Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Alzheimer's Research & Therapy 1/2022

Open Access 01.12.2022 | Research

Reading activities compensate for low education-related cognitive deficits

verfasst von: Yue Wang, Shinan Wang, Wanlin Zhu, Na Liang, Chen Zhang, Yuankun Pei, Qing Wang, Shiping Li, Jiong Shi

Erschienen in: Alzheimer's Research & Therapy | Ausgabe 1/2022

Abstract

Background

The incidence of cognitive impairment is increasing with an aging population. Developing effective strategies is essential to prevent dementia. Higher education level is associated with better baseline cognitive performance, and reading activities can slow down cognitive decline. However, it is unclear whether education and reading activities are synergistic or independent contributors to cognitive performance.

Methods

This was a sub-study of an ongoing prospective community cohort of China National Clinical Research Center Alzheimer’s Disease and Neurodegenerative Disorder Research (CANDOR). Demographic and clinical information, educational levels, and reading activities were collected. All participants finished neuropsychological testing batteries and brain MRIs. We analyzed cognitive performance and brain structures with education and reading activities.

Results

Four hundred fifty-nine subjectively cognitively normal participants were enrolled in the study. One hundred sixty-nine (36.82%) of them had regular reading activities. Participants in the reading group had better performance in all cognitive tests compared with those in the non-reading group, but no difference in brain MRI variables. Participants with higher education levels (more than 13 years) had better cognitive performance and higher hippocampal volumes. In low education groups (less than 12 years), more reading activities were associated with better cognitive test scores.

Conclusions

Both education and reading activities are important and synergistic for baseline cognitive function. Higher education level is associated with larger hippocampal volumes. Education may stimulate the growth and development of the hippocampus. Reading activities help to maintain and improve cognitive function in people with low levels of education.

Trial registration

NCT04320368.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s13195-022-01098-1.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
CANDOR
China National Clinical Research Center Alzheimer's Disease and Neurodegenerative Disorder Research
FINGER
Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability
AD
Alzheimer’s disease
MRI
Magnetic resonance imaging
NC
Normal cognition
MCI
Mild cognitive impairment
MMSE
Mini-Mental State Examination
CDR
Clinical Dementia Rating
MoCA
Montreal Cognitive Assessment
RAVLT
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
ROCF
Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test
TMT
Trail Making Test
CDT
Clock drawing test
BNT
Boston Naming Test
DST
Digit Span Test
SDMT
Symbol Digit Modalities Test
NPI
Neuropsychiatry Inventory
SD
Standard deviations
OR
Odds ratio
CI
Confidence interval
WM
White matter
eTIV
Estimated total intracranial volume

Introduction

Aging is the most important risk factor for dementia. With an aging population, dementia has cast an enormous social and economic burden around the world [1, 2]. Developing effective strategies is essential to prevent dementia [3]. It has been reported there are modifiable risk factors for dementia and modifying 12 of them may prevent or delay up to 40% of dementia [4]. The Finnish Geriatric Intervention Study to Prevent Cognitive Impairment and Disability (FINGER), a multicenter randomized controlled trial, reported beneficial effects on cognition through multimodal intervention including cognitive training [5]. Reading activities and other mental stimulation help to slow down cognitive decline [6, 7].
High education level is associated with better cognitive performance and lower likelihood to have Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [8, 9]. High education level may delay cognitive decline in individuals with subjective cognitive decline [10, 11]. However, although education is associated with baseline cognitive performance, it doesn’t affect the rate of cognitive decline [12], nor does it affect the neuropathological changes related to dementia, such as amyloid plaques and tangles [13].
Previous research compared the influence of reading activities and education on cognition and found that reading activities were associated with a lower risk of dementia even in late life, independent of education and other related factors [6, 7], while another study demonstrated that reading activities have a stronger relationship than education with executive function tests [14].
It is inconclusive whether reading activities and education are synergistic or independent contributors to cognitive performance. In this prospective community-based cohort study, we try to answer the following questions. First, what are the relationships of education and reading activities with cognitive performance on domain-specific tests? Second, are education and/or reading activities associated with brain structure? Third, can reading activities compensate for lower levels of education?

