Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Journal of Pediatrics 7/2007

Open Access 01.07.2007 | Review

Long-term cognitive and behavioral consequences of neonatal encephalopathy following perinatal asphyxia: a review

verfasst von: Mariëlle van Handel, Hanna Swaab, Linda S. de Vries, Marian J. Jongmans

Erschienen in: European Journal of Pediatrics | Ausgabe 7/2007

Abstract

Neonatal encephalopathy (NE) following perinatal asphyxia (PA) is considered an important cause of later neurodevelopmental impairment in infants born at term. This review discusses long-term consequences for general cognitive functioning, educational achievement, neuropsychological functioning and behavior. In all areas reviewed, the outcome of children with mild NE is consistently positive and the outcome of children with severe NE consistently negative. However, children with moderate NE form a more heterogeneous group with respect to outcome. On average, intelligence scores are below those of children with mild NE and age-matched peers, but within the normal range. With respect to educational achievement, difficulties have been found in the domains reading, spelling and arithmetic/mathematics. So far, studies of neuropsychological functioning have yielded ambiguous results in children with moderate NE. A few studies suggest elevated rates of hyperactivity in children with moderate NE and autism in children with moderate and severe NE. Conclusion: Behavioral monitoring is required for all children with NE. In addition, systematic, detailed neuropsychological examination is needed especially for children with moderate NE.
Abkürzungen
NE
neonatal encephalopathy
PA
perinatal asphyxia
HIE
hypoxic ischemic encephalopathy
ADHD
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
CP
cerebral palsy
EEG
electro encephalogram
MRI
magnetic resonance imaging
IQ
intelligence quotient

Introduction

It has been widely accepted that severe NE following PA can lead to serious motor disabilities, mental retardation and seizure disorders [59]. Traditionally, research in this area has focused on developmental consequences at a very young age. In addition, the effects of NE have often been treated as an all-or-nothing phenomenon; patients have been believed to show a completely normal development or become severely disabled. So far, few studies have looked at milder consequences of NE at school age.

From perinatal asphyxia to neonatal encephalopathy

PA can be defined as impaired respiratory gas exchange accompanied by the development of acidosis [30]. The pathway of PA can have its onset in the antepartum, intrapartum or postpartum period. Estimates of the incidence of PA vary from 1 to 8 per 1,000 live births [4]. This wide range can be largely attributed to problems in selecting indicators to identify children with PA. As a consequence, researchers have used many different sets of inclusion criteria when studying the effects of PA. For this reason, it is difficult to compare results regarding developmental outcome of infants with PA across studies.
In the past, single-marker definitions were commonly used to diagnose infants with PA (e.g., low Apgar score). However, these single markers have relatively low sensitivity and specificity for accurate diagnosis [54]. Moreover, they lack predictive power [3, 20, 3537, 40, 51]. Therefore, a shift has been made towards multiple-marker definitions based on combinations of indicators [16, 35]. Among the perinatal indicators are fetal distress (abnormal heart rate or meconium-stained amniotic fluid), delay in onset of spontaneous respiration, low Apgar score (<6 or 7 at 5 min), need for resuscitation and/or ventilation, and metabolic acidosis (cord pH <7.0 or 7.1 and base deficit >12 mmol/l). Postnatal indicators of PA include NE, multi-organ failure and abnormal findings on brain imaging. Of the above, the concept of NE is generally accepted as an essential criterion for the diagnosis of PA [4, 30]. NE has been defined as a clinical syndrome of disturbed neurological function in the earliest days of life in term infants, manifested by difficulty with initiating and maintaining respiration, depression of tone and reflexes, subnormal level of consciousness and often by seizures [30].
Recent studies of the protective effects of head cooling and systemic hypothermia after NE following PA used a combination of clinical and physiological criteria of PA (including a pH of 7.0 or less or a base deficit of 16 mmol per liter or more) and a neurological examination to determine the severity of NE [22, 52]. These inclusion criteria are closely related to a set of criteria postulated by The American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists (ACOG). There has been extensive debate about the relationships between PA, NE and serious possible consequences such as CP [25]. Contrary to what was once believed, only a small part of all cases of CP are caused by intrapartum events [10]. Likewise, many cases of NE are not associated with a history of PA [6, 38]. However, NE has to be present in the pathway from PA to subsequent CP [25]. The ACOG and the International Cerebral Palsy Task Force proposed a set of criteria to define an acute intrapartum hypoxic event that could subsequently lead to CP [4, 30]. The presence of moderate or severe NE and metabolic acidosis (pH <7 and base deficit ≥12 mmol/l) are considered to be essential markers for hypoxic ischemic injury. In addition, these criteria underline the importance of excluding other identifiable causes of NE before assuming asphyxia as the underlying etiology.
Both in clinical practice and for scientific purposes, patients suffering from NE are often assigned an encephalopathy score. Several classification systems can be identified. Leviton and Nelson provide an overview of qualitative and quantitative grading schemes [27]. In addition, scoring systems were developed that are applicable in low-income settings. Moreover, a new grading system has gained interest in the literature [19, 33]. The classification system of Sarnat and Sarnat developed in 1976 [50] is still widely used and the basis for most modern classification systems. In this review, the Sarnat system is used to grade NE because in most studies that were included this system it had been applied. According to the Sarnat system children are assigned a score of 1, 2 or 3 (1= mild NE; 2= moderate NE; 3= severe NE). Stage 1 lasts less than 24 h and is characterized by hyperalertness, uninhibited Moro and stretch reflexes, sympathetic effects and a normal EEG. Stage 2 is marked by obtundation, hypotonia, strong distal flexion and multifocal seizures. Infants with the most severe stage of NE (stage 3) are stuporous and flaccid, and brain stem and autonomic functions are suppressed. In addition, the EEG shows abnormal patterns of brain activity.

Grades of neonatal encephalopathy in relation to developmental outcome

Encephalopathy scores have often been used to predict neurodevelopmental outcome [43, 44, 46]. Although strong relationships exist between the categories of NE and developmental outcome, diversity of outcome is reported within individual grades [41]. This diversity may be partly explained by individual researchers using different neurological signs for category inclusion [27]. The Sarnat grading system has helped to clarify the NE categories and improved their usefulness in studies of prognosis [45].
Most outcome studies have focused on neurological functioning and severe deficits in young children (<4 years). In general, very few children with mild encephalopathy show neurological impairments or have developed severe mental or motor retardation at preschool age. In contrast, children with severe encephalopathy nearly always die or develop severe impairments such as CP, mental retardation, epilepsy and in some cases sensorineural hearing loss or cortical visual impairment [4, 45, 47]. While dyskinetic CP or quadriplegia is more commonly seen following “acute near total type of asphyxia” with MRI abnormalities present in the deep grey nuclei and in the perirolandic cortex, milder forms of CP, such as spastic diplegia, are more common among those with “subacute partial asphyxia” with MRI abnormalities in the watershed areas. Hemiplegia can be seen following “perinatal arterial stroke.” Although this condition will present with NE and seizures, it is less commonly associated with PA [13, 34]. Children who have suffered moderate encephalopathy seem to form a more heterogeneous group; research shows varying rates of infant death and morbidity [16].

