Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Molecular Brain 1/2014

Open Access 01.12.2014 | Research

Enhanced stability of hippocampal place representation caused by reduced magnesium block of NMDA receptors in the dentate gyrus

verfasst von: Yuichiro Hayashi, Yoko Nabeshima, Katsunori Kobayashi, Tsuyoshi Miyakawa, Koichi Tanda, Keizo Takao, Hidenori Suzuki, Eisaku Esumi, Shigeru Noguchi, Yukiko Matsuda, Toshikuni Sasaoka, Tetsuo Noda, Jun-ichi Miyazaki, Masayoshi Mishina, Kazuo Funabiki, Yo-ichi Nabeshima

Erschienen in: Molecular Brain | Ausgabe 1/2014

Abstract

Background

Voltage-dependent block of the NMDA receptor by Mg2+ is thought to be central to the unique involvement of this receptor in higher brain functions. However, the in vivo role of the Mg2+ block in the mammalian brain has not yet been investigated, because brain-wide loss of the Mg2+ block causes perinatal lethality. In this study, we used a brain-region specific knock-in mouse expressing an NMDA receptor that is defective for the Mg2+ block in order to test its role in neural information processing.

Results

We devised a method to induce a single amino acid substitution (N595Q) in the GluN2A subunit of the NMDA receptor, specifically in the hippocampal dentate gyrus in mice. This mutation reduced the Mg2+ block at the medial perforant path–granule cell synapse and facilitated synaptic potentiation induced by high-frequency stimulation. The mutants had more stable hippocampal place fields in the CA1 than the controls did, and place representation showed lower sensitivity to visual differences. In addition, behavioral tests revealed that the mutants had a spatial working memory deficit.

Conclusions

These results suggest that the Mg2+ block in the dentate gyrus regulates hippocampal spatial information processing by attenuating activity-dependent synaptic potentiation in the dentate gyrus.
Begleitmaterial
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.​1186/​1756-6606-7-44) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Yuichiro Hayashi, Yoko Nabeshima, Katsunori Kobayashi, Tsuyoshi Miyakawa contributed equally to this work.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Authors’ contributions

YN and Y-IN developed the gene-manipulation method. YN, EE, SN, YM, TS, TN, JM, MM and Y-IN generated the knock-in mice. YH and KF performed in vivo electrophysiology experiments. KK and HS performed slice electrophysiology experiments. TM, KoT and KeT performed behavioral experiments. YH, YN, KK, TM and Y-IN designed and wrote the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Abkürzungen
NMDAR
N-methyl D-aspartate receptor
DG
Dentate gyrus
GC
Granule cells
WT
Wild type
STP
Short-term potentiation
LTP
Long-term potentiation
SGZ
Subgranular zone
BrdU
5-bromo-2′-deoxyuridine.

Background

NMDA receptors (NMDARs) contribute to the majority of excitatory synaptic transmission in the brain, and are key molecules involved in synaptic plasticity, learning, and memory [1]. The activation of NMDAR channels is linked to the membrane potential by the voltage-dependent Mg2+ block [2, 3], allowing these channels to sense the coincidence of pre- and postsynaptic activity [4]. The use of pharmacological blockades and transgenic animals has shown that the NMDARs play a crucial role in various types of learning [5]. Recently, it has been demonstrated that elevated brain magnesium concentrations enhance memory function [6], suggesting that Mg2+ block of NMDARs is involved in memory. Furthermore, Drosophila overexpressing an NMDAR defective for Mg2+ block has defects in long-term memory [7]. However, the in vivo role of the Mg2+ block in vertebrates still remains unclear. NMDARs defective for the Mg2+ block cause perinatal lethality in mice expressing these receptors [8].
A single amino acid substitution is known to greatly change the Mg2+ blockade of the NMDAR. Functional NMDARs are likely hetero-oligomers comprising two types of subunits, GluN1 and GluN2. Each subunit has four predicted membrane-associated segments (M1–M4). A single asparagine residue in M2 is critical for voltage-dependent Mg2+ blockade. Replacement of asparagine 598 of the GluN1 subunit with glutamine strongly reduces the sensitivity of the NMDAR channel to Mg2+ block [9, 10]. A similar mutation in the GluN2 subunits (N595Q) also strongly reduces the block by Mg2+[9]. In the present study, we developed a new method for introducing a desired mutation into the gene of interest in a spatially restricted manner. Using this technique, we selectively introduced a single amino acid mutation (N595Q) into the GluN2A subunit in the dentate gyrus (DG) and tested the involvement of the Mg2+ block of NMDARs in hippocampal computational function.
The hippocampus is one of the most widely studied brain regions because of its simple structure and central role in memory formation in both humans and other mammals [11, 12]. Anatomically, the hippocampus can be divided into three structurally dissimilar areas: the DG, CA3, and CA1, and the DG is the first region involved in the hippocampal trisynaptic circuit [13]. We used hippocampal place cell activity to test the role of the DG Mg2+ block in hippocampal information processing. Place cells are neurons that exhibit a high firing rate when an animal is in a specific location in an environment [14, 15]. The properties of place cells have been used to gain insights into neural computation in the hippocampus [1618].
Here we found that expression of receptors with the N595Q substitution in GluN2A in the granule cells of the dentate gyrus reduced the Mg2+ block of NMDAR-mediated synaptic currents and facilitated activity-dependent synaptic potentiation at medial perforant path–granule cell synapses. Hippocampal place representation in the mutants was more stable than that of the controls and place representation showed low sensitivity to visual differences. These results imply that enhanced synaptic potentiation resulting from the decrease in Mg2+ blocking stabilizes place representation but impairs pattern separation. The mutants also showed deficits in working memory, indicating that the Mg2+ block contributes to spatial learning.

Results

Generation of the GluN2A (N595Q) mouse

We selected GluN2A for the dentate GC-specific abrogation of Mg2+ blockade of NMDARs because it is abundantly expressed in the hippocampus of adults, but not during early development [19]. To replace the asparagine residue 595 (N595) with glutamine (N595Q) in the GluN2A subunit, we constructed the targeting vector shown in Figure 1A and Additional file 1: Figure S1. We performed a two-step selection with G418 and Cre recombinase to obtain the desired recombinant ES clones (Figure 1A), and these were used to generate the flox homozygous (Figure 1C) mice. The mutually exclusive pre-mRNA splicing of adjacent exons is determined by the proximity of the downstream exon branch point to the upstream exon splice donor site, and a proximity of less than 51 nucleotides completely prevents the adjacent exons from being spliced together [20]. Therefore, the length of the spacer elements between the splice donor site of the wild-type (WT) exon 10 (Figure 1B, orange box) and the branching point of the mutant exon 10 (Figure 1B, green box) was designed to be 48 nucleotides. If the WT exon 10 between two loxP sequences were deleted by the action of Cre recombinase, the mutant exon 10 would then be spliced into mRNA (Figure 1C). GluN2Aflox/flox mice were first crossed with Nestin-Cre mice to confirm that the desired recombination event takes place, and their offspring (GluN2A+/flox-NestinCre) were subjected to analysis since the majority of homozygous GluN2A flox/flox-NestinCre mice died within 2 weeks after birth. In agreement with the distribution of Cre protein (data not shown), Cre-loxP recombination products were observed in the brain and spinal cord (Figure 2A). As expected, there was no significant difference in the size and expression levels of GluN2A mRNA and GluN2A subunits between genotypes (Figure 2B). We next confirmed the exclusive recruitment of WT exon 10 in the GluN2A flox/flox mice prior to Cre-loxP recombination and the substitution of WT mRNA (N595) with mutant mRNA (595Q) after Cre-loxP recombination (Figure 2C). These results clearly demonstrate that our method allows the introduction of a desired mutation into any gene of interest in a tissue-restricted manner without altering the level and pattern of target gene expression.

Induction of the N/Q mutation in the DG

Under control of the Purkinje Cell Protein 2 (PCP2) promoter [21], one of the transgenic (Tag) lines obtained were mice (TDGCre mice) expressing Cre recombinase intensively in the dentate GCs of the hippocampus (Figure 3A) and in Purkinje cells of the cerebellum, with slight expression in the thalamus (Additional file 2: Figure S2). These expression patterns became detectable postnatally after approximately 3 weeks and were sustained into adulthood. We crossed GluN2Aflox/flox mice with heterozygous TDGCre mice to generate GluN2A+/flox-TDGCre and GluN2Aflox/flox-TDGCre mice. These mice are useful for analyzing the role of GluN2A subunits in the dentate GCs, because (i) the GluN2A subunit is reported to be expressed predominantly in the adult hippocampus and granular layer of the cerebellum, but not in the Purkinje cells, and (ii) NMDAR function in the thalamus is largely dependent on GluN1/GluN2B, GluN2C, and GluN2D complexes [19].
As expected, the Cre-loxP recombination of genomic DNA was selectively induced in the brain, but not in the liver, kidney, muscle, or testis (data not shown). To confirm the switching from wild-type mRNA (N595) to mutant mRNA (595Q) in the DG, brain RNA preparations were analyzed by RT-PCR amplification, followed by digestion of amplified DNA with Nco I (common to both mRNAs) and Eco RI (specific to mutant mRNA) (Figure 3B). Digestion with Eco RI resulted in two bands in the PCR products from the DG of mutant mice, but not from other brain regions (Figure 3B), indicating dentate-specific induction of the N/Q mutation. Consistent with the fact that Cre-recombinase was expressed in nearly half of the dentate GCs in TDG-Cre mice (Figure 3A), approximately half of RT-PCR products from the DG of homozygous mice (GluN2Aflox/flox-TDGCre) were successfully digested with Eco RI (Figure 3B). Notably, we also histologically confirmed that the brains from mutant mice did not show any obvious morphological abnormalities (data not shown).
Based on the above results, we analyzed the physical and neurological features, along with the behavior, of GluN2A+/flox-TDGCre and GluN2Aflox/flox-TDGCre mice. Homozygous and heterozygous mutant mice were viable, fertile, and appeared grossly normal under standard conditions. No significant effects on physical characteristics, neurological reflexes, and pain sensitivity were observed. Because slight abnormalities in motor function (wire hanging and rotarod tests) were observed in homozygous, but not in heterozygous, mice (Table 1 and Additional file 3: Figure S3), we analyzed the physiological and behavioral functions of heterozygous mice (GluN2A+/flox-TDGCre for the mutant and GluN2A+/flox for the control) in detail.
Table 1
General physical characteristics and sensory-motor functions
 
Controls
Mutants
p value
Physical Characteristics
Age (week)
10~13
10~13
 
Weight (g) (10~13w)
25.8 (±0.3)
24.7 (±0.3)
0.0153
Weight (g) (14~17w)
30.0 (±0.4)
28.8 (±0.5)
0.244
Whiskers (% with)
79
84
 
Fur (% with normal fur)
100
100
 
Rectal temperature (°C)
36.5 (±0.2)
36.2 (±0.2)
0.2697
Sensory Motor Reflex
Ear twitch (% with quick response)
89%
95%
 
Whisker twitch (% with normal response)
100%
95%
 
Righting reflex (% with normal response)
100%
100%
 
Motor tests
Wire hang (latency to fall: sec)
55 (±2)
54 (±2)
0.7684
Data represent the mean ± S.E.M. (mutant: n = 19, control: n = 19).

