Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Behavioral and Brain Functions 1/2017

Open Access 01.12.2017 | Research

Effects of GABAB receptors in the insula on recognition memory observed with intellicage

verfasst von: Nan Wu, Feng Wang, Zhe Jin, Zhen Zhang, Lian-Kun Wang, Chun Zhang, Tao Sun

Erschienen in: Behavioral and Brain Functions | Ausgabe 1/2017

Abstract

Background

Insular function has gradually become a topic of intense study in cognitive research. Recognition memory is a commonly studied type of memory in memory research. GABABR has been shown to be closely related to memory formation. In the present study, we used intellicage, which is a new intelligent behavioural test system, and a bilateral drug microinjection technique to inject into the bilateral insula, to examine the relationship between GABABR and recognition memory.

Methods

Male Sprague–Dawley rats were randomly divided into control, Sham, Nacl, baclofen and CGP35348 groups. Different testing procedures were employed using intellicage to detect changes in rat recognition memory. The expression of GABABR (GB1, GB2) in the insula of rats was determined by immunofluorescence and western blotting at the protein level. In addition, the expression of GABABR (GB1, GB2) was detected by RT-PCR at the mRNA level.

Results

The results of the intellicage test showed that recognition memory was impaired in terms of position learning, punitive learning and punitive reversal learning by using baclofen and CGP35348. In position reversal learning, no significant differences were found in terms of cognitive memory ability between the control groups and the CGP and baclofen groups. Immunofluorescence data showed GABABR (GB1, GB2) expression in the insula, while data from RT-PCR and western blot analysis demonstrated that the relative expression of GB1 and GB2 was significantly increased in the baclofen group compared with the control groups. In the CGP35348 group, the expression of GB1 and GB2 was significantly decreased, but there was no significant difference in GB1 or GB2 expression in the control groups.

Conclusions

GABABR expression in the insula plays an important role in the formation of recognition memory in rats.
Hinweise
Nan Wu and Feng Wang contributed to the work equally and should be regarded as co-first authors
Abkürzungen
GABA
gamma-aminobutyric acid
PL
position learning
revPL
position reversal learning
NE
novelty of exploration
F5d
five days before
S5d
the second five days
F3d
three days before
S3d
the last three days

Background

The insula in humans is located in the deep side of the lateral fissure, which is also known as the “hidden fifth lobe”. The position of the insula is deep, the surrounding structure is complex, and there is contact with the vast majority of brain areas [1]. In recent years, with advances in functional imaging and the development of deep brain electrical technology, the function of the insula has received substantially more attention. Recent studies have suggested that the insular cortex is the key node of the brain salience network (Salience network) [2]. The insula integrates body perception, produces subjective feelings, determines stimulus-driven attentional capture, coordinates neural resources, and causes the body to respond to stimuli. Additionally, studies have found that the insula of rodents plays a central role in the formation of taste and visual recognition memory [3, 4], and the anterior insula is an important area for perception and arousal in humans [5].
Recognition memory is a subcategory of declarative memory, which is an important index with which to evaluate the level of memory consolidation. Recognition memory includes at least two different memory processes: recollection and familiarity [6]. Recognition memory depends on many memory sub-systems of the brain network, including the visual pathway, temporal lobe medial structure (hippocampus and olfactory cortex) [4], frontal lobe and parietal cortex. Moreover, different brain regions are interrelated, highly integrated, and play different roles in recognition memory [7]. At present, many studies have focused on the role of the medial temporal lobe structure and frontal cortex in recognition memory. Although the structure of the insula has been studied, most studies utilized fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) [5, 8]. The use of stereotactic microinjection technology to study the relationship between the insula and recognition memory is still rare [1].
GABABR is a metabotropic receptor of GABA that mediates slow and sustained inhibitory effects. GABABR is composed of two subunits, GB1 and GB2. GABA plays a role in relieving stress and calming the excitement of nerves by acting on GABABR. When GABA binds to GABABR, the G protein is activated. Then, a reduction in the presynaptic Ca2+ influx and inhibition of the release of a neurotransmitter or an increase in postsynaptic membrane K+ efflux leads to posterior membrane hyperpolarization and has an effect on G protein activation [9]. Post-synaptic, GABABR activation can also enhance GABAAR function that is outside of the synapse to maintain normal network function [10]. Recent studies have shown that GABABR participates in many important physiological activities and pathological changes. GABABR directly interacts with transcription factors, regulates long-term protein synthesis and metabolic activities, plays a central role in hippocampal neuron hyperactivity [11] and is important for memory consolidation [10, 12]. Moreover, GABABR is a key regulator of neurogenesis, synaptic plasticity and the long-term potentiation (LTP), which are important for guiding the regulation of long-term memory [1214]. The GABABR agonist baclofen impairs learning and memory, while the GABABR antagonist CGP35348 improves cognitive processing. Baclofen inhibits spatial learning in mice by activating the TREK-2K+ channel through the PKA pathway [15]; it also promotes the disappearance of normal elastic memory traces and disrupts the consolidation of conditioned reward memory [16]. Although the contribution of GABABR to memory has been widely recognized, some contradictory conclusions exist due to differences in behavioural tasks, animal strains, gender, drug concentrations, time, and pathways used in experimental animals, so the relationship between the regulation of learning and memory and GABABR expression still warrants further research [14, 17]. Furthermore, the role and mechanism of GABABR in the recognition of the insula is not yet clearly understood.
In the present study, we focused on cognitive function in normal rats after treatment with the GABAB receptor agonist baclofen and the antagonist CGP35348, which were injected into the bilateral insula. Furthermore, by controlling the changes in GABABR expression in the insula, we examined the behavioural changes in recognition memory via intellicage.