Methods

Study design and participants

This study was a sub-study of an ongoing prospective community-based cohort study of the China National Clinical Research Center Alzheimer's Disease and Neurodegenerative Disorder Research (CANDOR). CANDOR was started in July 2019 and planned to enroll one thousand and five hundred participants, including individuals with normal cognition (NC), mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and dementia. Demographic information and past medical history were collected. All participants were required to have a study partner to provide an independent evaluation of daily and social functions. They underwent detailed assessments for cognition and functional abilities, a comprehensive neuropsychological battery (described below in “Neuropsychological assessment”) including the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR), and brain MRIs. All enrolled participants for this study (1) were subjectively cognitively normal; (2) aged 40–100 years old; (3) had at least 3 years of education; (4) had no condition known to affect cognitive function, such as Alzheimer’s Disease, vascular dementia, Lewy body dementia, frontotemportal dementia, Parkinson’s disease, epilepsy, stroke, hydrocephalus, multiple sclerosis, traumatic brain injuries, genetic disorders affecting cognition, alcoholism, uncontrolled depression, or other psychiatric disorders; (5) had no uncontrolled neoplasia, or severe pulmonary, cardiovascular, metabolic, infectious, inflammatory, or endocrine diseases. We excluded individuals with less than 3 years of education because people started to learn how to read and write in the third year of elementary school in China. Therefore, people who have less than three years of education will have difficulties in reading.
To assess the relationship between education and leisure reading activities, we defined regular reading activities as reading at least one book per month on average for at least one year. We divided the participants as follows. First, participants were divided into 2 groups based on their reading activities. Reading activities were detailed, including (1) reading materials, such as paper books, e-books, and audio-books; (2) reading content, such as literature books, and non-literature books; (3) the total number of books, which was calculated as the average number of books read per month ×12 months × years of reading. In participants with reading habits, we divided them further into several groups based on reading years, reading content and reading materials. Second, participants were divided based on their education. Previous studies analyzed education by ≤9, 10–12, and ≥13 years [15, 16]. In our study, the average education years of all participants were 12.12 years. Therefore, we used a 12-year cut-off to divide participants into two groups: low education (≤12 years, high school education or below, under the average education level) and high education (≥13 years, college education or above, over the average education level). Third, participants were divided into four groups based on education years and reading activities: low education (educational years ≤12) with and without reading activities (groups 1 and 2), and high education (educational years ≥13) with and without reading activities (groups 3 and 4).

Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents

This protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Beijing Tiantan Hospital (approval number: KY 2019-004-007) and was in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant.

Neuropsychological assessment

Thirteen neuropsychological tests were completed at the visit, including (1) tests for overall cognitive performance: Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), and Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) with global scores; (2) Tests for specific cognitive domain: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) [17], Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCF) [18], Stroop Color-Word Test-Victoria version [19], Trail Making Test-A (TMT- A) and Trail Making Test B (TMT -B) [20], clock drawing test (CDT), Boston Naming Test (BNT), Digit Span Test (DST), and Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); and (3) Neuropsychiatry Inventory (NPI). These tests were administered by experienced neuropsychologists who were blinded to group assignment.

MRI assessment

All participants completed the brain MRI to exclude other demonstrable neurological diseases. Quantitative measures of signal-to-noise ratio, uniformity, and geometric distortion were conducted in each research center. 3.0 T-MRI was used with the scanning thickness not exceeding 1.5mm. The three-dimensional T1 weighted images were corrected for intensity non-uniformity with the N4 algorithm. Brain surface was reconstructed using FreeSurfer (version 7.2.0, http://​surfer.​nmr.​mgh.​harvard.​edu/​) recon-all pipeline. The cortical thickness and volume of the total brain, nuclei, gray matter white matter, and white matter lesion were all obtained with this pipeline. Regional cortical thickness was obtained and statistical analysis was performed.

Statistical analysis

The analysis was conducted with SPSS 24.0. Continuous variables were characterized as mean plus and/or minus standard deviations (SD). T-test or nonparametric test was used by the characteristic of the distribution. Categorical variables were analyzed by Pearson’s χ2 tests. We performed logistic regression analysis to evaluate the association between reading and CDR (CDR=0 or >0), linear regression analysis for the association of reading and neuropsychological assessment, and linear regression analysis for education and brain structure. The regression analyses were independent of age and sex in Table 2 model 1 and Table 5. The regression analyses were independent of age, sex, and education in Table 2 model 2. We also performed the collinearity analysis in every linear regression analysis, and all the results showed no collinearity between every included independent variable.

Results

From July 31, 2019, to August 1, 2021, 694 individuals were screened from communities in Beijing, Shijiazhuang, and Langfang, all in northern China. They completed standard baseline assessments. 459 were enrolled in the study who had both valid brain MRI examination and cognitive evaluation (Fig. 1). Among them, 169 (36.82%) had regular reading activities. There was no significant difference in age, sex, medical history, and mood assessment (NPI) between the two groups (Table 1). The reading group had more years of education and better cognitive performance than the non-reading group, including CDR, MMSE, MoCA, DST, RAVLT, ROCF, Stroop D and W time, TMT-A and B, BNT, SDMT, and CDT. However, there was no difference in cortical thickness and hippocampal volume in either hemisphere between the two groups.
Table 1
Demographic, clinical information, cognitive test scores, and MRI variables in reading and non-reading groups
 
