Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Brain Structure and Function 2/2020

Open Access 15.02.2020 | Original Article

Functional connectivity correlates of infant and early childhood cognitive development

verfasst von: Muriel M. K. Bruchhage, Giang-Chau Ngo, Nora Schneider, Viren D’Sa, Sean C. L. Deoni

Erschienen in: Brain Structure and Function | Ausgabe 2/2020

Abstract

Childhood is defined by the development of cognitive abilities as well as brain growth and function. While prior neuroimaging studies have investigated early development fragmentally, we studied the typical development of functional network connectivity continuously from infancy to childhood (average of 24 months) in 196 singleton term born children, as well as their emergence with age and visual, motor, and language abilities as assessed using the Mullen Scales of Early Learning. We demonstrate a cross-age shift to networks linked to higher-order cognitive processes, paralleling previous findings about developmental courses of functional connectivity networks. When investigating skill associations with functional connectivity independent of age, we revealed distinct network connectivity patterns for visual, motor, and language skills as each of them become more and more refined along childhood development. Specifically, the amount of functional networks recruited increases with skill complexity, with an exceeding involvement of higher order networks enabling daily maintenance and coordination of cognitive functions. Further, both motor and language network connectivity patterns overlapped in network connectivity patterns for the default mode, visual, salience, and dorsal attention networks, possibly implicating their overarching contribution to each other’s and higher cognitive development.

Introduction

Infancy and early childhood are periods of rapid brain and cognitive development. This developmental period is defined by early motor and language milestones, which are facilitated by dynamic changes in underlying and sub-serving structural and functional brain networks (Silbereis et al. 2016). Structurally, processes such as myelination, dendritic growth, and synaptic pruning help to establish rapid and efficient communication pathways within and across neural networks. Through increased messaging speed and synchrony, these structural changes help drive the maturation of functional networks, which may incorporate several structurally connected brain regions that work together to perform cognitive tasks. Further, the topology and organization of functional networks may change with age and cognitive development, as specificity and specialization of related brain regions increases.
Functional networks, assessed using resting state functional connectivity imaging (rsFMRI), appear to be present already during prenatal brain development (Perani et al. 2011). Throughout infancy and childhood, these networks are highly dynamic and flexibly interact with each other and display differential developmental trajectories with age (Marrus et al. 2018), mirroring the temporal pattern of emerging cognitive function. Examining the topology of networks through both longitudinal studies and cross-sectional comparisons between infants and adults suggest a general evolution from local to distributed organization (Power et al. 2011). To lay the foundation for the development of more complex cognitive skills, essential functions need to be present very early during development. For example, one of the first functions to develop in utero is visual reception. To enable this, visual networks are present and functioning at birth, and amongst the first to reach adult-like status (Gao et al. 2017).
Paralleling cognitive skill development, motor and language networks develop subsequently together with skill ability. For example, Lin et al. (2008) have shown that the strength and extent of sensorimotor networks increase more rapidly than those of visual networks throughout early childhood (assessed at ages 2 weeks, 1 year, and 2 years) setting the foundation for the development of more complex skills. Investigating networks associated with walking, Marrus et al. (2018) recently reported increasing connectivity in motor networks and ability with age (at ages 12 and 24 months), possibly implying a continuous functional connectivity relationship with motor abilities originating in early development (Marrus et al. 2018). Similarly, language networks are already established at birth and progressively mature towards left hemispheric functional dominance, a characteristic of adult networks (Perani et al. 2011).
Therefore, past studies have provided insight into how functional brain systems organize with age, in step with evolving and maturing cognitive functions. However, due to the age ranges and developmental periods investigated, there remains a fragmented view of the relationships between functional networks, cognitive development, and performance, particularly across early childhood (birth to 5 years of age; for example, Zielinski et al. 2010). It is especially important to describe typical cognitive functioning in this age range, as they are essential for the understanding and detection of abnormal development.
To address this gap, we report on the patterns of correlation between resting state functional network connectivity and measures of language, motor, and visual functioning in a large cohort of neuro-typically developing children, 3 months to 6 years of age. Using rsFMRI data acquired from 196 healthy children without major risk factors for developmental abnormalities, we show an expected trend of increasing functional network connectivity with age, with little differences between boys and girls. Next, exploring the correlation between cognitive skills assessed using the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (Mullen 1995), we found expected overlaps in anatomical specificity and domain ability. Specifically, visual network connectivity increased with visual reception scores, sensorimotor network connectivity increased with both gross and fine motor scores, and language network connectivity increased with both receptive and expressive language scores. We also show more dynamic connectivity patterns with higher order networks such as the default mode, attention, and salience networks. As our findings investigate and describe the distinct underlying functional network connectivity patterns underlying different motor and cognitive abilities acquired throughout infancy and early childhood, it has the potential for a landmark to detect subclinical early aberrant brain—behavior patterns.

