Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Alzheimer's Research & Therapy 1/2022

Open Access 01.12.2022 | Research

A low-dimensional cognitive-network space in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia

verfasst von: Lorenzo Pini, Siemon C de Lange, Francesca Benedetta Pizzini, Ilaria Boscolo Galazzo, Rosa Manenti, Maria Cotelli, Samantha Galluzzi, Maria Sofia Cotelli, Maurizio Corbetta, Martijn P van den Heuvel, Michela Pievani

Erschienen in: Alzheimer's Research & Therapy | Ausgabe 1/2022

Abstract

Background

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and frontotemporal dementia (FTD) show network dysfunctions linked with cognitive deficits. Within this framework, network abnormalities between AD and FTD show both convergent and divergent patterns. However, these functional patterns are far from being established and their relevance to cognitive processes remains to be elucidated.

Methods

We investigated the relationship between cognition and functional connectivity of major cognitive networks in these diseases. Twenty-three bvFTD (age: 71±10), 22 AD (age: 72±6), and 20 controls (age: 72±6) underwent cognitive evaluation and resting-state functional MRI. Principal component analysis was used to describe cognitive variance across participants. Brain network connectivity was estimated with connectome analysis. Connectivity matrices were created assessing correlations between parcels within each functional network. The following cognitive networks were considered: default mode (DMN), dorsal attention (DAN), ventral attention (VAN), and frontoparietal (FPN) networks. The relationship between cognition and connectivity was assessed using a bootstrapping correlation and interaction analyses.

Results

Three principal cognitive components explained more than 80% of the cognitive variance: the first component (cogPC1) loaded on memory, the second component (cogPC2) loaded on emotion and language, and the third component (cogPC3) loaded on the visuo-spatial and attentional domains. Compared to HC, AD and bvFTD showed impairment in all cogPCs (p<0.002), and bvFTD scored worse than AD in cogPC2 (p=0.031). At the network level, the DMN showed a significant association in the whole group with cogPC1 and cogPC2 and the VAN with cogPC2. By contrast, DAN and FPN showed a divergent pattern between diagnosis and connectivity for cogPC2. We confirmed these results by means of a multivariate analysis (canonical correlation).

Conclusions

A low-dimensional representation can account for a large variance in cognitive scores in the continuum from normal to pathological aging. Moreover, cognitive components showed both convergent and divergent patterns with connectivity across AD and bvFTD. The convergent pattern was observed across the networks primarily involved in these diseases (i.e., the DMN and VAN), while a divergent FC-cognitive pattern was mainly observed between attention/executive networks and the language/emotion cognitive component, suggesting the co-existence of compensatory and detrimental mechanisms underlying these components.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s13195-022-01145-x.
Martijn P van den Heuvel and Michela Pievani shared co-last authorship.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
AD
Alzheimer’s disease
CCA
Canonical correlation analysis
BOLD
Blood oxygen level dependent
bvFTD
Behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia
DAN
Dorsal attention network
DMN
Default mode network
FC
Functional connectivity
FPN
Frontoparietal network
PCA
Principal component analysis
Rs-fMRI
Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging
VAN
Ventral attention network

Background

Neurodegenerative disorders are among the top leading cause of death and disabilities worldwide [1]. Among people aged more than 65 years old, Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common form of neurodegeneration, while frontotemporal dementia (FTD) represents the first cause of cognitive impairment in younger individuals. In AD, memory is typically the earliest sign of cognitive deterioration. FTD serves as an umbrella term for several clinical syndromes, including the behavioral variant FTD (bvFTD), usually characterized by behavioral disturbances in the earliest stages [2, 3]. To date, no cure is available for these diseases. Recent significant advancement in the pharmacological field has been done, although findings are still far from being conclusive [46]. A better understanding of the pathophysiological mechanisms underlying these cognitive/behavioral symptoms might pave the way to novel treatments and rehabilitation options [7].
Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) is widely used to assess the putative functional architecture of the brain at rest. This technique can investigate in vivo brain oscillations in the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal between different brain regions. Brain areas showing temporal BOLD synchronization are assumed to be functionally grouped into neural networks. Functional connectivity (FC) exhibits a low-dimensional spatiotemporal pattern [8, 9]. This functional scaffold might have a representational role for cognitive abilities [10, 11]. The default mode network (DMN) is associated with episodic memory performance and shows a gradual shrinking with aging, in line with the natural decline of memory performance in the elderly [12]. Similarly, a group of “attentional networks” is linked with executive, language, and attentional abilities, that is the frontoparietal (FPN), the dorsal attention (DAN), and the ventral attention (VAN) networks.
In typical AD, breakdown of DMN is linked with core symptoms, i.e., impaired episodic memory [13]. By contrast, bvFTD manifests reduced FC of the VAN (also referred to as salience network) that is linked with clinical severity [14]. Other cognitive functions and networks are involved during disease progression, such as attentional networks/functions in both conditions [15, 16]. However, a simple 1:1 relationship between (lower) FC and (impaired) cognition is too simplistic to explain the complex pattern of cognitive and brain changes. Large-scale networks are closely interconnected and alterations in one network can have effects on other networks and undermine the balance of this functional scaffold. The triple network theory states that aberrant dynamic cross-network interactions of the VAN, FPN, and DMN underlie a wide range of cognitive/behavioral disturbances [17]. This theory posits that VAN integrates external information acting as an interface between DMN and FPN, regulating their competing inter-network activity and promoting appropriate behavioral response. Similarly, the VAN acts as a circuit breaker when attention is reoriented to relevant environmental stimuli, interrupting ongoing activity in the DAN, which in turn shifts attention to the new source of information [18, 19]. These studies suggested a general role in switching between networks supporting cognitive functions, which may explain previous evidence of between-network alterations in neurological disorders. Brain stroke lesions increase connectivity between networks commonly anti-correlated, such as the DMN and the DAN, with detrimental consequences on cognitive abilities [20]. In neurodegenerative disorders, the pivotal study of Zhou et al. [14] reported a divergent connectivity pattern in AD and bvFTD, whereby reduced connectivity of the DMN in AD was accompanied by hyper-connectivity of the salience network, while the opposite was seen in bvFTD. More recently, the same group observed that AD and bvFTD show divergent abnormalities in the topological organization of functional brain networks extending into subcortical and inter-network connections [21]. These studies pointed out a complex pattern of network connectivity alterations within the connectivity gradient. However, the relationship between this divergent functional pattern in AD and bvFTD and cognition is still unclear. A “classical” cognitive-network approach, which considers the relationship between a single test score (or a composite score across apriori defined domains) with FC might mask some latent relationships, considering also that cognitive scores are highly correlated. Here, we aimed at identifying the latent cognitive space in the continuum from normal to pathological aging. A low latent behavioral space was previously reported in stroke patients, supporting the validity of this approach in neurological diseases. These studies showed that three main cognitive components explained the large majority of variance in cognitive performance [22, 23], linked with a specific spatiotemporal trajectory of functional networks [24]. Based on these premises, in the present study, we sought to investigate whether each cognitive “motif” may capture a specific spatiotemporal pattern of neural cortical connectivity across different neurodegenerative diseases. To this aim, we used (i) a principal component analysis (PCA) to identify low-dimensional representations of cognition and (ii) a connectome analysis to assess FC patterns in core cognitive networks. We then assessed convergent and divergent cognitive-connectivity relationships using univariate and multivariate analyses. We hypothesize to find, within a latent low-dimensional space, both shared and divergent cognitive-FC patterns.

