Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 12/2020

Open Access 05.02.2020 | Original Contribution

Investigating the association between family connectedness and self-control in adolescence in a genetically sensitive design

verfasst von: Yayouk E. Willems, Odilia M. Laceulle, Meike Bartels, Catrin Finkenauer

Erschienen in: European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry | Ausgabe 12/2020

Abstract

Family connectedness is key for the development of self-control in early and middle childhood. But is family connectedness still important during the transitional phase of adolescence, when adolescents demand more independence from their parents and rely more on their peers? The aim of the present study was to investigate the association between family connectedness and self-control, and whether it still holds in adolescence using a genetically sensitive design. Data were used from a large sample of twins aged 14 (N = 11,260) and aged 16 (N = 8175), all enrolled in the Netherlands Twin Register. We applied bivariate twin models and monozygotic twin difference models to investigate the association between family connectedness and self-control and to unravel to what extent genetic and environmental factors explain this association. The results showed that more family connectedness is significantly related to better self-control in adolescence, albeit with a small effect size. Twin analyses revealed that this association was mainly explained by common genetic factors and that the effects of environmental factors were small. The current findings confirm the role of family connectedness in adolescent self-control. Importantly, however, the results demonstrate that phenomena we see within families seem the product of parent and children sharing the same genes rather than being exclusively attributable to environmental processes.
Hinweise

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s00787-020-01485-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Y. E. Willems and O. M. Laceulle shared first author.

Introduction

Family connectedness encompasses the feeling of trust, understanding, and support within the family, and is robustly associated with healthy child development [13, 25]. In other words, family connectedness is the emotional connection within the family, also referred to as family warmth or responsiveness, and capturing the way family members give emotional support, show affection and strengthen family bonds [13]. Children develop over time, and so does the influence of the family on the development of the child [57]. This development is especially pronounced during adolescence, as adolescents increasingly become active agents in their own development, demand more independence from their parents, and rely more on connectedness with peers than on connectedness with the family [10]. The developmental transition from being dependent on parents to independence yields an important question: is family connectedness still related to person characteristics in adolescence and, if so, what is the nature of this association? Thus far, most of the researches examining this question focused on early and middle childhood rather than adolescence. Moreover, few studies take the possibility of genetic confounding into account. Unraveling the underlying genetic and environmental mechanisms is important to understand how adolescents develop within, and in interaction with, their social world [30, 49].

Self-control

A key person characteristic in adolescent development is self-control. Self-control is the capacity to alter unwanted impulses and behaviors to bring them into agreement with internal and external standards [19, 23]. Self-control is proposed to be especially important during adolescence, as adolescents with high self-control capacities have higher-quality interpersonal relationships, better school grades, healthier lifestyles, less psychological problems, and report more happiness than their adolescent peers with less self-control capacities [11, 22, 41]. Given these findings, self-control has also been coined a hallmark for adolescents to become well-adjusted adults [10, 11, 41]. As such, it is important to understand causes and consequences of individual differences in adolescent self-control capacities.

The association between family connectedness and self-control

Research in early and middle childhood shows that family connectedness and self-control are associated (with associations around r = 0.20, see [46]). When parents create a context where family members feel accepted and supported, children get the opportunity to learn how to self-regulate their behaviors and impulses [7, 46]. Additionally, family connectedness is closely related to higher-quality parenting which, in turn, affects children’s opportunities to learn how to self-regulate their impulses, behaviors, and emotions [58]. Importantly, the association between family connectedness and self-control can also be explained by a child’s level of self-control evoking certain family responses. For example, children with high self-control elicit trust, warmth, and affection from their parents and siblings [9], feelings which strengthen family connectedness [59]. Thus, there seems to be a reciprocal association between family connectedness and a child’s self-control.
Research in adolescence on the association between family connectedness and self-control is more inconclusive. For example, while some longitudinal studies in adolescents find no significant bidirectional association [15, 43], others find significant associations with small effect sizes (r = 0.15–0.20, [34, 59]), and again others find moderate effect sizes for the association between family connectedness and self-control (r = 0.30–0.35, [31]). Important to note is that, even small changes in self-control levels can affect the developmental trajectory of individuals [11].
A particular problem in the current literature is that the associations reported in the studies are commonly interpreted as reflecting causal effects between family connectedness and self-control. Most of these studies, however, are correlational and thereby do not necessarily provide evidence of true direction of effect. An alternative explanation for the association between family connectedness and self-control is that underlying factors influence them both (i.e., no direct causal relationship but a third underlying factor that drives the association between the two, [4, 27]). One key underlying factor may be genetic factors.

Genetic influences on family connectedness and self-control

Over the last decade, accumulating research shows that traits are at least partly heritable [51]. For example, a recent meta-analysis showed that the heritability of self-control is 60%, with stable heritability estimates across middle childhood and adolescence [64]. Traits closely related to self-control show similar heritability estimates such as grit (37%, [55]), effortful control (49%, [67]), delay discounting (51%, [1]), and attention skills (70%, [52]). Importantly, not only person characteristics, but also contextual factors are partly influenced by genetic factors. Genes do not in any direct way “code” people for specific environments, however, individual’s genetic make-up influences their perception and selection of contexts [49].
The heritability of family connectedness ranges between 30 and 40% [61], and the way children perceive parenting is correlated with their genes (gene–environment correlation) [17, 26, 28].
Considering the genetic contribution to both family connectedness and self-control, it may thus well be that their observed associations are explained by common genetic factors that simultaneously influence both family connectedness and self-control [2, 48, 56]. Thus far, however, studies on the association between family connectedness and self-control specifically applying genetically sensitive designs are scarce. Two studies using a genetic sensitive design found no significant association between parental socialization and self-control in adolescence after controlling for genetic influences. Yet, the study had limited statistical power to solidify these assumptions (monozygotic twin pairs N = 289, dizygotic twin pairs N = 452, [4, 66]). To further our knowledge, it is important to assess whether family connectedness and self-control are the result of a true directional effect or, alternatively, by a confounding third factor such as common genetic factors simultaneously influencing both the family context and child outcomes (genetic pleiotropy, [48]).
One design allowing researchers to investigate the association between family connectedness and self-control in adolescents, while simultaneously unraveling to what extent the association is influenced by genetic or environmental factors, is the bivariate twin design. This design is built upon the premise that monozygotic twin pair correlations (100% genetically identical) and dizygotic twin pair correlations (on average 50% genetically identical) can be parsed into environmental and genetic influences [6].
Additionally, twin data allow researchers to apply a monozygotic difference design [3, 12, 16]. If there is a causal relationship between family connectedness and self-control, it could be expected that in genetically identical twins, the twin who reported more family connectedness has higher self-control than his/her co-twin, or vice versa. Applying both twin designs, the bivariate twin design and the monozygotic difference design, thereby allows us to better understand the nature of the association between family connectedness and self-control.