Need for long-term follow-up and focus on cognitive and behavioral outcomes

Because the majority of studies have focused on detecting major developmental abnormalities at a very young age, still little is known about the milder difficulties children may experience in the long run. Recently, however, research has started to focus more on the subtle problems of children without major neurological deficits [8, 23, 32, 35, 43, 44, 46]. Knowledge of the outcome for this group of children is important because these subtle problems can affect later scholastic achievement and behavioral adjustment.
Cognitive and behavioral difficulties can be expected because of the patterns of brain injury that have been associated with NE. The hippocampus and striatum are among the brain structures that can be affected [7, 21, 31, 42, 56]. These structures have been associated with specific cognitive functions such as memory and attention and hypothesized to play a role in the pathogenesis of ADHD, autism and schizophrenia [1416, 28, 57].
To gain insight into the long-term effects of NE on development, follow-up is required throughout the school-age period. Specific cognitive functions continue to develop throughout childhood. As it is impossible to examine a function that has not developed yet, short-term follow-up cannot exclude minor cognitive and behavioral difficulties at school age. Moreover, subtle functional deficits usually do not become apparent until a child faces increasing demands to master complex abilities in school.
The aim of this review was to discuss the results of previously published studies on the possible effects of NE on general cognitive functioning, educational achievement, neuropsychological functioning and behavior from infancy through adolescence. A literature search was performed using Web of Science. Studies were screened that had been published before October 2006. Search terms were ’asphyxia neonatorum,’ ‘neonatal encephalopathy,’ ‘newborn encephalopathy,’ ‘hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy’ and ‘perinatal asphyxia’ combined with the terms ‘cognition/cognitive’ and ‘behavior(-al)’. Reference lists of retrieved articles were screened for additional relevant publications. Studies were selected that reported the developmental outcome of NE or HIE following PA of children until the age of 18 years. The presence of NE was required for selection. When no additional indicators of PA were used, the asphyxial etiology of the NE had to be assumed by the authors. Studies were excluded when NE was partially caused by other conditions than PA, for example [17, 34]. Also, studies were excluded in which outcome was presented in preset categories (e.g., normal versus abnormal), in which several outcome measures were combined, for example [11, 18, 26]. In these cases it was not possible to judge whether a child had been assigned to the category ‘abnormal’ because of cognitive or behavioral problems or because of other problems, e.g., motor difficulties. A third reason for excluding studies from this review was the selection of children from a larger group of eligible patients. For example, some researchers selected only those children with abnormal findings on MRI in order to examine the correlation with outcome [61]. In this review, outcome is reported according to grade of NE (mild, moderate or severe). Most authors of the included studies used the classification system of Sarnat when determining the severity of the NE. However, in three cases the authors significantly deviated from this system [32, 47, 53]. In order to increase comparability across studies, the patients in these studies were reclassified according to Sarnat criteria. Articles were reviewed that reported general intellectual ability, levels of specific cognitive functions and/or behavioral problems. Details of the samples, inclusion criteria, outcome measures and results are described in Tables 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7 and 8.
Table 1
Griffiths Mental Development Scale* at 1 year
 
CP
No CP
Carli et al. (2004) [12]
Moderate NE, N=13
Moderate NE, N=27
• N=10: <−2 SD
• N=4: between −2 SD and −1 SD
• N=1: between −2 SD and −1 SD
• N=23: >−1 SD
• N=1: >−1 SD
 
• N=1: untestable
 
Gray et al. (1993) [24]
Moderate NE, N=10
Moderate NE, N=9
• N=9: <55
Mean =118 (N=1 not assessed)
• N=1: 87
 
Thompson et al. (1997) [55]
No grades of NE recorded, N=16
No grades of NE recorded, N=24
Mean =47, range: 7–101
Mean =16, range 94–128
Barnett et al. (2004) [9]
 
No grades of NE recorded, N=59
 
Mean =100.34, SD=15.05, range: 55–130
Rutherford et al. (1996) [49]
Moderate NE, N=8
Mild NE, N=4
All below normal (cut-off point not reported)
All normal
Severe NE, N=1
 
Below normal
 
*Griffiths mean of standardization sample =100.18, SD=12.76
Table 2
IQ at 2–6 years (no CP)
 
Age (years)
Mild NE
Moderate NE
Severe NE
Barnett et al. (2004)b [9]
2
N=45,f Griffiths: mean =103.53, SD=12.33 (range: 81–140)
Robertson & Finer (1985)a [43]
3.5
N=66, Stanford-Binet: mean IQ =101.5, SD=14.0
N=94 (21.3% handicapped) Stanford-Binet2: mean IQ =92.3, SD=23.2
N=7, Stanford-Binet2: mean IQ =37.1, SD=26.7
Shankaran (1991)c b [53]
5
N=14,f McCarthy: 64%: >−1 SD, 86%: >−2 SD
Barnett et al. (2002)b d [8]
5.5–6.5
N=20, WPPSI-R: mean IQ =109.7, SD=14.6 (N=1: 76, rest: >90)
N=12, WPPSI-R: (N=1: untestable) N=11: mean IQ =106.2, SD=11.8 (N=1: 84, rest: >94)
 
Barnett et al. (2004)b [9]
5–6
N=53,f WPPSI-R 85%: >−1 SD, mean IQ =101.98, SD=16.06, range: 69–139
Robertson & Finer (1988)e [44]
5.5
N=56, Stanford-Binet: mean IQ =106, SD=12
N=71, Stanford-Binet: mean IQ =99, SD=18)
 
Griffiths Mental Developmental Scales: mean of standardization sample =100.18, SD=12.76; Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales: mean of standardization sample =100, SD=16; McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities: mean of standardization sample =100, SD=16; Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-revised: mean of standardization sample =100, SD=15. aAll groups are significantly different from the other groups; p=0.001. bNo significance tests performed. cPatients were reclassified by author (MH) according to the classification system of Sarnat [50]. dIn the original article, results are presented for individual cases. Group means and standard deviations have been calculated by one of the authors (MH). eMild NE not significantly below comparison group; moderate NE significantly below mild NE and comparison group. fCombined group of mild, moderate and severe NE
Table 3
IQ at 7–9 years (no CP)
 
Age (years)
Mild NE
Moderate NE
Severe NE
Marlow (2005)b [32]
7
 
N=65, BAS-II:
 
Group 1 (N=34): GCS=112.3, SD=11.2
Group 2d (N=31): GCS=102.7, SD=13.2
Robertson et al. (1989)a [46]
8
N=56, WISC-R: mean IQ =106, SD=13
N=66, WISC-R: mean IQ =102, SD=17
5, WISC-R: mean IQ =36, SD=7
Robertson (1997)c [47]
9
 
N=64, WISC-R: mean IQ =100, SD=14
 
WISC-R: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-revised: mean of standardization sample =100, SD=15; BAS-II: British Ability Scales, GCS: General Cognitive Score: mean of standardization sample =100, SD=15. aIQ of mild NE is not significantly lower than IQ of comparison group. IQ of moderate and severe NE is significantly lower than IQ of mild NE and comparison group (p < 0.001). bGSC of group 1 is not significantly lower than GSC of comparison group. GCS of group 2 is significantly lower than GCS of comparison group (p < 0.01). cMean IQ is significantly lower than IQ of comparison group. dMarlow refers to this group as ‘severe NE.’ This group is reclassified by one fo the authors (MH) according to the classification system of Sarnat [50]
Table 4
Scholastic abilities at 7–13 years (no CP)
 