Methods

Reagents

Primary antibodies against GB1R and GB2R were purchased from Abcam (Cambridge, UK). GABAB receptor agonist Baclofen and antagonist CGP35348 were obtained from Sigma (St. Louis, US). The First Strand cDNA Synthesis Kit was purchased from Thermo Fisher. The PCR primers were designed and synthesized by Sangon Biotech (Shanghai, CN). The BCA Protein Assay Kit and Total Protein Extraction Kit were purchased from Jiangsu KeyGEN BioTECH Corp, Ltd.

Animals

Male Sprague–Dawley rats (6–8 weeks old, 250–300 g) were provided from the Animal Center of Ningxia Medical University. Each rat was singly housed with an alternating 12:12 h light/dark cycle. These rats were randomly divided into a control group (Control), Sham operation group (Sham), saline group (Nacl), Baclofen group (BLF), and CGP 35348 group (CGP), (sham group was similar in operation with saline group, but was injected with drugs and saline), with five animals in each group. All animal use procedures were approved by the Ningxia Medical University Medical Center Animal Care and Use Committee and were conducted in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. (No. 2016-124).

Model establishment

After an acclimatization period of at least 1 week, the animals received surgical implantation of cannulae aimed at the insular cortex according to a standardized protocol [18].

Surgery

Rats were implanted with bilateral canulas aimed at the granular insular cortex. Before surgery, animals were anesthetized with 10% chloral (4 ml/kg, ip.). The animals were mounted into a stereotaxic frame used to position the 22-gauge stainless steel guide canula in the granular insulars. Coordinates obtained from the Paxinos and Watson brain atlas (mm from bregma: AP = +1.2; ML = ±5.5; mm from skull surface: DV = −6.5). The guide canula was anchored to the skull using stainless steel screws and acrylic cement [19]. The animals were allowed 14 days for recovery after guide canula surgeries before the behavioural test.

Microinjection procedure

All microinjections were done slowly (1 µl/0.5 min) using a 5 µl Hamilton syringe connected by Pe-20 polyethylene tube. The stainless steel injection needle (50 G) was cut to protrude 0.5 mm beyond the tips of the guide cannulae and left in place for 1 min after injection to allow diffusion of the solution and to prevent back flow. Saline (0.3 nmol/μl), Baclofen (125 ng/μl) [20], and CGP35348 (12.5 µg/µl) [21], were injected bilaterally into the granular insular 30 min before the start of the behavioural test each day.

Implanted signal transponders

After 14 days recovery from guide canula surgeries and 24 h before introduction into intellicage, the rats were anesthetized with 10% chloral (4 ml/kg, ip.) and the transponders were implanted subcutaneously into the scapula of rats using the injector system delivered with the intellicage. The detector was then used to verify their appropriate subdermal location by read-out of the transponder [22].

Intellicage

The intellicage (TSE Systems GmbH,Germany; http://​www.​newbehavior.​com) is an automated group-housing apparatus allowing experimental testing within the home cage. The cage (410 × 190 × 435 cm) is equipped with four operant conditioning chambers located in each corner. Each conditioning chamber contains two water-drinking bottles and is accessible by a small opening containing a transponder reader antenna that registers the microchip of the entering rat. Access to each water bottle is controlled by gated nosepoke holes containing infrared beam-break sensors, which can be programmed to open or remain closed upon visit or nosepoke response. As each rat was implanted with a unique microchip, corner entry and nosepoke data could be integrated with microchip readings collected by each conditioning corner’s antenna, allowing data to be separated by each individual rat.

Behavioural test

Two weeks after surgery, rats were transferred to IntelliCages. Learning and memory information were collected from 9:00 a.m. to 12:00 a.m. Drug perfusion was performed half an hour before the experiment. At the end of each trial, rats were removed, fed freely in a single cage and water-cut (7:00 a.m.–9:00 a.m.) (Fig. 1).
Intellicage learning module design:
1.
Free exploration free exploration to allow rats to become, familiar with the environment for 10 days. All doors can be opened to reach the water bottle. The number of corner visits and nosepokes was monitored to assess rat exploratory power and corner preferences.
 
2.
Nosepoke learning for a total of 10 days, all doors were closed and rats must complete the nosepoke to open the door to drink water. The number of corner visits and nosepokes was monitored to assess rat exploratory power and corner preferences. Special attention was directed to determine the least preferred corner of each rat in order to guide the next module design.
 
3.
Behavioural extinguishment rats were allowed to explore for 1 day to extinguish the previous learning behaviour, during which time all rats were able to access all corners and vial vents.
 
4.
Position learning for a total of 10 days, the rat’s least preferred corner of the nosepoke adaptation period was designated as “correct”, while the remaining corners were designated as “error”. All rats were able to visit all the corners, but only when the corner was “correct”, would the door could be opened and drinking allowed. The position learning ability was measured by calculating the number of correct corner visits.
 
5.
Position reversal learning for a total of 10 days, the opposite corner of the “correct” corner in the position learning was designated as the new “correct” corner and remaining corners were designated as “error”. Rats were able to enter all places freely, The position reversal learning ability was measured by calculating the number of correct corner visits.
 
6.
Behavioural extinguishment the same as in step3.
 
7.
Novelty exploration for a total of 4 days, the LED lights of one corner were opened randomly. The light shifted in a counter-clockwise direction on each day. All rats were free to enter all corners, the number of corners visited was calculated, and the “novelty object preference” was evaluated.
 
8.
Behavioural extinguishment the same as in step3.
 
9.
Punitive learning for 7 days, the leftside of all the corners was assigned as its “correct” (reward) side. Unlike position learning, in this module, rat nosepoke to the “error” side incurred a blow penalty (aversion to irritation).
 