Materials and methods

Participant demographics

Data used in this study were drawn from the ongoing BAMBAM (Brown university Assessment of Myelination and Behavioral development Across Maturation) study of neurotypical brain and cognitive development, based at Brown University and located in Providence, RI, USA. For overview, BAMBAM was designed as an accelerated-longitudinal study of a large community cohort of healthy children. Approximately half of the study cohort was enrolled between 2 and 8 months of age; and the remainder between 2 and 4 years of age. Depending on child age, study visits occur every 6 months (under age 2) or 12 months, and include multi-modal MRI, performance, and parent reported measures of cognitive and behavioral functioning, anthropometry, and biospecimen collection. Participants were recruited with a wide range of different approaches, including online and newspaper advertisements, flyers, as well as referrals from pediatric hospitals.
Children with known major risk factors for developmental abnormalities at enrolment were excluded. Exclusion criteria included: in utero alcohol, cigarette or illicit substance exposure; preterm (< 37 weeks gestation) birth; small for gestational age or less than 1500 g; fetal ultrasound abnormalities; complicated pregnancy including preeclampsia, high blood pressure, or gestational diabetes; 5 min APGAR scores < 8; NCU admission; neurological disorder (e.g. head injury resulting in loss of consciousness, epilepsy); and psychiatric or learning disorder in the infant, parents or siblings (including maternal depression requiring medication in the year prior to pregnancy). In addition to screening at the time of enrollment, on-going screening for worrisome behaviors associated with neurodevelopmental disorders such as autism spectrum disorder using validated tools were performed to identify at-risk children and remove them from subsequent analysis. These included the modified checklist for autism (Robins et al. 1999), parent report measures (e.g. Child Behavior Checklist; Bilenberg et al. 1999), as well as medical and educational history data.
From the BAMBAM cohort, 196 out of 204 children were selected for analysis in this study (see details below), with each providing a single neuroimaging and neurocognitive dataset. With a 67% Caucasian and 33% non-white ratio, spanning families with income ranges lower than $10,000–$200,000 and above annual income with an mean average of $50,000–$69,999 real household income, our cohort is representative of the general US population (60.4% white and 39.6% non-white, average income of $61,372 as of 2017; Fontenot et al. 2018). Yearly household income was assessed in groups: (1) less than $10,000; (2) $10,000–$29,000; (3) $30,000–$49,000; (4) $50,000–$69,000; (5) $70,000–$89,000; (6) $90,000–$109,000; (7) $110,000–$149,000; (8) $150,000–$199,000; (9) $200,000 and more, as well as the option to prefer not to give information on family income.
Specific inclusion criteria were: (1) complete and high quality anatomical and rsFMRI dataset; (2) MRI data were acquired during natural sleep (so as to not mix awake and asleep rsFMRI data); (3) had a complete medical and family history that included birth outcome information; and (4) was a singleton full-term birth of healthy weight (more than 1500 g at birth). Participant demographics are provided in Table 1.
Table 1
Participant demographics
 
Boys
Girls
Number
109
87
Age at scan in months
24.9 (18.1)
23.7 (18.3)
Gestation time in weeks
39.6 (1.0)
39.2 (1.3)
Birth weight in lbs
7.4 (1.0)
7.4 (1.0)
Maternal education
2.83 (3.0)
2.43 (2.9)
Mullen
  
Early learning composite
96.83 (17.98)
105.94 (14.81)
Gross motor
18.01 (7.85)
17.45 (9.42)
Fine motor
22.65 (12.21)
22.49 (13.33)
Visual reception
25.07 (14.10)
24.95 (14.81)
Receptive language
21.97 (13.17)
22.21 (13.54)
Expressive language
21.02 (13.17)
20.99 (14.15)
Mean participant demographics with standard deviations in brackets. Average gestation time is given in weeks, birth weight is calculated in pounds, and maternal education was assessed with the Hollingshead scale

