Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 6/2018

Open Access 04.01.2018 | Invited Reviews

Vantage sensitivity: a framework for individual differences in response to psychological intervention

verfasst von: Bernadette de Villiers, Francesca Lionetti, Michael Pluess

Erschienen in: Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology | Ausgabe 6/2018

Abstract

Purpose

People differ significantly in their response to psychological intervention, with some benefitting more from treatment than others. According to the recently proposed theoretical framework of vantage sensitivity, some of this variability may be due to individual differences in environmental sensitivity, the inherent ability to register, and process external stimuli. In this paper, we apply the vantage sensitivity framework to the field of psychiatry and clinical psychology, proposing that some people are more responsive to the positive effects of psychological intervention due to heightened sensitivity.

Methods

After presenting theoretical frameworks related to environmental sensitivity, we review a selection of recent studies reporting individual differences in the positive response to psychological intervention.

Results

A growing number of studies report that some people benefit more from psychological intervention than others as a function of genetic, physiological, and psychological characteristics. These studies support the vantage sensitivity proposition that treatment response is influenced by factors associated with heightened sensitivity to environmental influences. More recently, studies have also shown that sensitivity can be measured with a short questionnaire which appears to predict the response to psychological intervention.

Conclusions

Vantage sensitivity is a framework with significant relevance for our understanding of widely observed heterogeneity in treatment response. It suggests that variability in response to treatment is partly influenced by people’s differing capacity for environmental sensitivity, which can be measured with a short questionnaire. Application of the vantage sensitivity framework to psychiatry and clinical psychology may improve our knowledge regarding when, how, and for whom interventions work.

Introduction

Extensive empirical evidence demonstrates that psychological intervention is an effective way to treat mental health problems [16]. The average beneficial effect of psychological treatment is widely accepted as substantial [79] and long lasting [1, 10]. However, while many people benefit from psychological therapy, between 15–45% experience no clinically significant improvement of symptoms [1116]. This variation in treatment response is a consistent finding across all kinds of treated mental health conditions and associated therapeutic interventions. Factors well known to influence treatment response include age and sex, severity, chronicity and comorbidity of symptoms, clinician and treatment context factors, as well as social support [1720], to mention some of the most important identified moderators of treatment response. Over the last decade, there has been pronounced scrutiny of whether the effectiveness of interventions depends also upon inherent characteristics of the individual, such as genes and personality traits. Recent psychological theories, such as vantage sensitivity, suggest that people may vary in how sensitive they are to supportive environmental influences and that this sensitivity affects their likelihood to experience the beneficial effects of psychological therapy. Hence, according to the notion of vantage sensitivity, differences in treatment outcomes may emerge—in addition to other established moderating factors—as a function of differing levels of environmental sensitivity. If such sensitivity can be measured in advance, treatment response which has direct implications for future practice may be predicted more adequately.
In this paper, we apply the concept of vantage sensitivity, a theory of how individuals respond differently to positive experiences, to the field of psychiatry and clinical psychology. We provide an up-to-date review of a selection of recent studies providing empirical evidence that genetic, physiological and psychological markers of sensitivity moderate the positive effects of psychological intervention. We then discuss the central mechanism which is hypothesized to underlie vantage sensitivity and propose existing questionnaires that can be used to measure higher-order sensitivity as a trait, before examining how increasing and converging evidence of vantage sensitivity factors may directly inform therapeutic practice.

Theories of individual differences in environmental sensitivity

The earliest systematic framework for the description of individual differences in response to environmental exposure was the diathesis–stress model. As one of the key concepts in psychopathology [21, 22], the diathesis-stress model posits that psychological problems result from the interaction between two sets of factors: (a) an individual’s inherent propensity for vulnerability, and (b) some sort of external life stressor. The model proposes that some individuals may be predisposed toward vulnerability, while others have naturally greater funds of resilience corresponding to fewer or less severe vulnerability factors, or more endogenous (e.g., genetic) resilience (see Fig. 1a).
Gene–environment interaction research, however, provides compelling evidence that the features by which we are composed do not align neatly on separate pathways to either vulnerability or resilience. Several genetic variants associated with heightened risk for maladaptive development, for example, are highly prevalent in the general population, yet in most cases do not result in maladaptive outcomes [23]. These would not be conserved should their operative properties be purely dysfunctional. Correspondingly, genetic variants found to associate with negative outcomes have been found to also associate with positive outcomes, in other circumstances [24, 25]. In more detail, numerous findings in developmental research demonstrate that the same traits that are associated with an increased risk for problematic development in negative environmental conditions also predict a higher probability to benefit from positive exposures [2629], illustrating that vulnerability for risk and the potential for enhanced development are not mutually exclusive.
These findings converged to form an important idea, expressed in the differential susceptibility [25, 30] hypothesis: certain genetic, neurobiological, or other individual features may operate as levers, associating with different developmental outcomes depending on the quality of the environment. The theory defined by the differential susceptibility hypothesis is that some people may be more sensitive neurologically and physiologically, causing them to perceive, process, and subsequently react to experiential stimuli more strongly, both negatively and positively. This provides an explanation for the conservation of genes associated with maladaptive outcomes when experiencing adversity: the same genes may also associate with improved reproductive fitness, with the liability conferred under some conditions outweighed by advantages conferred under other conditions [3134].
Biological sensitivity to context [35, 36] and the personality concept of sensory processing sensitivity [37] are two further important models of individual differences in sensitivity. Biological sensitivity to context demonstrates psychobiological reactivity to stress (e.g., high cortisol reactivity) with the potential to produce negative effects in adverse conditions and positive effects in supportive contexts [38, 39], whereas sensory processing sensitivity, developed by Aron (1997), is proposed as a measurable personality dimension, in which heightened sensitivity to external stimuli is attuned to greater depth of cognitive processing and high emotional reactivity [40].
Diathesis–stress, differential susceptibility, vantage sensitivity, biological sensitivity to context, and sensory processing sensitivity can all be contained under the broader conceptual meta-framework of environmental sensitivity [41]. As an overarching theoretical framework, environmental sensitivity proposes fundamental and consequential differences in how sensitively and acutely individuals perceive and absorb environmental stimuli, and that these differences—which lead some people to be more environmentally sensitive than others—are genetically influenced products of evolutionary adaptation. Importantly, the meta-framework of environmental sensitivity houses models that describe sensitivity to negative environmental exposures only, sensitivity to positive ones, as well as sensitivity to both. Although some research aims at determining statistically which of the different sensitivity models fit results of a study best [42], support for one versus other models may often depend on methodological differences between studies (e.g., type and range of outcome measures ). Hence, it may be more useful to adopt the broader view of environmental sensitivity when considering mechanisms of individual differences in response to environmental influences—whether they are negative or positive—but refer to particular models when formulating specific hypotheses or describing results. For example, in the case of individual differences in response to psychological treatment as a function of sensitivity, vantage sensitivity is the appropriate sensitivity model given that the environmental influence (i.e., psychological intervention) captures the presence/absence of a positive exposure but not the presence/absence of a negative one (which would be required to test a diathesis–stress or differential susceptibility hypothesis).