Reduction of voltage-dependent Mg2+ block of NMDA excitatory postsynaptic currents in the dentate GCs

To verify the incorporation of the mutant GluN2A subunit into the functional NMDAR in the DG, we recorded excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) mediated by NMDAR in the dentate GCs. The medial perforant path (MPP) input was activated, and the current–voltage relationship of pharmacologically isolated NMDA EPSCs was examined. While NMDA EPSCs showed the typical J-shaped voltage dependence in the control and mutant mice, the relative EPSC amplitudes at −60 mV and −80 mV were significantly larger in the mutant mice compared to the control mice (Figure 4A and B). Thus, as expected, the voltage-dependent Mg2+ block of synaptic NMDARs was reduced in the GCs of the mutant mice. The reduction of the Mg2+ block was apparent in approximately half of the recordings (Figure 4C), which correlates with the expression ratio of Cre-recombinase in the GCs shown in Figure 3A.
Next, we recorded field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) to examine synaptic plasticity at the MPP input in standard physiological saline. The input–output relationship of fEPSPs was indistinguishable between control and mutant mice (Figure 4D). Paired-pulse stimulation induced the depression of fEPSPs, and the depression ratio was indistinguishable between these mice (0.659 ± 0.016, n = 15 in controls; 0.659 ± 0.019, n = 15 in mutants). Thus, basal synaptic transmission, largely mediated by the AMPA receptor, is apparently not affected in mutant mice. LTP in the DG requires activation of NMDARs and is often difficult to induce in in vitro slices with intact synaptic inhibition [22, 23]. In our experimental conditions, tetanic stimulation at the test intensity failed to induce lasting potentiation and caused reversible short-term potentiation (STP) in both control and mutant mice. This STP was significantly larger in the mutant mice (Figure 4E). Whereas the tetanic stimulation at the increased stimulus intensity failed to induce LTP in the control mice, it consistently induced LTP in the mutant mice (Figure 4F). These results indicate that the induction of both STP and LTP is facilitated in the DG of the mutants.

Hippocampal place representation in the mutant mice

To test the role of the Mg2+ block of DG NMDAR in hippocampal computational function, we analyzed place cell activity in the hippocampus. Place cells are neurons whose firing rate increases when an animal is in a specific location in a given space [14]. Their activity is thought to be involved in spatial navigation. We recorded hippocampal CA1 neurons, the major output of the hippocampal circuit. Mice were allowed to move freely in an open, white, round-cornered square box for a 15-min habituation session once daily (Figure 5A). After at least 7 days of habituation, CA1 place cell activity was recorded in the same box for 15 min (Figure 5B). We recorded the activity of 93 CA1 pyramidal cells and seven interneurons from control mice (n = 8) and 31 CA1 pyramidal cells and six interneurons from mutant mice (n = 5) under freely moving conditions. We first measured the basic firing properties of pyramidal cells and interneurons during exploratory behavior. As shown in Table 2, the mean firing rate, firing rate variability (measured by the Fano factor), and spike shape of the cell types were not significantly different. These data show that CA1 cells from mutants have similar firing properties to those from control mice.
Table 2
Basic firing property of CA1 cells
 
Controls
Mutants
p value
Pyramidal cells
Mean firing rate (Hz)
1.22 ± 0.14
1.16 ± 0.21
0.76
Spike width (μs)
691 ± 64.4
685 ± 69.4
0.31
Fano factor (100ms bin)
1.54 ± 0.052
1.44 ± 0.67
0.67
Interneurons
Mean firing rate (Hz)
22.7 ± 9.4
29.8 ± 17.5
0.63
Spike width (μs)
236 ± 45.2
246 ± 41.6
0.69
Data represent the mean ± S.E.M. (mutant: n = 93 cells, control: n = 31 cells). Significance evaluated by the Wilcoxon rank sum test.
We then assessed the place cell properties of CA1 pyramidal cells in mutant mice. As shown in Figure 5C and Additional file 4: Figure S4, CA1 pyramidal cells in the mutant mice showed robust place-specific discharge. Therefore, the DG-restricted NMDAR mutation did not interfere with place field formation by CA1 cells. To quantitatively compare the specificity of the place fields, we measured the size of the place fields. As shown in Figure 5E, CA1 cells in the mutant mice exhibited no significant change in the size of the firing field. However, we found a difference in the properties of the place fields. In both controls and mutants, many CA1 cells have more than one firing field (Figure 5C and Additional file 4: Figure S4). To explore the properties of the place fields in more detail, we compared the size of the largest firing field (main place field) and the number of firing fields. We found that mutants have a significantly larger main firing field (Figure 5F) and the total number of firing fields was smaller (3.6 ± 0.18 in controls; 2.4 ± 0.20 in mutants, p = 0.00052, Wilcoxon). Measurement of the mean running speed revealed no differences between genotypes (5.89 ± 0.91 cm/s for controls, 5.82 ± 0.02 cm/s for mutants, p = 0.84, Wilcoxon). Therefore, although running speed can influence place cell activity [24], it fails to explain the observed differences in place cell properties.

Place fields in the mutant mice were more stable

Because NMDARs have been shown to regulate place field stability [25], one possible mechanism for the larger main place field and the smaller number of firing fields in the mutants is that the temporal fluctuations in the place fields are diminished by the mutation. To test this, we used the Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient for two place fields recorded successively in the same environment (Figure 5D, left). Place fields are known to exhibit fluctuations even when the animal is in the same environment [26], and in particular, larger fluctuations are observed in the freely-moving condition than in food-seeking [27]. Place fields of the mutant mice showed higher correlations than control mice (p = 0.00063, Wilcoxon) (Figure 5G), indicating that the mutants have more stable place fields. In contrast, the shift in the place fields between the two recordings (Figure 5D, right), another measure of similarity between two place fields, exhibited no detectable differences (Figure 5H). These two measures have different properties: place field shift (Figure 5H) is only affected by changes in the high-firing-rate field, whereas the correlations for the place fields (Figure 5G) evaluate the differences between entire place field maps. Therefore, the latter is more sensitive to small changes in place fields. Taking into account the different characteristic of these two measures, the mutation does not affect the persistence of place receptive fields, but reduces small fluctuations in the place fields.

Impaired separation of place representations in two different environments

The DG has long been proposed to perform pattern separation, an ability to distinguish between similar input patterns. Therefore, we tested the pattern separation of hippocampal place representation by comparing the place fields of two visually similar but distinct environments. The two testing environments shared the same recording room and box, but the local cue (a paper towel) was rotated 90 degrees clockwise (Figure 6). Place fields were first recorded in the ‘original cue’ condition and then recorded in the ‘rotated cue’ condition. During cue translocation, mice were allowed to rest in their home cage (~1 min). In control mice, most of the cells changed their place fields after local cue rotation (Figure 6, upper and Additional file 5: Figure S5A, upper), which is consistent with previous results in mice [27, 28]. In contrast, as shown in cross correlation histograms (Figure 6, lower-left and Additional file 5: Figure S5 A, lower) and place field shift histograms (Figure 6, lower-right), more than half of the mutant cells were insensitive to local cue rotation. Statistical analyses confirmed that the place fields of the mutant mice in the two environments were more similar than those of the control mice (cross correlation; p = 0.000077, Wilcoxon, and place field shift; p = 0.0072, Wilcoxon). We used a distal cue-centric coordinate frame for this analysis, because correlation coefficients for local cue-centric coordinate frames were significantly lower than those for distal cue (recording room, door, etc)-centric coordinates in both controls and mutants (Additional file 5: Figure S5B and Additional file 6: Figure S6). These results indicate that place representation in the CA1 of the mutant mice is less sensitive to environmental differences. Another possible interpretation is that attention to local cues was only decreased in the mutants. However, this is unlikely because no significant difference was detected in the correlation coefficient for local cue-centric coordinates between controls and mutants (Additional file 6: Figure S6).

Learning deficit in the mutant mice

We showed that place representations in the mutants were more stable and less sensitive to environmental differences compared with those of the controls (Figures 5 and 6). To test whether the mutation affects hippocampal-related behavior, we examined the spatial working memory of the mutants. In the eight-arm radial maze test, mutants showed spatial learning deficits in the initial phase of training, but they acquired normal performance in the late phase (Figure 7). This result indicates that the mutants have a slight but significant defect in the performance of a task requiring spatial working memory.