Current study

Previous studies have mainly focused on the association between family connectedness and self-control in middle and early childhood. We aim to extend this line of work to adolescence, a transformative phase for families, parents, and children. Additionally, few studies thus far have investigated the association between family connectedness and self-control in a genetically sensitive design. Such a design can provide information on the extent to which environmental or genetic factors influence this association. The goal of this study is therefore to investigate the nature of the association between family connectedness and self-control. We aim to do so by investigating the following three sub-questions in a large longitudinal sample of adolescent twins aged 14 years and aged 16 years: (1) are family connectedness and self-control associated over the course of adolescence? (2) to what extent is the association explained by genetic or environmental influences? (3) can we determine the directionality of the association between family connectedness and self-control?

Method

Sample and procedure

Longitudinal survey data were collected in twins registered with the Netherlands Twin Register, a population-based study initiated in 1987 in the Netherlands at the Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam. Upon parental consent, 14- and 16-year-old twins received questionnaires on family functioning, physical health, and psychological well-being (see [60, 61] for more details on data collection). Data collection was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee at the Vrije Universiteit Medical Center (2003/182).
The dataset comprised 14-year-old twins (57.6% females, MZ twin pairs N = 1,905, DZ twin pairs N = 3,353), and 16-year-old twins (58.1% females, MZ twin pairs N = 1,483, DZ twin pairs N = 2,476). See Table S1 Supplemental Material for an overview of the sample size for each effect size. For 28.1% of the same-sex twin pairs, zygosity was determined based on DNA typing or blood group. For the remaining same-sex twins, zygosity was determined based on questionnaire items filled in by parents (e.g., “is it difficult to discern the two siblings from one another”), resulting in accurate determination of zygosity in 93% of the cases [54].

Measures

Family connectedness was assessed with an adolescent self-report subscale of the McMaster Family Assessment Device (FAD) [21]. We used the Dutch translation which shows good psychometrical properties [61, 62], a one-factor structure, and a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.84 at age 14 and 0.85 at age 16. The subscale consisted of six items tapping into family connectedness such as, “in times of crisis, we can turn to each other for support”, “we feel free to express our feelings within the family”, and “we trust each other”. Items were scored on a 4-point scale, ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 4 = strongly agree. Scores on individual items were summed, so that higher scores reflected more family connectedness.
Self-control was assessed with the adolescent self-report of the ASEBA Self-Control Scale (ASCS) [63]. This scale is shown to be psychometrically sound to assess self-control [63], with a one-factor structure, good test–retest reliability (test–retest correlations of 0.55), and a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.73 at age 14 and 0.70 at age 16. The scale consists of eight items tapping into self-control with items such as “I fail to finish things that I start” and “I am inattentive or easily distracted”. The response format of the items is a 3-point scale, with response options not true (coded 0), somewhat or sometimes true (coded 1), and very true or often true (coded 2). We recoded the items such that higher sum scores reflected higher overall levels of self-control.

Statistical analyses

All analyses were conducted in Mplus version 7 [45], evaluating goodness of fit using the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and the comparative fit index (CFI) with the cutoff scores defined by Hu and Bentler [33]. We compared the fit statistics of different models. If the constrained model (i.e., the more parsimonious model) showed better fit statistics (lower RMSEA, higher CFI) or a non-significant p value to the χ2 test (meaning that the fit of the constrained model was not significantly worse than the fit of the more complex model), we considered this model to better represent the data. Considering our multiple tests (phenotypic correlations, cross-sectional twin analyses at age 14, cross-sectional twin analyses at age 16, longitudinal twin analyses self-control at age 14 to family connectedness at age 16, and longitudinal twin analyses family connectedness at age 14 and self-control at age 16), we applied the more conservative alpha level of 0.01. We applied structural equation modeling (SEM) for all our analyses, applying maximum likelihood estimator (MLR) and controlling for non-independence of observations by clustering data around the family identification variable [53].

Bivariate twin analyses

We applied bivariate twin analyses to investigate to what extent genetic and environmental factors contribute to family connectedness and self-control in adolescence. The observed phenotype and the association between two phenotypes can be decomposed into (1) genetic effects which can be additive genetic effects (A), and/or dominance genetic effects (D), (2) shared environmental effects (C), and (3) unique environmental effects (E), which is the part of the total variance that is unique to a certain individual, and also includes measurement error (see Fig. 1). Additionally, it allows us to calculate genetic (rg) and environmental correlations (re), which quantify the extent to which an association is influenced by the same genes or by the same environmental factors (see Fig. 1).

Monozygotic twin difference model

To further explore direction of causation between family connectedness and self-control in adolescence, we applied monozygotic within-individual change model. This is a method to test whether the monozygotic twin who reports an increase in family connectedness from age 14 to 16 shows an increase in self-control from age 14 to 16, as compared to the co-twin with a lesser increase in family connectedness over time. Because monozygotic twins are genetically identical, any phenotypic difference between them cannot be the result of genetic influences. This model is therefore particularly strong, because it examines an association controlling for genetic confounding and under the causal hypotheses these monozygotic differences should be significantly associated [3, 16].

Results

Descriptives

Means, standard deviations, and sample sizes are provided in Table 1. There were no significant mean or variance differences between boys and girls, nor between monozygotic and dizygotic twins. Constraining the twin correlations to be equal across gender did not deteriorate model fit, indicating the absence of gender differences in the genetic architecture of family connectedness or self-control, respectively (see Table S2, Supplemental Material). Most twin pairs consisted of complete pairs (age 14 82% complete MZ twin pairs, age 16 74% complete DZ twin pairs, 77% complete MZ twin pairs, 60% complete DZ twin pairs). Of the adolescents participated at age 14, 54% participated at age 16.
Table 1
Means, standard deviations, and phenotypic correlations with 95% confidence intervals
#
Variable
Age
M
SD
N
1
2
3
1
Family connectedness
14
19.42
2.78
10,685
   
2
Self-control
14
11.73
2.76
11,260
0.21* [0.19, 0.23]
  
3
Family connectedness
16
19.04
2.87
7430
0.42* [0.38, 0.45]
0.15* [0.12, 0.18]
 
4
Self-control
16
11.64
2.69
8175
0.17* [0.14, 0.20]
0.58* [0.56, 0.60]
0.19* [0.17, 0.21]
*p < 0.01

Is there an association between family connectedness and self-control in adolescence?