Methods

Participants and study design

Patients were enrolled at the IRCCS Istituto Centro San Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli in Brescia (Italy) as part of the NetCogBS project [25] (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier NCT03422250). Patients underwent a clinical, cognitive, and imaging assessment. Cognitive and imaging variables were collected also in a group of age-matched healthy controls (HC). The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki principles and approved by the local ethics committee of the IRCCS Istituto Centro San Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli in Brescia (Italy). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.
We included patients with a clinical diagnosis of AD or bvFTD [3, 26]. Inclusion criteria for patients were (i) age between 50 and 85 years, (ii) ability to provide written informed consent, and (iii) availability of a collateral source. We excluded patients with moderate/severe dementia (Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score < 18), any medical condition that could interfere with assessments, and contraindications for MRI (metal implants, pacemakers, prosthetic heart valves, claustrophobia). Inclusion criteria for HC were a normal neuropsychological performance on the cognitive battery, with no personal history of neurological, psychiatric, or cerebrovascular disorders. Exclusion criteria are reported in Pini et al. [25]. For the present study, we included all the patients with available cognitive assessment and MRI examination performed at the baseline. The whole procedure of analysis is depicted in Fig. 1.

MRI acquisition

Rs-fMRI and structural MRI data were acquired on a 3T Philips Achieva system equipped with an 8-channel head coil (University Hospital of Verona, Italy). The following sequences and parameters were used: 2D gradient echo echo-planar imaging (GRE-EPI) sequence for functional connectivity analysis (time repetition; TR/echo time; TE=3000/30ms; flip angle=80°, resolution=3mm isotropic; 48 axial slices; volumes=200) and 3D structural T1-weighted (TR/TE=8/3.7ms; flip angle=8°; resolution=1mm isotropic; 180 sagittal slices). Four fMRI volumes with reversed phase encoding directions were acquired for distortion correction purposes. Subjects were instructed to lie still in the scanner and to keep eyes closed but not to fall asleep while images were collected.

Imaging processing and computation

The first 5 scans were removed for the stability of the signal. Scans were corrected for distortions using the FMRIB’s Software Library (FSL, fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/) topup tool [27]. Imaging preprocessing was performed according to a previously validated approach used by our group [28]. Specifically, we (i) computed motion parameters through a custom preprocessing script; (ii) computed the affine registration matrix between rs-fMRI and the T1 image [29]; (iii) processed the T1 image using FreeSurfer version 6.0 (surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) to segment gray and white matter, and parcellate the cortex into 114 cortical regions using a subparcellation of the Desikan-Killiany atlas [30]; and (iv) applied the brain parcellation to the rs-fMRI data using the computed affine registration matrix. The BOLD signal was corrected by regressing out effects of motion (six motion parameters) and mean signal in CSF and left-right white matter (from Freesurfer). The signal was additionally band-pass filtered (0.01–0.1 Hz) and scrubbed by removing frames with potential movement artifacts (framewise displacement larger than 0.25 and a DVARS value 1.5×IQR above the third quartile). After this procedure, 1 bvFTD patient was excluded due to excessive motion. Additionally, 1 frame preceding each frame with potential movement artifacts was also removed to accommodate temporal smoothing of the signal. Finally, for each cognitive network from Yeo’s template, we calculated the average connectivity strength, computed as the correlation average of each mean time-course parcel included in the network template according to the following formula:
$$FC\ net=\frac{\sum_{k=1}^m\textrm{corr}(ij)}{m}$$
where corr(ij) represents Pearson’s correlation between each pair of parcels belonging to a specific cognitive network from Yeo’s template (DMN, DAN, FPN, and VAN), and m represents the number of all the pairs belonging to the network.
Finally, hippocampal volume was computed through FreeSurfer. For each subject, left and right hippocampal volumes were first corrected for intracranial volume and then averaged to compute a unique hippocampal metric. This metric was used to investigate the association with cognitive components.

Cognitive and clinical assessments

The neuropsychological evaluation included the following tests: Auditory Verbal Learning Test, immediate and delayed recall [31], Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure recall [32], story recall [33], paired associates learning test (PAL) [34], digit span backward test [35], verbal fluency (phonemic and semantic) tasks [36], Token Test [37], Trail Making Test part A (TMT-A) and part B (TMT-B) [38, 39], Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure copy [32], Reading the Mind in the Eyes [40], and the 60 Ekman faces tests [41]. Patients’ score at each test was z-transformed based on the performance distribution of the whole sample (patients and age-matched HC). z-scores for reaction times (i.e., PAL, TMT-A, and TMT-B) were inverted for congruency with performance scores of the other tests, i.e., higher scores representing better performance. Due to the high proportion of missing value in the TMT-B (50% missing data in AD and 34% in bvFTD), this test was excluded from the PCA analysis, to avoid possible biases. The clinical assessment included the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Scale (global and Sum of Boxes (CDR-SOB) scores) [42], the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) [43], the Frontal Behavior Inventory (only for bvFTD) [44], and the Instrumental Activities of Daily Living scale (IADL) [45]. Whole-population-based cognitive z-scores were then used to compute both PCA and composite scores.

Cognitive component characterization

The subjects × cognitive z-scores matrix was fed into a PCA using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS – Inc., version 23.0. Chicago). We expected the components to be correlated, so an oblique rotation was used, in line with the previous literature [23]. Components had to satisfy two criteria: (i) the eigenvalues had to be > 1; (ii) the percentage of variance accounted for had to be > 5%. We excluded 1 AD and 1 bvFTD patient due to cognitive data missing to avoid possible biases in the PCA computation.
We further compared the cognitive components (referred to as cogPC) with cognitive composite scores. z-scores from each test within a specific domain were averaged to compute 5 different composite scores, according to previously published procedure [25]: memory included the Auditory Verbal Learning Test, immediate and delayed recall, the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure recall, the story recall, the digit span backward test, and the paired associates learning test; language included the verbal fluency (phonemic and semantic) tasks and the Token Test; executive functions included the Trail Making Test part A and part B; visuo-constructional abilities included the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure copy and the clock test; emotion recognition included the Reading the Mind in the Eyes and the 60 Ekman faces tests. We compared each cogPC with the composite scores by means of a linear regression analysis. Different models were computed, each one having cogPC and composite scores as dependent and independent variables, respectively. We further investigated Spearman’s correlation between each cogPC with clinical outcomes (i.e., IADL, NPI, CDR-SOB, and FBI (only in bvFTD) outcomes). Statistical differences among groups in cogPC scores were assessed with the nonparametric Kruskal–Wallis test (AD vs bvFTD vs HC). Finally, to further characterize cogPC scores, we investigated the (Spearman’s) relationship between each component and hippocampal volume.