Adult neurogenesis in the mutant mice

Adult neurogenesis in the DG [29] has been shown to contribute to pattern separation [3032], and NMDARs are involved in the survival of newborn neurons in the DG [33]. This raised the possibility that the NMDAR mutation might affect the number of adult-born dentate granule cells. To test this, we counted the number of neurons newly produced in the subgranular zone (SGZ) of the DG. There was no detectable difference in numbers of BrdU-positive cells in the SGZ and hilus between the control and mutant mice at 24 h (Additional file 7: Figure S7) and 28 days (data not shown) after BrdU administration. Therefore, impaired pattern separation in our mutant mice was unlikely due to changes in adult neurogenesis.

Discussion

Studies of gene function using gene-manipulation techniques have allowed many breakthroughs in the biological sciences, including neuroscience. Here we introduced a mutation into the GluN2A gene by applying regulatory splicing mechanisms, in conjunction with Cre-loxP recombination, to elucidate the function of NMDARs in the DG of the hippocampus. Our system is widely applicable and has great advantages for introducing desired mutations into genes of interest in a spatiotemporally restricted manner, without altering the level and pattern of the expression of the relevant gene. Thus, this method is expected to open up new possibilities for studying the physiological functions of critical amino acid residue(s) in target genes in vivo.
NMDARs are predicted to be tetramers composed of two GluN1 and two GluN2 subunits [34]. We estimated the composition of NMDARs in the heterozygous mutant mice according to Seeburg’s prediction [35]. Twenty-five percent should be WT receptors (2 × GluN1-2 × GluN2AN) with normal Ca2+ conductance and voltage-dependent Mg2+ block. Fifty percent should be hybrid receptors comprised of one WT and one mutated subunit (2 × GluN1-GluN2AN-GluN2AQ), and the remaining 25% would be purely mutant receptors containing two mutated subunits (2 × GluN1-2 × GluN2AQ) (Additional file 8: Figure S8). Therefore, 75% of GluN1-GluN2A receptors in a single Cre-expressing cell may contain mutant GluN2A subunits, and such mutations should be expressed in nearly half of the GCs in the DG as described above (Figure 3A). Because the Mg2+ block of NMDA EPSCs was altered as predicted, our genetic manipulation indeed modified synaptic NMDARs in the GCs (Figure 4). Previous studies have analysed the effects of modulating the Mg2+ block. Overexpression of Mg2+ block-deficient NMDARs has been shown to impair long-term memory in Drosophila[7]. In addition, elevating extracellular Mg2+ levels leads to an increase in LTP in cultured hippocampal neurons [6, 36] or in the CA1 region of hippocampal slices from rats receiving magnesium supplements [6, 36] and improves learning and memory [6, 36]. The present study demonstrates that reduced Mg2+ block facilitates STP and LTP at the PP-DG synapse in mouse hippocampal slices, which is apparently contradictory to previous observations in rats. Intriguingly, the mutation increased NMDAR currents only at −60 to −80 mV range (Figure 4C). This suggests that facilitated synaptic potentiation is likely due to this enhancement of NMDAR currents near the resting potential, but not at the potential range for coincidence detection (> −30 mV). One possible mechanism for this is that the enhanced NMDAR currents in the basal condition promote the tetanus-induced activation of intracellular signaling pathways required for synaptic potentiation, possibly via enhanced Ca2+ influx. Another possibility is that the increased NMDAR current enhances depolarization during tetanic stimulation, thereby leading to the facilitation of synaptic potentiation.
To explore the role of the Mg2+ block of NMDARs in hippocampal information processing, we analyzed the place cell activity of hippocampal CA1 cells. In spite of the significant change in electrophysiological properties in the dentate GCs (Figure 4), mutant mice exhibited normal place-specific discharges (Figure 5C and Additional file 4: Figure S4). The CA1 receives synaptic input directly from the entorhinal cortex (EC), as well as indirectly from the EC through the DG, and previous lesion experiments have shown that the direct EC-CA1 pathway is sufficient for forming the CA1 place field [37]. In the mutant mice, mutant GluN2A was not expressed in the EC (Additional file 2: Figure S2), thus the direct EC-CA1 pathway remains intact. This may explain why the DG-restricted mutation does not affect place field formation in the CA1. Despite normal place field activity in the mutants, the number of recorded cells was smaller than that of controls (11.6 cells/animal for controls, and 6.2 cells/animal for mutants). This raised the possibility that the ratio of active cells to entire cells in a given environment was lower in the mutants. Because a much larger portion of hippocampal neurons are active during sleep and anesthesia than in the awake exploring animal [38], comparing activity during sleep and exploring would provide information on the population of cells active during exploration. However, we unfortunately did not record activity during rest/sleep in the present study, so we are unable to estimate the proportion of behaviorally active cells. Therefore, we cannot rule out the possibility that the mutation decreased the population of cells active during exploration.
Although the place fields of the mutants appeared normal, we found some differences in the dynamic properties of the place fields. Specifically, the CA1 place fields in the mutants showed reduced fluctuations (Figure 5G), and the place fields for the two different environments are more similar in the mutants than in the controls (Figure 6). The DG has been considered a site of pattern separation [39], and the latter result presented here is consistent with this hypothesis. McHugh et al. (2007) have also shown impaired pattern separation in animals lacking functional NMDARs in the DG [40]. It is interesting to note that both the hyperfunction of NMDARs due to the loss of the Mg2+ block (Figure 4) and the loss of NMDARs in the DG [40] can cause impairments in pattern separation. This raises the possibility that there exist a range of NMDAR activity optimal for the function of pattern separation.
The expression pattern of Cre in the DG (Figure 3A) and the results of the in vitro electrophysiological experiment (Figure 4C) demonstrate that the GCs of the mutants were mosaic for mutated GluN2A expression. However, the activity of each CA1 cell is governed by a large number of GCs: each CA3 cell receives contacts from approximately 50 GCs, and a single CA1 neuron receives inputs from more than 5000 CA3 pyramidal cells [41]. Therefore, the effect of this mosaicism in the mutants would be averaged out on CA1 cells.
Although NMDARs have classically been considered not to be functional in cerebellar Purkinje cells [42, 43], a recent report suggests that the NMDARs in the cerebellar Purkinje cells of adult rodents contribute to plasticity at the synapses between climbing fibers and Purkinje cells [44]. In our mutant mice, Cre/loxP recombination was observed in the cerebellum (Figure 2A), and slight motor abnormalities were found in homozygous mice (see Results), suggesting the possibility that mutated GluN2A in the cerebellum may contribute to the mutant phenotype even in heterozygotes. The cerebellum is known to process self-motion signals [45, 46], and a recent report showed that a transgenic mouse line with impaired protein kinase C-dependent plasticity at parallel fiber–Purkinje cell synapses has impaired hippocampal place field activity under specific circumstances, i.e., when animals rely on self-motion cues, or when there is conflict between visual cues and self-motion cues [47]. This report indicated a role for the cerebellum in processing self-motion signals for shaping hippocampal place field activity. In our place cell experiment, visual cues were always presented, and there was no conflict between visual cues and self-motion cues in the recording sessions (Figures 5 and 6). Therefore, it seems unlikely that mutated GluN2A in the cerebellar Purkinje cells is involved in the place cell abnormalities described here, but we cannot rule out the possibility that NMDARs in cerebellar Purkinje cells contribute to place cell activity during visually guided locomotion.
How does the mutation cause these changes in place cell properties? When the animal is exposed to a different environment, spatial maps change completely (“remapping”) [48]. Each spatial map is thought to be an attractor state in the recurrent network, and possibly resides in the CA3 [49, 50]. Recurrent networks can have a finite number of stable firing patterns, and they tend to move to and settle into these stable firing patterns. In support of this idea, the activity of the CA1 depends upon the output of the CA3, and has been shown to change abruptly when the input changes gradually [16]. In such a discrete output system, a small input difference is usually ignored in nature. However, the addition of an appropriate amount of noise to the input of the system can assist in state transitions that result from subthreshold differences in input. This noise-assisted enhancement of input sensitivity is called stochastic resonance and has been found in several sensory systems [51, 52]. Interestingly, hippocampal place fields exhibit large fluctuations even when the animal remains in the same environment [26], and these fluctuations are not due to random variation, but can be interpreted as switching between multiple spatial maps [53]. This raises the possibility that place field fluctuations serve to output different patterns in response to small input differences. Therefore, if the mutants have difficulty switching between spatial maps, their place fields would be stable and pattern separation would be impaired. If so, the next question is how a reduction in the Mg2+ block in the mutants impairs spatial map switching. A previous theoretical study has suggested that large NMDA EPSCs and the facilitated STP and LTP observed in our mutant mice can inhibit attractor state switching [54]. The CA3 has been regarded as the primary attractor network in the hippocampus because of its extensive recurrent collaterals [50]. Feedback connections from hilar mossy cells to the dentate GCs are another candidate for the attractor network in the hippocampus. Therefore, NMDARs in the dentate GCs could modulate attractor network properties [55]. Using neural network simulations, Rolls et al. 2008 reported that an increase in NMDAR conductance stabilizes attractor states and makes it more difficult to switch to another state in response to a new external stimulus. Thus, the reduced Mg2+ block in the GCs of our mutant mice may stabilize the spatial map by increasing NMDAR conductance.
The DG is one of the two brain regions where adult neurogenesis occurs [29, 56]. Recently, young adult-born GCs in the DG were shown to specifically mediate pattern separation [32]. In our mutant mice, the mutated version of GluN2A was expressed in mature GCs, which constitute the majority (~95%) of total GCs (Figure 3A), but we did not investigate whether mutated GluN2A is also expressed in young GCs. Determining the cell type responsible for abnormalities in place cell properties in the mutants is a goal for future experiments.
We showed that the reduced Mg2+ block of the NMDARs in the DG stabilizes place fields and impairs pattern separation (Figures 5 and 6). The former would be advantageous to learning, but the latter would not. As stated above, if spatial map switching is the basis for both place field stability and pattern separation, better pattern separation would be inherently incompatible with high place field stability. Considering this trade-off, the spatial working memory deficit in the mutants (Figure 7) suggests that the Mg2+ block in the dentate gyrus regulates hippocampal spatial information processing to maximize learning ability.