The phenotypic correlations between family connectedness and self-control are presented in Table 1. All correlations were small, positive, and significant, with a cross-sectional correlation of r = 0.21, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.19, 0.23] at age 14, and a cross-sectional correlation of r = 0.19, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.17, 0.21] at age 16. The longitudinal association between family connectedness at age 14 and self-control at age 16 was r = 0.17, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.14, 0.20]. The longitudinal association between self-control at age 14 and family connectedness at age 16 was r = 0.15, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.12, 0.18].
These results suggest that family connectedness and self-control are associated in adolescence, with more family connectedness associated with higher adolescent self-control and, vice versa, higher adolescent self-control associated with more family connectedness. Thus, on average, in families where adolescents report more family connectedness, adolescents report higher self-control and, vice versa, adolescents with higher self-control report their families to be more connected, albeit with a small effect size.

What are the genetic and environmental contributions to this association?

Twin correlations. The twin correlations are summarized in Table 2. All correlations were significant, and all the monozygotic twin correlations were stronger than the dizygotic twin correlations, suggesting the presence of genetic influences on both family connectedness and self-control. For family connectedness, the dizygotic twin correlations were close to the monozygotic twin correlations, which suggests an influence of the shared environment (C), while for self-control the monozygotic correlations were more than twice as high as the dizygotic correlations, indicating the presence of genetic dominance (D).
Table 2
Twin correlations and cross-twin cross-trait correlations with 95% confidence intervals
Correlations
MZ
DZ
Twin correlations
 Family connectedness at age 14
0.35*[0.31, 0.39]
0.25*[0.21, 0.29]
 Self-control at age 14
0.49*[0.46, 0.53]
0.18*[0.14, 0.22]
 Family connectedness at age 16
0.39*[0.34, 0.44]
0.24*[0.19, 0.29]
 Self-control at age 16
0.47*[0.43, 0.51]
0.18*[0.13, 0.23]
Cross-twin cross-trait correlations
 Family connectedness at age 14–self-control at age 14
0.18*[0.15, 0.20]
0.09*[0.07, 0.10]
 Self-control at age 16–family connectedness at age 16
0.12*[0.09, 0.16]
0.06*[0.04, 0.08]
 Family connectedness at age 14–self-control at age 16
0.17*[0.14, 0.21]
0.09*[0.06, 0.11]
 Self-control at age 14–family connectedness at age 16
0.11*[0.06, 0.15]
0.05*[0.03, 0.08]
*p < 0.01
Bivariate twin model. The results of the bivariate genetic twin model are presented in Table 4 (univariate standardized A, C/D, E estimates for family connectedness and self-control, respectively, are presented in Table 3). Full twin models were used in the bivariate twin analyses to avoid biases due to the drop of parameters (that is, we kept ACE for family connectedness and ADE for self-control). For the bivariate estimations, it is not possible to estimate both C and D in the same model, so in line with the classical twin model we estimated an AE model for the overlap between family connectedness and self-control. We modeled the bivariate genetic, and unique environmental effects but not the bivariate shared environmental effects, because it is not possible to estimate both C and D in the same model, and shared environmental influences did not contribute to variation in self-control [65]. All bivariate twin models showed good model fit (see Table S3, Supplemental Material).
Table 3
Additive genetic (A), dominant genetic (D), common shared environmental (C), and unique environmental (E) estimates [95% confidence interval] to family connectedness and self-control, respectively
Age
Family connectedness
Self-control
 
 
A
C
E
A
D
E
Age 14
0.19 [0.08, 0.31]
0.15 [0.07, 0.24]
0.65 [0.61, 0.70]
0.22 [0.08, 0.37]
0.27 [0.11, 0.43]
0.51 [0.47, 0.54]
Age 16
0.30 [0.16, 0.45]
0.09 [− 0.02, 0.20]
0.61 [0.56, 0.66]
0.23 [0.04, 0.43]
0.23 [0.03, 0.44]
0.53 [0.49, 0.58]
For the cross-sectional associations, genetic factors largely contributed to the association between family connectedness and self-control. At age 14, the bivariate heritability between family connectedness and self-control was 83%, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.71, 0.94], with the remaining 18%, p < 0.01, 95% CI [0.06, 29] explained by unique environmental factors. At age 16, the bivariate heritability was 66%, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.50, 0.83] with the remaining 34%, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.17, 0.50] explained by unique environmental factors.
For the associations from age 14 to 16, genetic factors solely contributed to the link between family connectedness and self-control. The bivariate heritability between family connectedness at age 14 and self-control at age 16 was 95%, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.76, 0.100]. Environmental effects on the covariance were non-significant: 5%, p = 0.96, 95% CI [− 0.23, 0.24]. Similarly, the bivariate heritability between self-control at age 14 and family connectedness at age 16 was 72%, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.47, 0.97]. Environmental effects on the covariance were non-significant: 28%, p = 0.03, 95% CI [0.03, 0.54].
While the bivariate heritability estimates elucidate the contribution of genetic and environmental factors to the phenotypic association between family connectedness and self-control, the genetic correlations (rg) and environmental correlations (re) quantify the extent to which the two are influenced by the same genes or by the same environmental factors. These correlations are presented in Table 4 and showed that rg ranged between 0.36 and 0.63 while re ranged between 0.00 and 0.07. This indicates that, over and above the strong influence of genes on the association between family connectedness and self-control, there is also overlap between the genes involved in both traits. The overlap between environmental factors is small or close to zero.
Table 4
Genetic and environmental contributions (95% confidence interval) to the association between family connectedness and self-control
 
A
E
rg
re
Cross-sectional
    
 Family connectedness at age 14–self-control at age 14
0.83*[0.71, 0.94]
0.18*[0.06, 0.29]
0.63*[.40, 0.86]
0.06*[0.02, 0.10]
 Family connectedness at age 16–self-control at age 16
0.66*[0.50, 0.83]
0.34*[0.17, 0.50]
0.37*[0.21, 0.52]
0.10*[0.05, 0.15]
Longitudinal
 Family connectedness at age 14–self-control at age 16
0.95*[0.76, 1.00]
0.05 [− 0.23, 0.24]
0.57*[0.34, 0.81]
0.00 [− 0.07, 0.07]
 Self-control at age 14–family connectedness at age 16
0.72*[0.47, 0.97]
0.28 [0.03, 0.54]
0.36*[0.20, 0.57]
0.07 [0.01, 0.14]
The estimates A and E represent the proportion of the phenotypic correlation that is due to genetic (A) and environmental (E) variance, rg = genetic correlations, re = environmental correlation
*p <0 .01

Is there a direction of effect between family connectedness and self-control?

