMC Medicine, 17(1), 25.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Nyblade, L., Stockton, M. A., Giger, K., Bond, V., Ekstrand, M. L., Lean, R. M., et al. (2019). Stigma in health facilities: Why it matters and how we can change it. BMC Medicine, 17(1), 25.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pedersen, C. B., Mors, O., Bertelsen, A., Waltoft, B. L., Agerbo, E., McGrath, J. J., Mortensen, P. B., & Eaton, W. W. (2014). A comprehensive nationwide study of the incidence rate and lifetime risk for treated mental disorders. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(5), 573–581.PubMedCrossRef Pedersen, C. B., Mors, O., Bertelsen, A., Waltoft, B. L., Agerbo, E., McGrath, J. J., Mortensen, P. B., & Eaton, W. W. (2014). A comprehensive nationwide study of the incidence rate and lifetime risk for treated mental disorders. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(5), 573–581.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pescosolido, B. A. (2013). The public stigma of mental illness: what do we think; what do we know; what can we prove? Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 54(1), 1–21.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Pescosolido, B. A. (2013). The public stigma of mental illness: what do we think; what do we know; what can we prove? Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 54(1), 1–21.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pescosolido, B. A., Martin, J. K., Long, J. S., Medina, T. R., Phelan, J. C., & Link, B. G. (2010). “A disease like any other”? A decade of change in public reactions to schizophrenia, depression, and alcohol dependence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(11), 1321–1330.CrossRef Pescosolido, B. A., Martin, J. K., Long, J. S., Medina, T. R., Phelan, J. C., & Link, B. G. (2010). “A disease like any other”? A decade of change in public reactions to schizophrenia, depression, and alcohol dependence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(11), 1321–1330.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Raykov, T. (1997). Estimation of Composite Reliability for Congeneric Measures. Applied Psychological Measurement, 21, 173–184.CrossRef Raykov, T. (1997). Estimation of Composite Reliability for Congeneric Measures. Applied Psychological Measurement, 21, 173–184.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Read, J. (2007). Why promoting biological ideology increases prejudice against people labelled ‘“schizophrenic.”’ Australian Psychologist, 42, 118–128.CrossRef Read, J. (2007). Why promoting biological ideology increases prejudice against people labelled ‘“schizophrenic.”’ Australian Psychologist, 42, 118–128.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Read, J., & Harré, N. (2001). The role of biological and genetic causal beliefs in the stigmatization of ’mental patients’. Journal of Mental Health, 10, 223–235.CrossRef Read, J., & Harré, N. (2001). The role of biological and genetic causal beliefs in the stigmatization of ’mental patients’. Journal of Mental Health, 10, 223–235.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Read, J., Haslam, N., Sayce, L., & Davies, E. (2006). Prejudice and schizophrenia: A review of the “mental illness is an illness like any other” approach. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 114(5), 303–318.PubMedCrossRef Read, J., Haslam, N., Sayce, L., & Davies, E. (2006). Prejudice and schizophrenia: A review of the “mental illness is an illness like any other” approach. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 114(5), 303–318.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Reavley, N. J., McCann, T. V., Cvetkovski, S., & Jorm, A. F. (2014). A multifaceted intervention to improve mental health literacy in students of a multicampus university: A cluster randomised trial. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49(10), 1655–1666.PubMedCrossRef Reavley, N. J., McCann, T. V., Cvetkovski, S., & Jorm, A. F. (2014). A multifaceted intervention to improve mental health literacy in students of a multicampus university: A cluster randomised trial. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49(10), 1655–1666.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Revelle, W., & Zinbarg, R. (2009). Coefficients alpha, beta, omega, and the glb: Comments on Sijtsma. Psychometrika, 74, 145–154.CrossRef Revelle, W., & Zinbarg, R. (2009). Coefficients alpha, beta, omega, and the glb: Comments on Sijtsma. Psychometrika, 74, 145–154.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rüsch, N., Todd, A. R., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Corrigan, P. W. (2010). Biogenetic models of psychopathology, implicit guilt, and mental illness stigma. Psychiatry Research, 179(3), 328–332.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Rüsch, N., Todd, A. R., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Corrigan, P. W. (2010). Biogenetic models of psychopathology, implicit guilt, and mental illness stigma. Psychiatry Research, 179(3), 328–332.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sartorius, N., & Schulze, H. (2005). Reducing the Stigma of Mental Illness. A Report from a Global Programme of the World Psychiatric Association. Cambridge University Press.CrossRef Sartorius, N., & Schulze, H. (2005). Reducing the Stigma of Mental Illness. A Report from a Global Programme of the World Psychiatric Association. Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schneeweiss, H., & Mathes, H. (1995). Factor analysis and principal components. Journal of Multivariate Analysis, 55, 105–124.CrossRef Schneeweiss, H., & Mathes, H. (1995). Factor analysis and principal components. Journal of Multivariate Analysis, 55, 105–124.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schnyder, N., Michel, C., Panczak, R., Ochsenbein, S., Schimmelmann, B. G., & Schultze-Lutter, F. (2018). The interplay of etiological knowledge and mental illness stigma on healthcare utilisation in the community: A structural equation model. European Psychiatry, 51, 48–56.PubMedCrossRef Schnyder, N., Michel, C., Panczak, R., Ochsenbein, S., Schimmelmann, B. G., & Schultze-Lutter, F. (2018). The interplay of etiological knowledge and mental illness stigma on healthcare utilisation in the community: A structural equation model. European Psychiatry, 51, 48–56.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schnyder, N., Panczak, R., Groth, N., & Schultze-Lutter, F. (2017). Association between mental health-related stigma and active help-seeking: Systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 210(4), 261–268.CrossRef Schnyder, N., Panczak, R., Groth, N., & Schultze-Lutter, F. (2017). Association between mental health-related stigma and active help-seeking: Systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 210(4), 261–268.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schomerus, G., Matschinger, H., & Angermeyer, M. C. (2014). Causal beliefs of the public and social acceptance of persons with mental illness: A comparative analysis of schizophrenia, depression and alcohol dependence. Psychological Medicine, 44(2), 303–314.PubMedCrossRef Schomerus, G., Matschinger, H., & Angermeyer, M. C. (2014). Causal beliefs of the public and social acceptance of persons with mental illness: A comparative analysis of schizophrenia, depression and alcohol dependence. Psychological Medicine, 44(2), 303–314.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schomerus, G., Schwahn, C., Holzinger, A., Corrigan, P. W., Grabe, H. J., Carta, M. G., et al. (2012). Evolution of public attitudes about mental illness: A systematic review and meta-analysis***. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125(6), 440–452.PubMedCrossRef Schomerus, G., Schwahn, C., Holzinger, A., Corrigan, P. W., Grabe, H. J., Carta, M. G., et al. (2012). Evolution of public attitudes about mental illness: A systematic review and meta-analysis***. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125(6), 440–452.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schultze-Lutter, F., Michel, C., Ruhrmann, S., & Schimmelmann, B. G. (2014). Prevalence and clinical significance of DSM-5 –attenuated psychosis syndrome in adolescents and young adults in the general population: The Bern Epidemiological At-Risk (BEAR) Study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40, 1499–1508.PubMedCrossRef Schultze-Lutter, F., Michel, C., Ruhrmann, S., & Schimmelmann, B. G. (2014). Prevalence and clinical significance of DSM-5 –attenuated psychosis syndrome in adolescents and young adults in the general population: The Bern Epidemiological At-Risk (BEAR) Study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40, 1499–1508.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schultze-Lutter, F., Michel, C., Ruhrmann, S., & Schimmelmann, B. G. (2018a). Prevalence and clinical relevance of interview-assessed psychosis-risk symptoms in the young adult community. Psychological Medicine, 48(7), 1167–1178.PubMedCrossRef Schultze-Lutter, F., Michel, C., Ruhrmann, S., & Schimmelmann, B. G. (2018a). Prevalence and clinical relevance of interview-assessed psychosis-risk symptoms in the young adult community. Psychological Medicine, 48(7), 1167–1178.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schultze-Lutter, F., Schmidt, S. J., & Theodoridou, A. (2018b). Psychopathology-a precision tool in need of re-sharpening. Front Psychiatry, 9, 446.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Schultze-Lutter, F., Schmidt, S. J., & Theodoridou, A. (2018b). Psychopathology-a precision tool in need of re-sharpening. Front Psychiatry, 9, 446.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Schulze, B. (2007). Stigma and mental health professionals: a review of the evidence on an intricate relationship. International Review of Psychiatry 19(2), 137–155.PubMedCrossRef Schulze, B. (2007). Stigma and mental health professionals: a review of the evidence on an intricate relationship. International Review of Psychiatry 19(2), 137–155.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sijtsma, K. (2009). On the use, the misuse, and the very limited usefulness of Cronbach’s alpha. Psychometrika, 74(1), 107.PubMedCrossRef Sijtsma, K. (2009). On the use, the misuse, and the very limited usefulness of Cronbach’s alpha. Psychometrika, 74(1), 107.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Sijtsma, K., & van der Ark, L. A. (2015). Conceptions of reliability revisited and practical recommendations. Nursing Research, 64(2), 128–136.PubMedCrossRef Sijtsma, K., & van der Ark, L. A. (2015). Conceptions of reliability revisited and practical recommendations. Nursing Research, 64(2), 128–136.