Reading
n=169
Non-reading
n=290
All patients
n=459
P value
Average age
60.33±9.04
60.03±8.5
60.14±8.69
0.727
Sex female, (n, %)
89, 52.7%
171, 59.0%
260, 56.6%
0.189
Years of education
13.88±3.3
11.1±3.52
12.12±3.69
<0.001
Hypertension (n, %)
57, 33.7%
96, 33.1%
153, 33.3%
0.918
Diabetes (n, %)
19, 11.2%
34, 11.7%
53, 11.5%
0.882
Stroke or TIA (n, %)
11, 6.5%
21, 7.2%
32, 7.0%
0.851
Coronary heart disease (n, %)
14, 8.3%
18, 6.2%
32, 7.0%
0.449
Global CDR score
0.11±0.21
0.22±0.41
0.18±0.36
0.001
MMSE
26.15±2.85
24.04±4.82
24.82±4.32
<0.001
MoCA
23.38±3.82
20.13±5.64
21.33±5.29
<0.001
DST total
12.3±2.34
11.09±2.93
11.54±2.79
<0.001
RAVLT total learning
39.98±10.06
36.26±12.68
37.63±11.91
0.001
RAVLT long-delayed recall
8.01±3.44
6.85±3.85
7.28±3.74
0.001
ROCF copy
32.34±6.91
28.48±10
30.36±8.83
0.001
ROCF long-delayed recall
16.5±7.82
11.03±9.01
13.69±8.87
<0.001
Stroop D time
16.86±6.13
21.8±16.31
19.97±13.67
<0.001
Stroop W time
22.32±8.05
26.77±14.46
25.12±12.65
<0.001
TMT-A time
44.18±21.57
58.17±35.98
52.98±32.11
<0.001
TMT-B time
101.55±71.89
125.32±87.41
116.51±82.72
0.002
BNT
25.02±3.4
21.99±4.37
23.11±4.29
<0.001
SDMT
39.57±13.39
32.71±14.89
35.25±14.72
<0.001
CDT
8.79±1.79
8.09±2.42
8.35±2.23
<0.001
NPI
1.03±3.17
1.83±6.01
1.54±5.15
0.064
Brain structure
 Left hippocampal volume, mm2
3496.15±427.05
3436.04±451.65
3458.79±442.97
0.170
 Left amygdala volume, mm2
1463.18±355.93
1482.53±324.29
1475.21±336.34
0.561
 Left thalamus volume, mm2
6879.88±904.81
6876.82±939.2
6877.98±925.29
0.973
 Left caudate volume, mm2
3316.52±565.36
3297.41±581.55
3304.64±574.9
0.737
 Left putamen volume, mm2
4632.86±662.24
4613.26±719.29
4620.68±697.55
0.776
 Left pallidum volume, mm2
1940±269.78
1916.5±238.56
1925.39±250.78
0.343
 Left cortex volume, mm2
213,629.66±24,614.53
215,780.91±24,480.75
214,966.79±24,525.43
0.375
 Left cerebral white matter volume, mm2
221,073.68±32,180.9
222,387.88±29,129.85
221,890.53±30,290.75
0.661
 Left mean cortical thickness, mm
2.37±0.11
2.38±0.12
2.37±0.11
0.516
 Right hippocampal volume, mm2
3605.87±487.08
3571.79±460.36
3584.69±470.39
0.464
 Right amygdala volume, mm2
1643.52±356.73
1664.16±329.83
1656.35±340
0.539
 Right thalamus volume, mm2
6643.18±897.79
6669.33±823.35
6659.44±851.36
0.756
 Right caudate volume, mm2
3366.34±617.01
3358.4±559.61
3361.41±581.31
0.869
 Right putamen volume, mm2
4714.73±647.17
4743.82±707.15
4732.81±684.46
0.667
 Right pallidum volume, mm2
1937.36±281.85
1920.2±253.02
1926.7±264.11
0.511
 Right cortex volume, mm2
212,619.37±25,297.4
215,773.47±24,519.75
214,579.83±24,835.2
0.199
 Right cerebral white matter volume, mm2
219,923.7±31,876.29
221,611.7±28,804.31
220,972.9±29,978.85
0.569
 Right mean cortical thickness, mm
2.36±0.11
2.37±0.12
2.37±0.11
0.151
 Cortex volume, mm2
426,249.03±49,571.32
431,554.38±48,668.99
429,546.62±49,023.28
0.274
 Subcortex gray volume, mm2
54,301.14±6096.21
54,392.28±6088.1
54,357.79±6084.32
0.88
 Total gray volume, mm2
440,997.39±63,865.8
443,999.58±57,770.04
442,863.43±60,094.68
0.613
 Cerebral white matter volume, mm2
3788.28±5597.58
2941.34±4143.9
3261.86±4758.19
0.093
 WM hyperintensities volume, mm2
578,959.97±62,023.02
584,269.48±59,903.83
582,260.15±60,698.27
0.376
 Brain segmentation volume, mm2
1,077,237.37±118,406.82
1,084,041.34±113,069.4
1,081,466.44±115,030.46
0.488
 eTIV, mm2
1,430,023.25±162,478.34
1,438,793.2±151,423.56
1,435,474.3±155,572.46
0.499
 Brain segmentation volume to eTIV, %
75.59±5.81
75.55±5.38
75.56±5.54
0.941
Abbreviations: CDR Clinical Dementia Rating, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, DST Digit Span Test, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, ROCF Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, TMT Trail Making Test, BNT Boston Naming Test, SDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test, CDT clock drawing test, NPI Neuropsychiatry Inventory, WM white matter, eTIV estimated total intracranial volume
Logistic and linear regression were used to assess confounding factors (Table 2). Reading activities were associated with better cognitive performance, such as MMSE (beta 2.193, 95%CI: 1.463–2.923, P<0.001), independent of age and sex in model 1. In model 2, when education was taken into account, reading activities showed similar effects in MoCA and Boston Naming; significant but less effects in MMSE, DST, ROCF delayed recall, Stroop D and W time, TMT-A and SDMT, but no effects in CDR, RAVLT, ROCF copy, TMT-B, and CDT.
Table 2
The logistic and linear regression analysis of reading in all cognitive tests
Logistic regression
Model 1, OR, 95% CI
P
Model 2, OR, 95% CI
P
Global CDR score (=0), 330 (71.9%)
2.012, 1.258–3.218
0.004
1.416, 0.848–2.365
0.210
Linear regression
Model 1, Beta, 95%CI
P
Model 2, Beta, 95%CI
P
MMSE
2.193 [1.463, 2.923]
<0.001
1.044 [0.302, 1.787]
0.006
MOCA
3.342 [2.486, 4.198]
<0.001
1.546 [0.744, 2.348]
<0.001
DST total
1.240 [0.749, 1.731]
<0.001
0.496 [0.003, 0.988]
0.048
RAVLT learn total
4.162 [2.208, 6.121]
<0.001
1.235 [−0.758, 3.227]
0.224
RAVLT long-delayed recall
1.185 [0.544, 1.826]
<0.001
0.241 [−0.426, 0.907]
0.478
ROCF copy
3.800 [1.487, 6.114]
0.001
1.296 [−0.998, 3.590]
0.267
ROCF delayed recall
5.359 [3.138, 7.580]
<0.001
3.103 [0.892, 5.314]
0.006
Stroop D time
−5.162 [−7.712, −2.611]
<0.001
−3.080 [−5.789, −0.371]
0.026
Stroop W time
−4.802 [−7.074, −2.530]
<0.001
−3.304 [−5.694, −0.914]
0.007
TMT-A time
−14.499 [−19.792, −9.206]
<0.001
−8.246 [−13.798, −2.694]
0.004
TMT-B time
−26.290 [−40.525, −12.055]
<0.001
−7.465 [−22.449–7.519]
0.328
BNT
2.961 [2.252, 3.669]
<0.001
1.761 [1.048, 2.474]
<0.001
SDMT
7.385 [5.233, 9.538]
<0.001
2.719 [0.693, 4.746]
0.009
CDT
0.703 [0.284, 1.121]
0.001
0.219 [−0.218, 0.656]
0.325
Model 1 logistic or linear regression included age and sex
Model 2 logistic or linear regression included age, sex, and years of education
Abbreviations: OR odds ratio for logistic regression, CI confidence interval, CDR Clinical Dementia Rating, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, DST Digit Span Test, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, ROCF Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, TMT Trail Making Test, BNT Boston Naming Test, SDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test, CDT clock drawing test, NPI Neuropsychiatry Inventory
Education had a remarkable effect on cognitive performance (Table 3). Participants with high education scored higher in all cognitive tests than those with low education. They also have higher hippocampal volumes on both sides (Fig. 2).
Table 3
Cognitive performance and brain structure at different education levels
 