Relationship between cognitive components and cognitive networks

Baseline sociodemographic and cognitive profile of patients and controls were assessed with the Kruskal–Wallis or chi-squared tests as appropriate. A Mann–Whitney test was performed to investigate FC network differences between each patient group and HC (AD vs HC; bvFTD vs HC). We investigated both convergent and divergent associations between cogPC scores and network FC in the whole cohort. Statistical analyses and figures were done with Python v.3.

Univariate analysis

Convergence between FC and cognition in the whole cohort was investigated by means of a bootstrapping approach for Spearman’s correlations with 5000 samples, aimed at investigating the association between network FC and cognitive components in the whole dataset. Specifically, this analysis was performed between the four cognitive networks from Yeo’s atlas (DMN, FPN, DAN, and VAN) with each cogPC score. Moreover, we implemented a stepwise-removal-of-data analysis to confirm the bootstrapping results (see the supplementary material for the details of this analysis).
Divergent cognitive-connectivity coupling was assessed through a general linear model (GLM), assessing the diagnosis*network interaction for each cogPC. For the interaction analysis, we considered AD, bvFTD, and HC as diagnostic factors. For each analysis, we excluded network data points above or below the 1.5 interquartile range. Finally, the same GLM model was repeated only for the patient cohort (AD and bvFTD).

Multivariate analysis

To confirm the relationship between cognitive scores and cognitive networks, we applied a canonical correlation analysis (CCA). This approach quantifies the multivariate association between patterns of network connectivity measures and cognitive scores, seeking the maximal correlation between linear combinations of variables in two different sets, i.e., FC and cognitive performance. Cognitive networks showing robust convergent univariate associations (i.e., the convergent correlation-wise analysis) were included as the network dataset. In addition, the visual and sensorimotor networks were included as control networks, as we did not expect a significant association within a low-dimensional cognitive space in this cohort, as one would expect for different brain disorders, such as stroke [23, 46]. Before CCA, network FC values were z-scored according to the network distribution values of the whole cohort. The five cognitive z-scored composite scores were included as the cognitive dataset (see the “Cognitive component characterization” section). CCA modes exhibiting a significant correlation between variates from the whole group were compared between groups through analysis of variance (ANOVA), testing both main effects and interactions.

Results

Twenty-two AD and 23 bvFTD patients were included in the study. A sample of 20 age-matched individuals was included as the control group. Patients and HC were comparable for age (p=0.904), education (p=0.105), and gender (p=0.910). As expected, the MMSE scores were significantly lower in patients compared to HC (p<0.001), without differences between AD and bvFTD (p>0.05). Compared to HC, patients showed lower cognitive scores in all the cognitive tests (p<0.002 for all scores). As expected, bvFTD exhibited lower performance compared to AD for the phonemic fluency test, the Reading the Mind in the Eyes, and the 60 Ekman faces tests (Table 1). bvFTD compared to AD showed more severe behavioral disturbances (NPI: p<0.001; FBI: p=0.001) and disease severity (IADL: p=0.012; CDR-SOB: p=0.04). Both bvFTD and AD showed significantly lower hippocampal volumes compared to HC (around 20% of volume reduction for both patient groups; p<0.001).
Table 1
Baseline sociodemographic and cognitive profile of patients and controls
 
HC
n=20
AD
n=22
bvFTD
n=23
P
Age
72 ± 6
72 ± 6
71 ± 10
.904
Sex (% female)
50%
55%
57%
.910
Education
11 ± 5
9 ± 4
9 ± 4
.105
MMSE
29 ± 2
21 ± 2e
22 ± 4e
<.001
Left hippocampus (mm3)
3724 ± 339
2892 ± 433e
2916 ± 684e
<.001
Right hippocampus (mm3)
3794 ± 326
2951 ± 451e
3079 ± 708e
<.001
Cognitive tests
 RAVLT—immediate
46 ± 7 (33-65)
20 ± 7e (6-33)
21 ± 7e (12-39)
< .001
 RAVLT—recall
10 ± 2 (4-14)
1.1 ± 1.5e (0-5)
2.4 ± 2.6e (0-8)
< .001
 Episodic memory
13 ± 3 (9-21)
2.4 ± 2.2e,a (0-10)
3.9 ± 3.8e (0-14)
< .001
 PAL
34 ± 16 (8-64)
166 ± 28e (104-204)
146 ± 54e,a (22-217)
< .001
 ROCF—recall
15 ± 4 (9-26)
2.3 ± 3.2e,b (0-9)
5.2 ± 4.6e (0-17)
< .001
 Backward digit span
4 ± 1 (0-5)
2.6 ± 1.9 (0-5)
1.8 ± 2.0e (0-5)
0.002
 Clock test
12 ± 1 (8-13)
5. ± 3.4e (0-12)
7.4 ± 3.3e (3-12)
< .001
 ROCF—copy
30 ± 4 (22-36)
22 ± 10e,b (0-36)
20 ± 9e (3-32)
< .001
 TMT-A
46 ± 10 (26-68)
120 ± 85e,b (40-328)
134 ± 94e (43-443)
< .001
 TMT-B
129 ± 46 (60-232)
387 ± 194e,c (85-733)
267 ± 127e,d (121-531)
< .001
 Phonemic fluency test
35 ± 8 (23-52)
24 ± 10e (4-40)
13 ± 8e,f (1-28)
< .001
 Semantic fluency test
40 ± 6 (29-51)
19 ± 8e (7-33)
16 ± 8e (6-36)
< .001
 Token test
34 ± 2 (30-37)
28 ± 5e (17-33)
25 ± 6e (12-34)
< .001
 RMET
20 ± 3 (14-26)
16 ± 4e (9-25)
12 ± 4e,f (5-20)
< .001
 EK-60F
47 ± 5 (38-54)
39 ± 8e (23-50)
27 ± 10e,f (10-47)
< .001
Abbreviations: MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning test, PAL paired associates learning test, ROCF Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure, TMT Trail Making Test, RMET Reading the Mind in the Eyes test, EK-60F Ekman 60 Faces Test
aData from 22 subjects
bData from 21 subjects
cData from 11 subjects
dData from 8 subjects
eVariables statistically different from HC
fVariables significantly different between bvFTD and AD