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Speerforck, S., Schomerus, G., Matschinger, H., & Angermeyer, M. C. (2017). Treatment recommendations for schizophrenia, major depression and alcohol dependence and stigmatizing attitudes of the public: Results from a German population survey. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 267(4), 341–350.PubMedCrossRef Speerforck, S., Schomerus, G., Matschinger, H., & Angermeyer, M. C. (2017). Treatment recommendations for schizophrenia, major depression and alcohol dependence and stigmatizing attitudes of the public: Results from a German population survey. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 267(4), 341–350.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Speerforck, S., Schomerus, G., Pruess, S., & Angermeyer, M. C. (2014). Different biogenetic causal explanations and attitudes towards persons with major depression, schizophrenia and alcohol dependence: Is the concept of a chemical imbalance beneficial? Journal of Affective Disorders, 168, 224–228.PubMedCrossRef Speerforck, S., Schomerus, G., Pruess, S., & Angermeyer, M. C. (2014). Different biogenetic causal explanations and attitudes towards persons with major depression, schizophrenia and alcohol dependence: Is the concept of a chemical imbalance beneficial? Journal of Affective Disorders, 168, 224–228.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Svensson, B., & Hansson, L. (2016). How mental health literacy and experience of mental illness relate to stigmatizing attitudes and social distance towards people with depression or psychosis: A cross-sectional study. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 70(4), 309–313.PubMedCrossRef Svensson, B., & Hansson, L. (2016). How mental health literacy and experience of mental illness relate to stigmatizing attitudes and social distance towards people with depression or psychosis: A cross-sectional study. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 70(4), 309–313.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Trani, J. F., Ballard, E., & Peña, J. B. (2016). Stigma of persons with disabilities in Afghanistan: Examining the pathways from stereotyping to mental distress. Social Science & Medicine, 153, 258–265.CrossRef Trani, J. F., Ballard, E., & Peña, J. B. (2016). Stigma of persons with disabilities in Afghanistan: Examining the pathways from stereotyping to mental distress. Social Science & Medicine, 153, 258–265.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Tsang, W. H., Tam, K.C. P., Chan, F., et al. (2003). Stigmatizing attitudes towards individuals with mental illness in Hong Kong: Implications for their recovery. Journal of Community Psychology, 31, 383–396.CrossRef Tsang, W. H., Tam, K.C. P., Chan, F., et al. (2003). Stigmatizing attitudes towards individuals with mental illness in Hong Kong: Implications for their recovery. Journal of Community Psychology, 31, 383–396.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Van Brakel, W. H. (2006). Measuring health-related stigma—a literature review. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 3, 307–334.CrossRef Van Brakel, W. H. (2006). Measuring health-related stigma—a literature review. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 3, 307–334.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Von Lersner, U., Gerb, J., Hizli, S., Waldhuber, D., Wallerand, A. F., Bajbouj, M., et al. (2019). Stigma of mental illness in germans and turkish immigrants in Germany: The effect of causal beliefs. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 46.CrossRef Von Lersner, U., Gerb, J., Hizli, S., Waldhuber, D., Wallerand, A. F., Bajbouj, M., et al. (2019). Stigma of mental illness in germans and turkish immigrants in Germany: The effect of causal beliefs. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 46.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wahl, O. F. (1999). Mental health consumers’ experience of stigma. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25(3), 467–478.PubMedCrossRef Wahl, O. F. (1999). Mental health consumers’ experience of stigma. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25(3), 467–478.PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wang, P. S., Angermeyer, M., Borges, G., Bruffaerts, R., Tat Chiu, W., Girolamo, D. E., G., , et al. (2007). Delay and failure in treatment seeking after first onset of mental disorders in the World Health Organization’s World Mental Health Survey Initiative. World Psychiatry, 6(3), 177–185.PubMedPubMedCentral Wang, P. S., Angermeyer, M., Borges, G., Bruffaerts, R., Tat Chiu, W., Girolamo, D. E., G., , et al. (2007). Delay and failure in treatment seeking after first onset of mental disorders in the World Health Organization’s World Mental Health Survey Initiative. World Psychiatry, 6(3), 177–185.PubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
The Important Role of Stereotypes in the relation between Mental Health Literacy and Stigmatization of Depression and Psychosis in the Community
verfasst von
Carolin M. Doll
Chantal Michel
Linda T. Betz
Benno G. Schimmelmann
Frauke Schultze-Lutter
Publikationsdatum
26.05.2021
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Community Mental Health Journal / Ausgabe 3/2022
Print ISSN: 0010-3853
Elektronische ISSN: 1573-2789
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10597-021-00842-5

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2022

Community Mental Health Journal 3/2022 Zur Ausgabe

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.