10.
Penalty reversal learning: for 7 days, the rightside of all the corners was assigned as the “correct” (reward) side and nosepoke of the “error” side was penalized.
 

Sample collection

After behavioural tests, the rats were sacrificed following anaesthesia with 10% chloral hydrate and the insular tissues of some rats were collected. The other rats were perfused with 4% Paraformaldehyde solution. The isolated brains were stored in sucrose solution to dehydrate gradiently and embedded in OCT for frozen tissue sections.

Immunofluorescence

The immunofluorescence method of SABC (StreptAvidin Biotin Complex) staining was performed as below. After antigen retrieval using citric acid buffer, the sections were blocked with serum and incubated with antibodies (GB1, 1:300; GB2, 1:500) at 4 °C overnight. After washing in PBS, donkeyanti-rabbit-FITC (green) fluorescence second antibody drop, room temperature 1 h, PBS wash 10 min × 4 times. The cartridge was dried and directly blocked with anti-quencher. The insular GI areas of immunostained slides were observed and positive cells were quantitatively analyzed using Image J1.48 analysis system. Six fields of view were picked for every slide.

RT-PCR

Total RNA of insular tissues was extracted using TRIzol reagent according to the manufacturer’s protocol. First-strand cDNA was generated using M-MLV reverse transcriptase. PCR was performed to detect the mRNA expression of each gene. PCR application conditions were described as followed: denaturation at 94 °C for 3 min, followed by 40 cycles of denaturation at 94 °C for 30 s, annealing at 58 °C for 30 s and extension at 72 °C for 45 s. RT-PCR products were analyzed and visualized on 4% agarose gel containing ethidium bromide (EB). Images were captured by Tanon 3500 digital gel imaging system. The PCR primers used were listed in Table 1.
Table 1
Specific primers used in real-time PCR analysis
Gene
Primer
Sequence (5′→3′)
GAPDH
FW
RV
GAGTCAACGGATTTGGTCGT
GACAAGATTCCCGTTCTCAG
GB1
FW
RV
AGATTGTGGACCCCTTGCAC
AGAAAATGCCAAGCCACGTA
GB2
FW
RV
CACCGAGTGTGACAATGCAAA
CCAGATTCCAGCCTTGGAGG

Western blotting

Insular tissues were lysed with protein extraction kit on ice, and then total protein content was determined by BCA protein determination method. Protein from each sample was separated by SDS/PAGE and transfer on to a PVDF membrane. After blocking with 5% non-fat milk for 1 h, the membranes were immunoblotted with primary antibodies (GB1, 1:300; GB2, 1:500) overnight. After incubated with secondary antibody (1:5000), signal detection was performed by Odyssey infrared laser imaging system, followed by gray intensity analysis.

Statistical analysis

All results are expressed as the mean ± SD. Analysis was performed using SPSS 21.0 software. For the behaviour test, the significance of the difference between groups was measured using one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey test for comparison between two groups. For RT-PCR and Western blotting, the significance of the difference between groups was measured using one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s Multiple Comparison test for comparison between two groups. P < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant (*P < 0.05, **P < 0.01).

Results

Behavioural test

Intellicage

Free exploration: There was no statistically significant difference in terms of the visit [F (4, 245) = 2.272, P > 0.05] (Fig. 2a) and nosepoke [F (4, 245) = 0.693, P > 0.05] (Fig. 2b) measures of learning ability between the five groups of rats. The results showed that under normal conditions, the cognitive learning ability of the five groups of rats was basically the same.

Nosepoke learning

In terms of the number of visits and nosepokes, compared with the other three groups, the visit rate of the BLF and CGP groups was decreased [F (4, 245) = 48.12, P < 0.01]. Furthermore, the number of visits in the BLF group was significantly reduced compared with the CGP group [q = 5.385, P < 0.01] (Fig. 2c). This finding suggests that baclofen and CGP35348 can inhibit spatial learning in normal rats, but the baclofen inhibitory effect is stronger. Additionally, the number of nosepokes in the BLF group was significantly lower than in the CGP group [F (4, 245) = 57.15, P < 0.01], [q = 8.44, P < 0.01] (Fig. 2d). This finding indicates that baclofen and CGP35348 also inhibit the skills of learning ability of normal rats and that inhibition by baclofen was stronger.

Position learning

The correct number of visits of the BLF and CGP groups was significantly lower than those of the other three groups [F (4, 245) = 56.26, P < 0.01], while there was no significant difference between the BLF group and the CGP group (q = 3.164, P > 0.05) (Fig. 2e). This indicates that baclofen and CGP35348 can inhibit spatial learning and memory in normal rats.

Position reversal learning

There were no significant differences in the number of correct visits between the BLF and the CGP groups compared with the other three groups, but the number of correct visits for the BLF group was significantly lower than that of the CGP group [q = 4.45, P < 0.05] (Fig. 2f). This suggests that baclofen and CGP35348 have an effect on learning and memory and that baclofen has a stronger inhibitory effect on the reversal of spatial position learning, which may not affect the spatial learning ability of rats.

Novelty exploration

The learning ability of five groups of rats did not show statistical significance in terms of the number of visits [F (4, 95) = 1.039, P > 0.05] (Fig. 2g). The results showed that for the five groups of rats the novelty exploration of learning ability was basically the same.

Punitive learning

The number of correct nosepokes in the BLF group and the CGP group was significantly lower than that in the other three groups [F (4, 170) = 32.20, P < 0.01], and the BLF and CGP groups showed a statistically significant difference in nosepokes [q = 4.48, P < 0.05] (Fig. 2h). This result indicates that baclofen and CGP35348 could inhibit the spatial and skills learning components of recognition memory in normal rats and that the inhibitory effect of baclofen was stronger.