The vantage sensitivity framework

A principal contribution of the distinct but related sensitivity models is the observation that variability in sensitivity is not confined to adversity, but operates across the full range of environmental quality. More sensitive individuals are not only more vulnerable to adversity but likely also more sensitive to the positive effect of positive experiences. Vantage sensitivity [43, 44] is a relatively new concept referring to the proclivity of some people to benefit disproportionately from positive features of environmental experience, just as vulnerability depicts a propensity to succumb to the negative effects of adversity in the diathesis–stress framework. Vantage sensitive individuals, those more responsive to and positively influenced by features of the environment that promote well-being, are presumed to possess inherent genetic, physiological and psychological traits that subserve responsivity to positive experience. Vantage-resistant individuals, on the other hand, are presumed to possess inherent characteristics that confine their likelihood of responding and altering positively to the same experiences (see Fig. 1b).
While accordant and closely related to differential susceptibility, and other models of sensitivity, vantage sensitivity is not synonymous to differential susceptibility. Given that differential susceptibility refers to individual differences in response to both adverse and positive contexts, the studied features of environmental quality must, therefore, necessarily include both negative and positive aspects to be consistent with differential susceptibility. While there is evidence that individuals highly sensitive to adverse rearing and environment factors may also be those most responsive to therapy and intervention [45], vantage sensitivity is considering individual differences that pertain only to positive experience, or the absence thereof, without making claims about the potential response to adverse experiences. In other words, vantage sensitivity describes the “bright side” of differential susceptibility, whereas diathesis–stress refers to the “dark side” only.

Empirical evidence for vantage sensitivity

According to existing studies on individual differences in response to various positive experiences, endogenous markers of vantage sensitivity appear to fall into three different categories, as reviewed previously [43]: (a) genetic factors, such as the serotonin transporter-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) [46]; (b) physiological measures, such as cortisol reactivity [47], and (c) psychological traits, such as negative emotionality in infancy [48]. In what follows, we present a selection of new studies most of which have not been included in previous reviews [31, 43] to provide specific empirical evidence for vantage sensitivity to psychological intervention for each of the three categories.

Genetic factors

Several gene variants have been found to moderate the impact of psychological intervention in the so-called candidate gene–environment interaction studies [4951]. These studies tend to focus on the moderating effect of single gene variants, such as the serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) [52]. In a recent study, it was investigated whether 5-HTTLPR moderates the positive effects of a home-based intervention program aimed at promoting secure attachment in children. The randomized controlled trial included 279 South African mother–child dyads [53]. According to the results of the study, children carrying the short variant of the 5-HTTLPR were significantly more likely to be securely attached (84%) if included in the treatment condition compared to the control group (58%). In contrast, children with the long variant of the 5-HTTLPR had similar rates of secure attachment whether they were in the treatment or control group (71 and 70%, respectively). Hence, these findings suggest that the short variant of the 5-HTTLPR predicted the positive response to psychological intervention, providing evidence for vantage sensitivity as a function of 5-HTTLPR.
Similar results emerged in a randomized controlled trial aimed at investigating genetic moderation of a comprehensive intervention program focused on the prevention of externalizing behavioral problems in high-risk children [54]. The authors investigated whether several variants of the glucocorticoid receptor gene (NR3C1) influenced the response to intervention in two subsamples of European-American (N = 242) and African-American (N = 248) participants, enrolled at age 5 and tested for outcomes at age 25 years. According to the study, European-American individuals carrying the A allele of the single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) rs10482672 had the lowest prevalence of externalizing disorders at age 25 when they belonged to the intervention group (18%), whereas those with the same gene variant in the control group had a prevalence of 75%. Conversely, for those homozygous for the G allele, no difference in externalizing symptom rates emerged between treatment (56%) and control (57%) groups. Hence, findings suggest that the A-allele reflects higher vantage sensitivity to the positive effects of psychological treatment.
Over the last years, researchers have started to combine multiple gene variants into polygenic scores based on the understanding that common and complex traits are the function of many gene variants rather than single ones [55, 56]. For example, Chhangur et al. [57] tested whether a polygenic score based on five dopaminergic gene variants (DRD4, DRD2, DAT1, MAOA, and COMT) moderated the efficacy of a parenting program applying a randomized controlled trial design involving 341 families with children characterized by elevated behavior problems. Consistent with the notion of vantage sensitivity, boys carrying 3–5 sensitivity gene variants showed the most pronounced reduction in externalizing behavioral problems both directly after the intervention and at an 8-month follow-up assessment compared to their genetically less-sensitive peers with 0–2 sensitivity variants. Importantly, at the pre-intervention assessment, children carrying more sensitivity genes did not differ from those with fewer sensitivity genes, suggesting that the polygenic score of sensitivity was unrelated to initial behavior problems.
Overcoming the limitations of candidate gene studies [58], Keers and colleagues [55] recently explored the moderating role of a genome-wide polygenic score, based on about 20,000 different gene variants, regarding the efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) in the treatment of 973 children with anxiety problems. Findings suggested that children with a higher genetic sensitivity score responded better to higher quality individual CBT (remission rate 70.9%) than to lower quality group or brief parent-led CBT (remission rates 55.5 and 41.6%, respectively). Conversely, no relevant differences among treatment types were identified for their genetically less sensitive peers, suggesting that more sensitive children are particularly responsive to the quality of treatment they receive.