Mild NE
Moderate NE
Severe NE
Reading
Robertson & Finer (1989) [46]
13% >1 grade level below expected level for age
35% >1 grade level below expected level for agea
100% >1 grade level below expected level for ageb
Robertson (1997) [47]
 
41% >1 grade level below expected level for aged
 
Marlow et al. (2005) [32]
 
Group 1: < control groupc
 
  
Group 2f: < control groupd
 
Moster et al. (2002) [35]
NE < control groupd
NE < control groupd
NE < control groupd
Writing
Marlow et al. (2005) [32]
 
Group 1: = control group
 
Group 2f: < control groupe
Moster et al. (2002) [35]
NE = control group
NE = control group
NE = control group
Spelling
Robertson & Finer (1989) [46]
2% >1 grade level below expected level for age
18% >1 grade level below expected level for agea
100% >1 grade level below expected level for ageb
Marlow et al. (2005) [32]
 
Group 1: < control groupc
 
Group 2f: < control groupd
Moster et al. (2002) [35]
NE = control group
NE = control group
NE = control group
Math
Robertson & Finer (1989) [46]
16% >1 grade level below expected level for age
20% >1 grade level below expected level for agea
100% >1 grade level below expected level for ageb
Robertson (1997) [47]
 
39% >1 grade level below expected level for aged
 
Marlow et al. (2005) [32]
 
Group 1:  = control group
 
Group 2f: < control groupd
Moster et al. (2002) [35]
NE < control groupc
NE < control groupc
NE < control groupc
Robertson & Finer (1989) [46]: age 8 years; Marlow et al. (2005) [32]: age 7 years; Moster et al. (2002) [35]: age 8–13 years
aSignificant difference with mild NE and comparison group, p < 0.01; bstatistical analyses are not given because of small group and low scores; csignificant difference with comparison group, p < 0.05; dsignificant difference with comparison group, p < 0.001; esignificant difference with comparison group, p < 0.01; fMarlow refers to this group as ‘severe NE’. This group is reclassified by one of the authors (MH) according to the classification system of Sarnat [50]
Table 5
Neuropsychological functions [43, 44, 46]
Tests
3.5 years
5.5 years
8 years
Receptive vocabulary
Mild NE: in the mean range
Mild NE = control group
Mild NE = control group
Moderate NE < mild NEa
Moderate NE< mild NE+ control groupb
Moderate NE< mild NE+ control groupa
Severe NE < mild + moderate NEa
Severe NE: 100% < −1 SD c
Severe NE < mild NE + control group c
Visual- motor integration
Mild NE: in the mean range
Mild NE = control group
Mild NE = control group
Moderate NE < mild NEa
Moderate NE < mild NE + control groupa
Moderate NE < mild NE + control groupa Severe NE < mild NE + control group c
Severe NE < mild + moderate NEa
Severe NE: 100% < −1 SD c
 
Receptive vocabulary: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, visual-motor integration: Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration. ap < 0.001; bp < 0.01; cstatistical analyses are not given because of small group and low scores
Table 6
Neuropsychological functions (NEPSY): moderate NE compared to control group [32]
Age: 7 years
Moderate NE group 1
Moderate NE group 2*
Attention and executive
Group 1 = control group (p=0.08)
Group 2 < control group (p < 0.01)
Language
Group 1 < control group (p=0.01)
Group 2 < control group (p < 0.01)
Sensorimotor
Group 1 < control group (p=0.04)
Group 2 = control group (p=0.10)
Visuospatial
Group 1 = control group (p=0.14)
Group 2 < control group (p=0.02)
Memory and learning
Group 1 = control group (p=0.61)
Group 2 < control group (p < 0.01)
*Marlow refers to this group as ‘severe NE.’ This group is reclassified by one of the authors (MH) according to the classification system of Sarnat [50]
Table 7
Verbal memory (Rey’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test)* [31]
Age: 16 years
Moderate NE
Total words learned trial 1–5
NE: 52.5, SD=8.7
Control group: 57, SD=5.8
Delayed recall (p=0.034)
NE: 11.6, SD=2.2
Control group: 13.3, SD=1.6
Recognition (p=0.011)
NE: 14.2, SD=1.0
Control group: 15, SD=0.0
*Raw scores (number correct)
Table 8
Verbal memory (Children’s auditory verbal learning test)* [47]
Age: 9 years
Moderate NE
Level of learninga
NE: 98, SD=16
Control group: 104, SD=15
Immediate recalla
NE: 96, SD=18
Control group: 105, SD=15
Delayed recalla
NE: 95, SD=15
Control group: 103, SD=16
Recognition accuracy
NE: 22% delayedb
Control group: 27% delayed
Total intrusions
NE: 19% delayedb
Control group: 20% delayed
Immediate attention
NE: 97, SD=15
Control group: 100, SD=15
*Mean of standardization sample =100, SD=15, ap < 0.01; bDelayed is below the 17th percentile of the standardization sample

General cognitive development

Infancy (0–24 months)

Eight studies were identified that measured developmental levels between the ages of 12 and 24 months in children with NE [2, 9, 12, 24, 39, 49, 50, 55]. Two of these were performed in low-income, non-western countries [2, 55]. Standardized testing with the Griffiths Mental Development Scales showed that almost all children with moderate NE who had been diagnosed to have CP at the age of 12 months were severely developmentally delayed (Table 1). In these studies, the rates of CP among children with moderate NE varied between 23% [24] and 82% [49]. Children with moderate NE without CP at this age performed in the average range or above, although the range of scores varies widely across studies. In addition, other studies in which the Denver Developmental Screening Test or the Bayley Scales of Infant Development were used reported normal development in children with mild NE, none of whom had CP.

Two to six years, without CP

In all of the selected studies [8, 9, 43, 44, 53], children with mild NE showed average general intellectual abilities that equaled those of a healthy comparison group (Table 2). Although children with moderate NE also performed in the average range, their abilities were significantly below those of children with mild NE and a reference group. The general intellectual abilities of children with severe NE were assessed in only one study. These children showed very low levels of functioning: a mean IQ of 37.1 was found [43]. In two studies, no grades of NE were recorded [9, 53], which hampers interpretation of these data. Rates of average or above-average functioning were 85% and 64%, respectively.

Seven to nine years, without CP

At school age, children with mild NE showed age-appropriate levels of general intellectual ability and performed as well as healthy comprison children on standardized testing [32, 46, 47] (Table 3). Although most children with moderate NE obtained significantly lower scores on intelligence measures than children with mild NE and comparison groups, their ability levels were in the average range.