Monozygotic twin within-individual change model. The results show that, in genetically identical twin pairs, the twin showing the largest increase in family connectedness from age 14 to 16 did not report larger increase in self-control from age 14 to age 16 than the co-twin showing lower increase (or even decrease) in experienced family connectedness: r = 0.01, p = 0.83, 95% CI [− 0.08, 0.10].

Discussion

The present study investigated the association between family connectedness and self-control, examining whether the association still holds in adolescence as well as the nature of this association. In line with the literature on early and middle childhood, the results confirmed that more reported family connectedness is related to better self-control in adolescents, albeit with a small effect size. When investigating the nature of this association, we found that this correlation was mainly explained by common genetic factors, with the effects of environmental factors being small. That is, the monozygotic twin who reports more family connectedness did not show higher self-control than his/her co-twin and, vice versa, the monozygotic twin with more self-control did not report more family connectedness than his/her co-twin. These findings are in line with results from middle childhood, where genetic factors play a role in explaining the overlap between family connectedness and child outcomes [12, 29, 35]. However, environmental factors play a larger role in early and middle childhood, while over time genetic factors increasingly play a role in spelling out individual differences in adolescent outcomes [42].
These findings suggest that while the association between family connectedness and self-control holds in adolescence, the two traits are not likely to causally influence one another over time. Rather, an underlying common factor such as genetic pleiotropy with some additional unique environmental influences seems to drive their association. Although there is common awareness that correlation does not equal causation, past research may have overestimated the association and too quickly concluded that the significant phenotypic relationship implies a transfer effect. For example, earlier studies investigating person–environment interactions emphasize how socializing processes are the driving source behind optimal self-control development [15, 34, 43]. The results of this study emphasize that understanding person–environment transactions are more complex, specifically highlighting the key role of biological factors in socializing processes. As such, if we aim to understand the mechanisms underlying person–environment transaction, it is important to incorporate both environmental and biological factors [42]. Combining these factors, thereby bridging multiple scientific disciplines, is necessary to paint a more complete picture of the etiology of individual differences in self-control.
An explanation for the role of shared genetic factors is that individuals evoke an environment as a result of their person characteristic: their self-control evokes more family connectedness and this is genetically mediated (evocative gene–environment correlation, [49]). Passive gene–environment correlation could also be a possible explanation, when there is a correlation between the environment the child is raised and the genotype a child inherits. For example, parents with high self-control are more likely to create a house environment with family connectedness but also transfer their ‘self-control’ genes to their children [49]. Perhaps gene–environment interaction is also at play, with certain genotypes being more sensitive to certain environments, positive or negative, as suggested by the diathesis stress model and the differential susceptibility theory [5, 44].
In this study, we quantified the extent to which environmental and genetic factors contribute to family connectedness, self-control and their overlap. While this approach is increasingly being applied in psychology, it is also of great use to other social sciences which traditionally do not include biological mechanisms (e.g., sociology, criminology, economics). Incorporating both genetic and environmental mechanisms is key, as without this understanding it is hard to pinpoint causal processes underlying phenomena we encounter [18]. Namely, the association between the social context (e.g., family connectedness) and child outcomes (e.g., self-control) can be the result of a true directional effect or, alternatively, be caused by common genetic factors simultaneously influencing both the family context and child outcomes (genetic pleiotropy or genetic confounding [48]). Not taking this alternative pathway into account potentially confounds research findings, hindering an attempt to reveal causal mechanisms explaining the outcome. While some researchers keep distrusting twin models (e.g., Burt and Simons [8]), years of cumulating evidence have shown the use of these models over and above the limitations these might have [50]. All traits are to a certain extent influenced by biological factors [51], and simply ignoring genetic influences would be naïve. As it would be naïve to only focus on genetic factors when investigating a child in its context. It is both nature and nurture contributing to individual differences in health and behavior, and incorporating both is an important way forward in our understanding of the way we become.
A limitation of the present study is that while we can investigate the nature of the association, we cannot unravel the more complex mechanisms underlying the association. As such, we do not know whether the association is explained by genetic pleiotropy, gene–environment correlation, gene–environment interaction, or a combination of these. Research designs such as children-of-twin designs, sibling designs, adoption studies, transmitted versus non-transmitted alleles, and interactions between polygenic risk scores and environments would allow for a deeper understanding of such mechanisms and are highly recommended for future research ([14, 2939, 47, 65]). Another limitation of the study is that we focused a certain period in adolescence (i.e., adolescents aged 14 and 16). While the current approach allowed to examine cross-age effects of family connectedness on self-control and vice versa, longitudinal studies stretching across childhood would be an important next step in our understanding of the way adolescents develop within, and in interaction with, their social world. It is also important to emphasize that we only included self-reports, possibly inflating person-specific covariances due to single-informant non-random error, and we recommend replication of our findings in the future applying a multiple-rater approach.
Alternatively, the direction of causality (DoC, Duffy and Martin [20]; Heath et al. [32]) is a very elegant model of testing directionality. There are, however, a number of conditions to be met in order for this model to work. One main condition is that the correlation between the two traits (in this case family connectedness and self-control) should be moderate to strong. This is to ensure the model gives reliable prediction of the direction of causation. As the correlation between family connectedness and self-control was small, we do not have the confidence for this model to work well with the data. In the future, with more fine-grained molecular data at hand, methods such as Mendelian randomization and genomic SEM could be promising to further unravel the causal processes underlying self-control development [24, 40].
As a concluding note, it is important to emphasize that twin models assess variance differences which does not mean change [30]. So, while we do see that change occurs over time—family connectedness and self-control are significantly and positively associated over time—the current results demonstrate that most variance is explained by family members sharing the same genes. This implies that while we can still apply association studies such as correlational analyses and cross-lagged panel models, we should be more careful in the interpretation of the underlying causes of such associations. That is, phenomena we see within families can also be the product of parent and children sharing the same genes rather than being exclusively attributable to environmental processes.