Physiological factors

Several physiological characteristics have been identified as markers of sensitivity, including measures of the autonomic nervous system and hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis [36, 59, 60]. However, not many studies have yet investigated the role of these physiological characteristics from a perspective of vantage sensitivity. But evidence that cortisol reactivity functions as a marker of vantage sensitivity have been provided in a recent study on the efficacy of exposure-based psychotherapy, involving 26 female adults with panic disorder and agoraphobia [61]. Results showed that higher cortisol levels during exposure and a higher cortisol awaking response predicted faster and greater recovery. According to visual exploration of the reported interaction effects, there were no significant differences in any of the outcomes related to cortisol measures at the beginning of the intervention but over time those with higher cortisol showed more improvement than those with lower cortisol. Similar findings emerged in a recent study aimed at testing whether cortisol levels predicted the degree of reduction of post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms in a sample of 41 veterans treated with trauma-focused therapy (CBT integrated with eye movement desensitization and processing) [62]. The cortisol awakening response [2] accounted for 10% of the treatment effect, with higher CAR being associated with increased reduction of symptoms after completion of the treatment. However, a recent meta-analysis on basal cortisol as a predictor of psychological therapy response in patients with anxiety disorders [63] failed to find a significant association between cortisol levels and treatment response. This may mean that it is cortisol reactivity rather than basal levels that reflect sensitivity to the environment, a hypothesis which remains to be investigated in greater detail.
Evidence also suggests that measures of the brain, such as structure and function, could reflect individual differences in vantage sensitivity, and therefore, prove useful in the prediction of treatment response. For example, in a study involving 39 patients who underwent CBT for the treatment of social anxiety disorder, greater treatment response was predicted by greater pre-treatment brain activation in response to angry faces [64]. It was specifically regions in the dorsal and ventral occipitotemporal cortex in which initial activation for angry versus neutral faces significantly predicted treatment response. Importantly, these brain activation patterns were unrelated to initial social anxiety at pre-treatment. Further support for functional brain measures as marker of vantage sensitivity is provided in a study featuring a group of 21 patients with a diagnosis of generalized social anxiety disorder who underwent CBT [65]. Before treatment, patients participated in an attentional control task focused on emotion processing while their brain function was measured with an MRI scanner. Analyses revealed that higher activity in the right dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and less activity in the left amygdala during the pre-treatment attentional control task predicted stronger anxiety symptom improvement across the treatment.

Psychological factors

Personality and temperament traits have a long history in research as risk factors for the emergence of mental health problems, such as depression [66]. Over the last years, research has shown that personality and temperament traits can also reflect vantage sensitivity, by moderating intervention effects and predicting treatment response. For example, irritability in new born infants, objectively assessed within 30 days postpartum, has been shown to predict the positive effects of a parenting program in a randomized controlled trial study involving 174 mothers and their children [67]. The study showed that highly irritable children (of mothers with a secure attachment) were significantly more likely to benefit from the parenting intervention than their moderately irritable peers (i.e., 97 versus 57% probability for secure attachment, respectively). Conversely, no difference in attachment security emerged between highly and moderately irritable children in the control condition, suggesting highly irritable infants were more sensitive to the positive effects of the intervention.
Evidence of personality as moderator of intervention effects has also been reported in a recent study on a school-based intervention for children with externalizing behavioral problems [68]. Teacher-reported big five personality traits conscientiousness and extraversion emerged as significant predictors of short- and long-term treatment effects in a sample of 264 children aged 10 years. Children with lower scores of conscientiousness failed to respond to the intervention while children scoring low in extraversion (i.e., more introverted, shy children) benefited most. Interestingly, higher conscientiousness and lower extraversion have both been associated with sensory processing sensitivity [69, 70], suggesting that this more responsive personality profile may capture important components of environmental sensitivity. In a different randomized controlled trial involving 256 adolescents similar findings emerged [71]: higher conscientiousness and agreeableness both significantly predicted the positive response to a multimodal ambulant treatment for severe and persistently antisocial adolescents. However, these moderation effects only emerged at post-treatment and could no longer be observed at the follow-up assessment. Long-term effects of the moderating role of personality on treatment efficacy have been reported in a different randomized controlled trial study involving 159 female adolescents, and aimed at testing the impact of interpersonal psychotherapy for the prevention of excessive weight gain. Three years after the completion of the program, improvements in body mass index were found only among females with high trait anxiety, suggesting that trait anxiety may reflect a degree of vantage sensitivity [72].

Discussion

Mechanism of vantage sensitivity

Our selective review of recent empirical evidence for vantage sensitivity may suggest the involvement of different molecular, neurological, physiological, and psychological mechanisms. However, it is more likely that these different mechanisms all jointly orchestrate aspects of a higher-order mechanism of sensitivity. In other words, these various factors may reflect the same core sensitivity mechanism at different levels of analysis—a hypothesis which remains to be tested. As discussed in more detail elsewhere [34, 43, 73], there are several higher-order processes that involve the various detected individual sensitivity markers and may represent important candidate mechanisms underlying vantage sensitivity (or environmental sensitivity more generally): attentional processes, reward sensitivity, social sensitivity and stress responsivity [see 43]. These are each central nervous system processes, providing substantiation of the centrality of the nervous system in environmental sensitivity [40, 74]. According to this general hypothesis of “Neurosensitivity, both genetic and environmental factors contribute to heightened sensitivity of the central nervous system which manifests itself both physiologically and psychologically [34, 41, 73, 75]. In summary, environmental sensitivity—defined as the ability to perceive and process information about the environment [41, 69]—may be driven primarily by a more sensitive central nervous system on which experiences register more easily and more deeply. Integral to this core mechanism of sensitivity is the quantifiable neurobiological trait of sensory processing sensitivity, held to influence the depth and degree by which sensory stimuli are processed, which we propose as a key candidate for the measurement of vantage sensitivity [40, 76].

Measurement of vantage sensitivity

While all the reviewed genetic, physiological, and psychological traits appear to reflect sensitivity to positive features of the environment, they do not represent a direct measure of the proposed underlying higher-order sensitivity. Hence, they should be considered sensitivity markers—some more proximal than others—and as such may not be ideal or practical for the measurement of sensitivity. However, sensitivity to both negative and positive environmental influences can be measured reliably with the highly sensitive person (HSP) scale in adults [76] and the highly sensitive child (HSC) scale in children [69]. These are validated psychometric self-report questionnaires originally designed to capture sensory processing sensitivity, manifested in higher awareness of subtleties in the environment, heightened processing of sensory input, and the tendency to be more easily overwhelmed by emotionally and sensory stimulating environments, to name just a few of the items. First evidence of vantage sensitivity in relation to sensitivity measured with the HSC scale has been reported in a study examining individual differences in response to a school-based resilience-promoting program aimed at reducing depressive symptoms in adolescents [77]. The intervention proved effective in reducing depression symptoms up to the 6-month follow-up assessment (but was no longer significant at 12 months) [78]. When investigating whether HSC moderated treatment effects, it was found that children scoring low on the HSC scale failed to show any improvement at all (i.e., displaying vantage resistance) while those scoring high in sensitivity showed substantial reductions in depression symptoms all the way through to the 12-month follow-up assessment [77]. In other words, as hypothesized the HSC scale predicted individual differences in vantage sensitivity related to treatment response. These findings have recently been replicated in a large randomized control trial (N = 2024) testing the efficacy of a school-based anti-bullying intervention [79]. Although the intervention significantly decreased victimization and bullying across the whole sample, examination of moderation effects revealed that intervention effects were driven by children characterized by high sensitivity. Children scoring low on HSC, on the other hand, did not benefit from the intervention.