MRI acquisition and functional connectivity processing

All neuroimaging data were acquired on a 3 T Siemens Trio scanner with a 12-channel head RF array. rsFMRI data were acquired during natural sleep with the following parameters: TE = 34 ms, TR = 2.5 s, flip angle = 80°, field of view = 24 × 24cm2, imaging matrix = 80 × 80, and 32 interleaved 3.6 mm slices (for a voxel resolution: 3 × 3 × 3.6mm3), BW = 751 Hz/pixel, and GRAPPA acceleration factor of 2. We acquired 132 volumes acquired for a total acquisition time of approximately 7:00 min. To achieve successful scanning without the use of sedatives, scans were scheduled around the child’s natural nap time. Once asleep, the child was transferred from one of the napping beds that included a bottom layer of custom made plexiglass that in turn enabled the child to be placed asleep onto the scanner bed. To remain in the sleep state, we used custom made patting going inside the bore to quiet the scanning noise, custom head phone pieces playing soothing rain sounds, pulse socks to monitor pulse and asleep behavior, as well as padding for the child to remain in a fixed position. At least one research assistant was in the scanner with the child watching at all times in addition to a camera facing the scanner.
T1-weighted anatomical data were also acquired using a magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo scan was acquired of each child with an isotropic voxel volume of 1.4 × 1.4 × 1.4 mm3. Sequence specific parameters were: TE = 6.9 ms; TR = 16 ms; inversion preparation time = 950 ms; flip angle = 15°; BW = 450 Hz/Pixel. The acquisition matrix and field of view were varied according to child head size to maintain a constant voxel volume and spatial resolution across all ages.
To extract connectivity values, the rsFMRI data were first preprocessed (including realignment, centering, motion correction, and scrubbing) with the CONN-fMRI toolbox for SPM 8 (Whitfield-Gabrieli et al. 2012) on MATLAB and registered to our child study template using FSL FLIRT (Smith et al. 2004) and ANTS (Avants et al. 2014). ROI-to-ROI connectivity analyses were performed, computing the correlation of spontaneous BOLD activity between network regions. This enabled us to determine differences in brain network connectivity with age as well as the individual Mullen raw domain scores (see below). A set of 32 anatomical ROIs were used in the network analysis (Whitfield-Gabrieli et al. 2012) and a reference of the distribution of the networks in the infant brain can be found in Figure S1. Using the implemented CompCor strategy (Behzadi et al. 2007), the effect of nuisance covariates including BOLD signal fluctuations from CSF, white matter and their derivatives, as well as the realignment parameter noises were reduced. Data were band-pass filtered (0.008 < f < 0.09 HZ). In addition, preprocessed images were visually inspected for remaining motion after data preprocessing, reducing the inclusion number from 204 to 196 children.

Statistical analyses

We investigated the correlation of functional connectivity networks with age as well as the different Mullen raw scores using the CONN toolbox. Statistical analyses were performed for the following conditions: (1) correlation with child age (corrected to a 40-week gestation) for all participants; and (2) correlation with individual raw Mullen scores for visual reception, fine motor, gross motor, receptive language, and executive language, all corrected for child age. For analysis #2, all children were included in the analysis for fine motor, visual reception, and expressive and receptive language. Only children up to 2.5 years of age were included in the analysis for gross motor, since most children reach maximal score for this domain by 30 months (Table 1). All analysis for #1 and #2 were repeated for all children as well as for both sexes individually. Independent sample t-tests were used to determine differences between biological sex for Mullen raw scores using IBM SPSS version 24. For all analyses, significance was defined as p ≤ 0.05 false discovery rate (FDR) seed-level corrected for multiple comparisons.

Mullen scales of early learning

Child cognitive development was assessed using the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL), a standardized and population normalized tool for assessing fine and gross motor, expressive and receptive language, and visual reception functioning in children from birth through 68 months of age. In addition to 5 raw scores and age-normalized domain-specific T scores (mean of 50, standard deviation of 7.5), the MSEL also provides a composite score that combines overall visual, motor, and language functioning.

Results

Functional network connectivity changes with child age

When boys and girls were combined, we found dynamic changes in connectivity across all functional networks when examining overall changes with age (Fig. 1, Table 2). Specifically, most changes with age were observed in language, salience, default mode, and attention networks, with a combination of down- and up-regulation across networks (Table 2). Exploring these age-related trends further and investigating potential differences in boys and girls, we found significant overlap (Table S2). Though there were subtle but distinct differences, specifically less connectivity in sensorimotor and language networks in girls when compared to boys, these results suggest unsurprisingly that the general networks underlying individual cognitive skills and behaviors do not differ in boys and girls, and that they develop in a consistent manner.
Table 2
Resting state fMRI network connectivity with age
Seed network
Seed region
Connected network
Connected region
T
p corr
Default mode
PCC
Default mode
LP (r.)
3.66
.0046
 