Cognitive components

The PCA analysis revealed 3 main cognitive components in the whole dataset. A first component (cogPC1) accounted for around 63% of the variance across all tests and subjects. A second factor (cogPC2) accounted for around 11% of the variance, while a third factor (cogPC3) accounted for more than 7% of the variance (Fig. 2). In the whole dataset, the first component was significantly associated with hippocampal volume (rho=.649; p<0.001), while the second and third components were not (p>0.080).
The results for the linear regression analysis comparing each cogPC with the composite z-scores are reported in Table 2. In AD and bvFTD, the first component showed a significant association with the memory composite (p<0.001 for both), while the remaining composite scores were not significant (p>0.100 and p>0.20, respectively). The second component in AD showed a strong association with language and emotion recognition composite scores (both p<0.001). This result was echoed in bvFTD (emotion and language p<0.001), with an additional effect for memory (p=0.02). Finally, in AD, the third component showed a significant association with visuo-spatial abilities (p<0.001) and language composite scores (p=.012). In bvFTD, the third component showed a strong association with visuo-spatial abilities and executive composite scores (p<0.001). Based on these results, we interpreted the cogPCs as memory component (cogPC1), emotion-language component (cogPC2), and visuo-spatial attentional component (cogPC3), respectively.
Table 2
Linear regression analysis for composite and component scores from the principal component analysis
Composite scores
Beta
P-value
Beta
P-value
AD
bvFTD
cogPC 1
R2=0.644, p=.05
R2=0.873, p<.001
 Memory
0.955
< 0.001
1.134
< 0.001
 Executive functions
−0.056
0.783
−0.137
0.254
 Visuo-spatial abilities
0.219
0.344
0.026
0.852
 Language
−0.091
0.683
−0.184
0.354
 Emotion recognition
−0.382
0.107
−0.145
0.431
cogPC 2
R2=0.964, p<.001
R2=975, p<.001
 Memory
−0.111
0.095
−0.142
0.019
 Executive functions
−0.039
0.547
−0.034
0.525
 Visuo-spatial abilities
−0.108
0.150
−0.061
0.337
 Language
0.525
< 0.001
0.596
< 0.001
 Emotion recognition
0.738
< 0.001
0.557
< 0.001
cogPC 3
R2=0.893, p<.001
R2=0.904, p<.001
 Memory
0.044
0.688
−0.064
0.555
 Executive functions
0.140
0.216
0.412
0.001
 Visuo-spatial abilities
0.696
< 0.001
0.691
< 0.001
 Language
0.340
0.012
0.049
0.733
 Emotion recognition
−0.169
0.130
−0.120
0.452
All these cognitive components were different among groups. Post hoc analysis showed that cogPC1 and cogPC3 were different in both patient groups compared to HC (p<0.002). By contrast, we reported a gradient in the cogPC2, with HC showing the greatest score, followed by AD and then by bvFTD (p=0.031). The difference between AD and bvFTD was significant (p=0.015) (Fig. 2).
When investigating the relationship between cogPCs with clinical outcomes, the first component showed a strong relationship with IADL, NPI, and CDR-SOB in both patient groups. Lower cognitive scores were positively associated with IADL (indicating greater functional disability) and negatively with NPI and CDR-SOB (indicating greater behavioral disturbances and higher disease severity). Notably, in AD, the other two components were unrelated with clinical outcomes, while in bvFTD, we found an association between the second component with both behavioral disturbances (FBI) and disease severity (CDR-SOB), and the latter with disease severity (CDR-SOB). These associations were positive, indicating a relation between lower scores and greater disabilities (Fig. 2).

Brain functional networks and connectivity-cognitive coupling

As shown in Supplementary Fig. S1, VAN connectivity was lower in bvFTD compared to HC (p=0.01), while connectivity values for DAN, FPN, and DMN were lower in AD compared to HC (p<0.04).
The bootstrapping analysis revealed a significant correlation between the first two cognitive components with the DMN and VAN (Fig. 3). Specifically, we found a significant correlation between DMN and cogPC1 (95% CI 0.05–0.53) and between both DMN and VAN with cogPC2 (95% CI 0.07–0.52 and 0.04–0.51, respectively). Conversely, the correlation between cognitive networks and cogPC3 was not significant. Results are illustrated in Fig. 3. The stepwise-removal analysis echoed these results, confirming a robust and stable correlation between these components and networks (see Supplementary material; Supplementary Fig. S2).
The interaction analysis showed that the lack of a significant relationship between cognition and the DAN/FPN in the whole dataset was due to a divergent coupling effect between groups. Specifically, after the exclusion of two outlier data points (1.5 interquartile network distribution range), the GLM showed a significant diagnosis*cogPC2 for the DAN (z=−2.389; p=0.029) but not for the interaction between diagnosis and cogPC3 (z=−1.152; p=0.249) (Fig. 3). In bvFTD, lower cognitive scores were associated with lower FC, while the opposite was seen in AD. Similarly, for the FPN, after the exclusion of one outlier data point, we reported a significant diagnosis*cogPC2 effect (z=−2.809; p=0.005), but the relationship was reversed: lower scores were linked with lower FC in AD and the opposite in bvFTD. The cogPC3 showed no significant interaction (z=−1.376; p=0.169) (Fig. 3). No evidence of an interaction was found between diagnosis and cogPC1 with both DAN and FPN (p>0.10 for all the analysis). DMN and VAN showed no evidence of divergent patterns with cogPC scores (p>0.30), in line with the convergent analysis. These results were confirmed when considering only patients (AD and bvFTD; Supplementary Fig. S3).
Within-diagnosis network-cognitive interaction effects, that is FPN*DAN interactions separately for AD and bvFTD, confirmed the previous results. We found a divergent within-diagnosis effect between cogPC2 with FPN and DAN in both cohorts (DAN*FPN interaction effect p=0.012 for both patient groups). A trend was reported for the DAN*FPN interaction with cogPC3 (AD: p=0.078; bvFTD: p=0.128) (Supplementary Fig. S4).

Multivariate association between cognitive performance and connectivity

We considered cognitive networks showing a robust univariate association with cognition (i.e., DMN and VAN), as the CCA seeks the maximal correlation between linear combinations of variables in two different sets. We identified 2 pairs of modes that significantly correlated the network variables and cognitive performance (mode 1: r=0.51, p<0.001; mode 2: r=0.48; p<0.001). The first mode mainly loaded on memory, language, and emotion recognition, while the corresponding network mode loaded on the DMN. The second mode mainly loaded on language and emotion recognition on the cognitive side, and on VAN on the corresponding network side (Fig. 4). ANOVA showed a significant difference for both cognitive modes (p<0.001) (Fig. 4). Post hoc Tukey’s test revealed that the first mode was significantly different between HC and both disease groups (AD: p<0.001; bvFTD: p<0.001), while AD and bvFTD showed no significant differences. The second mode was different between groups (p<0.001), mirroring cogPC2 result in bvFTD, as this group showed significant differences compared to both AD (p=0.033) and HC (p<0.001). No differences were reported between AD and HC (p=0.285).
Finally, we tested the GLM interaction model between the second mode (echoing the cogPC2), between groups with DAN and FPN connectivity, after removing outliers according to the 1.5 interquartile range. We confirmed a divergent association between this cognitive mode with DAN (p=0.008), while FPN did not show a significant effect (p=0.258) (Fig. 4).