Acknowledgements

This work was partially funded by the Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek (Grant number 406-15-132, Onderzoekstalent); FP7 Ideas: European Research Council (Grant number 602768); European Research Council (Grant number 771057 WELL-BEING).
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Psychiatrie, den Premium-Inhalten der psychiatrischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Anhänge

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Anokhin AP, Golosheykin S, Grant JD, Heath AC (2011) Heritability of delay discounting in adolescence: a longitudinal twin study. Behav Genet 41(2):175–183 Anokhin AP, Golosheykin S, Grant JD, Heath AC (2011) Heritability of delay discounting in adolescence: a longitudinal twin study. Behav Genet 41(2):175–183
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Ayoub M, Briley DA, Grotzinger A, Patterson MW, Engelhardt LE, Tackett JL, Tucker-Drob EM (2019) Genetic and environmental associations between child personality and parenting. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 10(6):711–721 Ayoub M, Briley DA, Grotzinger A, Patterson MW, Engelhardt LE, Tackett JL, Tucker-Drob EM (2019) Genetic and environmental associations between child personality and parenting. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 10(6):711–721
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Bartels M, de Moor MHM, van der Aa N, Boomsma DI, de Geus EJC (2012) Regular exercise, subjective wellbeing, and internalizing problems in adolescence: causality or genetic pleiotropy? Front Genet 3:4 Bartels M, de Moor MHM, van der Aa N, Boomsma DI, de Geus EJC (2012) Regular exercise, subjective wellbeing, and internalizing problems in adolescence: causality or genetic pleiotropy? Front Genet 3:4
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Beaver KM, Ferguson CJ, Lynn-Whaley J (2010) The association between parenting and levels of self-control: a genetically informative analysis. Crim Justice Behav 37(10):1045–1065 Beaver KM, Ferguson CJ, Lynn-Whaley J (2010) The association between parenting and levels of self-control: a genetically informative analysis. Crim Justice Behav 37(10):1045–1065
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH (2007) For better and for worse: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 16(6):300–304 Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH (2007) For better and for worse: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 16(6):300–304
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Boomsma D, Busjahn A, Peltonen L (2002) Classical twin studies and beyond. Nat Rev Genet 3(11):872–882 Boomsma D, Busjahn A, Peltonen L (2002) Classical twin studies and beyond. Nat Rev Genet 3(11):872–882
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Bowlby J (1969) Attachment and loss, Vol.1: attachment. Basic Books, New York Bowlby J (1969) Attachment and loss, Vol.1: attachment. Basic Books, New York
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Burt CH, Simons RL (2014) Pulling back the curtain on heritability studies: biosocial criminology in the postgenomic era. Criminology 52(2):223–262 Burt CH, Simons RL (2014) Pulling back the curtain on heritability studies: biosocial criminology in the postgenomic era. Criminology 52(2):223–262
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Buyukcan-Tetik A, Finkenauer C, Siersema M, Vander Heyden K, Krabbendam L (2015) Social relations model analyses of perceived self-control and trust in families. J Marriage Fam 77(1):209–223 Buyukcan-Tetik A, Finkenauer C, Siersema M, Vander Heyden K, Krabbendam L (2015) Social relations model analyses of perceived self-control and trust in families. J Marriage Fam 77(1):209–223
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Casey B, Caudle K (2013) The teenage brain: Self-control. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 22(2):82–87 Casey B, Caudle K (2013) The teenage brain: Self-control. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 22(2):82–87
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Harrington H, Hogan S, Ramrakha S, Moffitt TE (2016) Childhood forecasting of a small segment of the population with large economic burden. Nat Hum Behav 1(1):0005 Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Harrington H, Hogan S, Ramrakha S, Moffitt TE (2016) Childhood forecasting of a small segment of the population with large economic burden. Nat Hum Behav 1(1):0005
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Cecil CAM, Barker ED, Jaffee SR, Viding E (2012) Association between maladaptive parenting and child self-control over time: cross-lagged study using a monozygotic twin difference design. Br J Psychiatry 201(4):291–297 Cecil CAM, Barker ED, Jaffee SR, Viding E (2012) Association between maladaptive parenting and child self-control over time: cross-lagged study using a monozygotic twin difference design. Br J Psychiatry 201(4):291–297
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Chu PS, Saucier DA, Hafner E (2010) Meta-Analysis of the relationships between social support and well-being in children and adolescents. J Soc Clin Psychol 29(6):624–645 Chu PS, Saucier DA, Hafner E (2010) Meta-Analysis of the relationships between social support and well-being in children and adolescents. J Soc Clin Psychol 29(6):624–645
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Connolly EJ (2019) Further evaluating the relationship between adverse childhood experiences, antisocial behavior, and violent victimization: a sibling-comparison analysis. Youth Violence Juv Justice 18(1):3–23 Connolly EJ (2019) Further evaluating the relationship between adverse childhood experiences, antisocial behavior, and violent victimization: a sibling-comparison analysis. Youth Violence Juv Justice 18(1):3–23
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Craig JM (2016) Which bond matters more? Assessing the differential strengths of parental bonding measures on adolescent delinquency over time. Youth Violence Juv Justice 14(3):225–242 Craig JM (2016) Which bond matters more? Assessing the differential strengths of parental bonding measures on adolescent delinquency over time. Youth Violence Juv Justice 14(3):225–242
16.
Zurück zum Zitat De Moor MHM, Boomsma DI, Stubbe JH, Willemsen G, de Geus EJC (2008) Testing causality in the association between regular exercise and symptoms of anxiety and depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65(8):897–905 De Moor MHM, Boomsma DI, Stubbe JH, Willemsen G, de Geus EJC (2008) Testing causality in the association between regular exercise and symptoms of anxiety and depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65(8):897–905
17.
Zurück zum Zitat D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, Harden KP, Slutske WS, Heath AC, Martin NG (2007) A genetically informed study of the intergenerational transmission of marital instability. J Marriage Fam 69(3):793–809 D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, Harden KP, Slutske WS, Heath AC, Martin NG (2007) A genetically informed study of the intergenerational transmission of marital instability. J Marriage Fam 69(3):793–809
18.
Zurück zum Zitat D’Onofrio BM, Lahey BB, Turkheimer E, Lichtenstein P (2013) Critical need for family-based, quasi-experimental designs in integrating geneticand social science research. Am J Public Health 103(S1):S46–S55 D’Onofrio BM, Lahey BB, Turkheimer E, Lichtenstein P (2013) Critical need for family-based, quasi-experimental designs in integrating geneticand social science research. Am J Public Health 103(S1):S46–S55
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Duckworth A, Steinberg L (2015) Unpacking self-control. Child Dev Perspect 9(1):32–37 Duckworth A, Steinberg L (2015) Unpacking self-control. Child Dev Perspect 9(1):32–37
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Duffy DL, Martin NG (1994) Inferring the direction of causation in cross-sectional twin data: theoretical and empirical considerations. Genet Epidemiol 11(6):483–502 Duffy DL, Martin NG (1994) Inferring the direction of causation in cross-sectional twin data: theoretical and empirical considerations. Genet Epidemiol 11(6):483–502
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Epstein NB, Baldwin LM, Bishop DS (1983) The McMaster family assessment device. J Marital Fam Ther 9(2):171–180 Epstein NB, Baldwin LM, Bishop DS (1983) The McMaster family assessment device. J Marital Fam Ther 9(2):171–180
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Finkenauer C, Engels R, Baumeister R (2005) Parenting behaviour and adolescent behavioural and emotional problems: the role of self-control. Int J Behav Dev 29(1):58–69 Finkenauer C, Engels R, Baumeister R (2005) Parenting behaviour and adolescent behavioural and emotional problems: the role of self-control. Int J Behav Dev 29(1):58–69
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Gillebaart M (2018) The ‘operational’ definition of self-control. Front Psychol 9:1231 Gillebaart M (2018) The ‘operational’ definition of self-control. Front Psychol 9:1231
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Grotzinger AD, Rhemtulla M, de Vlaming R, Ritchie SJ, Mallard TT, Hill WD, Koellinger PD (2019) Genomic structural equation modelling provides insights into the multivariate genetic architecture of complex traits. Nat Hum Behav 3(5):513 Grotzinger AD, Rhemtulla M, de Vlaming R, Ritchie SJ, Mallard TT, Hill WD, Koellinger PD (2019) Genomic structural equation modelling provides insights into the multivariate genetic architecture of complex traits. Nat Hum Behav 3(5):513