Educational achievement

Relative strengths and weaknesses in cognitive functioning can become visible when children have to acquire specific scholastic abilities in school. A test of school readiness showed that 5.5-year-old children with mild and moderate NE were not significantly behind compared to comparison children [24]. However, children with moderate NE achieved lower scores in all domains, and 42% of them scored more than 1 SD below the average of the standardization group. Children with severe NE showed low school readiness scores at the age of 5.5 years. Table 4 gives an overview of the results of four studies, which assessed reading, writing, spelling and mathematics/arithmetic in school-aged children [32, 35, 46, 47]. Survivors of mild NE compared to a comparison group were not significantly more often more than one grade behind in reading, spelling and arithmetic. In these domains, all children with severe NE were more than one grade behind the expected level for their age. The group with moderate NE appeared to have distinct difficulties in reading, spelling and arithmetic/mathematics. Writing has been found to be less developed in only one subgroup of one of the studies.

Neuropsychological functions

Studies of specific neuropsychological functions yielded results that are similar to the findings on general intellectual ability and educational achievement (Tables 5, 6, 7 and 8). Children who suffered from mild NE tended to show normal receptive vocabulary and visual-motor integration at 3.5, 5.5 and 8 years of age [43, 44, 46]. As is the case in other cognitive domains, the severe group was impaired, and their ability levels were below those of children with mild or moderate NE and comparison groups. The moderate group showed an ambiguous pattern of performance. Deficits were found in receptive vocabulary, language and visual-motor integration. In contrast, attention and executive functions, visuospatial ability, and memory and learning were normal in a subgroup of 7-year-old children [29]. Although memory and learning did not seem to be impaired in this subgroup, these children had difficulties in the sub-domains narrative memory and sentence repetition, compared to intact memory for faces, names, everyday events and intact orientation. In addition, auditory verbal learning and delayed recall was impaired in a group of 9- and a group of 16-year-old children with moderate NE [31, 47]. Tests of other auditory skills in the former showed intact auditory attention and concentration, language and binaural separation, in contrast to impaired binaural integration, and sequencing and labeling [47].

Behavioral problems

So far, few researchers have addressed the issue of behavioral problems in children with a history of NE. Four studies were identified that met the selection criteria. In general, hyperactivity was more often present in children with moderate NE, but not in children with mild NE [32, 35, 44]. In addition, one study that used parent’s observations of their child’s behavior found more problems related to tractability, aggression, passivity and anxiety in a mixed group of children with NE compared to a control group [35]. No differences were found, however, related to attention, habituation and impulsivity. A recent study reported an unexpectedly high proportion of children with moderate and severe NE that developed an autism spectrum disorder [5]. However, the inclusion criteria differed considerably from those of Sarnat. Both groups can be classified as moderate NE when Sarnat’s criteria are applied. In addition, the authors explicitly did not exclude etiologies of NE other than PA.

Discussion and conclusion

In the introduction section of this review, problems were mentioned with regard to the comparability of different outcome studies of NE. Even after careful selection on the basis of predetermined criteria, comparing studies remained difficult. The main reason for this was the way test results were presented. In many cases, outcome of the children was reported in two or more categories, e.g., normal/mildly delayed/abnormal. One of the problems that arise due to such categorization, is the subjectivity involved in determining the cut-off points of the categories. Often, these cut-off points seemed to be chosen arbitrarily or not justified at all. Another issue that adds to this problem is the use of different tests of development, cognitive ability or behavioral problems. One can hardly compare, for example, the proportions of children with developmental delay in two studies when these are represented by a score below 85 on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development in one of the studies and by a score below 70 on the Griffiths Mental Developmental Scales in the other. To enable readers to compare studies, results of significance testing between subgroups of children with NE and comparison groups were added to the tables whenever available.
Children with a history of NE who suffer from CP at the age of 12 months are often also severely developmentally delayed. Estimates vary between 77% [12] and 90% [24]. Of the children without CP, those with mild NE have an excellent prognosis. Their intelligence, educational achievement and neuropsychological functions are comparable to healthy peers at least until middle childhood.
Children with severe NE appear to be impaired in every cognitive domain. They are less intelligent and perform worse at school and at neuropsychological tests than both healthy controls and children with mild or moderate NE.
As was expected, the children with moderate NE seem to form the most heterogeneous group. In general, these children are significantly less intelligent than children with mild NE and healthy peers, but their scores are still in the average range. And although these children are equally ready to go to school as other children at the age of 5.5 years, when measured in middle childhood, they perform less well in the domains reading, spelling and mathematics. On the basis of these results, it is possible that children with moderate NE fall behind in school, but do not show clear deficits at standardized testing. Another issue is raised by the relatively broad range of performance of children with moderate NE. Calculating mean IQ scores masks individual differences, which are large in groups of children with moderate NE. Therefore, means do not describe this group well, and it would be more informative to look at the distribution of scores. The description of neuropsychological functions in this review is based on six studies, in which five different tests were used in five different samples of children with moderate NE. These studies do not provide sufficient and comparable data to draw reliable conclusions about patterns of neuropsychological strengths and difficulties. The heterogeneous nature of the group of children with moderate NE raises the question whether these children really form one group. Marlow et al. [32] divided a group of children with moderate NE according to the severity of the symptoms of the NE. The group with the least severe neonatal symptoms showed a few discrete neuropsychological difficulties, but was more intelligent and performed better in school than the group with the most severe neonatal symptoms. Marlow suggested a dose-response effect of hypoxia within this group with moderate NE. This idea of a continuum of casualty with regard to morbidity has already been proposed by Low [29]. He suggested a theory of a critical threshold of asphyxia beyond which brain damage occurs. Beyond this threshold, a spectrum of minor and major deficits would occur. So, although the classification system of Sarnat [50] appears to be very useful for the prediction of the outcome of children with mild or severe NE, in case of moderate NE this qualitative system lacks predictive power. Future research should therefore focus on developing a reliable, more gradual model of prediction.
Another issue regarding prediction of outcome after NE also mainly concerns moderately affected children. It is still unclear whether NE has a general effect on intellectual abilities or leads to specific patterns of cognitive strengths and difficulties. This review shows that systematic, detailed research of neuropsychological function is still scarce. A few case reports, which are not included in this review, have shown specific impairments in episodic memory (memory for events), with relatively intact semantic memory (memory for facts) [21, 58]. However, these findings, which were based on small numbers of children, do not justify generalization to all children with NE and should therefore be replicated in larger, well-defined samples.
The introduction section of this review referred to possible associations between brain areas that are vulnerable to perinatal hypoxia and psychopathology, such as ADHD and autism. Long-term behavioral outcome of survivors of NE has received very little attention compared to neurological outcome and general intellectual ability. Only a few studies looked at the behavioral consequences of NE. Those studies found elevated rates of hyperactivity and autism in children with moderate NE. Evidence of hyperactivity and impulsive behavior has also been found in animal studies of PA [1, 60]. These findings suggest that, in addition to cognitive impairment, children with NE could be at risk of developing behavioral problems. Because of the clinical implications this would have, future research should include behavioral monitoring of all children with NE.
Long-term follow-up of children with NE has been advocated before, including the optimal ages of assessment of specific functions [48]. Recommendations include assessment of general cognitive and adaptive behavioral functioning at preschool age, and, in some cases, school achievement tests and a neuropsychological screening at school age. The authors of this review would like to go one step further in recommending standard screening for behavioral problems in all children with NE, regardless of their level of cognitive functioning. In addition, detailed neuropsychological assessment is recommended especially in all children with moderate NE.
From this review it is concluded that general intellectual, educational and neuropsychological outcomes are consistently positive for children with mild NE and negative for severely affected children. However, children with moderate NE form a more heterogeneous group with respect to outcome. On average, intelligence scores are below those of children with mild NE and age-matched peers, but within the normal range. Difficulties have been found in the domains reading, spelling and arithmetic/mathematics. So far, neuropsychological functioning of children with NE has received relatively little attention. The studies that were selected for this review have yielded ambiguous results in children with moderate NE. A few studies suggest elevated rates of hyperactivity in children with moderate NE and autism in children with moderate and severe NE. Therefore, behavioral monitoring is required for all children with NE. In addition, systematic, detailed neuropsychological examination is needed especially for children with moderate NE.