Conclusions

Here we found that the N595Q substitution in GluN2A reduced the Mg2+ block of NMDAR-mediated synaptic currents in dentate granule cells and facilitated activity-dependent synaptic potentiation at the medial perforant path–granule cell synapses. Hippocampal place representation in the mutants was more stable than that in the controls and less sensitive to visual differences. These results imply that the enhanced synaptic potentiation resulting from the reduction of the Mg2+ block stabilizes place representation but impairs pattern separation. The mutants also showed working memory deficits, indicating that the Mg2+ block contributes to spatial learning.

Methods

Targeting construct design

Because N595 is located in exon 10 of the GluN2A gene, [57], we generated a mouse line (GluN2Aflox) harboring a tandem array of the wild-type (WT) exon 10 encoding N595 and a mutant exon 10 encoding Q595 separated by a 70-nucleotide (nt) artificial intron. This artificial intron was comprised of a 5′-donor sequence (9 nt) and a 3′-acceptor sequence, including the branch point (27 nt) from the mouse β-globin intron and loxP sequences (34 nt) that were inserted between the donor and acceptor sequences (Figure 1B). The length of the spacer elements between the splice donor site of the WT exon 10 (Figure 1B, orange box) and the branch point of the mutant exon 10 (Figure 1B, green box) was designed to be 48 nt. However, it is possible that on occasions, the 5′ normal exon will be skipped due to unforeseen mechanisms. In order to ensure that the normal exon 10 is exclusively spliced into the mRNA, the splicing acceptor element of normal exon 10 was substituted with a 73-nt Sxl-binding splicing acceptor sequence that is preferentially selected among mutually spliced exons in the absence of Sxl protein (notably, Sxl-like protein has not been found in mice) [58] (Figure 1B, violet). The Neo-TK cassette, consisting of neomycin-resistant and thymidine-kinase genes flanked by loxP sequences, was located on the 5′ side of the Sxl-binding splicing acceptor sequences (Figure 1A and B). Two-step selection was performed to obtain the desired recombinant ES clones. The targeting vector was electroporated into TT2 ES cells [59]. After G418 selection, one homologous recombinant clone (out of 399 G418-resistant clones) was identified. This clone was expanded, transfected with a Cre recombinase-expression plasmid (pIC-Cre), and further selected with gancyclovir. We confirmed that one clone out of 216 gancyclovir-resistant clones exhibited precise excision of the loxP-Neo-TK-loxP cassette by Cre-loxP recombination. This secondary recombinant ES clone was then microinjected into 8-cell embryos to generate flox homozygous (GluN2Aflox/flox) mice using standard procedures, and was confirmed by Southern blot analyses.

PCR analysis for genotyping and Cre-loxP recombination

After the fidelity of PCR genotyping for GluN2A was confirmed, subsequent genotyping performed by PCR using primers Pr137: 5′GTGGTAAAATCCAGTTAGATAG3′ and E1R: 5′GGGTTATAGAATGGATGGTTA3′. PCR conditions were as follows; denaturation at 94°C for 1 min, followed by 30 cycles of 1 min at 94°C, 1 min at 60°C, 1 min at 72°C, a final extension at 72°C for 5 min, and storage at 4°C. The floxed, wild-type and Cre-mediated recombined GluN2A genes correspond to PCR products of 807, 599 and 454 bp, respectively. For analysis of Cre-loxP recombination in various tissues by PCR, genomic DNA samples from various tissue were subjected to the same PCR condition as above, using primers CRE10: 5′CAACGAGTGATGAGGTTCGCAA3′ and CRE13: 5′CCCCAGAAATGCCAGATTACGT3′.

RT-PCR analysis

The GluN2A cDNA fragments were amplified using total RNA by RT-PCR with primers mE1F: 5′ATCAACGAGCAGTTATGGCC3′ and mE1R: 5′GTCATGAGGTCTCTGGAACT3′. The amplified DNA fragment of 521 bp was digested by EcoRI or NcoI.

Generation of transgenic mice

Nestin-Cre and TDG-Cre transgenic lines were generated. The Nestin-Cre mice were produced carrying tandem arrays of the Cre-recombinase gene driven by the rat nestin promoter, the internal ribosome entry sites (IRES) sequence, lacZ gene, and a neuron specific enhancer [60]. In TDG-Cre we used a Purkinje Cell Protein 2 (PCP2) promoter to drive the expression of Cre-recombinase. We used a transgenic strain, CAG-CAT-Z, as a reporter line [61].

Neurological screen

A neurological screen was conducted as previously described [62]. The righting, whisker touch, and ear twitch reflexes were evaluated. A number of physical features were also recorded, including the presence of whiskers or bald patches in the hair.

Wire hanging test

Neuromuscular strength was tested with the wire hanging test. The mouse was placed on a wire mesh and the mesh was then inverted, to force the animals to grip the wire. Latency to fall was recorded, with a 60 sec cut-off time.

Rotarod test

Motor coordination and balance were tested using an accelerating rotarod (UGO Basile Accelerating Rotarod). The test was performed by placing a mouse on a rotating drum (3 cm diameter) and the time each animal was able to maintain its balance on the rod was measured. The speed of the rotarod accelerated from 4 to 40 rpm over a 5-min period.

Hot plate test

The hot plate test was used to evaluate sensitivity to a painful stimulus. Mice were placed on a 55.0 (±0.3) °C hot plate (Columbus Instruments, Columbus, Ohio), and latency to the first hind-paw response was recorded. The hind-paw response was either a foot shake or a paw lick.

Slice electrophysiology

Adult male mutant and control mice (14–17 weeks old) were used. Mice were decapitated under halothane anesthesia and both hippocampi were isolated. Transverse hippocampal slices (370–400 μm) were obtained using a tissue slicer (VT1000S; Leica Microsystems, Nussloch, Germany or Vibratome 3000 plus; Vibratome Company, St. Louis, MO, USA) in sucrose-containing saline composed of (in mM): sucrose, 72; NaCl, 80; KCl, 2.5; NaH2PO4, 1.0; NaHCO3, 26.2; glucose, 20; CaCl2, 0.5; and MgCl2, 7 (equilibrated with 95% O2/5% CO2) for whole-cell recordings or in standard saline (see below) for field potential recordings. Slices were incubated for 30 min at 30°C in a standard saline solution composed of (in mM): NaCl, 125; KCl, 2.5; NaH2PO4, 1.0; NaHCO3, 26.2; glucose, 11; CaCl2, 2.5; and MgCl2, 1.3 (equilibrated with 95% O2/5% CO2) and maintained in a humidified interface holding chamber at room temperature (24–27°C) before use. Electrophysiological recordings were made from slices placed in a submersion-type chamber superfused at 2 mL/min. The bath temperature was maintained at 26.5–27.5°C using an automated temperature controller.
Whole-cell recordings were made from GCs in the DG using a blind whole-cell patch-clamp technique. Voltage-clamp recordings were made with a pipette filled with a solution composed of (in mM) CsCl, 140; HEPES, 20; NaCl, 8; MgATP, 2; Na2GTP, 0.3; EGTA, 0.5; and CaCl2, 0.05 (pH adjusted to 7.3 with CsOH). The recording pipette was placed in the middle third of the GC layer. Synaptic currents were evoked at 0.2 Hz by bipolar tungsten stimulating electrodes that were placed in the middle third of the dentate molecular layer to activate the input from the medial perforant path (MPP). Activation of MPP input was verified by the depression of synaptic currents in response to paired stimuli. EPSCs mediated by NMDARs were recorded in standard saline solution supplemented with 6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (CNQX, 20 μM), picrotoxin (100 μM), and SR95531 (2 μM). EPSC amplitudes were measured on analysis. Series resistance was monitored during the recordings, and data deviating by more than 15% were excluded. fEPSPs at the MPP synapse were recorded in the middle third of the molecular layer using a glass pipette filled with 2 M NaCl. The MPP was stimulated at 0.05 Hz in standard saline. The initial slopes of fEPSPs were measured.
All recordings were done using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Axon Instruments, Union City, CA, USA), filtered at 2–5 kHz, and stored in a personal computer via an interface (digitized at 10 kHz). CNQX and d-2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid (d-APV) were purchased from Tocris (Bristol, UK). Picrotoxin was from Wako Pure Chemical Industries, Ltd. (Osaka, Japan). SR95531 was from Sigma (St. Louis, MO, USA). All values are expressed as the mean ± S.E.M. Statistical significance was evaluated using two-tailed Mann–Whitney tests, Student’s t-tests, or ANOVA.