Discussion

The core results of this study are (a) a low-dimensional cognitive space in the aging and age-related pathology continuum and (b) both divergent and convergent FC patterns linked with this low cognitive space. These findings could shed light into the relationship between cognitive alterations and brain alterations in AD and bvFTD, suggesting possible detrimental and compensatory mechanisms.

Low cognitive dimensional space

In this study, we identified three main cognitive components across normal aging, AD, and bvFTD explaining more than 80% of the variance. The first memory component explained the largest amount of variance in our sample and was significantly related to hippocampal volumes, congruently with the previous literature [47]. This component was also associated with the clinical severity in both dementia groups and may therefore represent a common neuropsychological impairment across AD and bvFTD [48, 49]. The second component was represented by a language-emotion factor that was involved in both AD and bvFTD but was more impaired in the latter group and linked with both clinical severity and behavioral disturbances only in this cohort. This component may therefore capture a neuropsychological feature specific to bvFTD. Emotion and language are indeed two functions highly impacted in this disorder [50, 51], and their inclusion within a common factor is not surprising as language plays a fundamental role in emotion. Previous researches highlighted that access to the meaning of emotional words (i.e., with emotional meaning) is a fundamental component to understand emotional facial expressions [52]. Similarly, language disturbances commonly co-occur with the impairment of the socio-emotional behavior in bvFTD. Severity of language deficits seems to be linked with disease severity and frontotemporal atrophy suggesting a close link between bvFTD, speech and language deficits, and disease’s core neural disruptions [53]. Finally, we identified a third component, mainly loading to visuo-spatial executive tests. This component, although altered in both patient groups, was linked with the clinical severity only in bvFTD, again suggesting that this factor may capture a bvFTD-specific neuropsychological feature. Overall, these findings suggest a low-dimensional cognitive pattern within the aging and age-related pathology continuum, where only three cognitive dimensions explained a large amount of variance of cognitive outcomes. Previous research identified a similar low-dimensional pattern in stroke; despite the great heterogeneity of brain lesions, it has been suggested a low-dimensional pattern of cognitive deficits, involving three main different components [23]. These factors might help to identify a low space for behavioral phenotypes in neurodegeneration, moving beyond the classical “composite” score approach. Indeed, the latter approach is critically dependent upon the apriori definition of the cognitive domains measured by the different tests, thereby neglecting the frequent co-occurrence of deficits within and across domains.

Divergent and convergent relationships between cognition and connectivity

This low-dimensional cognitive manifold was linked with the FC pattern of higher-order cognitive networks. FC of the DMN was positively associated with memory and emotion-language scores in the whole cohort, with no significant interaction between groups. The association between DMN and memory is in line with a vast body of literature [54], while the association between DMN and emotion-language suggests that this network plays an important role for constructing discrete emotional experiences [55]. Additionally, we reported a robust association between the VAN and the emotion-language scores, with no significant interaction between groups, linking the VAN with social functioning [56]. Previous studies highlighted that the VAN can be briefly activated by external stimuli of behavioral relevance [19], suggesting that stimuli encoded in VAN areas are defined also by emotional experience [57]. These results were confirmed by the multivariate analysis, showing a robust linear relationship between DMN with a cognitive mode represented by memory, language, and emotion recognition, and between a cognitive mode mirroring the cogPC2 with VAN connectivity (as shown in Fig. 4). Although DMN and VAN showed a selective vulnerability in AD and bvFTD, respectively, these associations suggest shared multi-dimensional network mechanisms between these disorders, congruent with the network-cognitive relationships observed in physiological conditions. The link between DMN with memory and VAN with emotion-language might be ubiquitous within the aging-pathology continuum, although bvFTD showed higher levels of deficit in both language-emotions and VAN connectivity, suggesting that failure in these networks is associated with the decline of memory and social domains.
Along with these commonalities, we reported divergent connectivity-cognitive couplings, confirmed by both the univariate and the multivariate analysis. For both DAN and FPN, we reported a divergent pattern between the emotion-language component and diagnosis. This effect was confirmed by the analysis performed in the patient cohort, in addition to a significant divergent effect between the DAN and the visuo-spatial component. Overall, this pattern suggests that divergent network-level effects might emerge as a consequence of aberrant connectivity observed in the primary affected networks in these disorders. In the last years, it has been well established the role of DAN as a “network gate” facilitating top-down attention processing by suppressing VAN signals to exclude irrelevant bottom-up information [18, 58, 59]. We speculate that in bvFTD, given normal connectivity of the DAN but reduced connectivity of the VAN, the positive relationship between DAN and non-memory cognitive components may reflect compensatory neural mechanisms for attentional/emotional/language processing typifying this disorder. Indeed, DAN and VAN dynamically interact to control the information to be processed [60, 61] indicating that DAN connectivity in bvFTD might compensate VAN failure. On the other side, AD showed the opposite pattern, i.e., reduced connectivity of the DAN and normal VAN pattern. This may imply in AD a defective role of DAN in facilitating top-down processes inhibiting irrelevant information as well as reduced regulation of attentional networks (i.e., VAN and FPN). Thus, a less flexible dynamic response might result in a less efficient maintenance of the cognitive set. A similar divergent pattern between connectivity, cognition, and diagnosis was observed for the FPN and the language/emotion cognitive component. In this case, the association was reversed. FPN was significantly reduced in AD compared to HC, suggesting that this pattern might highlight residual functionality, that is patients with lower FC have worse cognition, while those with higher cognition show relatively preserved cognitive performance. A coupled activity between DMN and FPN supports cognitive demand for goal-directed task [62]. Moreover, the positive association between these two networks was associated to between-network compensatory mechanisms in mild cognitive impairment patients [63], indicating that FPN connectivity might sustain DMN failure. Again, in bvFTD, this pattern was reversed, suggesting an emerging defective role of FPN over cognitive functions. Overall, these results point to the presence of both common and divergent patterns among AD and bvFTD patients, suggesting that cognitive alterations are distributed among a connectivity dysfunctional gradient, which may reflect reduced variability in network dynamics. FC of distant cognitive networks represents a dynamic process and might influence cognitive demands and neural resources, which may reflect either compensation or network failure.