Implications

There is now cogent evidence that environmental sensitivity factors explain individual differences in response to both adverse and supportive experiences [43, 44, 80]. In the context of psychotherapy, the converging evidence that people respond to a greater or lesser degree to enhancing, supportive experiences as a function of endogenous factors, hypothesized to be associated with higher-order sensitivity mechanisms, has important implications for clinical practice. Most importantly, differences in response to psychological treatment should not only be expected but already considered when making decisions about the provision of treatment. Given that sensitivity can be measured as a phenotype, there is a significant potential for the application of existing sensitivity measures such as the HSP [76] and HSC [69] scales as well as for the development of further screening tools for the detection of individuals more or less likely to respond to psychological intervention as a function of inherent sensitivity. The measurement of sensitivity before treatment contributes to a personalized medicine approach [81], allowing the clinician to select the treatment that is most likely to help the patient based on her/his individual degree of sensitivity. To enable such a personalized treatment approach, continued investigation and cataloging of sensitivity markers is crucial to achieve an objective, testable profile of vantage sensitivity and vantage resistance that can be incorporated into diagnostic and clinical practices. However, as environmental sensitivity is likely an outcome of many molecular and neurobiological mechanisms, it may be most promising to focus on measurable higher-order traits rather than the various underlying properties. Several studies using the HSC scale, for example, demonstrate that it may be possible to predict individuals least and most likely to respond to therapeutic intervention without collection of genetic and physiological data [77, 79].
The currently limited ability to accurately predict what effects a psychotherapeutic intervention is likely to have, and for whom, remains a significant challenge. One central factor that contributes to this challenge may be of predominately methodological nature: the focus on main effects when evaluating the efficacy of treatment often means that the consideration of individual differences is neglected. Group means provide no information on how much individuals differ in response to treatment. As a consequence, aggregating treatment outcomes can lead to considerable overestimation (in the case of vantage resistant individuals) and underestimation (in the case of vantage sensitive individual) of the effectiveness of interventions [14, 82]. Failure to investigate degrees of individual difference in response to treatment, and specifically to identify systematic heterogeneity in response to treatment, may explain, at least in part, the stark contrast in the successful development of drugs to treat physical illness and disease compared to the limited progress made in the treatment of psychiatric disorders [13].
The practical benefits of focusing interventions better are clear, and pertain not just to financial costs and provision of service on a more effective basis, but also to the consideration of patients for whom current therapeutic interventions do not work. While individuals most sensitive and responsive to environmental influence may require shorter or lower intensity programs of intervention, vantage-resistant individuals may require interventions of greater duration, intensity, simultaneous application of two or more treatment types—or it may be found that for some people for whom vantage resistance is very pronounced, it is not just a question of intensity and duration of therapeutic intervention(s), but a redefinition of therapeutic strategy. Importantly, being less sensitive to one type of treatment may not necessarily mean lacking sensitivity to all treatments. Future research will have to investigate whether treatment-specific vantage sensitivity factors exist and whether those that are resistant to intervention due to low sensitivity may require more intensive intervention approaches or rather alternative types of treatment (e.g., medication).
Implications of vantage sensitivity go beyond therapeutic intervention. Applications in educational and social care plans are also conceivable. For example, many children and young people in institutional care go through multiple failed residential placements, with the level and type of residential care ‘stepped up’ each time a placement breaks down. Measures to better assess the different levels of care that children require, at the point where children enter care, may have an enormous impact on the lives and developmental trajectories of many children.
There are two reasons children and young people may be a critical focus, should vantage sensitivity be incorporated into diagnostic and clinical practices. First, vantage sensitivity factors may build over time, according to positive exposures. That is, there is some evidence that biomarkers related to increased vantage sensitivity also predict their own increased expression during early developmental periods [83], in contexts of positive exposures, suggesting that individual propensity for vantage sensitivity may spiral upwards over time, subject to conditions. This leads to a second important question: whether vantage sensitivity itself can be directly influenced through intervention. While it is not possible to change genetic structure, it may be possible to increase responsivity to positive exposures, or anatomically affect biological substrates (i.e., brain structure and function), through interventions that specifically target higher-order characteristics of vantage sensitivity.

Future directions

The notion of vantage sensitivity is still relatively new, and much remains to be investigated. Presiding questions include whether vantage sensitivity describes positive response to all kinds of positive experiences and exposures, or whether it is domain specific, with individuals differing in what types of positive exposures they are sensitive to. Whether vantage sensitivity can itself be influenced, and if so, during which the developmental periods have important social and psychological implications. More research on vantage sensitivity is required in relation to psychotherapy to replicate findings, to identify alternative therapeutic approaches for individuals that appear to show vantage resistance to standard treatment, and to develop a more fine-grained assessment of vantage sensitivity factors in the context of psychological therapy. Significant further investigation of specific endogenous factors and mechanistic pathways that predict differential response to interventions is also required. In particular, identifying endophenotypes and examining candidate genetic and neurophysiological markers of higher-order sensitivity traits as well as whether and how these various sensitivity markers are associated with self-reported sensitivity assessed with questionnaires. Furthermore, future research should also investigate whether there are specific vantage sensitivity factors that do not also increase vulnerability to the negative effects of adverse experiences.
It is eminently possible that the measurement of environmental sensitivity may not only contribute to the development of screening tools for tailored psychotherapeutic and other intervention programs, but also be the critical element therein. Measurement of environmental sensitivity is feasible with the HSP and HSC scales, and can be readily introduced into current research and clinical settings. Notwithstanding, while sensory processing sensitivity may capture important characteristics of environmental sensitivity in a robust way, more specific measures of environmental sensitivity are required, and HSP and HSC scales can be refined with more research to better predict treatment response.

Conclusion

Heterogeneity of response to therapeutic interventions is a fact that is widely observable. According to the concept of vantage sensitivity people differ in their sensitivity to positive influences, including psychotherapy, as a function of inherent and relatively stable factors. Empirical studies confirm vantage sensitivity in relation to psychological intervention. Differences in vantage sensitivity, which can be gauged through questionnaires that measure environmental sensitivity, should be considered in clinical practice. Applying the framework of vantage sensitivity to the fields of psychiatric and psychological research may elucidate when, how, and for whom interventions work.

Acknowledgements

This work was conducted with the support of a Marie Sklodowska-Curie individual fellowship from the European Commission awarded to Francesca Lionetti (H2020—MSCA—IF—2015–704283).