In vivo electrophysiology

All procedures were approved by our Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Adult male mutant and control mice (14–21 weeks old) were used. The control mice consisted of six WT mice (GluN2A+/+) and two floxed mice (GluN2A+/flox). We did not detect any significant differences in firing properties and place field measurements between the WT and floxed animals.
Animals were chronically implanted with a miniature microdrive equipped with four electrodes (Neuralynx, Tucson, AZ). Each electrode was composed of four individually insulated tungsten wires (12.7 μm diameter) that were twisted together. The impedance of the electrodes was approximately 400 kΩ at 1 kHz. Animals were anesthetized with a mixture of 0.9% ketamine and 0.2% xylazine. The skull was exposed, and four stainless screws were inserted into the bone for structural support. A small hole (<0.5 mm) was made over the left hemisphere (1.5 mm lateral to the midline, 2.0 mm posterior to the bregma), and the electrodes were inserted through the hole. The electrode tips were positioned 0.9 mm deep from the brain surface. The space between the bone and the microdrive was filled with petroleum jelly, and the microdrive and the bone were glued using dental acrylic.
The electrodes were connected to a unity gain buffer consisting of an operational amplifier (TL074; Texas Instruments, Dallas, TX). Signals from the buffer amplifier were amplified (×3000), and filtered between 300 Hz and 5000 Hz. Amplified signals were sampled by a data acquisition card (PCI-6259; National Instruments, Austin, TX) at 20 kHz. The location of the animal was tracked using an overhead video camera, which tracks a pair of infrared LEDs placed on the top of the microdrive. Neural waveforms and video images were processed and stored using custom software written in LABVIEW7 (National Instruments, Austin, TX).
At least one week before surgery, animals were handled daily and allowed to explore the testing platform (a white, round-cornered square box, 40 × 40 cm, 30 cm high) for 15 min each day. The box was housed in a 180 × 120 cm soundproof room. A brown paper towel was hung on the wall of the box as a local cue. Four incandescent bulbs in 15-cm hemispherical reflectors were mounted in the corners of the room (Figure 5A). One week after surgery, electrodes were screened daily for complex spike cells. Complex spike cells were identified as those that had both a wide peak-to-trough width (>0.3 ms) and complex spike bursts. If no complex spike cell was found, electrodes were lowered by 40 μm and were screened the next day until complex spike cells were found. The recording session began only if complex spike cells persisted for at least 1 day. The animals were exposed to both cue configurations (‘orig’ and ‘rot’) for 15 min each during the cell screening period (11–19 days, depending on individuals) and recording sessions (5 days). The complex spiking cells recorded in the CA1 appear to be pyramidal cells [63, 64]; thus, we designated the complex spiking cells in the CA1 as pyramidal cells. The data were excluded if the mouse crossed less than 90% of the platform in a single session or the mean firing rates were below 0.1 Hz. At the end of the recordings, marker lesions were made at the electrode tips by passing a current (20 μA, 10 s). The location of a recording site was determined by estimating the distance along the electrode track associated with the microelectrode position at the time of recording.

Spike data analyses

Recorded neural waveforms were digitally high-pass filtered above 800 Hz. Spikes with amplitudes 3.5 times greater than noise were extracted for further analyses. The spikes were segregated into multiple single units using automatic clustering software Klustakwik [65] and manual cluster cutting software Klusters [66]. The presence of a refractory period in isolated units was inspected visually by using Klusters. Units with a clear refractory period (>2 ms) in their autocorrelograms were included in the present analysis. The firing rate maps of the complex spike cells were constructed by finding the spike number at each location bin (2 cm × 2 cm) divided by the dwell time in the bin. To eliminate nonspecific firing at rest, spikes occurring when the animal's velocity was less than 1 cm/s were filtered out [28]. The maps were smoothed using a Gaussian spatial and temporal filter with a standard deviation of 1 pixel (2 cm) and 2.5 s, respectively. A place field was defined as a group of at least one contiguous pixel in which the firing rates exceeded the overall mean firing rate plus one standard deviation. “Main place field” was defined as the largest place field for each cell. Signal processing and statistical analyses were performed using MATLAB 6.5 (Mathworks, Natick, MA) and GraphPad Prism5 (GraphPad Software, La Jolla, CA).

Eight-arm radial maze test

All procedures were approved by our Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Adult male mutant and control mice (10–13 or 17–21 weeks old) were used. Mice were housed in a room with a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 a.m.) with free access to food and water. Behavioral testing was performed between 9:00 a.m. and 6:00 p.m. After the tests, all apparatus were cleaned with super hypochlorous water to remove the scent of mice. The eight-arm radial maze test was conducted in a manner similar to that described previously [62]. The floor of the maze consisted of white plexiglass, and the wall (25 cm high) consisted of transparent plexiglass. Each arm (9 × 40 cm) radiated from an octagonal central starting platform (perimeter 12 × 8 cm) like the spokes of a wheel. Identical food wells (1.4 cm deep and 1.4 cm in diameter) with pellet sensors were placed at the distal end of each arm. The pellet sensors were able to automatically record pellet intake by the mice. The maze was elevated 75 cm above the floor and placed in a dimly lit room with several extra-maze cues. During the experiment, the maze was maintained in a constant orientation. One week before pre-training, animals were deprived of food until their body weight was reduced to 80–85% of the initial level. Pre-training started on day 8. Each mouse was placed in the central starting platform and allowed to explore and consume food pellets scattered on the whole maze for a 5-min period (one session per mouse). After completion of the initial pre-training, mice received another pre-training to take a pellet from each food well after being placed at the distal end of each arm. A trial was finished after the subject consumed the pellet. This series of experiments was repeated eight times, using eight different arms, for each mouse. After these pre-training trials, the actual maze acquisition trials were performed. All eight arms were baited with food pellets. Mice were placed on the central platform and allowed to find all eight pellets within 25 min. A trial was terminated immediately after all eight pellets were consumed or 25 min had elapsed. An “arm visit” was defined as traveling more than 5 cm from the central platform. The mice were confined in the center platform for 5 s after each arm choice. The animals went through one trial per day (34 trials in total). For each trial, choices of arms, latency to get all pellets, distance traveled, number of different arms chosen within the first eight choices, the number of revisiting, and omission errors were automatically recorded. Data acquisition, control of guillotine doors, and data analyses were performed using Image RM software written by Tsuyoshi Miyakawa (available through O’Hara & Co., Tokyo, Japan). The software is based on NIH Image (http://​rsb.​info.​nih.​gov/​nih-image/​). Statistical analyses were conducted using StatView (SAS Institute).

BrdU labeling and immunohistochemistry

To assess the effects of GluN2A mutations on the number of BrdU-positive cells, mice (18 weeks old) were administered BrdU, 4 × 75 mg/kg i.p., dissolved in saline, every 2 h. Mice were sacrificed 24 h after the last BrdU injection. After anesthesia, mice were transcardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde and brains were collected for immunohistochemistry. All brains were post-fixed overnight in 4% paraformaldehyde at 4°C and soaked in a series of 10%, 20% and 30% sucrose at 4°C. Serial sections of the brains (30 μm sections) were cut through the entire hippocampus on a cryostat. DNA denaturation was conducted by incubation for 2 h in 50% formamide/2× SSC at 65°C, followed by several rinses. Sections were then incubated for 30 min in 2 N HCl and then for 10 min in boric acid. After washing with PBS, sections were incubated in 3% H2O2 for 30 min to eliminate endogenous peroxidases. After blocking with 3% normal goat serum in 0.01% Triton X-100, cells were incubated with anti-mouse BrdU (1:100; Becton Dickinson) overnight at 4°C. Sections were then incubated for 1 h with secondary antibody (1:1000; biotinylated goat anti-mouse; Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) followed by amplification using an avidin-biotin complex (Vector Laboratories) and visualization with DAB (Vector Laboratories).

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by CREST, Japan Science and Technology Agency.
This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Authors’ contributions

YN and Y-IN developed the gene-manipulation method. YN, EE, SN, YM, TS, TN, JM, MM and Y-IN generated the knock-in mice. YH and KF performed in vivo electrophysiology experiments. KK and HS performed slice electrophysiology experiments. TM, KoT and KeT performed behavioral experiments. YH, YN, KK, TM and Y-IN designed and wrote the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Jetzt e.Med zum Sonderpreis bestellen!

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Jetzt bestellen und 100 € sparen!

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Neurologie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes, den Premium-Inhalten der neurologischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Neurologie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Anhänge