Punitive reversal learning

The number of correct nosepokes in the BLF and CGP groups was significantly lower than that in the other three groups [F (4, 170) = 23.01, P < 0.01], while the BLF and CGP groups showed a statistically significant difference in terms of the number of correct nosepokes [q = 4.46, P < 0.05] (Fig. 2i). This finding indicates that baclofen and CGP35348 had inhibitory effects on spatial and skills learning of recognition memory and that baclofen had a stronger inhibitory effect.
The conversion efficiency of learning and memory:
1.
During the first 5 days of position reversal learning, the BLF group showed less frequent visits compared with the other four groups [F (4, 245) = 5.611, P < 0.01]. In addition, the number of correct visits decreased significantly in the BLF group compared with the CGP group [q = 4.14, P < 0.01] (Fig. 3a). However, in the second 5 days, the BLF and CGP groups showed the same performance as the control groups in terms of the number of correct visits. The number of correct visits in the BLF group was significantly lower than that in the CGP group [q = 3.95, P < 0.01] (Fig. 3b), which indicates that, at the beginning of reversal learning, baclofen inhibits the conversion efficiency of learning more than CGP35348. The response ability of rats to spatial localization transformation is poor, but after a period of learning, Baclofen’s inhibition slowly weakens.
 
2.
In the BLF and CGP groups, the number of correct nosepokes decreased significantly from the first 3 days [F (4, 70) = 25.56, P < 0.01] to the last 3 days of the reversal of learning [F (4, 70) = 68.17, P < 0.01]. The BLF and CGP groups differed significantly from each other [q = 5.56, P < 0.01] (Fig. 3c), [q = 6.76, P < 0.01] (Fig. 3d). This finding indicated that baclofen and CGP35348 could inhibit the learning efficiency of rats. Baclofen produced a more obvious reduction in the reversal of learning efficiency.
 

Nissl

Nissl staining showed that the target of the insula was in accordance with the experimental requirement (Fig. 4).

Immunofluorescence

Immunofluorescence showed normal expression of GB1 and GB2 in the insula, indicating that the normal rat insula contains GABABR (Fig. 5).

RT-PCR

The expression of GB1and GB2 was much higher in the Baclofen group but lower in the CGP group compared to the other groups [GB1F (4, 20) = 15.51, P < 0.01] [GB2F (4, 20) = 35.98, P < 0.01] (Fig. 6).

Western blotting

The expression of GB1 and GB2 was much higher in the Baclofen group, but lower in the CGP group compared to the other groups [GB1F (4, 20) = 20.17, P < 0.01] [GB2F (4, 20) = 19.01, P < 0.01] (Fig. 7).