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

Neuer Inhalt

Print-Titel

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Weitere Produktempfehlungen anzeigen
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychological A (2013) Recognition of psychotherapy effectiveness. Psychotherapy (Chicago Ill) 50(1):102CrossRef American Psychological A (2013) Recognition of psychotherapy effectiveness. Psychotherapy (Chicago Ill) 50(1):102CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Carr A (2009) The effectiveness of family therapy and systemic interventions for child-focused problems. J Family Ther 31(1):3–45CrossRef Carr A (2009) The effectiveness of family therapy and systemic interventions for child-focused problems. J Family Ther 31(1):3–45CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Cuijpers P et al (2014) Psychological treatment of generalized anxiety disorder: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 34(2):130–140CrossRefPubMed Cuijpers P et al (2014) Psychological treatment of generalized anxiety disorder: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 34(2):130–140CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Cuijpers P et al (2008) Psychotherapy for depression in adults: a meta-analysis of comparative outcome studies. J Consult Clin Psychol 76(6):909–922CrossRefPubMed Cuijpers P et al (2008) Psychotherapy for depression in adults: a meta-analysis of comparative outcome studies. J Consult Clin Psychol 76(6):909–922CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Mayo-Wilson E et al (2014) Psychological and pharmacological interventions for social anxiety disorder in adults: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 1(5):368–376CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Mayo-Wilson E et al (2014) Psychological and pharmacological interventions for social anxiety disorder in adults: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 1(5):368–376CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
6.
Zurück zum Zitat National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health (2010) Depression: the treatment and management of depression in adults (updated edition). British Psychological Society, Leicester National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health (2010) Depression: the treatment and management of depression in adults (updated edition). British Psychological Society, Leicester
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Chorpita BF et al (2011) Evidence-based treatments for children and adolescents: an updated review of indicators of efficacy and effectiveness. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 18(2):154–172CrossRef Chorpita BF et al (2011) Evidence-based treatments for children and adolescents: an updated review of indicators of efficacy and effectiveness. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 18(2):154–172CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Glass G, Smith M, Miller T (1980) The benefits of psychotherapy. John Hopkins Un, Baltimore Glass G, Smith M, Miller T (1980) The benefits of psychotherapy. John Hopkins Un, Baltimore
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Wampold BE, Imel ZE (2015) The great psychotherapy debate: the evidence for what makes psychotherapy work. Routledge, New York, NYCrossRef Wampold BE, Imel ZE (2015) The great psychotherapy debate: the evidence for what makes psychotherapy work. Routledge, New York, NYCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Grant PM et al (2012) Randomized trial to evaluate the efficacy of cognitive therapy for low-functioning patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69(2):121–127CrossRefPubMed Grant PM et al (2012) Randomized trial to evaluate the efficacy of cognitive therapy for low-functioning patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69(2):121–127CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Fisher PL, Wells A (2005) How effective are cognitive and behavioral treatments for obsessive–compulsive disorder? A clinical significance analysis. Behav Res Ther 43(12):1543–1558CrossRefPubMed Fisher PL, Wells A (2005) How effective are cognitive and behavioral treatments for obsessive–compulsive disorder? A clinical significance analysis. Behav Res Ther 43(12):1543–1558CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Hansen NB, Lambert MJ, Forman EM (2002) The psychotherapy dose-response effect and its implications for treatment delivery services. Clin Psycho Sci Pract 9(3):329–343CrossRef Hansen NB, Lambert MJ, Forman EM (2002) The psychotherapy dose-response effect and its implications for treatment delivery services. Clin Psycho Sci Pract 9(3):329–343CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Jacobson NS, Follette WC, Revenstorf D (1984) Psychotherapy outcome research: methods for reporting variability and evaluating clinical significance. Behav Ther 15(4):336–352CrossRef Jacobson NS, Follette WC, Revenstorf D (1984) Psychotherapy outcome research: methods for reporting variability and evaluating clinical significance. Behav Ther 15(4):336–352CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Lambert MJ, Hansen NB, Bauer S (2008) Assessing the clinical significance of outcome results. In: Nezu AM, Nezu CM (eds) Evidence-based outcome research: a practical guide to conducting randomized controlled trials for psychosocial interventions. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 359–378 Lambert MJ, Hansen NB, Bauer S (2008) Assessing the clinical significance of outcome results. In: Nezu AM, Nezu CM (eds) Evidence-based outcome research: a practical guide to conducting randomized controlled trials for psychosocial interventions. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 359–378
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Loerinc AG et al (2015) Response rates for CBT for anxiety disorders: need for standardized criteria. Clin Psycho Rev 42:72–82CrossRef Loerinc AG et al (2015) Response rates for CBT for anxiety disorders: need for standardized criteria. Clin Psycho Rev 42:72–82CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Öst L-G et al (2015) Cognitive behavioral treatments of obsessive–compulsive disorder. A systematic review and meta-analysis of studies published 1993–2014. Clin Psychol Rev 40:156–169CrossRefPubMed Öst L-G et al (2015) Cognitive behavioral treatments of obsessive–compulsive disorder. A systematic review and meta-analysis of studies published 1993–2014. Clin Psychol Rev 40:156–169CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Fox S, Herman M, Beck AT (1984) Severity of psychiatric symptoms as a predictor of benefits from psychotherapy: the veterans administration-Penn study. Am J Psychiatry 141(10):1172–1177CrossRefPubMed Fox S, Herman M, Beck AT (1984) Severity of psychiatric symptoms as a predictor of benefits from psychotherapy: the veterans administration-Penn study. Am J Psychiatry 141(10):1172–1177CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Kraemer HC et al (2002) Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59(10):877–883CrossRefPubMed Kraemer HC et al (2002) Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59(10):877–883CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Lambert MJ, Barley DE (2001) Research summary on the therapeutic relationship and psychotherapy outcome. Psychother Theory Res Pract Train 38(4):357CrossRef Lambert MJ, Barley DE (2001) Research summary on the therapeutic relationship and psychotherapy outcome. Psychother Theory Res Pract Train 38(4):357CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Sotsky SM et al (1991) Patient predictors of response to psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy: findings in the NIMH treatment of depression collaborative research program. Am J Psychiatry 148(8):997–1008CrossRefPubMed Sotsky SM et al (1991) Patient predictors of response to psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy: findings in the NIMH treatment of depression collaborative research program. Am J Psychiatry 148(8):997–1008CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Monroe SM, Simons AD (1991) Diathesis–stress theories in the context of life stress research: implications for the depressive disorders. Psychol Bull 110(3):406CrossRefPubMed Monroe SM, Simons AD (1991) Diathesis–stress theories in the context of life stress research: implications for the depressive disorders. Psychol Bull 110(3):406CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Zuckerman M (1999) Diathesis-stress models. In: Zuckerman M (ed) Vulnerability to psychopathology: a biosocial model. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, pp 3–23CrossRef Zuckerman M (1999) Diathesis-stress models. In: Zuckerman M (ed) Vulnerability to psychopathology: a biosocial model. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, pp 3–23CrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH (2011) Differential susceptibility to rearing environment depending on dopamine-related genes: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Dev Psychopathol 23(1):39–52CrossRefPubMed Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH (2011) Differential susceptibility to rearing environment depending on dopamine-related genes: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Dev Psychopathol 23(1):39–52CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky J, Pluess M (2009) Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol Bull 135(6):885–908CrossRefPubMed Belsky J, Pluess M (2009) Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol Bull 135(6):885–908CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky J (2005) Differential susceptibility to rearing influences: an evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence. In: Ellis B, Bjorklund D (eds) Origins of the social mind: evolutionary psychology and child development. Guilford, New York, pp 139–163 Belsky J (2005) Differential susceptibility to rearing influences: an evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence. In: Ellis B, Bjorklund D (eds) Origins of the social mind: evolutionary psychology and child development. Guilford, New York, pp 139–163
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky JAY, Hsieh K-H, Crnic K (1998) Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys’ externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years: differential susceptibility to rearing experience? Dev Psychopathol 10(2):301–319CrossRefPubMed Belsky JAY, Hsieh K-H, Crnic K (1998) Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys’ externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years: differential susceptibility to rearing experience? Dev Psychopathol 10(2):301–319CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim S, Kochanska G (2012) Child temperament moderates effects of parent–child mutuality on self-regulation: a relationship-based path for emotionally negative infants. Child Dev 83(4):1275–1289CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kim S, Kochanska G (2012) Child temperament moderates effects of parent–child mutuality on self-regulation: a relationship-based path for emotionally negative infants. Child Dev 83(4):1275–1289CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Pluess M, Belsky J (2009) Differential susceptibility to rearing experience: the case of childcare. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 50(4):396–404CrossRefPubMed Pluess M, Belsky J (2009) Differential susceptibility to rearing experience: the case of childcare. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 50(4):396–404CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Ellis BJ et al (2011) Differential susceptibility to the environment: an evolutionary–neurodevelopmental theory. Dev Psychopathol 23(1):7–28CrossRefPubMed Ellis BJ et al (2011) Differential susceptibility to the environment: an evolutionary–neurodevelopmental theory. Dev Psychopathol 23(1):7–28CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Pluess M (2017) Vantage sensitivity: environmental sensitivity to positive experiences as a function of genetic differences. J Pers 85(1):38–50CrossRefPubMed Pluess M (2017) Vantage sensitivity: environmental sensitivity to positive experiences as a function of genetic differences. J Pers 85(1):38–50CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Uher R (2009) The role of genetic variation in the causation of mental illness: an evolution-informed framework. Mol Psychiatry 14(12):1072–1082CrossRefPubMed Uher R (2009) The role of genetic variation in the causation of mental illness: an evolution-informed framework. Mol Psychiatry 14(12):1072–1082CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky J, Pluess M (2013) Beyond risk, resilience, and dysregulation: phenotypic plasticity and human development. Dev Psychopathol 25(4 Pt 2):1243–1261CrossRefPubMed Belsky J, Pluess M (2013) Beyond risk, resilience, and dysregulation: phenotypic plasticity and human development. Dev Psychopathol 25(4 Pt 2):1243–1261CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Boyce WT et al (1995) Psychobiologic reactivity to stress and childhood respiratory illnesses: results of two prospective studies. Psychosom Med 57(5):411–422CrossRefPubMed Boyce WT et al (1995) Psychobiologic reactivity to stress and childhood respiratory illnesses: results of two prospective studies. Psychosom Med 57(5):411–422CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Boyce WT, Ellis BJ (2005) Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary–developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Dev Psychopathol 17(2):271–301CrossRefPubMed Boyce WT, Ellis BJ (2005) Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary–developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Dev Psychopathol 17(2):271–301CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Aron EN, Aron A (1997) Sensory-processing sensitivity and its relation to introversion and emotionality. J Personal Soc Psychol 73(2):345CrossRef Aron EN, Aron A (1997) Sensory-processing sensitivity and its relation to introversion and emotionality. J Personal Soc Psychol 73(2):345CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Ellis BJ, Boyce WT (2008) Biological sensitivity to context. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 17(3):183–187CrossRef Ellis BJ, Boyce WT (2008) Biological sensitivity to context. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 17(3):183–187CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Ellis BJ, Essex MJ, Boyce WT (2005) Biological sensitivity to context: II. Empirical explorations of an evolutionary–developmental theory. Dev Psychopathol 17(2):303–328CrossRefPubMed Ellis BJ, Essex MJ, Boyce WT (2005) Biological sensitivity to context: II. Empirical explorations of an evolutionary–developmental theory. Dev Psychopathol 17(2):303–328CrossRefPubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Aron EN, Aron A, Jagiellowicz J (2012) Sensory processing sensitivity: a review in the light of the evolution of biological responsivity. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 16(3):262–282CrossRef Aron EN, Aron A, Jagiellowicz J (2012) Sensory processing sensitivity: a review in the light of the evolution of biological responsivity. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 16(3):262–282CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Pluess M (2015) Individual differences in environmental sensitivity. Child Dev Perspect 9(3):138–143CrossRef Pluess M (2015) Individual differences in environmental sensitivity. Child Dev Perspect 9(3):138–143CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Roisman GI et al (2012) Distinguishing differential susceptibility from diathesis–stress: recommendations for evaluating interaction effects. Dev Psychopathol 24(2):389–409CrossRefPubMed Roisman GI et al (2012) Distinguishing differential susceptibility from diathesis–stress: recommendations for evaluating interaction effects. Dev Psychopathol 24(2):389–409CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Pluess M, Belsky J (2013) Vantage sensitivity: Individual differences in response to positive experiences. Psychol Bull 139(4):901CrossRefPubMed Pluess M, Belsky J (2013) Vantage sensitivity: Individual differences in response to positive experiences. Psychol Bull 139(4):901CrossRefPubMed