Electronic supplementary material

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Nakanishi S, Nakajima Y, Masu M, Ueda Y, Nakahara K, Watanabe D, Yamaguchi S, Kawabata S, Okada M: Glutamate receptors: brain function and signal transduction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1998, 26: 230-235. 10.1016/S0165-0173(97)00033-7.PubMedCrossRef Nakanishi S, Nakajima Y, Masu M, Ueda Y, Nakahara K, Watanabe D, Yamaguchi S, Kawabata S, Okada M: Glutamate receptors: brain function and signal transduction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1998, 26: 230-235. 10.1016/S0165-0173(97)00033-7.PubMedCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Mayer ML, Westbrook GL, Guthrie PB: Voltage-dependent block by Mg2+ of NMDA responses in spinal cord neurones. Nature. 1984, 309: 261-263. 10.1038/309261a0.PubMedCrossRef Mayer ML, Westbrook GL, Guthrie PB: Voltage-dependent block by Mg2+ of NMDA responses in spinal cord neurones. Nature. 1984, 309: 261-263. 10.1038/309261a0.PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Nowak L, Bregestovski P, Ascher P, Herbet A, Prochiantz A: Magnesium gates glutamate-activated channels in mouse central neurones. Nature. 1984, 307: 462-465. 10.1038/307462a0.PubMedCrossRef Nowak L, Bregestovski P, Ascher P, Herbet A, Prochiantz A: Magnesium gates glutamate-activated channels in mouse central neurones. Nature. 1984, 307: 462-465. 10.1038/307462a0.PubMedCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Bliss TV, Collingridge GL: A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature. 1993, 361: 31-39. 10.1038/361031a0.PubMedCrossRef Bliss TV, Collingridge GL: A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature. 1993, 361: 31-39. 10.1038/361031a0.PubMedCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Rezvani AH: Involvement of the NMDA System in Learning and Memory. Animal Models of Cognitive Impairment. Edited by: Levin ED, Buccafusco JJ. 2006, Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press Rezvani AH: Involvement of the NMDA System in Learning and Memory. Animal Models of Cognitive Impairment. Edited by: Levin ED, Buccafusco JJ. 2006, Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Slutsky I, Abumaria N, Wu LJ, Huang C, Zhang L, Li B, Zhao X, Govindarajan A, Zhao MG, Zhuo M, Tonegawa S, Liu G: Enhancement of learning and memory by elevating brain magnesium. Neuron. 2010, 65: 165-177. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.026.PubMedCrossRef Slutsky I, Abumaria N, Wu LJ, Huang C, Zhang L, Li B, Zhao X, Govindarajan A, Zhao MG, Zhuo M, Tonegawa S, Liu G: Enhancement of learning and memory by elevating brain magnesium. Neuron. 2010, 65: 165-177. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.026.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Miyashita T, Oda Y, Horiuchi J, Yin JC, Morimoto T, Saitoe M: Mg(2+) block of Drosophila NMDA receptors is required for long-term memory formation and CREB-dependent gene expression. Neuron. 2012, 74: 887-898. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.039.PubMedCrossRef Miyashita T, Oda Y, Horiuchi J, Yin JC, Morimoto T, Saitoe M: Mg(2+) block of Drosophila NMDA receptors is required for long-term memory formation and CREB-dependent gene expression. Neuron. 2012, 74: 887-898. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.039.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Single FN, Rozov A, Burnashev N, Zimmermann F, Hanley DF, Forrest D, Curran T, Jensen V, Hvalby O, Sprengel R, Seeburg PH: Dysfunctions in mice by NMDA receptor point mutations NR1(N598Q) and NR1(N598R). J Neurosci. 2000, 20: 2558-2566.PubMed Single FN, Rozov A, Burnashev N, Zimmermann F, Hanley DF, Forrest D, Curran T, Jensen V, Hvalby O, Sprengel R, Seeburg PH: Dysfunctions in mice by NMDA receptor point mutations NR1(N598Q) and NR1(N598R). J Neurosci. 2000, 20: 2558-2566.PubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Burnashev N, Schoepfer R, Monyer H, Ruppersberg JP, Gunther W, Seeburg PH, Sakmann B: Control by asparagine residues of calcium permeability and magnesium blockade in the NMDA receptor. Science. 1992, 257: 1415-1419. 10.1126/science.1382314.PubMedCrossRef Burnashev N, Schoepfer R, Monyer H, Ruppersberg JP, Gunther W, Seeburg PH, Sakmann B: Control by asparagine residues of calcium permeability and magnesium blockade in the NMDA receptor. Science. 1992, 257: 1415-1419. 10.1126/science.1382314.PubMedCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Mori H, Masaki H, Yamakura T, Mishina M: Identification by mutagenesis of a Mg(2+)-block site of the NMDA receptor channel. Nature. 1992, 358: 673-675. 10.1038/358673a0.PubMedCrossRef Mori H, Masaki H, Yamakura T, Mishina M: Identification by mutagenesis of a Mg(2+)-block site of the NMDA receptor channel. Nature. 1992, 358: 673-675. 10.1038/358673a0.PubMedCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Scoville WB, Milner B: Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1957, 20: 11-21. 10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Scoville WB, Milner B: Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1957, 20: 11-21. 10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Morris RG, Garrud P, Rawlins JN, O'Keefe J: Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature. 1982, 297: 681-683. 10.1038/297681a0.PubMedCrossRef Morris RG, Garrud P, Rawlins JN, O'Keefe J: Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature. 1982, 297: 681-683. 10.1038/297681a0.PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Amaral DG, Witter MP: The three-dimensional organization of the hippocampal formation: a review of anatomical data. Neuroscience. 1989, 31: 571-591. 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90424-7.PubMedCrossRef Amaral DG, Witter MP: The three-dimensional organization of the hippocampal formation: a review of anatomical data. Neuroscience. 1989, 31: 571-591. 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90424-7.PubMedCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat O'Keefe J, Dostrovsky J: The hippocampus as a spatial map: preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 1971, 34: 171-175.PubMedCrossRef O'Keefe J, Dostrovsky J: The hippocampus as a spatial map: preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res. 1971, 34: 171-175.PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat O'Keefe J, Nadel L: The hippocampus as a cognitive map. 1978, Oxford: Oxford University Press O'Keefe J, Nadel L: The hippocampus as a cognitive map. 1978, Oxford: Oxford University Press
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Wills TJ, Lever C, Cacucci F, Burgess N, O'Keefe J: Attractor dynamics in the hippocampal representation of the local environment. Science. 2005, 308: 873-876. 10.1126/science.1108905.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Wills TJ, Lever C, Cacucci F, Burgess N, O'Keefe J: Attractor dynamics in the hippocampal representation of the local environment. Science. 2005, 308: 873-876. 10.1126/science.1108905.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Nakazawa K, Quirk MC, Chitwood RA, Watanabe M, Yeckel MF, Sun LD, Kato A, Carr CA, Johnston D, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S: Requirement for hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors in associative memory recall. Science. 2002, 297: 211-218. 10.1126/science.1071795.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Nakazawa K, Quirk MC, Chitwood RA, Watanabe M, Yeckel MF, Sun LD, Kato A, Carr CA, Johnston D, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S: Requirement for hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors in associative memory recall. Science. 2002, 297: 211-218. 10.1126/science.1071795.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S, Moser MB, Moser EI: Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science. 2007, 315: 961-966. 10.1126/science.1135801.PubMedCrossRef Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S, Moser MB, Moser EI: Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science. 2007, 315: 961-966. 10.1126/science.1135801.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Monyer H, Burnashev N, Laurie DJ, Sakmann B, Seeburg PH: Developmental and regional expression in the rat brain and functional properties of four NMDA receptors. Neuron. 1994, 12: 529-540. 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90210-0.PubMedCrossRef Monyer H, Burnashev N, Laurie DJ, Sakmann B, Seeburg PH: Developmental and regional expression in the rat brain and functional properties of four NMDA receptors. Neuron. 1994, 12: 529-540. 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90210-0.PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Smith CW, Nadal-Ginard B: Mutually exclusive splicing of alpha-tropomyosin exons enforced by an unusual lariat branch point location: implications for constitutive splicing. Cell. 1989, 56: 749-758. 10.1016/0092-8674(89)90678-8.PubMedCrossRef Smith CW, Nadal-Ginard B: Mutually exclusive splicing of alpha-tropomyosin exons enforced by an unusual lariat branch point location: implications for constitutive splicing. Cell. 1989, 56: 749-758. 10.1016/0092-8674(89)90678-8.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang XM, Ng AH, Tanner JA, Wu WT, Copeland NG, Jenkins NA, Huang JD: Highly restricted expression of Cre recombinase in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Genesis. 2004, 40: 45-51. 10.1002/gene.20062.PubMedCrossRef Zhang XM, Ng AH, Tanner JA, Wu WT, Copeland NG, Jenkins NA, Huang JD: Highly restricted expression of Cre recombinase in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Genesis. 2004, 40: 45-51. 10.1002/gene.20062.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Hanse E, Gustafsson B: Long-term potentiation and field EPSPs in the lateral and medial perforant paths in the dentate gyrus in Vitro: a comparison. Eur J Neurosci. 1992, 4: 1191-1201. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1992.tb00144.x.PubMedCrossRef Hanse E, Gustafsson B: Long-term potentiation and field EPSPs in the lateral and medial perforant paths in the dentate gyrus in Vitro: a comparison. Eur J Neurosci. 1992, 4: 1191-1201. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1992.tb00144.x.PubMedCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Wigstrom H, Gustafsson B: Large long-lasting potentiation in the dentate gyrus in vitro during blockade of inhibition. Brain Res. 1983, 275: 153-158. 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90428-6.PubMedCrossRef Wigstrom H, Gustafsson B: Large long-lasting potentiation in the dentate gyrus in vitro during blockade of inhibition. Brain Res. 1983, 275: 153-158. 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90428-6.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, O'Keefe J: The contributions of position, direction, and velocity to single unit activity in the hippocampus of freely-moving rats. Exp Brain Res. 1983, 52: 41-49.PubMedCrossRef McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, O'Keefe J: The contributions of position, direction, and velocity to single unit activity in the hippocampus of freely-moving rats. Exp Brain Res. 1983, 52: 41-49.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Kentros C, Hargreaves E, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER, Shapiro M, Muller RV: Abolition of long-term stability of new hippocampal place cell maps by NMDA receptor blockade. Science. 1998, 280: 2121-2126. 10.1126/science.280.5372.2121.PubMedCrossRef Kentros C, Hargreaves E, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER, Shapiro M, Muller RV: Abolition of long-term stability of new hippocampal place cell maps by NMDA receptor blockade. Science. 1998, 280: 2121-2126. 10.1126/science.280.5372.2121.PubMedCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Fenton AA, Muller RU: Place cell discharge is extremely variable during individual passes of the rat through the firing field. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998, 95: 3182-3187. 10.1073/pnas.95.6.3182.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Fenton AA, Muller RU: Place cell discharge is extremely variable during individual passes of the rat through the firing field. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998, 95: 3182-3187. 10.1073/pnas.95.6.3182.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Kentros CG, Agnihotri NT, Streater S, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER: Increased attention to spatial context increases both place field stability and spatial memory. Neuron. 2004, 42: 283-295. 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00192-8.PubMedCrossRef Kentros CG, Agnihotri NT, Streater S, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER: Increased attention to spatial context increases both place field stability and spatial memory. Neuron. 2004, 42: 283-295. 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00192-8.PubMedCrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Cho YH, Giese KP, Tanila H, Silva AJ, Eichenbaum H: Abnormal hippocampal spatial representations in alphaCaMKIIT286A and CREBalphaDelta- mice. Science. 1998, 279: 867-869. 10.1126/science.279.5352.867.PubMedCrossRef Cho YH, Giese KP, Tanila H, Silva AJ, Eichenbaum H: Abnormal hippocampal spatial representations in alphaCaMKIIT286A and CREBalphaDelta- mice. Science. 1998, 279: 867-869. 10.1126/science.279.5352.867.PubMedCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Altman J, Das GD: Autoradiographic and histological evidence of postnatal hippocampal neurogenesis in rats. J Comp Neurol. 1965, 124: 319-335. 10.1002/cne.901240303.PubMedCrossRef Altman J, Das GD: Autoradiographic and histological evidence of postnatal hippocampal neurogenesis in rats. J Comp Neurol. 1965, 124: 319-335. 10.1002/cne.901240303.PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Clelland CD, Choi M, Romberg C, Clemenson GD, Fragniere A, Tyers P, Jessberger S, Saksida LM, Barker RA, Gage FH, Bussey TJ: A functional role for adult hippocampal neurogenesis in spatial pattern separation. Science. 2009, 325: 210-213. 10.1126/science.1173215.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Clelland CD, Choi M, Romberg C, Clemenson GD, Fragniere A, Tyers P, Jessberger S, Saksida LM, Barker RA, Gage FH, Bussey TJ: A functional role for adult hippocampal neurogenesis in spatial pattern separation. Science. 2009, 325: 210-213. 10.1126/science.1173215.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Sahay A, Scobie KN, Hill AS, O'Carroll CM, Kheirbek MA, Burghardt NS, Fenton AA, Dranovsky A, Hen R: Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve pattern separation. Nature. 2011, 472: 466-470. 10.1038/nature09817.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Sahay A, Scobie KN, Hill AS, O'Carroll CM, Kheirbek MA, Burghardt NS, Fenton AA, Dranovsky A, Hen R: Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve pattern separation. Nature. 2011, 472: 466-470. 10.1038/nature09817.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Nakashiba T, Cushman JD, Pelkey KA, Renaudineau S, Buhl DL, McHugh TJ, Rodriguez Barrera V, Chittajallu R, Iwamoto KS, McBain CJ, Fanselow MS, Tonegawa S: Young dentate granule cells mediate pattern separation, whereas old granule cells facilitate pattern completion. Cell. 2012, 149: 188-201. 10.1016/j.cell.2012.01.046.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Nakashiba T, Cushman JD, Pelkey KA, Renaudineau S, Buhl DL, McHugh TJ, Rodriguez Barrera V, Chittajallu R, Iwamoto KS, McBain CJ, Fanselow MS, Tonegawa S: Young dentate granule cells mediate pattern separation, whereas old granule cells facilitate pattern completion. Cell. 2012, 149: 188-201. 10.1016/j.cell.2012.01.046.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Tashiro A, Sandler VM, Toni N, Zhao C, Gage FH: NMDA-receptor-mediated, cell-specific integration of new neurons in adult dentate gyrus. Nature. 2006, 442: 929-933. 10.1038/nature05028.PubMedCrossRef Tashiro A, Sandler VM, Toni N, Zhao C, Gage FH: NMDA-receptor-mediated, cell-specific integration of new neurons in adult dentate gyrus. Nature. 