Acknowledgement

This research is supported by a grant of the Princess Beatrix Fund (MAR03-0122).
Open Access This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License ( https://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by-nc/​2.​0 ), which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Jetzt e.Med zum Sonderpreis bestellen!

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Jetzt bestellen und 100 € sparen!

e.Med Pädiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Pädiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Pädiatrie, den Premium-Inhalten der pädiatrischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Pädiatrie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Adriani W, Giannakopoulou D, Bokulic Z, Jernej B, Alleva E, Laviola G (2006) Response to novelty, social and self-control behaviors, in rats exposed to neonatal anoxia: modulatory effects of an enriched environment. Psychopharmacology 184:155–165CrossRefPubMed Adriani W, Giannakopoulou D, Bokulic Z, Jernej B, Alleva E, Laviola G (2006) Response to novelty, social and self-control behaviors, in rats exposed to neonatal anoxia: modulatory effects of an enriched environment. Psychopharmacology 184:155–165CrossRefPubMed
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Aggarwal P, Chaudhari S, Bhave S, Pandit A, Barve S (1998) Clinical predictors of outcome in hypoxic ischaemic encephalopathy in term neonates. Ann Trop Paediatr 18:117–121PubMed Aggarwal P, Chaudhari S, Bhave S, Pandit A, Barve S (1998) Clinical predictors of outcome in hypoxic ischaemic encephalopathy in term neonates. Ann Trop Paediatr 18:117–121PubMed
3.
Zurück zum Zitat American Academy of Pediatrics CoFaN (1996) Use and abuse of the Apgar score. Pediatrics 98:141–142 American Academy of Pediatrics CoFaN (1996) Use and abuse of the Apgar score. Pediatrics 98:141–142
4.
Zurück zum Zitat American College of Obstetrics and Gynecology: Task force on Neonatal Encephalopathy (2003) Neonatal encephalopathy and cerebral palsy: Defining the pathogenesis and pathophysiology. American College of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Washington DC American College of Obstetrics and Gynecology: Task force on Neonatal Encephalopathy (2003) Neonatal encephalopathy and cerebral palsy: Defining the pathogenesis and pathophysiology. American College of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Washington DC
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Badawi N, Dixon G, Felix JF, Keogh JM, Petterson B, Stanley FJ, Kurinczuk JJ (2006) Autism following a history of newborn encephalopathy: more than a coincidence? Dev Med Child Neurol 48:85–89CrossRefPubMed Badawi N, Dixon G, Felix JF, Keogh JM, Petterson B, Stanley FJ, Kurinczuk JJ (2006) Autism following a history of newborn encephalopathy: more than a coincidence? Dev Med Child Neurol 48:85–89CrossRefPubMed
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Badawi N, Kurinczuk JJ, Keogh JM, Alessandri LM, O’Sullivan F, Burton PR, Pemberton PJ, Stanley FJ (1998) Intrapartum risk factors for newborn encephalopathy: the Western Australian case-control study. BMJ 317:1554–1558PubMed Badawi N, Kurinczuk JJ, Keogh JM, Alessandri LM, O’Sullivan F, Burton PR, Pemberton PJ, Stanley FJ (1998) Intrapartum risk factors for newborn encephalopathy: the Western Australian case-control study. BMJ 317:1554–1558PubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Barkovich AJ (1992) MR and CT evaluation of profound neonatal and infantile asphyxia. Am J Neuroradiol 13:959–972PubMed Barkovich AJ (1992) MR and CT evaluation of profound neonatal and infantile asphyxia. Am J Neuroradiol 13:959–972PubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Barnett A, Mercuri E, Rutherford M, Haataja L, Frisone MF, Henderson S, Cowan F, Dubowitz L (2002) Neurological and perceptual-motor outcome at 5–6 years of age in children with neonatal encephalopathy: relationship with neonatal brain MRI. Neuropediatrics 33:242–248CrossRefPubMed Barnett A, Mercuri E, Rutherford M, Haataja L, Frisone MF, Henderson S, Cowan F, Dubowitz L (2002) Neurological and perceptual-motor outcome at 5–6 years of age in children with neonatal encephalopathy: relationship with neonatal brain MRI. Neuropediatrics 33:242–248CrossRefPubMed
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Barnett AL, Guzzetta A, Mercuri E, Henderson SE, Haataja L, Cowan F, Dubowitz L (2004) Can the Griffiths scales predict neuromotor and perceptual-motor impairment in term infants with neonatal encephalopathy? Arch Dis Child 89:637–643CrossRefPubMed Barnett AL, Guzzetta A, Mercuri E, Henderson SE, Haataja L, Cowan F, Dubowitz L (2004) Can the Griffiths scales predict neuromotor and perceptual-motor impairment in term infants with neonatal encephalopathy? Arch Dis Child 89:637–643CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Blair E, Stanley FJ (1988) Intrapartum asphyxia-a rare cause of cerebral palsy. J Pediatr 112:515–519CrossRefPubMed Blair E, Stanley FJ (1988) Intrapartum asphyxia-a rare cause of cerebral palsy. J Pediatr 112:515–519CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Caravale B, Allemand F, Libenson MH (2003) Factors predictive of seizures and neurologic outcome in perinatal depression. Pediatr Neurol 29:18–25CrossRefPubMed Caravale B, Allemand F, Libenson MH (2003) Factors predictive of seizures and neurologic outcome in perinatal depression. Pediatr Neurol 29:18–25CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Carli G, Reiger I, Evans N (2004) One-year neurodevelopmental outcome after moderate newborn hypoxic ischaemic encephalopathy. J Paediatr Child Health 40:217–220CrossRefPubMed Carli G, Reiger I, Evans N (2004) One-year neurodevelopmental outcome after moderate newborn hypoxic ischaemic encephalopathy. J Paediatr Child Health 40:217–220CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Cowan F (2000) Outcome after intrapartum asphyxia in term infants. Semin Neonatol 5:127–140CrossRefPubMed Cowan F (2000) Outcome after intrapartum asphyxia in term infants. Semin Neonatol 5:127–140CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat de Haan M, Wyatt JS, Roth S, Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian D, Mishkin M (2006) Brain and cognitive-behavioural development after asphyxia at term birth. Dev Sci 9(4):350–358CrossRefPubMed de Haan M, Wyatt JS, Roth S, Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian D, Mishkin M (2006) Brain and cognitive-behavioural development after asphyxia at term birth. Dev Sci 9(4):350–358CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat DeLong GR (1992) Autism, amnesia, hippocampus, and learning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 16:63–70CrossRefPubMed DeLong GR (1992) Autism, amnesia, hippocampus, and learning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 16:63–70CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Dilenge ME, Majnemer A, Shevell MI (2001) Long-term developmental outcome of asphyxiated term neonates. J Child Neurol 16:781–792PubMed Dilenge ME, Majnemer A, Shevell MI (2001) Long-term developmental outcome of asphyxiated term neonates. J Child Neurol 16:781–792PubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Dixon G, Badawi N, Kurinczuk JJ, Keogh JM, Silburn SR, Zubrick SR, Stanley FJ (2002) Early developmental outcomes after newborn encephalopathy. Pediatrics 109:26–33CrossRefPubMed Dixon G, Badawi N, Kurinczuk JJ, Keogh JM, Silburn SR, Zubrick SR, Stanley FJ (2002) Early developmental outcomes after newborn encephalopathy. Pediatrics 109:26–33CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Ellis M, Manandhar N, Shrestha PS, Shrestha L, Manandhar DS, Costello AM (1999) Outcome at 1 year of neonatal encephalopathy in Kathmandu, Nepal. Dev Med Child Neurol 41:689–695CrossRefPubMed Ellis M, Manandhar N, Shrestha PS, Shrestha L, Manandhar DS, Costello AM (1999) Outcome at 1 year of neonatal encephalopathy in Kathmandu, Nepal. Dev Med Child Neurol 41:689–695CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Freeman JM, Nelson KB (1988) Intrapartum asphyxia and cerebral palsy. Pediatrics 82(2):240–249PubMed Freeman JM, Nelson KB (1988) Intrapartum asphyxia and cerebral palsy. Pediatrics 82(2):240–249PubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Gadian DG, Aicardi J, Watkins KE, Porter DA, Mishkin M, Vargha-Khadem F (2000) Developmental amnesia associated with early hypoxic-ischaemic injury. Brain 123:499–507CrossRefPubMed Gadian DG, Aicardi J, Watkins KE, Porter DA, Mishkin M, Vargha-Khadem F (2000) Developmental amnesia associated with early hypoxic-ischaemic injury. Brain 123:499–507CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Gluckman PD, Wyatt JS, Azzopardi D, Edwards AD, Ballard R, Ferriero DM, Polin RA, Robertson CM, Thoresen M, Whitelaw A, Gunn AJ (2005) Selective head cooling with mild systemic hypothermia after neonatal encephalopathy: Multicentre randomised trial. Lancet 365:663–670PubMed Gluckman PD, Wyatt JS, Azzopardi D, Edwards AD, Ballard R, Ferriero DM, Polin RA, Robertson CM, Thoresen M, Whitelaw A, Gunn AJ (2005) Selective head cooling with mild systemic hypothermia after neonatal encephalopathy: Multicentre randomised trial. Lancet 365:663–670PubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Gonzalez FF, Miller SP (2006) Does perinatal asphyxia impair cognitive function without cerebral palsy? Arch Dis Child 91:F454–F459 Gonzalez FF, Miller SP (2006) Does perinatal asphyxia impair cognitive function without cerebral palsy? Arch Dis Child 91:F454–F459