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Hagerty BM, Lynch-Sauer J, Patusky KL, Bouwsema M, Collier P (1992) Sense of belonging: a vital mental health concept. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 6(3):172–177 Hagerty BM, Lynch-Sauer J, Patusky KL, Bouwsema M, Collier P (1992) Sense of belonging: a vital mental health concept. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 6(3):172–177
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Harden KP, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, D'Onofrio BM, Slutske WS, Heath AC, Martin NG (2007) Marital conflict and conduct problems in children of twins. Child Dev 78(1):1–18 Harden KP, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, D'Onofrio BM, Slutske WS, Heath AC, Martin NG (2007) Marital conflict and conduct problems in children of twins. Child Dev 78(1):1–18
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Harris JR (1995) Where is the child's environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychol Rev 102(3):458–489 Harris JR (1995) Where is the child's environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychol Rev 102(3):458–489
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Hannigan LJ, McAdams TA, Plomin R, Eley TC (2016) Etiological influences on perceptions of parenting: a longitudinal, multi-informant twin study. J Youth Adolesc 45(12):2387–2405 Hannigan LJ, McAdams TA, Plomin R, Eley TC (2016) Etiological influences on perceptions of parenting: a longitudinal, multi-informant twin study. J Youth Adolesc 45(12):2387–2405
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Harold GT, Leve LD, Barrett D, Elam K, Neiderhiser JM, Natsuaki MN, Shaw D, Reiss D, Thapar A (2013) Biological and rearing mother influences on child ADHD symptoms: revisiting the developmental interface between nature and nurture. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54(10):1038–1046 Harold GT, Leve LD, Barrett D, Elam K, Neiderhiser JM, Natsuaki MN, Shaw D, Reiss D, Thapar A (2013) Biological and rearing mother influences on child ADHD symptoms: revisiting the developmental interface between nature and nurture. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54(10):1038–1046
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Harold GT, Leve LD, Sellers R (2017) How can genetically informed research help inform the next generation of interparental and parenting interventions? Child Dev 88(2):446–458 Harold GT, Leve LD, Sellers R (2017) How can genetically informed research help inform the next generation of interparental and parenting interventions? Child Dev 88(2):446–458
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Hay C, Meldrum RC, Piquero AR (2013) Negative cases in the nexus between self-control, social bonds, and delinquency. Youth Violence Juv Justice 11(1):3–25 Hay C, Meldrum RC, Piquero AR (2013) Negative cases in the nexus between self-control, social bonds, and delinquency. Youth Violence Juv Justice 11(1):3–25
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Heath AC, Kessler RC, Neale MC, Hewitt JK, Eaves LJ, Kendler KS (1993) Testing hypotheses about direction of causation using cross-sectional family data. Behav Genet 23(1):29–50 Heath AC, Kessler RC, Neale MC, Hewitt JK, Eaves LJ, Kendler KS (1993) Testing hypotheses about direction of causation using cross-sectional family data. Behav Genet 23(1):29–50
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Hu L, Bentler PM (1999) Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model 6(1):1–55 Hu L, Bentler PM (1999) Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model 6(1):1–55
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Janssen HJ, Eichelsheim VI, Deković M, Bruinsma GJ (2016) How is parenting related to adolescent delinquency? A between-and within-person analysis of the mediating role of self-control, delinquent attitudes, peer delinquency, and time spent in criminogenic settings. Eur J Criminol 13(2):169–194 Janssen HJ, Eichelsheim VI, Deković M, Bruinsma GJ (2016) How is parenting related to adolescent delinquency? A between-and within-person analysis of the mediating role of self-control, delinquent attitudes, peer delinquency, and time spent in criminogenic settings. Eur J Criminol 13(2):169–194
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Knafo A, Plomin R (2006) Parental discipline and affection and children's prosocial behavior: genetic and environmental links. J Pers Soc Psychol 90(1):147–164 Knafo A, Plomin R (2006) Parental discipline and affection and children's prosocial behavior: genetic and environmental links. J Pers Soc Psychol 90(1):147–164
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Kong A, Thorleifsson G, Frigge ML, Vilhjalmsson BJ, Young AI, Thorgeirsson TE, Gudbjartsson DF (2018) The nature of nurture: effects of parental genotypes. Science 359(6374):424–428 Kong A, Thorleifsson G, Frigge ML, Vilhjalmsson BJ, Young AI, Thorgeirsson TE, Gudbjartsson DF (2018) The nature of nurture: effects of parental genotypes. Science 359(6374):424–428
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Leve LD, Neiderhiser JM, Shaw DS, Ganiban J, Natsuaki MN, Reiss D (2013) The early growth and development study: a prospective adoption study from birth through middle childhood. Twin Res Hum Genet 16(1):412–423 Leve LD, Neiderhiser JM, Shaw DS, Ganiban J, Natsuaki MN, Reiss D (2013) The early growth and development study: a prospective adoption study from birth through middle childhood. Twin Res Hum Genet 16(1):412–423
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Malanchini M, Wang Z, Voronin I, Schenker VJ, Plomin R, Petrill SA, Kovas Y (2017) Reading self-perceived ability, enjoyment and achievement: a genetically informative study of their reciprocal links over time. Dev Psychol 53(4):698–712 Malanchini M, Wang Z, Voronin I, Schenker VJ, Plomin R, Petrill SA, Kovas Y (2017) Reading self-perceived ability, enjoyment and achievement: a genetically informative study of their reciprocal links over time. Dev Psychol 53(4):698–712
39.
Zurück zum Zitat McAdams TA, Hannigan LJ, Eilertsen EM, Gjerde LC, Ystrom E, Rijsdijk FV (2018) Revisiting the children-of-twins design: improving existing models for the exploration of intergenerational associations. Behav Genet 48(5):397–412 McAdams TA, Hannigan LJ, Eilertsen EM, Gjerde LC, Ystrom E, Rijsdijk FV (2018) Revisiting the children-of-twins design: improving existing models for the exploration of intergenerational associations. Behav Genet 48(5):397–412
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Minică CC, Dolan CV, Boomsma DI, de Geus E, Neale MC (2018) Extending causality tests with genetic instruments: an integration of Mendelian randomization with the classical twin design. Behav Genet 48(4):337–349 Minică CC, Dolan CV, Boomsma DI, de Geus E, Neale MC (2018) Extending causality tests with genetic instruments: an integration of Mendelian randomization with the classical twin design. Behav Genet 48(4):337–349
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, Caspi A (2011) A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108(7):2693–2698 Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, Caspi A (2011) A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108(7):2693–2698
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Mõttus R, Briley DA, Zheng A, Mann FD, Engelhardt LE, Tackett JL, Harden KP, Tucker-Drob EM (2019) Kids becoming less alike: a behavioral genetic analysis of developmental increases in personality variance from childhood to adolescence J Personal. Soc Psychol 117(3):635–658 Mõttus R, Briley DA, Zheng A, Mann FD, Engelhardt LE, Tackett JL, Harden KP, Tucker-Drob EM (2019) Kids becoming less alike: a behavioral genetic analysis of developmental increases in personality variance from childhood to adolescence J Personal. Soc Psychol 117(3):635–658
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Moilanen KL, Rasmussen KE, Padilla-Walker LM (2015) Bidirectional associations between self-regulation and parenting styles in early adolescence. J Res Adolesc 25(2):246–262 Moilanen KL, Rasmussen KE, Padilla-Walker LM (2015) Bidirectional associations between self-regulation and parenting styles in early adolescence. J Res Adolesc 25(2):246–262
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Monroe SM, Simons AD (1991) Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: implications for the depressive disorders. Psychol Bull 110(3):406–425 Monroe SM, Simons AD (1991) Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: implications for the depressive disorders. Psychol Bull 110(3):406–425
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Muthén LK, Muthén BO (2012) Mplus Version 7 user’s guide. Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles Muthén LK, Muthén BO (2012) Mplus Version 7 user’s guide. Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Pallini S, Chirumbolo A, Morelli M, Baiocco R, Laghi F, Eisenberg N (2018) The relation of attachment security status to effortful self-regulation: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 144(5):501–531 Pallini S, Chirumbolo A, Morelli M, Baiocco R, Laghi F, Eisenberg N (2018) The relation of attachment security status to effortful self-regulation: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 144(5):501–531