2006, 442: 929-933. 10.1038/nature05028.PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Furukawa H, Singh SK, Mancusso R, Gouaux E: Subunit arrangement and function in NMDA receptors. Nature. 2005, 438: 185-192. 10.1038/nature04089.PubMedCrossRef Furukawa H, Singh SK, Mancusso R, Gouaux E: Subunit arrangement and function in NMDA receptors. Nature. 2005, 438: 185-192. 10.1038/nature04089.PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Seeburg PH, Single F, Kuner T, Higuchi M, Sprengel R: Genetic manipulation of key determinants of ion flow in glutamate receptor channels in the mouse. Brain Res. 2001, 907: 233-243. 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02445-3.PubMedCrossRef Seeburg PH, Single F, Kuner T, Higuchi M, Sprengel R: Genetic manipulation of key determinants of ion flow in glutamate receptor channels in the mouse. Brain Res. 2001, 907: 233-243. 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02445-3.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Slutsky I, Sadeghpour S, Li B, Liu G: Enhancement of synaptic plasticity through chronically reduced Ca2+ flux during uncorrelated activity. Neuron. 2004, 44: 835-849. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.013.PubMedCrossRef Slutsky I, Sadeghpour S, Li B, Liu G: Enhancement of synaptic plasticity through chronically reduced Ca2+ flux during uncorrelated activity. Neuron. 2004, 44: 835-849. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.013.PubMedCrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, Meltzer J, Sutherland RJ: Hippocampal granule cells are necessary for normal spatial learning but not for spatially-selective pyramidal cell discharge. Exp Brain Res. 1989, 76: 485-496. 10.1007/BF00248904.PubMedCrossRef McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, Meltzer J, Sutherland RJ: Hippocampal granule cells are necessary for normal spatial learning but not for spatially-selective pyramidal cell discharge. Exp Brain Res. 1989, 76: 485-496. 10.1007/BF00248904.PubMedCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Thompson LT, Best PJ: Place cells and silent cells in the hippocampus of freely-behaving rats. J Neurosci. 1989, 9: 2382-2390.PubMed Thompson LT, Best PJ: Place cells and silent cells in the hippocampus of freely-behaving rats. J Neurosci. 1989, 9: 2382-2390.PubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat O'Reilly RC, McClelland JL: Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: avoiding a trade-off. Hippocampus. 1994, 4: 661-682. 10.1002/hipo.450040605.PubMedCrossRef O'Reilly RC, McClelland JL: Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: avoiding a trade-off. Hippocampus. 1994, 4: 661-682. 10.1002/hipo.450040605.PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat McHugh TJ, Jones MW, Quinn JJ, Balthasar N, Coppari R, Elmquist JK, Lowell BB, Fanselow MS, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S: Dentate gyrus NMDA receptors mediate rapid pattern separation in the hippocampal network. Science. 2007, 317: 94-99. 10.1126/science.1140263.PubMedCrossRef McHugh TJ, Jones MW, Quinn JJ, Balthasar N, Coppari R, Elmquist JK, Lowell BB, Fanselow MS, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S: Dentate gyrus NMDA receptors mediate rapid pattern separation in the hippocampal network. Science. 2007, 317: 94-99. 10.1126/science.1140263.PubMedCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnston D, Amaral DG: Hippocampus. The synaptic organization of the brain. Edited by: Sheperd GM. 1998, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 417-458. Johnston D, Amaral DG: Hippocampus. The synaptic organization of the brain. Edited by: Sheperd GM. 1998, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 417-458.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Perkel DJ, Hestrin S, Sah P, Nicoll RA: Excitatory synaptic currents in Purkinje cells. Proc Biol Sci. 1990, 241: 116-121. 10.1098/rspb.1990.0074.PubMedCrossRef Perkel DJ, Hestrin S, Sah P, Nicoll RA: Excitatory synaptic currents in Purkinje cells. Proc Biol Sci. 1990, 241: 116-121. 10.1098/rspb.1990.0074.PubMedCrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Llano I, Marty A, Armstrong CM, Konnerth A: Synaptic- and agonist-induced excitatory currents of Purkinje cells in rat cerebellar slices. J Physiol. 1991, 434: 183-213.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Llano I, Marty A, Armstrong CM, Konnerth A: Synaptic- and agonist-induced excitatory currents of Purkinje cells in rat cerebellar slices. J Physiol. 1991, 434: 183-213.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Piochon C, Levenes C, Ohtsuki G, Hansel C: Purkinje cell NMDA receptors assume a key role in synaptic gain control in the mature cerebellum. J Neurosci. 2010, 30: 15330-15335. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4344-10.2010.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Piochon C, Levenes C, Ohtsuki G, Hansel C: Purkinje cell NMDA receptors assume a key role in synaptic gain control in the mature cerebellum. J Neurosci. 2010, 30: 15330-15335. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4344-10.2010.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Yakusheva TA, Shaikh AG, Green AM, Blazquez PM, Dickman JD, Angelaki DE: Purkinje cells in posterior cerebellar vermis encode motion in an inertial reference frame. Neuron. 2007, 54: 973-985. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.003.PubMedCrossRef Yakusheva TA, Shaikh AG, Green AM, Blazquez PM, Dickman JD, Angelaki DE: Purkinje cells in posterior cerebellar vermis encode motion in an inertial reference frame. Neuron. 2007, 54: 973-985. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.003.PubMedCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Angelaki DE, Yakusheva TA, Green AM, Dickman JD, Blazquez PM: Computation of egomotion in the macaque cerebellar vermis. Cerebellum. 2010, 9: 174-182. 10.1007/s12311-009-0147-z.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Angelaki DE, Yakusheva TA, Green AM, Dickman JD, Blazquez PM: Computation of egomotion in the macaque cerebellar vermis. Cerebellum. 2010, 9: 174-182. 10.1007/s12311-009-0147-z.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Rochefort C, Arabo A, Andre M, Poucet B, Save E, Rondi-Reig L: Cerebellum shapes hippocampal spatial code. Science. 2011, 334: 385-389. 10.1126/science.1207403.PubMedCrossRef Rochefort C, Arabo A, Andre M, Poucet B, Save E, Rondi-Reig L: Cerebellum shapes hippocampal spatial code. Science. 2011, 334: 385-389. 10.1126/science.1207403.PubMedCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Muller RU, Kubie JL: The effects of changes in the environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal complex-spike cells. J Neurosci. 1987, 7: 1951-1968.PubMed Muller RU, Kubie JL: The effects of changes in the environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal complex-spike cells. J Neurosci. 1987, 7: 1951-1968.PubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Treves A, Rolls ET: Computational constraints suggest the need for two distinct input systems to the hippocampal CA3 network. Hippocampus. 1992, 2: 189-199. 10.1002/hipo.450020209.PubMedCrossRef Treves A, Rolls ET: Computational constraints suggest the need for two distinct input systems to the hippocampal CA3 network. Hippocampus. 1992, 2: 189-199. 10.1002/hipo.450020209.PubMedCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Marr D: Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1971, 262: 23-81. 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078.PubMedCrossRef Marr D: Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1971, 262: 23-81. 10.1098/rstb.1971.0078.PubMedCrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Douglass JK, Wilkens L, Pantazelou E, Moss F: Noise enhancement of information transfer in crayfish mechanoreceptors by stochastic resonance. Nature. 1993, 365: 337-340. 10.1038/365337a0.PubMedCrossRef Douglass JK, Wilkens L, Pantazelou E, Moss F: Noise enhancement of information transfer in crayfish mechanoreceptors by stochastic resonance. Nature. 1993, 365: 337-340. 10.1038/365337a0.PubMedCrossRef
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Moss F, Ward LM, Sannita WG: Stochastic resonance and sensory information processing: a tutorial and review of application. Clin Neurophysiol. 2004, 115: 267-281. 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.09.014.PubMedCrossRef Moss F, Ward LM, Sannita WG: Stochastic resonance and sensory information processing: a tutorial and review of application. Clin Neurophysiol. 2004, 115: 267-281. 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.09.014.PubMedCrossRef
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Jackson J, Redish AD: Network dynamics of hippocampal cell-assemblies resemble multiple spatial maps within single tasks. Hippocampus. 2007, 17: 1209-1229. 10.1002/hipo.20359.PubMedCrossRef Jackson J, Redish AD: Network dynamics of hippocampal cell-assemblies resemble multiple spatial maps within single tasks. Hippocampus. 2007, 17: 1209-1229. 10.1002/hipo.20359.PubMedCrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Rolls ET, Loh M, Deco G: An attractor hypothesis of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur J Neurosci. 2008, 28: 782-793. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06379.x.PubMedCrossRef Rolls ET, Loh M, Deco G: An attractor hypothesis of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur J Neurosci. 2008, 28: 782-793. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06379.x.PubMedCrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Jackson MB, Scharfman HE: Positive feedback from hilar mossy cells to granule cells in the dentate gyrus revealed by voltage-sensitive dye and microelectrode recording. J Neurophysiol. 1996, 76: 601-616.PubMed Jackson MB, Scharfman HE: Positive feedback from hilar mossy cells to granule cells in the dentate gyrus revealed by voltage-sensitive dye and microelectrode recording. J Neurophysiol. 1996, 76: 601-616.PubMed
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Gage FH: Neurogenesis in the adult brain. J Neurosci. 2002, 22: 612-613.PubMed Gage FH: Neurogenesis in the adult brain. J Neurosci. 2002, 22: 612-613.PubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Meguro H, Mori H, Araki K, Kushiya E, Kutsuwada T, Yamazaki M, Kumanishi T, Arakawa M, Sakimura K, Mishina M: Functional characterization of a heteromeric NMDA receptor channel expressed from cloned cDNAs. Nature. 1992, 357: 70-74. 10.1038/357070a0.PubMedCrossRef Meguro H, Mori H, Araki K, Kushiya E, Kutsuwada T, Yamazaki M, Kumanishi T, Arakawa M, Sakimura K, Mishina M: Functional characterization of a heteromeric NMDA receptor channel expressed from cloned cDNAs. Nature. 1992, 357: 70-74. 10.1038/357070a0.PubMedCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Boggs RT, Gregor P, Idriss S, Belote JM, McKeown M: Regulation of sexual differentiation in D melanogaster via alternative splicing of RNA from the transformer gene. Cell. 1987, 50: 739-747. 10.1016/0092-8674(87)90332-1.PubMedCrossRef Boggs RT, Gregor P, Idriss S, Belote JM, McKeown M: Regulation of sexual differentiation in D melanogaster via alternative splicing of RNA from the transformer gene. Cell. 1987, 50: 739-747. 10.1016/0092-8674(87)90332-1.PubMedCrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Yagi T, Tokunaga T, Furuta Y, Nada S, Yoshida M, Tsukada T, Saga Y, Takeda N, Ikawa Y, Aizawa S: A novel ES cell line, TT2, with high germline-differentiating potency. Anal Biochem. 1993, 214: 70-76. 10.1006/abio.1993.1458.PubMedCrossRef Yagi T, Tokunaga T, Furuta Y, Nada S, Yoshida M, Tsukada T, Saga Y, Takeda N, Ikawa Y, Aizawa S: A novel ES cell line, TT2, with high germline-differentiating potency. Anal Biochem. 1993, 214: 70-76. 10.1006/abio.1993.1458.PubMedCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Imai F, Hirai S, Akimoto K, Koyama H, Miyata T, Ogawa M, Noguchi S, Sasaoka T, Noda T, Ohno S: Inactivation of aPKClambda results in the loss of adherens junctions in neuroepithelial cells without affecting neurogenesis in mouse neocortex. Development. 2006, 133: 1735-1744. 10.1242/dev.02330.PubMedCrossRef Imai F, Hirai S, Akimoto K, Koyama H, Miyata T, Ogawa M, Noguchi S, Sasaoka T, Noda T, Ohno S: Inactivation of aPKClambda results in the loss of adherens junctions in neuroepithelial cells without affecting neurogenesis in mouse neocortex. Development. 2006, 133: 1735-1744. 10.1242/dev.02330.PubMedCrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Araki K, Araki M, Miyazaki J, Vassalli P: Site-specific recombination of a transgene in fertilized eggs by transient expression of Cre recombinase. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1995, 92: 160-164. 10.1073/pnas.92.1.160.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Araki K, Araki M, Miyazaki J, Vassalli P: Site-specific recombination of a transgene in fertilized eggs by transient expression of Cre recombinase. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1995, 92: 160-164. 10.1073/pnas.92.1.160.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Miyakawa T, Yared E, Pak JH, Huang FL, Huang KP, Crawley JN: Neurogranin null mutant mice display performance deficits on spatial learning tasks with anxiety related components. Hippocampus. 2001, 11: 763-775. 10.1002/hipo.1092.PubMedCrossRef Miyakawa T, Yared E, Pak JH, Huang FL, Huang KP, Crawley JN: Neurogranin null mutant mice display performance deficits on spatial learning tasks with anxiety related components. Hippocampus. 2001, 11: 763-775. 10.1002/hipo.1092.PubMedCrossRef
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Fox SE, Ranck JB: Localization and anatomical identification of theta and complex spike cells in dorsal hippocampal formation of rats. Exp Neurol. 1975, 49: 299-313. 10.1016/0014-4886(75)90213-7.PubMedCrossRef Fox SE, Ranck JB: Localization and anatomical identification of theta and complex spike cells in dorsal hippocampal formation of rats. Exp Neurol. 1975, 49: 299-313. 10.1016/0014-4886(75)90213-7.PubMedCrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Fox SE, Ranck JB: Electrophysiological characteristics of hippocampal complex-spike cells and theta cells. Exp Brain Res. 1981, 41: 399-410.PubMed Fox SE, Ranck JB: Electrophysiological characteristics of hippocampal complex-spike cells and theta cells. Exp Brain Res. 1981, 41: 399-410.PubMed
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Harris KD, Henze DA, Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Buzsaki G: Accuracy of tetrode spike separation as determined by simultaneous intracellular and extracellular measurements. J Neurophysiol. 2000, 84: 401-414.PubMed Harris KD, Henze DA, Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Buzsaki G: Accuracy of tetrode spike separation as determined by simultaneous intracellular and extracellular measurements. J Neurophysiol. 2000, 84: 401-414.PubMed
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Hazan L, Zugaro M, Buzsaki G: Klusters, NeuroScope, NDManager: a free software suite for neurophysiological data processing and visualization. J Neurosci Methods. 2006, 155: 207-216. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.01.017.PubMedCrossRef Hazan L, Zugaro M, Buzsaki G: Klusters, NeuroScope, NDManager: a free software suite for neurophysiological data processing and visualization. J Neurosci Methods. 2006, 155: 207-216. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.01.017.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Enhanced stability of hippocampal place representation caused by reduced magnesium block of NMDA receptors in the dentate gyrus
verfasst von
Yuichiro Hayashi
Yoko Nabeshima
Katsunori Kobayashi
Tsuyoshi Miyakawa
Koichi Tanda
Keizo Takao
Hidenori Suzuki
Eisaku Esumi
Shigeru Noguchi
Yukiko Matsuda
Toshikuni Sasaoka
Tetsuo Noda
Jun-ichi Miyazaki
Masayoshi Mishina
Kazuo Funabiki
Yo-ichi Nabeshima
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2014
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Molecular Brain / Ausgabe 1/2014
Elektronische ISSN: 1756-6606
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-6606-7-44