Discussion

Research regarding the insula is a new method of understanding the cognitive function of the brain. Studies have shown that the insula is involved in the regulation of pain, formation of addiction, formation of disgust, generation of depression, regulation of cardiac activity, language planning, and empathy [1, 2, 23]. Herpes encephalitis, ischaemic stroke, glioma and other diseases often involve the insula. In addition, neurological and psychiatric disorders, such as temporal lobe epilepsy, schizophrenia, dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, anxiety, depression, and autism, are closely related to the insula [24]. Some research on recognition memory has shown that the main structural basis of recognition memory from the perspective of anatomy and function is the highlight network. However, the role and mechanism of the insula, which is the core of the highlight network, is unclear. In addition, there is still considerable controversy regarding the relationship between recollection and familiar memory, its various functional characteristics, the neural basis and other issues [19, 24]. However, GABA, as the most important inhibitory neurotransmitter in the central nervous system, plays an important role in the encoding, sorting and transmission of nerve information [25]. Several studies have shown that GABA is involved in the decision-making process [26] and that the dysfunction of GABABR is also associated with a variety of neurological diseases, including epilepsy, anxiety, depression, drug addiction and cognitive disorders [11, 2729]. To summarize the results of the research regarding GABA and the insula, first, neuropsychiatric diseases are closely associated to the insula and GABABR. Second, neuropsychiatric diseases are often accompanied by impaired cognitive function. Third, the recognition of cognitive memory is a good indicator of cognitive function. As both the insula and GABABR play important roles in recognition memory, we raise some questions of whether there is a link between them. This question was explored in our experiments.
Our behavioural testing utilized a new intelligent behaviour monitoring system, intellicage, with different recognition memory modules. This system is different from the previous water maze, dark test and other classical behavioural detection methods. One of the biggest drawbacks of these classic behavioural experiments is that they are man-made to provide animals a variety of stimuli to observe the animal’s learning and memory changes. Alternatively, intellicage is characterized by the location of rats in an environment that closely resembles a natural social environment. This setup avoids human intervention as much as possible, which can lead to behavioural changes in rats [30, 31]. After years of use, intellicage is gradually being taken seriously in the field of behavioural research. Using the intellicage operating system, we divided the testing programme into four modules, which represent four different types of recognition memory, to verify the effect of GABABR expression in the insula on recognition memory. First, we used nosepoke learning to explore the effects of rats’ recognition memory (skills learning ability). The second module used position learning and position reversal learning to explore rats’ spatial recognition memory changes. In the third module, we explored the use of novel exploration to evaluate the ability of rats to recognize memories of novel things. The fourth module utilized punitive learning and punitive reversal learning to explore a reconsidering memory that reflected spatial positioning and skills learning ability.
Our evidence confirms that after perfusing the GABABR-selective agonist baclofen and GABABR-specific antagonist CGP35348 in normal rat insula, the number of corner accesses was reduced in the nosepoke study. This finding indicated that GABABR upregulation and downregulation in the insula resulted in a decrease in the skills learning ability associated with recognition memory. Studies have confirmed that the anterior medial temporal lobes plays a role in food intake [32], and our results confirmed that GABABR in the insula also affects skills learning as rats learn to drink and remember how to obtain water. In position learning, modulation of rat GABABR expression also caused a reduction in spatial memory, but the change in GABABR expression did not affect the spatial recognition memory of the rat in position reversal learning. This finding suggests that upregulation and downregulation of GABABR in the insula could damage the rat’s spatial memory, but it did not affect spatial reversal learning associated with recognition memory. Studies have confirmed that the hippocampus is the node of the memory system [33], but we have determined through experiments that GABABR in the insula also has such a role. For punitive learning and punitive reversal learning, upregulation and downregulation of GABABR expression in the insula of rats could lead to a decrease in the number of correct nosepokes, which indicates that the comprehensive utilization of recognition memory is impaired and interrupted. In recent years, most studies on recognition memory have targeted the hippocampus [34], It has been reported that in the hippocampus, baclofen inhibits GABABR-induced spatial learning in normal rats by activating the TREK-2K+ channel [15]. CGP35348 inhibits the inhibitory postsynaptic potential (IPSP), enhances GABABR activation, and improves the memory formation process [35]. However, in our study, up-regulation and down-regulation of GABABR expression in the insula during the abovementioned test modules affected cognition in normal rats, and GABABR upregulation in the normal rat insula was more damaging to cognition. Therefore, it cannot be determined whether CGP35348 can improve cognitive function because there are different targets and the roles of the targets are also very different. Thus, increases or decreases in GABABR expression in the insula will damage memory function. Moreover, with a change in learning pattern, the cognitive changes associated with GABABR expression were different. Several studies have confirmed that GABA in the hippocampus plays an important role in novelty recognition and target identification [36, 37], but we found that the change in GABABR expression in the insula did not affect the ability of the animals to recognize new things, which indicates that the insula may not be the region involved in novelty recognition.
In this experiment, we also explored the conversion efficiency of rats’ recognition memory. In position reversal learning, we found that in the first 5 days, upregulation and downregulation of GABABR in the insula resulted in a marked decrease in the recognition memory of rats and that upregulation of GABABR expression made this reduction more obvious. However, in the last 5 days of learning, GABABR expression in the insula was not associated with a decline in recognition memory capacity. This finding indicates that at the beginning of the first 5 days, upregulation and downregulation of GABABR expression in the insula resulted in a reduction in the efficiency of rat spatial recognition of memory transfer, but with an increase in learning time, the efficiency of this conversion was restored in the last 5 days. However, upregulation and downregulation of GABABR expression, which occurred both in the first 3 days and in the second 3 days of punitive reversal learning, impaired the conversion efficiency of spatial location recognition memory and the recognition memory of the skills learning ability, suggesting that changes in GABABR expression in the insula influence the efficiency of the transformation of recognition memory. Combining the results of position reversal learning and punitive reversal learning, we conclude that GABABR changes in the insula can lead to a reduction in the efficiency of recognition memory, which is a single mode, and this reduction in efficiency will be mitigated with increased learning time; however, for more than one mode of recognition memory, the change in GABABR expression in the insula will continue to affect the conversion efficiency of recognition memory.
The insula is located near the temporal lobe. Research has confirmed that the medial temporal lobe and insula are associated with memory consolidation [6] and that temporal lobe lesions, such as temporal lobe epilepsy, can cause patients to have memory damage [38]. It has been found that the relationship between GABABR expression in the insula and memory recognition may provide a new target for the treatment of patients with impaired cognitive function. However, in future experiments, we need to study the link between the insula and other brain regions to investigate the behavioural changes of rats in a large sample and to explore the molecular mechanism of GABABR expression in insular and recognition memory.
In conclusion, our study demonstrates that GABABR plays an important role in the formation of recognition memory and may be become a new target for the study of memory.

Authors’ contributions

NW contributed to the design and planning of the experiment and all the tests, data analyses and writing the manuscript. ZZ and KLW took part in the planning and designing of the experiment and cognitive tests, data analyses and manuscript preparation. FW and TS contributed to the design and planning of the experiment. ZJ contributed to the design and data analyses. TS should be regarded as corresponding author. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Availability of data and materials

Data set for the design of Intellicage is from http://​www.​newbehavior.​com. All data are fully available without restriction.