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Pluess M, Belsky J (2015) Vantage sensitivity: genetic susceptibility to effects of positive experiences. In: Pluess M (ed) Genetics of psychological well-being. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 193–210CrossRef Pluess M, Belsky J (2015) Vantage sensitivity: genetic susceptibility to effects of positive experiences. In: Pluess M (ed) Genetics of psychological well-being. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 193–210CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky J (2013) Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Int J Child Care Educ Policy 7(2):15–31CrossRef Belsky J (2013) Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Int J Child Care Educ Policy 7(2):15–31CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Drury SS et al (2012) Genetic sensitivity to the caregiving context: the influence of 5httlpr and BDNF val66met on indiscriminate social behavior. Physiol Behav 106(5):728–735CrossRefPubMed Drury SS et al (2012) Genetic sensitivity to the caregiving context: the influence of 5httlpr and BDNF val66met on indiscriminate social behavior. Physiol Behav 106(5):728–735CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Obradovic J et al (2010) Biological sensitivity to context: the interactive effects of stress reactivity and family adversity on socio-emotional behavior and school readiness. Child Dev 81(1):270–289CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Obradovic J et al (2010) Biological sensitivity to context: the interactive effects of stress reactivity and family adversity on socio-emotional behavior and school readiness. Child Dev 81(1):270–289CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Ramchandani PG, van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ (2010) Differential susceptibility to fathers’ care and involvement: the moderating effect of infant reactivity. Family Sci 1(2):93–101CrossRef Ramchandani PG, van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ (2010) Differential susceptibility to fathers’ care and involvement: the moderating effect of infant reactivity. Family Sci 1(2):93–101CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Kohen R et al (2011) Response to psychosocial treatment in poststroke depression is associated with serotonin transporter polymorphisms. Stroke 42:2068–2070CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kohen R et al (2011) Response to psychosocial treatment in poststroke depression is associated with serotonin transporter polymorphisms. Stroke 42:2068–2070CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Eley TC et al (2012) Therapygenetics: the 5HTTLPR and response to psychological therapy. Mol Psychiatry 17:236–237CrossRefPubMed Eley TC et al (2012) Therapygenetics: the 5HTTLPR and response to psychological therapy. Mol Psychiatry 17:236–237CrossRefPubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ (2015) Genetic differential susceptibility on trial: meta-analytic support from randomized controlled experiments. Dev Psychopathol 27(1):151–162CrossRefPubMed van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ (2015) Genetic differential susceptibility on trial: meta-analytic support from randomized controlled experiments. Dev Psychopathol 27(1):151–162CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Lesch KP et al (1996) Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 274(5292):1527–1531CrossRefPubMed Lesch KP et al (1996) Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 274(5292):1527–1531CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Morgan B et al (2017) Serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) polymorphism and susceptibility to a home-visiting maternal-infant attachment intervention delivered by community health workers in South Africa: reanalysis of a randomized controlled trial. Plos Med 14(2):e1002237CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Morgan B et al (2017) Serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) polymorphism and susceptibility to a home-visiting maternal-infant attachment intervention delivered by community health workers in South Africa: reanalysis of a randomized controlled trial. Plos Med 14(2):e1002237CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Albert D et al (2015) Can genetics predict response to complex behavioral interventions? Evidence from a genetic analysis of the fast track randomized control trial. J Policy Anal Manag 34(3):497–518CrossRef Albert D et al (2015) Can genetics predict response to complex behavioral interventions? Evidence from a genetic analysis of the fast track randomized control trial. J Policy Anal Manag 34(3):497–518CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Keers R et al (2016) A genome-wide test of the differential susceptibility hypothesis reveals a genetic predictor of differential response to psychological treatments for child anxiety disorders. Psychother Psychosom 85(3):146–158CrossRefPubMed Keers R et al (2016) A genome-wide test of the differential susceptibility hypothesis reveals a genetic predictor of differential response to psychological treatments for child anxiety disorders. Psychother Psychosom 85(3):146–158CrossRefPubMed
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Pluess M, Belsky J (2013) Vantage sensitivity: Individual differences in response to positive experiences. Psychol Bull 139(4):901–916CrossRefPubMed Pluess M, Belsky J (2013) Vantage sensitivity: Individual differences in response to positive experiences. Psychol Bull 139(4):901–916CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Chhangur RR et al (2017) Genetic moderation of intervention efficacy: dopaminergic genes, the Incredible Years, and externalizing behavior in children. Child Dev 88(3):796–811CrossRefPubMed Chhangur RR et al (2017) Genetic moderation of intervention efficacy: dopaminergic genes, the Incredible Years, and externalizing behavior in children. Child Dev 88(3):796–811CrossRefPubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Duncan LE, Keller MC (2011) A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry 168(10):1041–1049CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Duncan LE, Keller MC (2011) A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry 168(10):1041–1049CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Sijtsema JJ et al (2013) Family cohesion, prosocial behavior, and aggressive/delinquent behavior in adolescence: moderating effects of biological sensitivity to context. The TRAILS study. Dev Psychopathol 25:699–712CrossRefPubMed Sijtsema JJ et al (2013) Family cohesion, prosocial behavior, and aggressive/delinquent behavior in adolescence: moderating effects of biological sensitivity to context. The TRAILS study. Dev Psychopathol 25:699–712CrossRefPubMed
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Somers JA, Ibrahim MH, Luecken LJ (2017) Biological sensitivity to the effects of childhood family adversity on psychological well-being in young adulthood. Child Maltreat 22(3):236–244CrossRefPubMed Somers JA, Ibrahim MH, Luecken LJ (2017) Biological sensitivity to the effects of childhood family adversity on psychological well-being in young adulthood. Child Maltreat 22(3):236–244CrossRefPubMed
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Meuret AE et al (2015) High cortisol awakening response and cortisol levels moderate exposure-based psychotherapy success. Psychoneuroendocrinolgy 51:331–340CrossRef Meuret AE et al (2015) High cortisol awakening response and cortisol levels moderate exposure-based psychotherapy success. Psychoneuroendocrinolgy 51:331–340CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Rapcencu AE et al (2017) Pre-treatment cortisol awakening response predicts symptom reduction in posttraumatic stress disorder after treatment. Psychoneuroendocrinology 82:1–8CrossRefPubMed Rapcencu AE et al (2017) Pre-treatment cortisol awakening response predicts symptom reduction in posttraumatic stress disorder after treatment. Psychoneuroendocrinology 82:1–8CrossRefPubMed
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Fischer S, Cleare AJ (2017) Cortisol as a predictor of psychological therapy response in anxiety disorders—systematic review and meta-analysis. J Anxiety Disord 47:60–68CrossRefPubMed Fischer S, Cleare AJ (2017) Cortisol as a predictor of psychological therapy response in anxiety disorders—systematic review and meta-analysis. J Anxiety Disord 47:60–68CrossRefPubMed