Education ≤12 years
n=294
Education ≥13 years
n=165
P
Average age
61.66±8.28
57.43±8.78
<0.001
Sex female (n, %)
177, 60.2%
83, 50.3%
0.040
Years of education
9.79±2.34
16.29±0.95
<0.001
Global CDR score
0.10±0.24
0.22±0.40
<0.001
MMSE
23.74±4.65
26.73±2.79
<0.001
MoCA
19.47±5.18
24.62±3.61
<0.001
DST total
10.72±2.51
12.98±2.68
<0.001
RAVLT total learning
8.93±3.32
11.07±2.79
<0.001
RAVLT long-delayed recall
6.04±3.7
8.65±3.53
<0.001
ROCF copy
33.18±6.55
28.8±9.53
<0.001
ROCF long-delayed recall
17.23±7.96
11.74±8.76
<0.001
Stroop D time
22.07±16.02
16.25±6.55
<0.001
Stroop W time
27.72±13.85
20.54±8.46
<0.001
TMT-A time
59.73±32.17
41.09±28.42
<0.001
TMT-B time
136.48±91.03
81.31±48.78
<0.001
BNT
21.78±4.47
25.44±2.69
<0.001
SDMT
30.22±13.05
44.06±13.285
<0.001
CDT
8.00±2.39
8.96±1.76
<0.001
NPI
1.75±5.86
1.16±3.55
0.242
Brain structure
 Left hippocampal volume, mm2
3386.57±435.93
3584.61±428.01
<0.001
 Left amygdala volume, mm2
1634.78±220.55
1709.65±215.06
0.145
 Left thalamus volume, mm2
6883.74±897.91
6867.95±973.97
0.864
 Left caudate volume, mm2
3313.07±580.16
3289.96±567.13
0.697
 Left putamen volume, mm2
4629.44±710.25
4605.42±676.78
0.73
 Left pallidum volume, mm2
1931.57±251.23
1914.63±250.43
0.498
 Left cortex volume, mm2
216,139.25±23,700.57
212,924.21±25,849.25
0.188
 Left cerebral white matter volume, mm2
223,576.87±29,777.23
218,952.71±31,041.08
0.125
 Left mean cortical thickness, mm
2.37±0.12
2.38±0.10
0.678
 Right hippocampal volume, mm2
3512.3±476.23
3710.8±433.37
<0.001
 Right amygdala volume, mm2
4691.13±49,986.08
1777.05±252.54
0.563
 Right thalamus volume, mm2
6696.93±830.19
6594.12±885.91
0.225
 Right caudate volume, mm2
3373.49±565.75
3340.36±608.73
0.474
 Right putamen volume, mm2
4743.54±678.03
4714.12±697.27
0.666
 Right pallidum volume, mm2
1928.99±259.36
1922.71±272.97
0.812
 Right cortex volume, mm2
216,046.23±23,917.16
212,025.16±26,239.96
0.104
 Right cerebral white matter volume, mm2
222,783.05±29,373.45
217,819.36±30,845.32
0.096
 Right mean cortical thickness, mm
2.37±0.12
2.37±0.11
0.800
 Cortex volume, mm2
432,185.48±47,269.6
424,949.37±51,768.69
0.138
 Subcortex gray volume, mm2
54,526.69±6033.69
54,063.54±6179.62
0.445
 Total gray volume, mm2
585,324.16±59,164.42
576,922.21±63,115.96
0.164
 Cerebral white matter volume, mm2
446,359.92±58,982.67
436,772.07±61,698.97
0.109
 WM hyperintensities volume, mm2
3219.53±4544.25
3335.6±5123.64
0.510
 Brain segmentation volume, mm2
1,089,199.77±112,197.33
1,067,993.91±118,965.54
0.110
 eTIV, mm2
1,443,152.53±151,357.93
1,422,097.74±162,271.98
0.199
 Brain segmentation volume to eTIV, %
75.67±5.19
75.37±6.11
0.590
Abbreviations: CDR Clinical Dementia Rating, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, DST Digit Span Test, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, ROCF Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, TMT Trail Making Test, BNT Boston Naming Test, SDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test, CDT clock drawing test, NPI Neuropsychiatry Inventory, WM white matter, eTIV estimated total intracranial volume
Reading years and reading content had little impact on cognitive performance and brain structure (Supplemental Tables 1 and 2). Reading e-books showed no obvious cognitive benefits than paper books, and listening to audio-books performed better in MoCA, BNT, and SDMT (Supplemental Table 3).
To assess if reading activities have a compensatory effect for low education, we divided participants into four groups: low education (educational years ≤12) with and without reading activities (groups 1 and 2), and high education (educational years ≥13) with and without reading activities (groups 3 and 4). Reading activities improved most cognitive tests (except RAVLT, ROCF copy, and TMT-B) in the low education group (group 1 better than group 2, Table 4). By reading more books, participants with low education could achieve similar or even better cognitive scores than those with high education in MMSE, MoCA, DST, and BNT (Fig. 3). In the high education groups, reading activities showed few effects probably due to ceiling effects (group 3 similar to group 4, Table 4).
Table 4
Cognitive performance and brain structure comparison by education level and reading activities
 