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Gray PH, Tudehope DI, Masel JP, Burns YR, Mohay HA, O’Callaghan MJ, Williams GM (1993) Perinatal hypoxic-ischaemic brain injury: prediction of outcome. Dev Med Child Neurol 35:965–973PubMedCrossRef Gray PH, Tudehope DI, Masel JP, Burns YR, Mohay HA, O’Callaghan MJ, Williams GM (1993) Perinatal hypoxic-ischaemic brain injury: prediction of outcome. Dev Med Child Neurol 35:965–973PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Hankins GD, Speer M (2003) Defining the pathogenesis and pathophysiology of neonatal encephalopathy and cerebral palsy. Obstet Gynecol 102:628–636CrossRefPubMed Hankins GD, Speer M (2003) Defining the pathogenesis and pathophysiology of neonatal encephalopathy and cerebral palsy. Obstet Gynecol 102:628–636CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Ilves P, Lintrop M, Metsvaht T, Vaher U, Talvik T (2004) Cerebral blood-flow velocities in predicting outcome of asphyxiated newborn infants. Acta Paediatr 93:523–528CrossRefPubMed Ilves P, Lintrop M, Metsvaht T, Vaher U, Talvik T (2004) Cerebral blood-flow velocities in predicting outcome of asphyxiated newborn infants. Acta Paediatr 93:523–528CrossRefPubMed
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Leviton A, Nelson KB (1992) Problems with definitions and classifications of newborn encephalopathy. Pediatric Neurology 8:85–90CrossRefPubMed Leviton A, Nelson KB (1992) Problems with definitions and classifications of newborn encephalopathy. Pediatric Neurology 8:85–90CrossRefPubMed
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Lou HC (1996) Etiology and pathogenesis of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): significance of prematurity and perinatal hypoxic-haemodynamic encephalopathy. Acta Paediatr 85:1266–1271PubMed Lou HC (1996) Etiology and pathogenesis of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): significance of prematurity and perinatal hypoxic-haemodynamic encephalopathy. Acta Paediatr 85:1266–1271PubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Low JA, Galbraith RS, Muir DW, Killen HL, Pater EA, Karchmar EJ (1988) Motor and cognitive deficits after intrapartum asphyxia in the mature fetus. Am J Obstet Gynecol 158(2):356–361PubMed Low JA, Galbraith RS, Muir DW, Killen HL, Pater EA, Karchmar EJ (1988) Motor and cognitive deficits after intrapartum asphyxia in the mature fetus. Am J Obstet Gynecol 158(2):356–361PubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat MacLennan A (1999) A template for defining a causal relation between acute intrapartum events and cerebral palsy: international consensus statement. BMJ 319:1054–1059PubMed MacLennan A (1999) A template for defining a causal relation between acute intrapartum events and cerebral palsy: international consensus statement. BMJ 319:1054–1059PubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Maneru C, Serra-Grabulosa JM, Junque C, Salgado-Pineda P, Bargallo N, Olondo M, Botet-Mussons F, Tallada M, Mercader JM (2003) Residual hippocampal atrophy in asphyxiated term neonates. J Neuroimaging 13(1):68–74CrossRefPubMed Maneru C, Serra-Grabulosa JM, Junque C, Salgado-Pineda P, Bargallo N, Olondo M, Botet-Mussons F, Tallada M, Mercader JM (2003) Residual hippocampal atrophy in asphyxiated term neonates. J Neuroimaging 13(1):68–74CrossRefPubMed
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Marlow N, Rose AS, Rands CE, Draper ES (2005) Neuropsychological and educational problems at school age associated with neonatal encephalopathy. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 90:F380–F387CrossRefPubMed Marlow N, Rose AS, Rands CE, Draper ES (2005) Neuropsychological and educational problems at school age associated with neonatal encephalopathy. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 90:F380–F387CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller SP, Latal B, Barnwell A, Barkovich AJ, Ferriero DM, Clark H, Partridge JC, Glidden D (2004) Clinical signs predict 30-month neurodevelopmental outcome after neonatal encephalopathy. Am J Obst Gyn 190:93–99CrossRef Miller SP, Latal B, Barnwell A, Barkovich AJ, Ferriero DM, Clark H, Partridge JC, Glidden D (2004) Clinical signs predict 30-month neurodevelopmental outcome after neonatal encephalopathy. Am J Obst Gyn 190:93–99CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Miller SP, Ramaswamy V, Michelson D, Barkovich AJ, Holshouser B, Wycliffe N, Glidden DV, Deming D, Partridge JC, Wu YW, Ashwal S, Ferriero DM (2005) Patterns of brain injury in term neonatal encephalopathy. J Pediatr 146:453–460CrossRefPubMed Miller SP, Ramaswamy V, Michelson D, Barkovich AJ, Holshouser B, Wycliffe N, Glidden DV, Deming D, Partridge JC, Wu YW, Ashwal S, Ferriero DM (2005) Patterns of brain injury in term neonatal encephalopathy. J Pediatr 146:453–460CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Moster D, Lie RT, Markestad T (2002) Joint association of Apgar scores and early neonatal symptoms with minor disabilities at school age. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 86:F16–F21CrossRefPubMed Moster D, Lie RT, Markestad T (2002) Joint association of Apgar scores and early neonatal symptoms with minor disabilities at school age. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 86:F16–F21CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson KB, Dambrosia JM, Ting TY, Grether JK (1996) Uncertain value of electronic fetal monitoring in predicting cerebral palsy. N Engl J Med 334:613–618CrossRefPubMed Nelson KB, Dambrosia JM, Ting TY, Grether JK (1996) Uncertain value of electronic fetal monitoring in predicting cerebral palsy. N Engl J Med 334:613–618CrossRefPubMed
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson KB, Ellenberg JH (1981) Apgar scores as predictors of chronic neurologic disability. Pediatrics 68:36–44PubMed Nelson KB, Ellenberg JH (1981) Apgar scores as predictors of chronic neurologic disability. Pediatrics 68:36–44PubMed
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Nelson KB, Leviton A (1991) How much of neonatal encephalopathy is due to birth asphyxia? Am J Dis Child 145:1325–1331PubMed Nelson KB, Leviton A (1991) How much of neonatal encephalopathy is due to birth asphyxia? Am J Dis Child 145:1325–1331PubMed
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Oygür N, Sőnmez O, Saka O, Yeğin O (1998) Predictive value of plasma and cerebrospinal fluid tumour necrosis factor-alpha and interleukin-1 beta concentrations on outcome of full term infants with hypoxic-ischaemic encephalopathy. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 79:F190–F193PubMed Oygür N, Sőnmez O, Saka O, Yeğin O (1998) Predictive value of plasma and cerebrospinal fluid tumour necrosis factor-alpha and interleukin-1 beta concentrations on outcome of full term infants with hypoxic-ischaemic encephalopathy. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 79:F190–F193PubMed
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Painter MJ, Scott M, Hirsch RP, O’Donoghue P, Depp R (1988) Fetal heart rate patterns during labor: neurologic and cognitive development at six to nine years of age. Am J Obstet Gynecol 159(4):854–858PubMed Painter MJ, Scott M, Hirsch RP, O’Donoghue P, Depp R (1988) Fetal heart rate patterns during labor: neurologic and cognitive development at six to nine years of age. Am J Obstet Gynecol 159(4):854–858PubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Peliowski A, Finer N (1992) Birth asphyxia in the term infant. In: Sinclair JC, Bracken MB (eds) Effective care of the newborn infant. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 249–280 Peliowski A, Finer N (1992) Birth asphyxia in the term infant. In: Sinclair JC, Bracken MB (eds) Effective care of the newborn infant. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 249–280