Limitations and strengths

This study has several limitations. First, our sample size was relatively small, although the clinical and demographical characteristics of patients and controls were well matched. Second, we did not collect amyloid and tau biomarkers; thus, we could not investigate whether FC-cognitive associations were driven by molecular pathology, as suggested by the cascading network failure hypothesis [64]. According to this model, tau-associated local network failure may be followed by a global compensatory phenomenon associated with Aβ [64]. Future studies examining the relationship between molecular pathology and divergent network connectivity will allow for a more nuanced clarification of the relationship between cognition and FC. Moreover, the inclusion of clinical subscales tailored for the different neurodegenerative disorders (e.g., the FTD-CDR for FTD) [65] might help future studies to better unravel the relationship between cognitive components and disease severity. Similarly, the number of tests included for each domain was different, which might result in “noisier” components when fewer tests are included.
Besides these limitations, this study has two main strengths: (1) investigating for the first time the cognitive dimensional space within the aging and age-related pathology continuum through a dimensionality reduction approach and (2) assessing the univariate and multivariate relationships between this new cognitive space and neural networks connectivity. Future studies could further confirm these patterns. The identification of divergent and shared neural mechanisms across neurodegenerative diseases would increase our understanding of network dynamics. Moreover, these findings would be useful to optimize non-invasive electric brain network stimulation intervention [7], improving target selection for stimulation protocols aiming at rehabilitating specific cognitive functions.

Conclusions

In conclusion, a PCA approach revealed a low cognitive dimensional space across aging, AD, and bvFTD. Cognitive deficits in patients are more accurately described by correlated deficit components rather than the collection of individual scores. We identified a few components that were consistent across different cohorts. The associated network coupling of the identified components showed both convergent and divergent patterns, suggesting both possible detrimental and compensatory effects, which might help to drive new effective interventions [7].