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2014

Molecular Brain 1/2014 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Nicht Creutzfeldt Jakob, sondern Abführtee-Vergiftung

29.05.2024 Hyponatriämie Nachrichten

Eine ältere Frau trinkt regelmäßig Sennesblättertee gegen ihre Verstopfung. Der scheint plötzlich gut zu wirken. Auf Durchfall und Erbrechen folgt allerdings eine Hyponatriämie. Nach deren Korrektur kommt es plötzlich zu progredienten Kognitions- und Verhaltensstörungen.

Schutz der Synapsen bei Alzheimer

29.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Mit einem Neurotrophin-Rezeptor-Modulator lässt sich möglicherweise eine bestehende Alzheimerdemenz etwas abschwächen: Erste Phase-2-Daten deuten auf einen verbesserten Synapsenschutz.

Sozialer Aufstieg verringert Demenzgefahr

24.05.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Ein hohes soziales Niveau ist mit die beste Versicherung gegen eine Demenz. Noch geringer ist das Demenzrisiko für Menschen, die sozial aufsteigen: Sie gewinnen fast zwei demenzfreie Lebensjahre. Umgekehrt steigt die Demenzgefahr beim sozialen Abstieg.

Hirnblutung unter DOAK und VKA ähnlich bedrohlich

17.05.2024 Direkte orale Antikoagulanzien Nachrichten

Kommt es zu einer nichttraumatischen Hirnblutung, spielt es keine große Rolle, ob die Betroffenen zuvor direkt wirksame orale Antikoagulanzien oder Marcumar bekommen haben: Die Prognose ist ähnlich schlecht.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.