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Doehrmann O et al (2013) Predicting treatment response in social anxiety disorder from functional magnetic resonance imaging. JAMA 70:87–97 Doehrmann O et al (2013) Predicting treatment response in social anxiety disorder from functional magnetic resonance imaging. JAMA 70:87–97
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Klumpp H et al (2014) Neural response during attentional control and emotion processing predicts improvement after cognitive behavioral therapy in generalized social anxiety disorder. Psychol Med 44:3109–3121CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Klumpp H et al (2014) Neural response during attentional control and emotion processing predicts improvement after cognitive behavioral therapy in generalized social anxiety disorder. Psychol Med 44:3109–3121CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Haukulinen C et al (2015) Personality and depressive symptoms: individual participant meta-analysis of 10 cohort studies. Depress Anxiety 32:461–470CrossRef Haukulinen C et al (2015) Personality and depressive symptoms: individual participant meta-analysis of 10 cohort studies. Depress Anxiety 32:461–470CrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Cassidy J et al (2011) Enhancing infant attachment security: an examination of treatment efficacy and differential susceptibility. Dev Psychopathol 23(1):131–148CrossRefPubMed Cassidy J et al (2011) Enhancing infant attachment security: an examination of treatment efficacy and differential susceptibility. Dev Psychopathol 23(1):131–148CrossRefPubMed
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Stoltz S et al (2013) Child personality as moderator of outcome in a school-based intervention for preventing externalising behaviour. Eur J Pers 27(3):271–279CrossRef Stoltz S et al (2013) Child personality as moderator of outcome in a school-based intervention for preventing externalising behaviour. Eur J Pers 27(3):271–279CrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Lionetti F et al (in press) Dandelions, tulips and orchids: evidence for the existence of low-sensitive, medium-sensitive and high-sensitive individuals. Transl Psychiatry Lionetti F et al (in press) Dandelions, tulips and orchids: evidence for the existence of low-sensitive, medium-sensitive and high-sensitive individuals. Transl Psychiatry
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Asscher J et al (2016) Do personality traits affect responsiveness of juvenile delinquents to treatment? J Res Pers 63:44–50CrossRef Asscher J et al (2016) Do personality traits affect responsiveness of juvenile delinquents to treatment? J Res Pers 63:44–50CrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Tanofsky-Kraff M et al (2017) Excess weight gain prevention in adolescents: three-year outcome following a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 85(3):218–227CrossRefPubMed Tanofsky-Kraff M et al (2017) Excess weight gain prevention in adolescents: three-year outcome following a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 85(3):218–227CrossRefPubMed
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky J, Pluess M (2016) Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. In: Cicchetti D (ed) Developmental psychopathology. Wiley, New York, p 59 Belsky J, Pluess M (2016) Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. In: Cicchetti D (ed) Developmental psychopathology. Wiley, New York, p 59
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Moore SR, Depue RA (2016) Neurobehavioral foundation of environmental reactivity. Psychol Bull 142(2):107–164CrossRefPubMed Moore SR, Depue RA (2016) Neurobehavioral foundation of environmental reactivity. Psychol Bull 142(2):107–164CrossRefPubMed
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Pluess M, Stevens S, Belsky J (2013) Differential susceptibility: developmental and evolutionary mechanisms of gene-environment interactions. In: Legerstee M, Haley DW, Bornstein MH (eds) The infant mind: origins of the social brain. Guilford, New York, pp 77–96 Pluess M, Stevens S, Belsky J (2013) Differential susceptibility: developmental and evolutionary mechanisms of gene-environment interactions. In: Legerstee M, Haley DW, Bornstein MH (eds) The infant mind: origins of the social brain. Guilford, New York, pp 77–96
76.
Zurück zum Zitat Aron EN, Aron A (1997) Sensory-processing sensitivity and its relation to introversion and emotionality. J Pers Soc Psychol 73(2):345–368CrossRefPubMed Aron EN, Aron A (1997) Sensory-processing sensitivity and its relation to introversion and emotionality. J Pers Soc Psychol 73(2):345–368CrossRefPubMed
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Pluess M, Boniwell I (2015) Sensory-Processing Sensitivity predicts treatment response to a school-based depression prevention program: evidence of vantage sensitivity. Personal Individ Differ 82(0):40–45CrossRef Pluess M, Boniwell I (2015) Sensory-Processing Sensitivity predicts treatment response to a school-based depression prevention program: evidence of vantage sensitivity. Personal Individ Differ 82(0):40–45CrossRef
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Pluess M et al (2017) Preliminary evaluation of a school-based resilience-promoting intervention in a high-risk population: application of an exploratory two-cohort treatment/control design. PLoS One 12(5):e0177191CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pluess M et al (2017) Preliminary evaluation of a school-based resilience-promoting intervention in a high-risk population: application of an exploratory two-cohort treatment/control design. PLoS One 12(5):e0177191CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Nocentini A, Menesini E, Pluess M (in revision) The personality trait of environmental sensitivity predicts children’s positive response to school-based anti-bullying intervention. Clin Psychol Sci Nocentini A, Menesini E, Pluess M (in revision) The personality trait of environmental sensitivity predicts children’s positive response to school-based anti-bullying intervention. Clin Psychol Sci
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Pluess M (2017) Vantage sensitivity: environmental sensitivity to positive experiences as a function of genetic differences. J Personal 85(1):38–50CrossRef Pluess M (2017) Vantage sensitivity: environmental sensitivity to positive experiences as a function of genetic differences. J Personal 85(1):38–50CrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Abrahams E, Silver M (2009) The case for personalized medicine. SAGE Publications, Thousand Oaks Abrahams E, Silver M (2009) The case for personalized medicine. SAGE Publications, Thousand Oaks
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH (2015) The hidden efficacy of interventions: Gene × environment experiments from a differential susceptibility perspective. Annu Rev Psychol 66:381–409CrossRefPubMed Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH (2015) The hidden efficacy of interventions: Gene × environment experiments from a differential susceptibility perspective. Annu Rev Psychol 66:381–409CrossRefPubMed
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Kok BE, Fredrickson BL (2010) Upward spirals of the heart: autonomic flexibility, as indexed by vagal tone, reciprocally and prospectively predicts positive emotions and social connectedness. Biol Psychol 85(3):432–436CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kok BE, Fredrickson BL (2010) Upward spirals of the heart: autonomic flexibility, as indexed by vagal tone, reciprocally and prospectively predicts positive emotions and social connectedness. Biol Psychol 85(3):432–436CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Vantage sensitivity: a framework for individual differences in response to psychological intervention
verfasst von
Bernadette de Villiers
Francesca Lionetti
Michael Pluess
Publikationsdatum
04.01.2018
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology / Ausgabe 6/2018
Print ISSN: 0933-7954
Elektronische ISSN: 1433-9285
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-017-1471-0

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 6/2018

Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 6/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.