Group 1
n=71
Group 2
n=223
P
Group 3
n=98
Group 4
n=67
P
Age
62.66±9.79
61.34±7.73
0.299
58.63±8.09
55.67±9.5
0.825
Gender female (n, %)
39, 54.9%
138, 61.9%
0.297
50, 51.0%
33, 49.3%
0.824
Education years
10.44±1.95
9.58±2.42
0.007
16.37±1.08
16.18±0.72
0.180
Global CDR score
0.14±0.23
0.24±0.44
0.323
0.08±0.20
0.13±0.28
0.236
MMSE
25.04±3.13
23.33±4.97
0.001
26.94±2.35
26.42±3.34
0.241
MoCA
21.26±4.05
18.9±5.37
0.001
24.9±2.82
24.22±4.52
0.280
DST total
11.59±2.27
10.45±2.53
0.001
12.82±2.26
13.21±3.19
0.387
RAVLT total learning
35.97±9.55
33.81±11.85
0.123
42.85±9.45
44.44±12.02
0.345
RAVLT long-delayed recall
6.07±3.43
6.03±3.79
0.931
8.73±3.29
8.53±3.88
0.726
ROCF copy
30.36±8.25
27.99±10.07
0.167
33.96±5.08
30.84±9.53
0.186
ROCF long-delayed recall
14.33±8.32
10.39±8.73
0.012
18.31±6.95
14.05±9.92
0.096
Stroop D time
18.19±7.84
23.31±17.69
0.001
15.91±4.35
16.75±8.88
0.419
Stroop W time
24.97±9.69
28.59±14.85
0.019
20.42±6
20.72±11.21
0.844
TMT-A time
50.89±25.93
62.56±33.49
0.003
39.39±16.34
43.62±40.19
0.419
TMT-B time
128.57±91.74
139.01±90.87
0.405
82.26±44.79
79.91±54.49
0.764
BNT
23.97±3.87
21.08±4.43
<0.001
25.77±2.81
24.96±2.45
0.057
SDMT
33.07±12.71
29.31±13.05
0.035
44.21±11.89
43.84±15.18
0.864
CDT
8.59±1.92
7.82±2.50
0.008
8.94±1.69
8.98±1.87
0.870
NPI
1.31±4.25
1.89±6.29
0.478
0.83±2.09
1.65±4.97
0.206
Brain structure
      
