Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Experimental Brain Research 3/2013

01.09.2013 | Research Article

Explicit knowledge and real-time action control: anticipating a change does not make us respond more quickly

verfasst von: Brendan D. Cameron, Darian T. Cheng, Romeo Chua, Paul van Donkelaar, Gordon Binsted

Erschienen in: Experimental Brain Research | Ausgabe 3/2013

Einloggen, um Zugang zu erhalten

Abstract

When the target of a goal-directed reach changes location, people normally respond rapidly and automatically to the target shift. Here, we investigate whether explicit knowledge about a moving target (knowing whether a location change is likely/unlikely) improves responsiveness, with the goal of understanding top-down effects on real-time reaching. In Experiment 1, we presented participants with pre-cues that indicated a 20 or 80 % likelihood of a target perturbation on that trial. When participants made pro-pointing responses to the target perturbations, their online response occurred later for 20 % trials than for 80 % trials, but this effect may have been due to suppression of the online response on 20 % trials, rather than enhancement of the response on 80 % trials. In Experiment 2, we presented participants with 50 and 100 % likelihood pre-cues, and observed no shortening of the latency on 100 % trials compared to 50 % trials, which suggests that expectation does not enhance the automatic response to a perturbation. However, we did observe more vigorous responses to the perturbation on the 100 % trials, and this contributed to shorter movement times relative to the 50 % trials. We also examined, in Experiment 2, whether prior knowledge about the direction of the target perturbation would shorten the latency of the online response, but we did not observe any reduction in latency. In sum, the onset of the automatic response appears to be suppressible, but not augmentable by top-down input. The possibility that the forcefulness of the automatic response is modifiable by expectation is examined, but not resolved.
Fußnoten
1
Our detection of response latency for each participant used the full set of jump trials for each pre-cue type. However, because there were more jump trials for the 80 % than for the 20 % pre-cues, we were concerned that the t tests used in 80 % case might detect earlier times as a result of greater statistical power. Therefore, we also carried out a test where we randomly selected a number of trials from 80 % that was equivalent to the number of trials in 20 % and applied the frame-by-frame t test analysis to those trials. We carried out this random sampling 100 times for each participant, and took the median of these deviation times (the median was used to eliminate the effects of some negative skewing in the distribution of deviation times). These values (one deviation time per participant for 20 % and one for 80 %) were then analyzed with a t test. This analysis resulted in the same statistical conclusion as the original procedure, though with a smaller mean difference (17 ms) and a smaller t value: t(9) = 2.6, p <  05.
 
2
We were not concerned that experimenter knowledge would be problematic, mainly because expectation about performance seems to be unrelated to real-time behaviour: participants do not expect their reach to go to a shifted target when they intend to stop it, yet it does (Pisella et al. 2000), and the latencies of the unexpected responses (the direct deviations in stop/anti-point) appear to be equivalent to the latencies of the expected corrections (corrections in pro-point) (Cressman et al. 2006; Day and Lyon 2000). If expectation about movement outcome does not influence real-time corrections, then experimenter participation should be innocuous. Note that this is different from expectation about target behaviour, which was directly manipulated here and was equivalent for all participants.
 