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Rademakers RP, van der Knaap MS, Verbeeten B Jr, Barth PG, Valk J (1995) Central cortico-subcortical involvement: a distinct pattern of brain damage caused by perinatal and postnatal asphyxia in term infants. J Comput Assist Tomogr 19:256–263CrossRefPubMed Rademakers RP, van der Knaap MS, Verbeeten B Jr, Barth PG, Valk J (1995) Central cortico-subcortical involvement: a distinct pattern of brain damage caused by perinatal and postnatal asphyxia in term infants. J Comput Assist Tomogr 19:256–263CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Robertson CM, Finer NN (1985) Term infants with hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy: outcome at 3.5 years. Dev Med Child Neurol 27:473–484PubMedCrossRef Robertson CM, Finer NN (1985) Term infants with hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy: outcome at 3.5 years. Dev Med Child Neurol 27:473–484PubMedCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Robertson CM, Finer NN (1988) Educational readiness of survivors of neonatal encephalopathy associated with birth asphyxia at term. J Dev Behav Pediatr 9:298–306CrossRefPubMed Robertson CM, Finer NN (1988) Educational readiness of survivors of neonatal encephalopathy associated with birth asphyxia at term. J Dev Behav Pediatr 9:298–306CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Robertson CM, Finer NN (1993) Long-term follow-up of term neonates with perinatal asphyxia. Clin Perinatol 20:483–500PubMed Robertson CM, Finer NN (1993) Long-term follow-up of term neonates with perinatal asphyxia. Clin Perinatol 20:483–500PubMed
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Robertson CM, Finer NN, Grace MG (1989) School performance of survivors of neonatal encephalopathy associated with birth asphyxia at term. J Pediatr 114:753–760CrossRefPubMed Robertson CM, Finer NN, Grace MG (1989) School performance of survivors of neonatal encephalopathy associated with birth asphyxia at term. J Pediatr 114:753–760CrossRefPubMed
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Robertson CM (1997) Long-term follow-up of term infants with perinatal asphyxia. In: Stevenson DK, Sunshine P (eds) Fetal and neonatal brain injury: mechanisms, management and the risks of practice, 2nd edn. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 615–630 Robertson CM (1997) Long-term follow-up of term infants with perinatal asphyxia. In: Stevenson DK, Sunshine P (eds) Fetal and neonatal brain injury: mechanisms, management and the risks of practice, 2nd edn. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 615–630
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Robertson CM, Perlman M (2006) Follow-up of the term infant after hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. Paediatric Child Health 11:278–282 Robertson CM, Perlman M (2006) Follow-up of the term infant after hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. Paediatric Child Health 11:278–282
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Rutherford M, Pennock J, Schwieso J, Cowan F, Dubowitz L (1996) Hypoxic-ischaemic encephalopathy: early and late magnetic resonance imaging findings in relation to outcome. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 75:F145–F151PubMedCrossRef Rutherford M, Pennock J, Schwieso J, Cowan F, Dubowitz L (1996) Hypoxic-ischaemic encephalopathy: early and late magnetic resonance imaging findings in relation to outcome. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 75:F145–F151PubMedCrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Sarnat HB, Sarnat MS (1976) Neonatal encephalopathy following fetal distress. A clinical and electroencephalographic study. Arch Neurol 33:696–705PubMed Sarnat HB, Sarnat MS (1976) Neonatal encephalopathy following fetal distress. A clinical and electroencephalographic study. Arch Neurol 33:696–705PubMed
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Seidman DS, Paz I, Laor A, Gale R, Stevenson DK, Danon YL (1991) Apgar scores and cognitive performance at 17 years of age. Obstet Gynecol 77(6):875–878PubMed Seidman DS, Paz I, Laor A, Gale R, Stevenson DK, Danon YL (1991) Apgar scores and cognitive performance at 17 years of age. Obstet Gynecol 77(6):875–878PubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Shankaran S, Laptook AR, Ehrenkranz RA, Tyson JE, McDonald SA, Donovan EF, Fanaroff AA, Poole WK, Wright LL, Higgins RD, Finer NN, Carlo WA, Duara S, Oh W, Cotten CM, Stevenson DK, Stoll BJ, Lemons JA, Guillet R, Jobe AH (2005) Whole-body hypothermia for neonates with hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. N Engl J Med 353:1574–1584CrossRefPubMed Shankaran S, Laptook AR, Ehrenkranz RA, Tyson JE, McDonald SA, Donovan EF, Fanaroff AA, Poole WK, Wright LL, Higgins RD, Finer NN, Carlo WA, Duara S, Oh W, Cotten CM, Stevenson DK, Stoll BJ, Lemons JA, Guillet R, Jobe AH (2005) Whole-body hypothermia for neonates with hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. N Engl J Med 353:1574–1584CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Shankaran S, Woldt E, Koepke T, Bedard MP, Nandyal R (1991) Acute neonatal morbidity and long-term central nervous system sequelae of perinatal asphyxia in term infants. Early Hum Dev 25:135–148CrossRefPubMed Shankaran S, Woldt E, Koepke T, Bedard MP, Nandyal R (1991) Acute neonatal morbidity and long-term central nervous system sequelae of perinatal asphyxia in term infants. Early Hum Dev 25:135–148CrossRefPubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Shevell MI, Majnemer A, Miller SP (1999) Neonatal neurologic prognostication: the asphyxiated term newborn. Pediatr Neurol 21:776–784CrossRefPubMed Shevell MI, Majnemer A, Miller SP (1999) Neonatal neurologic prognostication: the asphyxiated term newborn. Pediatr Neurol 21:776–784CrossRefPubMed
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Thompson CM, Puterman AS, Linley LL, Hann FM, van der Elst CW, Molteno CD, Malan AF (1997) The value of a scoring system for hypoxic ischaemic encephalopathy in predicting neurodevelopmental outcome. Acta Paediatr 86:757–761PubMed Thompson CM, Puterman AS, Linley LL, Hann FM, van der Elst CW, Molteno CD, Malan AF (1997) The value of a scoring system for hypoxic ischaemic encephalopathy in predicting neurodevelopmental outcome. Acta Paediatr 86:757–761PubMed
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Toft PB (1999) Prenatal and perinatal striatal injury: a hypothetical cause of attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder? Pediatr Neurol 21:602–610CrossRefPubMed Toft PB (1999) Prenatal and perinatal striatal injury: a hypothetical cause of attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder? Pediatr Neurol 21:602–610CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Petten C (2004) Relationship between hippocampal volume and memory ability in healthy individuals across the lifespan: review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 42(10):1394–1413CrossRefPubMed Van Petten C (2004) Relationship between hippocampal volume and memory ability in healthy individuals across the lifespan: review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 42(10):1394–1413CrossRefPubMed
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Watkins KE, Connelly A, Van Paesschen W, Mishkin M (1997) Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory. Science 277:376–380CrossRefPubMed Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Watkins KE, Connelly A, Van Paesschen W, Mishkin M (1997) Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory. Science 277:376–380CrossRefPubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Volpe JJ (2001) Neurology of the newborn. Saunders, Philadelphia Volpe JJ (2001) Neurology of the newborn. Saunders, Philadelphia
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Weitzdoerfer R, Pollak A, Lubec B (2004) Perinatal asphyxia in the rat has lifelong effects on morphology, cognitive functions, and behavior. Semin Perinatol 28:249–256CrossRefPubMed Weitzdoerfer R, Pollak A, Lubec B (2004) Perinatal asphyxia in the rat has lifelong effects on morphology, cognitive functions, and behavior. Semin Perinatol 28:249–256CrossRefPubMed
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Yokochi K, Fujimoto S (1996) Magnetic resonance imaging in children with neonatal asphyxia: correlation with developmental sequelae. Acta Paediatr 85:88–95PubMed Yokochi K, Fujimoto S (1996) Magnetic resonance imaging in children with neonatal asphyxia: correlation with developmental sequelae. Acta Paediatr 85:88–95PubMed
Metadaten
Titel
Long-term cognitive and behavioral consequences of neonatal encephalopathy following perinatal asphyxia: a review
verfasst von
Mariëlle van Handel
Hanna Swaab
Linda S. de Vries
Marian J. Jongmans
Publikationsdatum
01.07.2007
Verlag
Springer-Verlag
Erschienen in
European Journal of Pediatrics / Ausgabe 7/2007
Print ISSN: 0340-6199
Elektronische ISSN: 1432-1076
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00431-007-0437-8