 Left hippocampal volume, mm2
3391.25±442.13
3372.73±419.86
0.759
3580.93±455.11
3587.09±411.2
0.93
 Left amygdala volume, mm2
1487.93±400.96
1494.7±333.99
0.889
1444.95±319.72
1443.14±289.65
0.971
 Left thalamus volume, mm2
6927.86±912.39
6868.82±894.71
0.635
6844.53±902.39
6902.71±1077.97
0.713
 Left caudate volume, mm2
3274.19±590.97
3326.22±577.32
0.518
3347.72±546.78
3204.22±589.96
0.118
 Left putamen volume, mm2
4613.36±742.16
4634.88±700.9
0.827
4647.23±600.29
4543.36±777.51
0.344
 Left pallidum volume, mm2
1958.16±291.24
1922.58±236.29
0.307
1926.63±253.55
1896.81±246.62
0.463
 Left cortex volume, mm2
215,000.93±24,471.4
216,524.19±23,482.27
0.643
212,619.25±24,800.12
213,376.89±27,526.83
0.857
 Left cerebral white matter volume, mm2
225,861.59±35,115.03
222,804.26±27,795.56
0.51
217,545.76±29,530.28
221,041.16±33,286.15
0.488
 Left mean cortical thickness, mm
2.37±0.12
2.36±0.11
0.34
2.38±0.11
2.38±0.1
0.796
 Right hippocampal volume, mm2
3533.42±467.04
3449.84±500.66
0.205
3695.9±417.79
3720.84±445.47
0.723
 Right amygdala volume, mm2
1638.97±387.11
1671.78±333.05
0.495
1646.86±334.66
1639.52±320.52
0.89
 Right thalamus volume, mm2
6739.93±910.74
6682.39±802.96
0.617
6571.9±886.17
6627.11±891.5
0.701
 Right caudate volume, mm2
3326.03±602.96
3389.53±553.2
0.418
3396.05±628.68
3257.71±572.69
0.161
 Right putamen volume, mm2
4702.98±639.94
4757.25±691.4
0.564
4723.38±655.7
4700.38±759.94
0.839
 Right pallidum volume, mm2
1939.62±285.7
1925.39±250.47
0.692
1935.71±280.49
1903.42±262.42
0.466
 Right cortex volume, mm2
214,709.43±24,120.85
216,498.29±23,889.7
0.589
211,079.33±26,149.49
213,429.13±26,517.63
0.581
 Right cerebral white matter volume, mm2
225,512.97±34,160.89
221,859.89±27,599.24
0.42
215,805.29±29,593.22
220,808.98±32,622.88
0.317
 Right mean cortical thickness, mm
2.37±0.12
2.35±0.11
0.142
2.38±0.11
2.37±0.11
0.427
 Cortex volume, mm2
429,710.36±48,310.18
433,022.48±47,001.73
0.613
423,698.58±50,582.34
426,806.01±53,831.66
0.712
Subcortex gray volume, mm2
54,504.63±6480.09
54,534.15±5891.59
0.972
54,151.2±5827.67
53,933.42±6713.47
0.828
 Total gray volume, mm2
582,730.42±62,282.68
586,201.27±58,202.35
0.672
576,181.75±62,013.7
578,021.34±65,196.6
0.858
 Cerebral white matter volume, mm2
451,374.56±69,166.21
444,664.14±55,202.29
0.463
433,351.05±58,867.31
441,850.14±65,823.54
0.396
 WM hyperintensities volume, mm2
3602.52±4963.72
3090.02±4398.62
0.416
3925.16±6044.47
2460.48±3165.75
0.048
 Brain segmentation volume, mm2
1,093,169.74±123,689.54
1,087,857.27±108,324.4
0.733
1,065,497.73±113,587.95
1,071,699.19±127,357.94
0.748
 eTIV, mm2
1,440,816.13±159,459
1,443,942.62±148,911.25
0.882
1,422,070.61±165,054.25
1,422,138.01±159,346.57
0.998
 Brain segmentation volume to eTIV, %
76.03±5.26
75.55±5.18
0.507
75.26±6.19
75.54±6.03
0.783
Four groups: low education (educational years ≤12) with and without reading activities (groups 1 and 2) and high education (educational years ≥13) with and without reading activities (groups 3 and 4)
Abbreviations: CDR Clinical Dementia Rating, MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, DST Digit Span Test, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, ROCF Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, TMT Trail Making Test, BNT Boston Naming Test, SDMT Symbol Digit Modalities Test, CDT clock drawing test, NPI Neuropsychiatry Inventory, WM white matter, eTIV estimated total intracranial volume
The linear regression related to hippocampal volume on either side showed that years of education influenced hippocampal volume with beta1 14.999 [4.906, 25.092], P=0.004 and beta2 15.816 [4.949, 26.683], P=0.004 (Table 5), regardless of age and sex.
Table 5
The linear regression of education and hippocampal volume
 