Ethics approval

Rats were provided were provided from the Animal Center of Ningxia Medical University (China) and all experiments were in accordance with the Regulations of Experimental Animal Administration issued by the State Committee of Science and Technology of China on Oct. 31, 1988.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Nos. 81660226, 81460208) and the Ningxia Science and Technology Support Project.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Bermudez-Rattoni F. The forgotten insular cortex: its role on recognition memory formation. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;109:207–16.CrossRefPubMed Bermudez-Rattoni F. The forgotten insular cortex: its role on recognition memory formation. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014;109:207–16.CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Uddin LQ. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(1):55–61.CrossRefPubMed Uddin LQ. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(1):55–61.CrossRefPubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat O’Brien LD, Sticht MA, Mitchnick KA, Limebeer CL, Parker LA, Winters BD. CB1 receptor antagonism in the granular insular cortex or somatosensory area facilitates consolidation of object recognition memory. Neurosci Lett. 2014;578:192–6.CrossRefPubMed O’Brien LD, Sticht MA, Mitchnick KA, Limebeer CL, Parker LA, Winters BD. CB1 receptor antagonism in the granular insular cortex or somatosensory area facilitates consolidation of object recognition memory. Neurosci Lett. 2014;578:192–6.CrossRefPubMed
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Blonde GD, Bales MB, Spector AC. Extensive lesions in rat insular cortex significantly disrupt taste sensitivity to NaCl and KCl and slow salt discrimination learning. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(2):e0117515.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Blonde GD, Bales MB, Spector AC. Extensive lesions in rat insular cortex significantly disrupt taste sensitivity to NaCl and KCl and slow salt discrimination learning. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(2):e0117515.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Fischer DB, Boes AD, Demertzi A, Evrard HC, Laureys S, Edlow BL, Liu H, Saper CB, Pascual-Leone A, Fox MD, et al. A human brain network derived from coma-causing brainstem lesions. Neurology. 2016;87(23):2427–34.CrossRefPubMed Fischer DB, Boes AD, Demertzi A, Evrard HC, Laureys S, Edlow BL, Liu H, Saper CB, Pascual-Leone A, Fox MD, et al. A human brain network derived from coma-causing brainstem lesions. Neurology. 2016;87(23):2427–34.CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Balderas I, Rodriguez-Ortiz CJ, Bermudez-Rattoni F. Consolidation and reconsolidation of object recognition memory. Behav Brain Res. 2015;285:213–22.CrossRefPubMed Balderas I, Rodriguez-Ortiz CJ, Bermudez-Rattoni F. Consolidation and reconsolidation of object recognition memory. Behav Brain Res. 2015;285:213–22.CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Rodriguez P, Zhou W, Barrett DW, Altmeyer W, Gutierrez JE, Li J, Lancaster JL, Gonzalez-Lima F, Duong TQ. Multimodal randomized functional MR imaging of the effects of methylene blue in the human brain. Radiology. 2016;281(2):516–26.CrossRefPubMed Rodriguez P, Zhou W, Barrett DW, Altmeyer W, Gutierrez JE, Li J, Lancaster JL, Gonzalez-Lima F, Duong TQ. Multimodal randomized functional MR imaging of the effects of methylene blue in the human brain. Radiology. 2016;281(2):516–26.CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Padgett CL, Slesinger PA. GABAB receptor coupling to G-proteins and ion channels. Adv Pharmacol. 2010;58:123–47.CrossRefPubMed Padgett CL, Slesinger PA. GABAB receptor coupling to G-proteins and ion channels. Adv Pharmacol. 2010;58:123–47.CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Craig MT, McBain CJ. The emerging role of GABAB receptors as regulators of network dynamics: fast actions from a ‘slow’ receptor? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2014;26:15–21.CrossRefPubMed Craig MT, McBain CJ. The emerging role of GABAB receptors as regulators of network dynamics: fast actions from a ‘slow’ receptor? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2014;26:15–21.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Lang M, Moradi-Chameh H, Zahid T, Gane J, Wu C, Valiante T, Zhang L. Regulating hippocampal hyperexcitability through GABAB receptors. Physiol Rep. 2014;2(4):e00278.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lang M, Moradi-Chameh H, Zahid T, Gane J, Wu C, Valiante T, Zhang L. Regulating hippocampal hyperexcitability through GABAB receptors. Physiol Rep. 2014;2(4):e00278.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Cullen PK, Dulka BN, Ortiz S, Riccio DC, Jasnow AM. GABA-mediated presynaptic inhibition is required for precision of long-term memory. Learn Mem. 2014;21(4):180–4.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Cullen PK, Dulka BN, Ortiz S, Riccio DC, Jasnow AM. GABA-mediated presynaptic inhibition is required for precision of long-term memory. Learn Mem. 2014;21(4):180–4.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Pontes A, Zhang Y, Hu W. Novel functions of GABA signaling in adult neurogenesis. Front Biol. 2013;8(5):496–507.CrossRef Pontes A, Zhang Y, Hu W. Novel functions of GABA signaling in adult neurogenesis. Front Biol. 2013;8(5):496–507.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Heaney CF, Kinney JW. Role of GABA(B) receptors in learning and memory and neurological disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;63:1–28.CrossRefPubMed Heaney CF, Kinney JW. Role of GABA(B) receptors in learning and memory and neurological disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;63:1–28.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Deng PY, Xiao Z, Yang C, Rojanathammanee L, Grisanti L, Watt J, Geiger JD, Liu R, Porter JE, Lei S. GABA(B) receptor activation inhibits neuronal excitability and spatial learning in the entorhinal cortex by activating TREK-2K+ channels. Neuron. 2009;63:230–43.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Deng PY, Xiao Z, Yang C, Rojanathammanee L, Grisanti L, Watt J, Geiger JD, Liu R, Porter JE, Lei S. GABA(B) receptor activation inhibits neuronal excitability and spatial learning in the entorhinal cortex by activating TREK-2K+ channels. Neuron. 2009;63:230–43.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Heinrichs SC, Leite-Morris KA, Carey RJ, Kaplan GB. Baclofen enhances extinction of opiate conditioned place preference. Behav Brain Res. 2010;207(2):353–9.CrossRefPubMed Heinrichs SC, Leite-Morris KA, Carey RJ, Kaplan GB. Baclofen enhances extinction of opiate conditioned place preference. Behav Brain Res. 2010;207(2):353–9.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Rajalu M, Fritzius T, Adelfinger L, Jacquier V, Besseyrias V, Gassmann M, Bettler B. Pharmacological characterization of GABAB receptor subtypes assembled with auxiliary KCTD subunits. Neuropharmacology. 2015;88:145–54.CrossRefPubMed Rajalu M, Fritzius T, Adelfinger L, Jacquier V, Besseyrias V, Gassmann M, Bettler B. Pharmacological characterization of GABAB receptor subtypes assembled with auxiliary KCTD subunits. Neuropharmacology. 2015;88:145–54.CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Balderas I, Rodriguez-Ortiz CJ, Salgado-Tonda P, Chavez-Hurtado J, McGaugh J, Bermudez-Rattoni F. The consolidation of object and context recognition memory involve different regions of the temporal lobe. Learn Mem. 2008;15(9):618–24.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Balderas I, Rodriguez-Ortiz CJ, Salgado-Tonda P, Chavez-Hurtado J, McGaugh J, Bermudez-Rattoni F. The consolidation of object and context recognition memory involve different regions of the temporal lobe. Learn Mem. 2008;15(9):618–24.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Berman DE, Hazvi S, Neduva V, Dudai Y. The role of identified neurotransmitter systems in the response of insular cortex to unfamiliar taste: activation of ERK1-2 and formation of a memory trace. J Neurosci. 2000;20(18):7017–23.PubMed Berman DE, Hazvi S, Neduva V, Dudai Y. The role of identified neurotransmitter systems in the response of insular cortex to unfamiliar taste: activation of ERK1-2 and formation of a memory trace. J Neurosci. 2000;20(18):7017–23.PubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat St Onge JR, Floresco SB. Prefrontal cortical contribution to risk-based decision making. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20(8):1816–28.CrossRefPubMed St Onge JR, Floresco SB. Prefrontal cortical contribution to risk-based decision making. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20(8):1816–28.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Ataie Z, Babri S, Ghahramanian Golzar M, Ebrahimi H, Mirzaie F, Mohaddes G. GABAB receptor blockade prevents antiepileptic action of ghrelin in the rat hippocampus. Adv Pharm Bull. 2013;3(2):353–8.PubMedPubMedCentral Ataie Z, Babri S, Ghahramanian Golzar M, Ebrahimi H, Mirzaie F, Mohaddes G. GABAB receptor blockade prevents antiepileptic action of ghrelin in the rat hippocampus. Adv Pharm Bull. 2013;3(2):353–8.PubMedPubMedCentral