Declarations

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki principles and approved by the local ethics committee of the IRCCS Istituto Centro San Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli in Brescia (Italy) (Number 43/2014). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Anhänge
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Feigin VL, et al. Global, regional, and national burden of neurological disorders, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18:459–80.CrossRef Feigin VL, et al. Global, regional, and national burden of neurological disorders, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18:459–80.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Pasquier F. Telling the difference between frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2005;18:628–32.CrossRef Pasquier F. Telling the difference between frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2005;18:628–32.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Rascovsky K, et al. Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2011;134:2456–77.CrossRef Rascovsky K, et al. Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2011;134:2456–77.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Knopman DS, Jones DT, Greicius MD. Failure to demonstrate efficacy of aducanumab: an analysis of the EMERGE and ENGAGE trials as reported by Biogen, December 2019. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;17:696–701.CrossRef Knopman DS, Jones DT, Greicius MD. Failure to demonstrate efficacy of aducanumab: an analysis of the EMERGE and ENGAGE trials as reported by Biogen, December 2019. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;17:696–701.CrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Sevigny J, et al. The antibody aducanumab reduces Aβ plaques in Alzheimer’s disease. Nature. 2016;537:50–6.CrossRef Sevigny J, et al. The antibody aducanumab reduces Aβ plaques in Alzheimer’s disease. Nature. 2016;537:50–6.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Hunt LJ, Harrison KL, Covinsky KE. Instead of wasting money on aducanumab, pay for programs proven to help people living with dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2021;69:3690–2.CrossRef Hunt LJ, Harrison KL, Covinsky KE. Instead of wasting money on aducanumab, pay for programs proven to help people living with dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2021;69:3690–2.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Pievani M, Pini L, Cappa SF, Frisoni GB. Brain networks stimulation in dementia: insights from functional imaging. Curr Opin Neurol. 2016;29:756–62.CrossRef Pievani M, Pini L, Cappa SF, Frisoni GB. Brain networks stimulation in dementia: insights from functional imaging. Curr Opin Neurol. 2016;29:756–62.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Laird AR, et al. Behavioral interpretations of intrinsic connectivity networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23:4022–37.CrossRef Laird AR, et al. Behavioral interpretations of intrinsic connectivity networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23:4022–37.CrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Yeo BT, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:1125–65.CrossRef Yeo BT, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106:1125–65.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Pezzulo G, Zorzi M, Corbetta M. The secret life of predictive brains: what’s spontaneous activity for? Trends Cogn Sci. 2021;25:730–43.CrossRef Pezzulo G, Zorzi M, Corbetta M. The secret life of predictive brains: what’s spontaneous activity for? Trends Cogn Sci. 2021;25:730–43.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim D, Livne T, Metcalf NV, Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Spontaneously emerging patterns in human visual cortex and their functional connectivity are linked to the patterns evoked by visual stimuli. J Neurophysiol. 2020;124:1343–63.CrossRef Kim D, Livne T, Metcalf NV, Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Spontaneously emerging patterns in human visual cortex and their functional connectivity are linked to the patterns evoked by visual stimuli. J Neurophysiol. 2020;124:1343–63.CrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Staffaroni AM, et al. The longitudinal trajectory of default mode network connectivity in healthy older adults varies as a function of age and is associated with changes in episodic memory and processing speed. J Neurosci. 2018;38:2809–17.CrossRef Staffaroni AM, et al. The longitudinal trajectory of default mode network connectivity in healthy older adults varies as a function of age and is associated with changes in episodic memory and processing speed. J Neurosci. 2018;38:2809–17.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Pini L, et al. Breakdown of specific functional brain networks in clinical variants of Alzheimer’s disease. Ageing Res Rev. 2021;72:101482.CrossRef Pini L, et al. Breakdown of specific functional brain networks in clinical variants of Alzheimer’s disease. Ageing Res Rev. 2021;72:101482.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhou J, et al. Divergent network connectivity changes in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2010;133:1352–67.CrossRef Zhou J, et al. Divergent network connectivity changes in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2010;133:1352–67.CrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Agosta F, et al. Resting state fMRI in Alzheimer’s disease: beyond the default mode network. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:1564–78.CrossRef Agosta F, et al. Resting state fMRI in Alzheimer’s disease: beyond the default mode network. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:1564–78.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Filippi M, et al. Functional network connectivity in the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Cortex. 2013;49:2389–401.CrossRef Filippi M, et al. Functional network connectivity in the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Cortex. 2013;49:2389–401.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Menon V. Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:483–506.CrossRef Menon V. Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:483–506.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:201–15.CrossRef Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:201–15.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL. The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron. 2008;58:306–24.CrossRef Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL. The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron. 2008;58:306–24.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Corbetta M, Siegel JS, Shulman GL. On the low dimensionality of behavioral deficits and alterations of brain network connectivity after focal injury. Cortex. 2018;107:229–37.CrossRef Corbetta M, Siegel JS, Shulman GL. On the low dimensionality of behavioral deficits and alterations of brain network connectivity after focal injury. Cortex. 2018;107:229–37.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Ng ASL, et al. Distinct network topology in Alzheimer’s disease and behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2021;13:13.CrossRef Ng ASL, et al. Distinct network topology in Alzheimer’s disease and behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2021;13:13.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Bisogno AL, et al. A low-dimensional structure of neurological impairment in stroke. Brain Commun. 2021;3:fcab119.CrossRef Bisogno AL, et al. A low-dimensional structure of neurological impairment in stroke. Brain Commun. 2021;3:fcab119.CrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Corbetta M, et al. Common behavioral clusters and subcortical anatomy in stroke. Neuron. 2015;85:927–41.CrossRef Corbetta M, et al. Common behavioral clusters and subcortical anatomy in stroke. Neuron. 2015;85:927–41.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat McKhann GM, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:263–9.CrossRef McKhann GM, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:263–9.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Andersson JLR, Sotiropoulos SN. An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. NeuroImage. 2016;125:1063–78.CrossRef Andersson JLR, Sotiropoulos SN. An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. NeuroImage. 2016;125:1063–78.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat van den Heuvel MP, et al. Evolutionary modifications in human brain connectivity associated with schizophrenia. Brain. 2019;142:3991–4002.CrossRef van den Heuvel MP, et al. Evolutionary modifications in human brain connectivity associated with schizophrenia. Brain. 2019;142:3991–4002.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Greve DN, Fischl B. Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. NeuroImage. 2009;48:63–72.CrossRef Greve DN, Fischl B. Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. NeuroImage. 2009;48:63–72.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Cammoun L, et al. Mapping the human connectome at multiple scales with diffusion spectrum MRI. J Neurosci Methods. 2012;203:386–97.CrossRef Cammoun L, et al. Mapping the human connectome at multiple scales with diffusion spectrum MRI. J Neurosci Methods. 2012;203:386–97.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Carlesimo GA, Caltagirone C, Gainotti G, Nocentini U. Batteria per la valutazione del deterioramento mentale: II. Standardizzazione e affidabilità diagnostica nell’identificazione di pazienti affetti da sindrome demenziale. [The Mental Deterioration Battery: II. Standardization and diagnostic reliability in the identification of demented patients.]. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr. 1995;56:471–88. Carlesimo GA, Caltagirone C, Gainotti G, Nocentini U. Batteria per la valutazione del deterioramento mentale: II. Standardizzazione e affidabilità diagnostica nell’identificazione di pazienti affetti da sindrome demenziale. [The Mental Deterioration Battery: II. Standardization and diagnostic reliability in the identification of demented patients.]. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr. 1995;56:471–88.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Caffarra P, Vezzadini G, Dieci F, Zonato F, Venneri A. Rey-Osterrieth complex figure: normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurol Sci. 2002;22:443–7.CrossRef Caffarra P, Vezzadini G, Dieci F, Zonato F, Venneri A. Rey-Osterrieth complex figure: normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurol Sci. 2002;22:443–7.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Novelli G, et al. Tre test clinici di memoria verbale a lungo termine: Taratura su soggetti normali [Three clinical tests for the assessment of verbal long-term memory function: Norms from 320 normal subjects]. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr. 1986;47:278–96. Novelli G, et al. Tre test clinici di memoria verbale a lungo termine: Taratura su soggetti normali [Three clinical tests for the assessment of verbal long-term memory function: Norms from 320 normal subjects]. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr. 1986;47:278–96.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Connell H, et al. Early detection of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) using the CANTAB paired Associates Learning Test. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2004;19:1207–8.CrossRef O’Connell H, et al. Early detection of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) using the CANTAB paired Associates Learning Test. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2004;19:1207–8.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Monaco M, Costa A, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo GA. Forward and backward span for verbal and visuo-spatial data: standardization and normative data from an Italian adult population. Neurol Sci. 2013;34:749–54.CrossRef Monaco M, Costa A, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo GA. Forward and backward span for verbal and visuo-spatial data: standardization and normative data from an Italian adult population. Neurol Sci. 2013;34:749–54.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Novelli G, et al. Tre test clinici di ricerca e produzione lessicale. Taratura su sogetti normali [Three clinical tests to research and rate the lexical performance of normal subjects]. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr. 1986;47:477–506. Novelli G, et al. Tre test clinici di ricerca e produzione lessicale. Taratura su sogetti normali [Three clinical tests to research and rate the lexical performance of normal subjects]. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr. 1986;47:477–506.
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Spinnler H, Tognoni G. Standardizzazione e Taratura italiana di test neuropsicologici. 1987. p. 120. Spinnler H, Tognoni G. Standardizzazione e Taratura italiana di test neuropsicologici. 1987. p. 120.
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Amodio P, et al. Variability of Trail Making Test, Symbol Digit Test and Line Trait Test in normal people. A normative study taking into account age-dependent decline and sociobiological variables. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2002;14:117–31.CrossRef Amodio P, et al. Variability of Trail Making Test, Symbol Digit Test and Line Trait Test in normal people. A normative study taking into account age-dependent decline and sociobiological variables. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2002;14:117–31.CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Giovagnoli AR, et al. Trail making test: normative values from 287 normal adult controls. Ital J Neurol Sci. 1996;17:305–9.CrossRef Giovagnoli AR, et al. Trail making test: normative values from 287 normal adult controls. Ital J Neurol Sci. 1996;17:305–9.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Serafin M, Surian L. Test degli Occhi: uno strumento per valutare la “teoria della mente”. G Ital Psicol. 2004;31(4):839–62. Serafin M, Surian L. Test degli Occhi: uno strumento per valutare la “teoria della mente”. G Ital Psicol. 2004;31(4):839–62.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Dodich A, et al. Emotion recognition from facial expressions: a normative study of the Ekman 60-Faces Test in the Italian population. Neurol Sci. 2014;35:1015–21.CrossRef Dodich A, et al. Emotion recognition from facial expressions: a normative study of the Ekman 60-Faces Test in the Italian population. Neurol Sci. 2014;35:1015–21.CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger W, Coben LA, Martin RL. A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 1982;140:566–72.CrossRef Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger W, Coben LA, Martin RL. A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 1982;140:566–72.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Cummings JL, et al. The neuropsychiatric inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology. 1994;44:2308.CrossRef Cummings JL, et al. The neuropsychiatric inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology. 1994;44:2308.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Kertesz A, Davidson W, Fox H. Frontal behavioral inventory: diagnostic criteria for frontal lobe dementia. Can J Neurol Sci J Can Sci Neurol. 1997;24:29–36.CrossRef Kertesz A, Davidson W, Fox H. Frontal behavioral inventory: diagnostic criteria for frontal lobe dementia. Can J Neurol Sci J Can Sci Neurol. 1997;24:29–36.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. 1969;9:179–86.CrossRef Lawton MP, Brody EM. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. 1969;9:179–86.CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Salvalaggio A, De Filippo De Grazia M, Zorzi M, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Corbetta M. Post-stroke deficit prediction from lesion and indirect structural and functional disconnection. Brain. 2020;143:2173–88.CrossRef Salvalaggio A, De Filippo De Grazia M, Zorzi M, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Corbetta M. Post-stroke deficit prediction from lesion and indirect structural and functional disconnection. Brain. 2020;143:2173–88.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Lisman J, et al. Viewpoints: how the hippocampus contributes to memory, navigation and cognition. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20:1434–47.CrossRef Lisman J, et al. Viewpoints: how the hippocampus contributes to memory, navigation and cognition. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20:1434–47.CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Schubert S, Leyton CE, Hodges JR, Piguet O. Longitudinal memory profiles in behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;51:775–82.CrossRef Schubert S, Leyton CE, Hodges JR, Piguet O. Longitudinal memory profiles in behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;51:775–82.CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Fernández-Matarrubia M, et al. Episodic memory dysfunction in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: a clinical and FDG-PET study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;57:1251–64.CrossRef Fernández-Matarrubia M, et al. Episodic memory dysfunction in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: a clinical and FDG-PET study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;57:1251–64.CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Hardy CJD, et al. The language profile of behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;50:359–71.CrossRef Hardy CJD, et al. The language profile of behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;50:359–71.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Kumfor F, et al. Beyond the face: how context modulates emotion processing in frontotemporal dementia subtypes. Brain. 2018;141:1172–85.CrossRef Kumfor F, et al. Beyond the face: how context modulates emotion processing in frontotemporal dementia subtypes. Brain. 2018;141:1172–85.CrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Lindquist KA, MacCormack JK, Shablack H. The role of language in emotion: predictions from psychological constructionism. Front Psychol. 2015;6:444.CrossRef Lindquist KA, MacCormack JK, Shablack H. The role of language in emotion: predictions from psychological constructionism. Front Psychol. 2015;6:444.CrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Geraudie A, et al. Speech and language impairments in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;131:1076–95.CrossRef Geraudie A, et al. Speech and language impairments in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;131:1076–95.CrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Smallwood J, et al. The default mode network in cognition: a topographical perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2021;22:503–13.CrossRef Smallwood J, et al. The default mode network in cognition: a topographical perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2021;22:503–13.CrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Satpute AB, Lindquist KA. The default mode network’s role in discrete emotion. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019;23:851–64.CrossRef Satpute AB, Lindquist KA. The default mode network’s role in discrete emotion. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019;23:851–64.CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Seeley WW, et al. Divergent social functioning in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer disease: reciprocal networks and neuronal evolution. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2007;21:S50–7.CrossRef Seeley WW, et al. Divergent social functioning in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer disease: reciprocal networks and neuronal evolution. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2007;21:S50–7.CrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Viviani R. Emotion regulation, attention to emotion, and the ventral attentional network. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:746.CrossRef Viviani R. Emotion regulation, attention to emotion, and the ventral attentional network. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:746.CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Bressler SL, Tang W, Sylvester CM, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Top-down control of human visual cortex by frontal and parietal cortex in anticipatory visual spatial attention. J Neurosci. 2008;28:10056–61.CrossRef Bressler SL, Tang W, Sylvester CM, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Top-down control of human visual cortex by frontal and parietal cortex in anticipatory visual spatial attention. J Neurosci. 2008;28:10056–61.CrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Stokes M, Thompson R, Cusack R, Duncan J. Top-down activation of shape-specific population codes in visual cortex during mental imagery. J Neurosci. 2009;29:1565–72.CrossRef Stokes M, Thompson R, Cusack R, Duncan J. Top-down activation of shape-specific population codes in visual cortex during mental imagery. J Neurosci. 2009;29:1565–72.CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Suo X, et al. Anatomical and functional coupling between the dorsal and ventral attention networks. NeuroImage. 2021;232:117868.CrossRef Suo X, et al. Anatomical and functional coupling between the dorsal and ventral attention networks. NeuroImage. 2021;232:117868.CrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Vossel S, Geng JJ, Fink GR. Dorsal and ventral attention systems: distinct neural circuits but collaborative roles. Neuroscientist. 2014;20:150–9.CrossRef Vossel S, Geng JJ, Fink GR. Dorsal and ventral attention systems: distinct neural circuits but collaborative roles. Neuroscientist. 2014;20:150–9.CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Spreng RN, Stevens WD, Chamberlain JP, Gilmore AW, Schacter DL. Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. NeuroImage. 2010;53:303–17.CrossRef Spreng RN, Stevens WD, Chamberlain JP, Gilmore AW, Schacter DL. Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. NeuroImage. 2010;53:303–17.CrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Pini L, et al. Quality of sleep predicts increased frontoparietal network connectivity in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 2020;95:205–13.CrossRef Pini L, et al. Quality of sleep predicts increased frontoparietal network connectivity in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 2020;95:205–13.CrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Jones DT, et al. Cascading network failure across the Alzheimer’s disease spectrum. Brain. 2016;139:547–62.CrossRef Jones DT, et al. Cascading network failure across the Alzheimer’s disease spectrum. Brain. 2016;139:547–62.CrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Miyagawa T, et al. Utility of the global CDR ® plus NACC FTLD rating and development of scoring rules: data from the ARTFL/LEFFTDS Consortium. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16:106–17.CrossRef Miyagawa T, et al. Utility of the global CDR ® plus NACC FTLD rating and development of scoring rules: data from the ARTFL/LEFFTDS Consortium. Alzheimers Dement. 2020;16:106–17.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
A low-dimensional cognitive-network space in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia
verfasst von
Lorenzo Pini
Siemon C de Lange
Francesca Benedetta Pizzini
Ilaria Boscolo Galazzo
Rosa Manenti
Maria Cotelli
Samantha Galluzzi
Maria Sofia Cotelli
Maurizio Corbetta
Martijn P van den Heuvel
Michela Pievani
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2022
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Alzheimer's Research & Therapy / Ausgabe 1/2022
Elektronische ISSN: 1758-9193
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-022-01145-x