Model 1, beta, 95% CI
P
Model 2, beta, 95% CI
P
Years of education
14.999, [4.906, 25.092]
0.004
15.816, [4.949, 26.683]
0.004
Education ≥13
23.020, [3.868, 42,172]
0.019
22.114, [1.476, 42.753]
0.036
Model 1, data of left hippocampal volume were analyzed as results, age, and sex were in linear regression
Model 2, data of right hippocampal volume were analyzed as results, age, and sex were in linear regression
Abbreviations: CI confidence interval

Discussion

In this community-based subjectively cognitively normal population, participants with regular reading activities showed better cognitive performance in overall cognitive abilities, attention, memory, language, visuospatial and executive function. This effect is independent of brain volume, especially hippocampal volume. A prospective cohort study showed that increased participation in cognitive activities (including reading) was associated with better memory [21]. Although reading activities involve multiple brain areas, subgroup analysis of the FINGER study has shown that the multi-domain intervention has no effects on brain volume, cortical thickness, and white matter lesion [22].
Education was related to cognition across all tested domains. Reading is associated with all tested domains controlled with age and sex. However, when education was included in the analysis, the effect of reading on cognitive assessment weakened, indicating a stronger correlation between education level and cognition. Reading is a complex task that involves various brain areas, including the insular and frontal opercular cortex, lateral temporal cortex, and early auditory cortex with the positive reaction and inferior temporal and motor cortex with the negative reaction [23]. However, we did not see a difference in the cortical thickness and the hippocampus between reading and non-reading groups. This suggests that reading activities may help to improve cognitive function in participants with low education level (≤12 years) independent of brain volume. In some cognitive domains, the cognitive performance gap caused by education level is decreased with the increase in reading activities. Reading is a good way to fill the cognitive gap brought about by lack of education, especially in language, non-verbal memory, and executive function.
In this study, participants with high education level had higher hippocampal volume. Larger hippocampal volumes may be associated with higher intelligence quotient (IQ), practice in hippocampus-related function (e.g., learning and memory), lifestyle, and medial/historical factors (neurotoxic effects of obesity, diabetes mellitus, hypertension, hypoxic brain injury, obstructive sleep apnea, bipolar disorder, clinical depression, and head trauma) [24]. Higher education level is favorable to the neurological task performance [10, 12, 2527], but not to AD-related pathology [28].
MMSE and MoCA are screening tests for cognition. Their cut-off scores are based on education levels. In this study, participants with low education but reading more books showed no difference in MMSE and MoCA compared with participants with high education level. It suggests that people with low education but who read a lot probably should be screened at the same level as those who are more educated.
Audio devices are a new form of reading activities and have become popular. It is suggested that audiobooks are probably better than non-audio books at improving cognitive function. Young children learned more words from the e-book and from the audio narrator than print books [29]. Different types of books may influence the ability to retrieve information. Listening to audiobooks may stimulate more brain areas to have positive effects on cognition, especially memory and executive function. Since poor vision is not uncommon in the elderly, audiobooks are a better tool for old people to enjoy reading activities.
The strength of this study is a large community-based cohort with detailed neuropsychological testing batteries and brain MRI analysis. But the study has several limitations. First, this is an observational, cross-sectional study. Correlation does not imply causation. To study the causative effect of reading activities on cognitive function, a randomized clinical trial is warranted. Participants with certain education levels would be assigned with different reading activities. Other intellectual activities besides leisure reading would also be taken into account. Second, we enrolled participants with subjectively normal cognitive function to represent community-based cohorts. The average CDR was 0.18 although a few participants with a CDR more than 0.5. Ongoing longitudinal follow-ups will allow us to assess the relationship between risk/protective factors and the conversion to dementia. Third, all participants were enrolled from northern China. There is likely a difference in culture, education, and environmental factors among different regions in China. To expand population sampling is needed in future studies. Fourth, higher education level is associated with larger hippocampal volumes. One explanation is that education may stimulate the growth and development of the hippocampus. Alternatively, people with larger hippocampal volume may have a better chance to acquire higher education. Fifth, the study may have a recall bias since reading activities were recorded by self-reported questionnaires. People might under- or overestimate the books they read. Objective measures (e.g., a shopping receipt of purchased books) may help to validate the finding. Finally, reading activities as measured by reading books are mainly leisure reading. It does not take into account of all activities related to intellectual activities. Individuals who do a lot of reading or research at work but have little time in reading books outside of work may be underestimated in reading activities.