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Urbach YK, Raber KA, Canneva F, Plank AC, Andreasson T, Ponten H, Kullingsjo J, Nguyen HP, Riess O, von Horsten S. Automated phenotyping and advanced data mining exemplified in rats transgenic for Huntington’s disease. J Neurosci Methods. 2014;234:38–53.CrossRefPubMed Urbach YK, Raber KA, Canneva F, Plank AC, Andreasson T, Ponten H, Kullingsjo J, Nguyen HP, Riess O, von Horsten S. Automated phenotyping and advanced data mining exemplified in rats transgenic for Huntington’s disease. J Neurosci Methods. 2014;234:38–53.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Feng Q, Chen X, Sun J, Zhou Y, Sun Y, Ding W, Zhang Y, Zhuang Z, Xu J, Du Y. Voxel-level comparison of arterial spin-labeled perfusion magnetic resonance imaging in adolescents with internet gaming addiction. Behav Brain Funct. 2013;9(1):33.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Feng Q, Chen X, Sun J, Zhou Y, Sun Y, Ding W, Zhang Y, Zhuang Z, Xu J, Du Y. Voxel-level comparison of arterial spin-labeled perfusion magnetic resonance imaging in adolescents with internet gaming addiction. Behav Brain Funct. 2013;9(1):33.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Gogolla N, Takesian AE, Feng G, Fagiolini M, Hensch TK. Sensory integration in mouse insular cortex reflects GABA circuit maturation. Neuron. 2014;83(4):894–905.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Gogolla N, Takesian AE, Feng G, Fagiolini M, Hensch TK. Sensory integration in mouse insular cortex reflects GABA circuit maturation. Neuron. 2014;83(4):894–905.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Nussbaum D, Honarmand K, Govoni R, Kalahani-Bargis M, Bass S, Ni X, Laforge K, Burden A, Romero K, Basarke S. An eight component decision-making model for problem gambling: a systems approach to stimulate integrative research. J Gambl Stud. 2011;27(4):523–63.CrossRefPubMed Nussbaum D, Honarmand K, Govoni R, Kalahani-Bargis M, Bass S, Ni X, Laforge K, Burden A, Romero K, Basarke S. An eight component decision-making model for problem gambling: a systems approach to stimulate integrative research. J Gambl Stud. 2011;27(4):523–63.CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Enna SJ, Blackburn TP. GABAB receptor pharmacology—a tribute to Norman Bowery. Preface. Adv Pharmacol. 2010;58:123–47.CrossRef Enna SJ, Blackburn TP. GABAB receptor pharmacology—a tribute to Norman Bowery. Preface. Adv Pharmacol. 2010;58:123–47.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Fu Z, Yang H, Xiao Y, Zhao G, Huang H. The gamma-aminobutyric acid type B (GABAB) receptor agonist baclofen inhibits morphine sensitization by decreasing the dopamine level in rat nucleus accumbens. Behav Brain Funct. 2012;8:20.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Fu Z, Yang H, Xiao Y, Zhao G, Huang H. The gamma-aminobutyric acid type B (GABAB) receptor agonist baclofen inhibits morphine sensitization by decreasing the dopamine level in rat nucleus accumbens. Behav Brain Funct. 2012;8:20.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Onishchenko N, Tamm C, Vahter M, Hokfelt T, Johnson JA, Johnson DA, Ceccatelli S. Developmental exposure to methylmercury alters learning and induces depression-like behavior in male mice. Toxicol Sci. 2007;97(2):428–37.CrossRefPubMed Onishchenko N, Tamm C, Vahter M, Hokfelt T, Johnson JA, Johnson DA, Ceccatelli S. Developmental exposure to methylmercury alters learning and induces depression-like behavior in male mice. Toxicol Sci. 2007;97(2):428–37.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Krackow S, Vannoni E, Codita A, Mohammed Ah, Cirulli F, Branchi I, Alleva E, Reichelt A, Willuweit A, Voikar V. Consistent behavioral phenotype differences between inbred mouse strains in the IntelliCage. Genes Brain Behav. 2010;9(7):722–31.CrossRefPubMed Krackow S, Vannoni E, Codita A, Mohammed Ah, Cirulli F, Branchi I, Alleva E, Reichelt A, Willuweit A, Voikar V. Consistent behavioral phenotype differences between inbred mouse strains in the IntelliCage. Genes Brain Behav. 2010;9(7):722–31.CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Coppin G. The anterior medial temporal lobes: their role in food intake and body weight regulation. Physiol Behav. 2016;167:60–70.CrossRefPubMed Coppin G. The anterior medial temporal lobes: their role in food intake and body weight regulation. Physiol Behav. 2016;167:60–70.CrossRefPubMed
33.
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Lee JQ, Zelinski EL, McDonald RJ, Sutherland RJ. Heterarchic reinstatement of long-term memory: a concept on hippocampal amnesia in rodent memory research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:154–66.CrossRefPubMed Lee JQ, Zelinski EL, McDonald RJ, Sutherland RJ. Heterarchic reinstatement of long-term memory: a concept on hippocampal amnesia in rodent memory research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:154–66.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Gillani Q, Iqbal S, Arfa F, Khakwani S, Akbar A, Ullah A, Ali M, Iqbal FA. Effect of GABAB receptor antagonist (CGP35348) on learning and memory in albino mice. Sci World J. 2014;2014:983651.CrossRef Gillani Q, Iqbal S, Arfa F, Khakwani S, Akbar A, Ullah A, Ali M, Iqbal FA. Effect of GABAB receptor antagonist (CGP35348) on learning and memory in albino mice. Sci World J. 2014;2014:983651.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Terunuma M, Revilla-Sanchez R, Quadros IM, Deng Q, Deeb TZ, Lumb M, Sicinski P, Haydon PG, Pangalos MN, Moss SJ. Postsynaptic GABAB receptor activity regulates excitatory neuronal architecture and spatial memory. J Neurosci. 2014;34(3):804–16.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Terunuma M, Revilla-Sanchez R, Quadros IM, Deng Q, Deeb TZ, Lumb M, Sicinski P, Haydon PG, Pangalos MN, Moss SJ. Postsynaptic GABAB receptor activity regulates excitatory neuronal architecture and spatial memory. J Neurosci. 2014;34(3):804–16.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Khanegheini A, Nasehi M, Zarrindast MR. The modulatory effect of CA1 GABAb receptors on ketamine-induced spatial and non-spatial novelty detection deficits with respect to Ca(2+). Neuroscience. 2015;305:157–68.CrossRefPubMed Khanegheini A, Nasehi M, Zarrindast MR. The modulatory effect of CA1 GABAb receptors on ketamine-induced spatial and non-spatial novelty detection deficits with respect to Ca(2+). Neuroscience. 2015;305:157–68.CrossRefPubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Schipper S, Aalbers MW, Rijkers K, Lagiere M, Bogaarts JG, Blokland A, Klinkenberg S, Hoogland G, Vles JS. Accelerated cognitive decline in a rodent model for temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2016;65:33–41.CrossRefPubMed Schipper S, Aalbers MW, Rijkers K, Lagiere M, Bogaarts JG, Blokland A, Klinkenberg S, Hoogland G, Vles JS. Accelerated cognitive decline in a rodent model for temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2016;65:33–41.CrossRefPubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1986. Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1986.
Metadaten
Titel
Effects of GABAB receptors in the insula on recognition memory observed with intellicage
verfasst von
Nan Wu
Feng Wang
Zhe Jin
Zhen Zhang
Lian-Kun Wang
Chun Zhang
Tao Sun
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2017
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Behavioral and Brain Functions / Ausgabe 1/2017
Elektronische ISSN: 1744-9081
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-017-0125-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2017