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 7/2007

European Journal of Pediatrics 7/2007 Zur Ausgabe

Mit dem Seitenschneider gegen das Reißverschluss-Malheur

03.06.2024 Urologische Notfallmedizin Nachrichten

Wer ihn je erlebt hat, wird ihn nicht vergessen: den Schmerz, den die beim Öffnen oder Schließen des Reißverschlusses am Hosenschlitz eingeklemmte Haut am Penis oder Skrotum verursacht. Eine neue Methode für rasche Abhilfe hat ein US-Team getestet.

Reanimation bei Kindern – besser vor Ort oder während Transport?

29.05.2024 Reanimation im Kindesalter Nachrichten

Zwar scheint es laut einer Studie aus den USA und Kanada bei der Reanimation von Kindern außerhalb einer Klinik keinen Unterschied für das Überleben zu machen, ob die Wiederbelebungsmaßnahmen während des Transports in die Klinik stattfinden oder vor Ort ausgeführt werden. Jedoch gibt es dabei einige Einschränkungen und eine wichtige Ausnahme.

Alter der Mutter beeinflusst Risiko für kongenitale Anomalie

28.05.2024 Kinder- und Jugendgynäkologie Nachrichten

Welchen Einfluss das Alter ihrer Mutter auf das Risiko hat, dass Kinder mit nicht chromosomal bedingter Malformation zur Welt kommen, hat eine ungarische Studie untersucht. Sie zeigt: Nicht nur fortgeschrittenes Alter ist riskant.

Begünstigt Bettruhe der Mutter doch das fetale Wachstum?

Ob ungeborene Kinder, die kleiner als die meisten Gleichaltrigen sind, schneller wachsen, wenn die Mutter sich mehr ausruht, wird diskutiert. Die Ergebnisse einer US-Studie sprechen dafür.

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.