Conclusions

Participants in reading groups with less education (educational years ≤12) had better cognitive performance than the ones in non-reading groups. Education affects more than reading habits in every cognitive domain and in hippocampal volume.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the participants and their caregivers who participated in the study.

Declarations

This protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Beijing Tiantan Hospital (approval number: KY 2019-004-007) and was in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Written informed consent was obtained from each participant.
Not involving any person’s data in any form.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, Levälahti E, Ahtiluoto S, Antikainen R, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2015;385(9984):2255–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(15)60461-5.CrossRefPubMed Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, Levälahti E, Ahtiluoto S, Antikainen R, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2015;385(9984):2255–63. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​S0140-6736(15)60461-5.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson AS, Flicker LJ, Lichtenberg PA. Reading ability mediates the relationship between education and executive function tasks. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006;12(1):64–71.CrossRef Johnson AS, Flicker LJ, Lichtenberg PA. Reading ability mediates the relationship between education and executive function tasks. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006;12(1):64–71.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment, (2nd ed). New York: Oxford University Press; 1983. Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment, (2nd ed). New York: Oxford University Press; 1983.
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Spreen O, Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998 Spreen O, Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Lezak MDHD, Loring DW, Hannay HJ, Fischer JS. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. Lezak MDHD, Loring DW, Hannay HJ, Fischer JS. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004.
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Hall CB, Derby CA, Kuslansky G, et al. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med. 2003;348(25):2508–16.CrossRef Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Hall CB, Derby CA, Kuslansky G, et al. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med. 2003;348(25):2508–16.CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Reading activities compensate for low education-related cognitive deficits
verfasst von
Yue Wang
Shinan Wang
Wanlin Zhu
Na Liang
Chen Zhang
Yuankun Pei
Qing Wang
Shiping Li
Jiong Shi
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2022
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Alzheimer's Research & Therapy / Ausgabe 1/2022
Elektronische ISSN: 1758-9193
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-022-01098-1

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2022

Alzheimer's Research & Therapy 1/2022 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Nicht Creutzfeldt Jakob, sondern Abführtee-Vergiftung

29.05.2024 Hyponatriämie Nachrichten

Eine ältere Frau trinkt regelmäßig Sennesblättertee gegen ihre Verstopfung. Der scheint plötzlich gut zu wirken. Auf Durchfall und Erbrechen folgt allerdings eine Hyponatriämie. Nach deren Korrektur kommt es plötzlich zu progredienten Kognitions- und Verhaltensstörungen.

Schutz der Synapsen bei Alzheimer

29.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Mit einem Neurotrophin-Rezeptor-Modulator lässt sich möglicherweise eine bestehende Alzheimerdemenz etwas abschwächen: Erste Phase-2-Daten deuten auf einen verbesserten Synapsenschutz.

Sozialer Aufstieg verringert Demenzgefahr

24.05.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Ein hohes soziales Niveau ist mit die beste Versicherung gegen eine Demenz. Noch geringer ist das Demenzrisiko für Menschen, die sozial aufsteigen: Sie gewinnen fast zwei demenzfreie Lebensjahre. Umgekehrt steigt die Demenzgefahr beim sozialen Abstieg.

Hirnblutung unter DOAK und VKA ähnlich bedrohlich

17.05.2024 Direkte orale Antikoagulanzien Nachrichten

Kommt es zu einer nichttraumatischen Hirnblutung, spielt es keine große Rolle, ob die Betroffenen zuvor direkt wirksame orale Antikoagulanzien oder Marcumar bekommen haben: Die Prognose ist ähnlich schlecht.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.