Behavioral and Brain Functions 1/2017 Zur Ausgabe

Neu in den Fachgebieten Neurologie und Psychiatrie

Niedriger diastolischer Blutdruck erhöht Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Komplikationen

25.04.2024 Hypotonie Nachrichten

Wenn unter einer medikamentösen Hochdrucktherapie der diastolische Blutdruck in den Keller geht, steigt das Risiko für schwere kardiovaskuläre Ereignisse: Darauf deutet eine Sekundäranalyse der SPRINT-Studie hin.

Frühe Alzheimertherapie lohnt sich

25.04.2024 AAN-Jahrestagung 2024 Nachrichten

Ist die Tau-Last noch gering, scheint der Vorteil von Lecanemab besonders groß zu sein. Und beginnen Erkrankte verzögert mit der Behandlung, erreichen sie nicht mehr die kognitive Leistung wie bei einem früheren Start. Darauf deuten neue Analysen der Phase-3-Studie Clarity AD.

Viel Bewegung in der Parkinsonforschung

25.04.2024 Parkinson-Krankheit Nachrichten

Neue arznei- und zellbasierte Ansätze, Frühdiagnose mit Bewegungssensoren, Rückenmarkstimulation gegen Gehblockaden – in der Parkinsonforschung tut sich einiges. Auf dem Deutschen Parkinsonkongress ging es auch viel um technische Innovationen.

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.