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2022

Alzheimer's Research & Therapy 1/2022 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Nicht Creutzfeldt Jakob, sondern Abführtee-Vergiftung

29.05.2024 Hyponatriämie Nachrichten

Eine ältere Frau trinkt regelmäßig Sennesblättertee gegen ihre Verstopfung. Der scheint plötzlich gut zu wirken. Auf Durchfall und Erbrechen folgt allerdings eine Hyponatriämie. Nach deren Korrektur kommt es plötzlich zu progredienten Kognitions- und Verhaltensstörungen.

Schutz der Synapsen bei Alzheimer

29.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Mit einem Neurotrophin-Rezeptor-Modulator lässt sich möglicherweise eine bestehende Alzheimerdemenz etwas abschwächen: Erste Phase-2-Daten deuten auf einen verbesserten Synapsenschutz.

Sozialer Aufstieg verringert Demenzgefahr

24.05.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Ein hohes soziales Niveau ist mit die beste Versicherung gegen eine Demenz. Noch geringer ist das Demenzrisiko für Menschen, die sozial aufsteigen: Sie gewinnen fast zwei demenzfreie Lebensjahre. Umgekehrt steigt die Demenzgefahr beim sozialen Abstieg.

Hirnblutung unter DOAK und VKA ähnlich bedrohlich

17.05.2024 Direkte orale Antikoagulanzien Nachrichten

Kommt es zu einer nichttraumatischen Hirnblutung, spielt es keine große Rolle, ob die Betroffenen zuvor direkt wirksame orale Antikoagulanzien oder Marcumar bekommen haben: Die Prognose ist ähnlich schlecht.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.