3
The delay, of course, had no effect on stationary trials.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Baldauf D, Wolf M, Deubel H (2006) Deployment of visual attention before sequences of goal-directed hand movements. Vis Res 46:4355–4374PubMedCrossRef Baldauf D, Wolf M, Deubel H (2006) Deployment of visual attention before sequences of goal-directed hand movements. Vis Res 46:4355–4374PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Boulinguez P, Nougier V (1999) Control of goal-directed movements: the contribution of orienting of visual attention and motor preparation. Acta Psychol 103:21–45CrossRef Boulinguez P, Nougier V (1999) Control of goal-directed movements: the contribution of orienting of visual attention and motor preparation. Acta Psychol 103:21–45CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (1997) Fast responses of the human hand to changes in target position. J Motor Behav 29:297–310CrossRef Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (1997) Fast responses of the human hand to changes in target position. J Motor Behav 29:297–310CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2003) Fast corrections of movements with a computer mouse. Spat Vis 16:365–376PubMedCrossRef Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2003) Fast corrections of movements with a computer mouse. Spat Vis 16:365–376PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cameron BD, Cressman EK, Franks IM, Chua R (2009) Cognitive constraint on the ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand: movement intention influences the hand’s susceptibility to involuntary online corrections. Conscious Cogn 18:646–652PubMedCrossRef Cameron BD, Cressman EK, Franks IM, Chua R (2009) Cognitive constraint on the ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand: movement intention influences the hand’s susceptibility to involuntary online corrections. Conscious Cogn 18:646–652PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cameron BD, Franks IM, Inglis JT, Chua R (2010) Reach adaptation to explicit vs. implicit target error. Exp Brain Res 203:367–380PubMedCrossRef Cameron BD, Franks IM, Inglis JT, Chua R (2010) Reach adaptation to explicit vs. implicit target error. Exp Brain Res 203:367–380PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cameron BD, Franks IM, Inglis JT, Chua R (2011) Reach adaptation to online target error. Exp Brain Res 209:171–180PubMedCrossRef Cameron BD, Franks IM, Inglis JT, Chua R (2011) Reach adaptation to online target error. Exp Brain Res 209:171–180PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cressman EK, Franks IM, Enns JT, Chua R (2006) No automatic pilot for visually guided aiming based on colour. Exp Brain Res 171:174–183PubMedCrossRef Cressman EK, Franks IM, Enns JT, Chua R (2006) No automatic pilot for visually guided aiming based on colour. Exp Brain Res 171:174–183PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Cressman EK, Cameron BD, Lam MY, Franks IM, Chua R (2010) Movement duration does not affect automatic online control. Hum Mov Sci 29:871–881PubMedCrossRef Cressman EK, Cameron BD, Lam MY, Franks IM, Chua R (2010) Movement duration does not affect automatic online control. Hum Mov Sci 29:871–881PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Day BL, Lyon IN (2000) Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target. Exp Brain Res 130:159–168PubMedCrossRef Day BL, Lyon IN (2000) Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target. Exp Brain Res 130:159–168PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Desmurget M, Grafton S (2000) Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cogn Sci 4:423–431PubMedCrossRef Desmurget M, Grafton S (2000) Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements. Trends Cogn Sci 4:423–431PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Desmurget M, Epstein CM, Turner RS, Prablanc C, Alexander GE, Grafton ST (1999) Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target. Nat Neurosci 2:563–567 Desmurget M, Epstein CM, Turner RS, Prablanc C, Alexander GE, Grafton ST (1999) Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target. Nat Neurosci 2:563–567
Zurück zum Zitat Deubel H, Schneider WX (1996) Saccade target selection and object recognition: evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vis Res 36:1827–1837PubMedCrossRef Deubel H, Schneider WX (1996) Saccade target selection and object recognition: evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vis Res 36:1827–1837PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Deubel H, Schneider WX, Paprotta I (1998) Selective dorsal and ventral processing: evidence for a common attentional mechanism in reaching and perception. Vis Cogn 5:81–107CrossRef Deubel H, Schneider WX, Paprotta I (1998) Selective dorsal and ventral processing: evidence for a common attentional mechanism in reaching and perception. Vis Cogn 5:81–107CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Diedrichsen J, Hashambhoy Y, Rane T, Shadmehr R (2005) Neural correlates of reach errors. J Neurosci 25:9919–9931PubMedCrossRef Diedrichsen J, Hashambhoy Y, Rane T, Shadmehr R (2005) Neural correlates of reach errors. J Neurosci 25:9919–9931PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Filevich E, Kuhn S, Haggard P (2012) Intentional inhibition in human action: the power of ‘no’. Neurosci Biobehav R 36:1107–1118CrossRef Filevich E, Kuhn S, Haggard P (2012) Intentional inhibition in human action: the power of ‘no’. Neurosci Biobehav R 36:1107–1118CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Goodale MA, Pelisson D, Prablanc C (1986) Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature 320:748–750 Goodale MA, Pelisson D, Prablanc C (1986) Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature 320:748–750
Zurück zum Zitat Gritsenko V, Yakovenko S, Kalaska JF (2009) Integration of predictive feedforward and sensory feedback signals for online control of visually-guided movement. J Neurophysiol 102:2375–2388CrossRef Gritsenko V, Yakovenko S, Kalaska JF (2009) Integration of predictive feedforward and sensory feedback signals for online control of visually-guided movement. J Neurophysiol 102:2375–2388CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Heath M, Maraj A, Maddigan M, Binsted G (2009) The antipointing task: vector inversion is supported by a perceptual estimate of visual space. J Motor Behav 41:383–392CrossRef Heath M, Maraj A, Maddigan M, Binsted G (2009) The antipointing task: vector inversion is supported by a perceptual estimate of visual space. J Motor Behav 41:383–392CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hick WE (1952) On the rate of gain of information. Q J Exp Psychol 4:11–26CrossRef Hick WE (1952) On the rate of gain of information. Q J Exp Psychol 4:11–26CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hoffman JE, Subramaniam B (1995) The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Atten Percept Psychophys 57:787–795CrossRef Hoffman JE, Subramaniam B (1995) The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Atten Percept Psychophys 57:787–795CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Magescas F, Prablanc C (2006) Automatic drive of limb motor plasticity. J Cogn Neurosci 18:75–83PubMedCrossRef Magescas F, Prablanc C (2006) Automatic drive of limb motor plasticity. J Cogn Neurosci 18:75–83PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat McIntosh RD, Mulroue A, Brockmole JR (2010) How automatic is the hand’s automatic pilot? Evidence from dual-task studies. Exp Brain Res 206:257–269PubMedCrossRef McIntosh RD, Mulroue A, Brockmole JR (2010) How automatic is the hand’s automatic pilot? Evidence from dual-task studies. Exp Brain Res 206:257–269PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10:437–442PubMedCrossRef Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10:437–442PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Pelisson D, Prablanc C, Goodale MA, Jeannerod M (1986) Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb. Exp Brain Res 62:303–311 Pelisson D, Prablanc C, Goodale MA, Jeannerod M (1986) Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb. Exp Brain Res 62:303–311
Zurück zum Zitat Pisella L, Grea H, Tilikete C, Vighetto A, Desmurget M, Rode G, Boisson D, Rossetti Y (2000) An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia. Nat Neurosci 3:729–736PubMedCrossRef Pisella L, Grea H, Tilikete C, Vighetto A, Desmurget M, Rode G, Boisson D, Rossetti Y (2000) An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia. Nat Neurosci 3:729–736PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Prablanc C, Martin O (1992) Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. J Neurophysiol 67:455–469PubMed Prablanc C, Martin O (1992) Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. J Neurophysiol 67:455–469PubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Striemer CL, Yukovsky J, Goodale MA (2010) Can intention override the “automatic pilot”? Exp Brain Res 202:623–632PubMedCrossRef Striemer CL, Yukovsky J, Goodale MA (2010) Can intention override the “automatic pilot”? Exp Brain Res 202:623–632PubMedCrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wijdenes LO, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2011) Fast and fine-tuned corrections when the target of a hand movement is displaced. Exp Brain Res 214:453–462CrossRef Wijdenes LO, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2011) Fast and fine-tuned corrections when the target of a hand movement is displaced. Exp Brain Res 214:453–462CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Explicit knowledge and real-time action control: anticipating a change does not make us respond more quickly
verfasst von
Brendan D. Cameron
Darian T. Cheng
Romeo Chua
Paul van Donkelaar
Gordon Binsted
Publikationsdatum
01.09.2013
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
Experimental Brain Research / Ausgabe 3/2013
Print ISSN: 0014-4819
Elektronische ISSN: 1432-1106
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3401-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2013