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Peyrot WJ, Milaneschi Y, Abdellaoui A, Sullivan PF, Hottenga JJ, Boomsma DI, Penninx BW (2014) Effect of polygenic risk scores ondepression in childhood trauma. Br J Psychiatry 205(2):113–119 Peyrot WJ, Milaneschi Y, Abdellaoui A, Sullivan PF, Hottenga JJ, Boomsma DI, Penninx BW (2014) Effect of polygenic risk scores ondepression in childhood trauma. Br J Psychiatry 205(2):113–119
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Pingault JB, O’reilly PF, Schoeler T, Ploubidis GB, Rijsdijk F, Dudbridge F (2018) Using genetic data to strengthen causal inference in observational research. Nat Rev Genet 19(9):566–580 Pingault JB, O’reilly PF, Schoeler T, Ploubidis GB, Rijsdijk F, Dudbridge F (2018) Using genetic data to strengthen causal inference in observational research. Nat Rev Genet 19(9):566–580
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Plomin R, Daniels D (2011) Why are children in the same family so different from one another? Int J Epidemiol 40(3):563–582 Plomin R, Daniels D (2011) Why are children in the same family so different from one another? Int J Epidemiol 40(3):563–582
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Plomin R, DeFries JC, Knopik VS, Neiderhiser JM (2016) Top 10 replicated findings from behavioral genetics. Perspect Psychol Sci 11(1):3–23 Plomin R, DeFries JC, Knopik VS, Neiderhiser JM (2016) Top 10 replicated findings from behavioral genetics. Perspect Psychol Sci 11(1):3–23
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Polderman TJ, Derks EM, Hudziak JJ, Verhulst FC, Posthuma D, Boomsma DI (2007) Across the continuum of attention skills: a twin study of the SWAN ADHD rating scale. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 48(11):1080–1087 Polderman TJ, Derks EM, Hudziak JJ, Verhulst FC, Posthuma D, Boomsma DI (2007) Across the continuum of attention skills: a twin study of the SWAN ADHD rating scale. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 48(11):1080–1087
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Polderman TJC, Benyamin B, de Leeuw CA, Sullivan PF, van Bochoven A, Visscher PM, Posthuma D (2015) Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nat Genet 47(7):702–709 Polderman TJC, Benyamin B, de Leeuw CA, Sullivan PF, van Bochoven A, Visscher PM, Posthuma D (2015) Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nat Genet 47(7):702–709
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Rebollo I, de Moor MHM, Dolan CV, Boomsma DI (2006) Phenotypic factor analysis of family data: correction of the bias due to dependency. Twin Res Hum Genet 9(3):367–376 Rebollo I, de Moor MHM, Dolan CV, Boomsma DI (2006) Phenotypic factor analysis of family data: correction of the bias due to dependency. Twin Res Hum Genet 9(3):367–376
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Rietveld MJ, van Der Valk JC, Bongers IL, Stroet TM, Slagboom PE, Boomsma DI (2000) Zygosity diagnosis in young twins by parental report. Twin Res Hum Genet 3(3):134–141 Rietveld MJ, van Der Valk JC, Bongers IL, Stroet TM, Slagboom PE, Boomsma DI (2000) Zygosity diagnosis in young twins by parental report. Twin Res Hum Genet 3(3):134–141
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Rimfeld K, Kovas Y, Dale PS, Plomin R (2016) True grit and genetics: predicting academic achievement from personality. J Pers Soc Psychol 111(5):780–789 Rimfeld K, Kovas Y, Dale PS, Plomin R (2016) True grit and genetics: predicting academic achievement from personality. J Pers Soc Psychol 111(5):780–789
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Rowe DC (1990) As the twig is bent? The myth of child-rearing influences on personality development. J Couns Dev 68(6):606–611 Rowe DC (1990) As the twig is bent? The myth of child-rearing influences on personality development. J Couns Dev 68(6):606–611
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Sameroff A (2010) A unified theory of development: a dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Dev 81(1):6–22 Sameroff A (2010) A unified theory of development: a dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Dev 81(1):6–22
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Sheffield Morris A, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Myers SS, Robinson LR (2007) The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc Dev 16(2):361–388 Sheffield Morris A, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Myers SS, Robinson LR (2007) The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc Dev 16(2):361–388
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Tiberio SS, Capaldi DM, Kerr DCR, Bertrand M, Pears KC, Owen L (2016) Parenting and the development of effortful control from early childhood to early adolescence: a transactional developmental model. Dev Psychopathol 28(3):837–853 Tiberio SS, Capaldi DM, Kerr DCR, Bertrand M, Pears KC, Owen L (2016) Parenting and the development of effortful control from early childhood to early adolescence: a transactional developmental model. Dev Psychopathol 28(3):837–853
60.
Zurück zum Zitat van Beijsterveldt CEM, Groen-Blokhuis M, Hottenga JJ, Franić S, Hudziak JJ, Lamb D, Boomsma DI (2013) The young netherlands twin register (YNTR): longitudinal twin and family studies in over 70,000 Children. Twin Res Hum Genet 16(01):252–267 van Beijsterveldt CEM, Groen-Blokhuis M, Hottenga JJ, Franić S, Hudziak JJ, Lamb D, Boomsma DI (2013) The young netherlands twin register (YNTR): longitudinal twin and family studies in over 70,000 Children. Twin Res Hum Genet 16(01):252–267
61.
Zurück zum Zitat van der Aa N, Boomsma DI, Rebollo-Mesa I, Hudziak JJ, Bartels M (2010) Moderation of genetic factors by parental divorce in adolescents’ evaluations of family functioning and subjective wellbeing. Twin Res Hum Genet 13(02):143–162 van der Aa N, Boomsma DI, Rebollo-Mesa I, Hudziak JJ, Bartels M (2010) Moderation of genetic factors by parental divorce in adolescents’ evaluations of family functioning and subjective wellbeing. Twin Res Hum Genet 13(02):143–162
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Wenniger WFMDB, Hageman WJJM, Arrindell WA (1993) Cross-national validity of dimensions of family functioning: first experiences with the Dutch version of the McMaster Family Assesment Device (FAD). Personal Individ Differ 14:769–781 Wenniger WFMDB, Hageman WJJM, Arrindell WA (1993) Cross-national validity of dimensions of family functioning: first experiences with the Dutch version of the McMaster Family Assesment Device (FAD). Personal Individ Differ 14:769–781
63.
Zurück zum Zitat Willems YE, Dolan CV, van Beijsterveldt CEM, de Zeeuw EL, Boomsma DI, Bartels M, Finkenauer C (2018) Genetic and environmental influences on self-control: assessing self-control with the ASEBA self-control scale. Behavior Genetics 48(2):1–12 Willems YE, Dolan CV, van Beijsterveldt CEM, de Zeeuw EL, Boomsma DI, Bartels M, Finkenauer C (2018) Genetic and environmental influences on self-control: assessing self-control with the ASEBA self-control scale. Behavior Genetics 48(2):1–12
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Willems YE, Boesen N, Li J, Finkenauer C, Bartels M (2019) The heritability of self-control: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 100:324–334 Willems YE, Boesen N, Li J, Finkenauer C, Bartels M (2019) The heritability of self-control: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 100:324–334
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Willems YE, de Zeeuw EL, van Beijsterveldt CE, Boomsma DI, Bartels M, Finkenauer C (2019) Out of control: examining the association between family conflict and self-control in adolescence in a genetically sensitive design. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 59(2):254–262 Willems YE, de Zeeuw EL, van Beijsterveldt CE, Boomsma DI, Bartels M, Finkenauer C (2019) Out of control: examining the association between family conflict and self-control in adolescence in a genetically sensitive design. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 59(2):254–262
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Wright J, Beaver K, Delisi M, Vaughn M (2008) Evidence of negligible parenting influences on self-control, delinquent peers, and delinquency in a sample of twins. Justice Q 25(3):544–569 Wright J, Beaver K, Delisi M, Vaughn M (2008) Evidence of negligible parenting influences on self-control, delinquent peers, and delinquency in a sample of twins. Justice Q 25(3):544–569
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Yamagata S, Takahashi Y, Kijima N, Maekawa H, Ono Y, Ando J (2005) Genetic and environmental etiology of effortful control. Twin Res Hum Genet 8(4):300–306 Yamagata S, Takahashi Y, Kijima N, Maekawa H, Ono Y, Ando J (2005) Genetic and environmental etiology of effortful control. Twin Res Hum Genet 8(4):300–306
Metadaten
Titel
Investigating the association between family connectedness and self-control in adolescence in a genetically sensitive design
verfasst von
Yayouk E. Willems
Odilia M. Laceulle
Meike Bartels
Catrin Finkenauer
Publikationsdatum
05.02.2020
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry / Ausgabe 12/2020
Print ISSN: 1018-8827
Elektronische ISSN: 1435-165X
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-020-01485-9

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 12/2020

European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 12/2020 Zur Ausgabe

Demenzkranke durch Antipsychotika vielfach gefährdet

23.04.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Wenn Demenzkranke aufgrund von Symptomen wie Agitation oder Aggressivität mit Antipsychotika behandelt werden, sind damit offenbar noch mehr Risiken verbunden als bislang angenommen.

Weniger postpartale Depressionen nach Esketamin-Einmalgabe

Bislang gibt es kein Medikament zur Prävention von Wochenbettdepressionen. Das Injektionsanästhetikum Esketamin könnte womöglich diese Lücke füllen.

„Psychotherapie ist auch bei sehr alten Menschen hochwirksam!“

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Kongressbericht

Die Kombination aus Medikamenten und Psychotherapie gilt als effektivster Ansatz bei Depressionen. Das ist bei betagten Menschen nicht anders, trotz Besonderheiten.

Auf diese Krankheiten bei Geflüchteten sollten Sie vorbereitet sein

22.04.2024 DGIM 2024 Nachrichten

Um Menschen nach der Flucht aus einem Krisengebiet bestmöglich medizinisch betreuen zu können, ist es gut zu wissen, welche Erkrankungen im jeweiligen Herkunftsland häufig sind. Dabei hilft eine Internetseite der CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention).

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.