Experimental Brain Research 3/2013 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Sozialer Aufstieg verringert Demenzgefahr

24.05.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Ein hohes soziales Niveau ist mit die beste Versicherung gegen eine Demenz. Noch geringer ist das Demenzrisiko für Menschen, die sozial aufsteigen: Sie gewinnen fast zwei demenzfreie Lebensjahre. Umgekehrt steigt die Demenzgefahr beim sozialen Abstieg.

Hirnblutung unter DOAK und VKA ähnlich bedrohlich

17.05.2024 Direkte orale Antikoagulanzien Nachrichten

Kommt es zu einer nichttraumatischen Hirnblutung, spielt es keine große Rolle, ob die Betroffenen zuvor direkt wirksame orale Antikoagulanzien oder Marcumar bekommen haben: Die Prognose ist ähnlich schlecht.

Was nützt die Kraniektomie bei schwerer tiefer Hirnblutung?

17.05.2024 Hirnblutung Nachrichten

Eine Studie zum Nutzen der druckentlastenden Kraniektomie nach schwerer tiefer supratentorieller Hirnblutung deutet einen Nutzen der Operation an. Für überlebende Patienten ist das dennoch nur eine bedingt gute Nachricht.

Thrombektomie auch bei großen Infarkten von Vorteil

16.05.2024 Ischämischer Schlaganfall Nachrichten

Auch ein sehr ausgedehnter ischämischer Schlaganfall scheint an sich kein Grund zu sein, von einer mechanischen Thrombektomie abzusehen. Dafür spricht die LASTE-Studie, an der Patienten und Patientinnen mit einem ASPECTS von maximal 5 beteiligt waren.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.