PCC
Language
pST (l)
3.57
.0046
   
pST (r.)
4.07
.0021
 
LP (r.)
Default mode
LP (l.)
2.74
.0483
   
PCC
3.66
.0046
  
Salience
Anterior insula (l.)
− 3.40
.0084
   
Anterior insula (r.)
− 4.64
.0002*
   
ACC
− 3.26
.0013
  
Cerebellar
Posterior
2.68
.0417
 
MPFC
Sensorimotor
Lateral (r.)
3.25
.0085
  
Language
pSTG (r.)
3.61
.0051
   
pSTG (l.)
3.57
.0046
   
IFG (r.)
4.09
.0020
   
IFG (l.)
3.54
.0051
Sensorimotor
Superior
Cerebellar
Posterior
− 4.38
.0006*
 
Lateral (r.)
Default mode
MPFC
3.25
.0212
  
Cerebellar
Anterior
− 2.91
.0424
   
Posterior
− 3.31
.0212
Visual
Lateral (l.)
Visual
lateral (r.)
5.17
.0000*
  
Dorsal attention
IPS (r.)
3.73
.0039
   
FEF (r.)
2.90
.0262
  
Default mode
LP (l.)
− 3.50
.0060
  
Frontoparietal
PPC (l.)
− 3.10
.0175
 
Lateral (r.)
Visual
Lateral (l.)
5.17
.0000*
  
Dorsal attention
IPS (l.)
3.99
.0014
   
IPS (r.)
3.30
.0071
   
FEF (l.)
2.68
.0357
   
FEF (r.)
3.68
.0031
  
Frontoparietal
PPC (r.)
− 3.49
.0046
  
Sensorimotor
Lateral (l.)
2.89
.0225
 
Occipital
Frontoparietal
LPFC (r.)
− 3.52
.0165
salience
SMG (l.)
Salience
IPS (l.)
− 3.37
.0280
   
IPS (r.)
− 2.98
.0497
 
SMG (r.)
Language
pSTG (l.)
− 3.46
.0203
 
RPFC (l.)
Salience
ACC
4.48
.0004*
 
RPFC (r.)
Salience
ACC
4.38
.0006*
 
Anterior insula (l.)
Salience
ACC
3.32
.0164
  
Default mode
LP (r.)
− 3.40
.0164
 
Anterior insula (r.)
Salience
ACC
2.68
.0417
  
Default mode
LP (r.)
− 4.64
.0002*
   
LP (l.)
− 2.69
.0417
  
Dorsal attention
FEF (r.)
− 3.39
.0108
  
Frontoparietal
IPS (l.)
− 3.33
.0108
   
IPS (r.)
− 2.71
.0417
 
ACC
Salience
RPFC (l.)
4.48
.0003*
   
RPFC (r.)
4.38
.0003*
   
Anterior insula (l.)
3.32
.0066
   
Anterior insula (r.)
2.68
.0284
  
Default mode
LP (l.)
− 3.79
.0021
   
LP (r.)
− 3.26
.0068
   
MPFC
− 3.42
.0059
   
PCC
− 2.67
.0284
  
Frontoparietal
PPC (l.)
− 3.00
.0137
Dorsal attention
FEF (l.)
Dorsal attention
IPS (l.)
4.57
.0001*
   
IPS (r.)
4.55
.0001*
 
FEF (r.)
Dorsal attention
IPS (r.)
4.03
.0025
   
IPS (l.)
3.45
.0065
  
Visual
Lateral (r.)
3.68
.0046
   
Lateral (l.)
2.90
.0262
  
Salience
Anterior insula (r.)
− 3.39
.0065
 
IPS (l.)
Dorsal attention
FEF (l.)
4.57
.0003*
   
FEF (r.)
3.45
.0054
   
IPS (r.)
3.83
.0018
  
Visual
Lateral (r.)
3.99
.0014
  
Salience
SMG (l.)
− 3.37
.0056
  
Frontoparietal
PPC (l.)
− 2.82
.0273
 
IPS (r.)
Dorsal attention
FEF (l.)
4.55
.0003*
   
FEF (r.)
4.03
.0012
   
IPS (l.)
3.83
.0018
  
Visual
Lateral (l.)
3.73
.0010
   
Lateral (r.)
3.30
.0071
  
Language
IFG (r.)
− 3.20
.0083
  
Salience
SMG (l.)
− 2.98
.0142
  
Default mode
PCC
− 2.56
.0437
Frontoparietal
LPFC (l.)
Frontoparietal
PPC (l.)
4.68
.0002*
  
Language
IFG (r.)
− 3.23
.0227
   
IFG (l.)
− 3.07
.0257
 
PPC (l.)
Frontoparietal
LPFC (l.)
4.68
.0002*
  
Salience
Anterior insula
− 3.33
.0162
   
ACC
− 3.00
.0240
  
Visual
Lateral (l.)
− 3.10
.0233
  
Dorsal attention
IPS (l.)
− 2.82
.0328
  
Cerebellar
Anterior
2.75
.0338
 
LPFC (r.)
Visual
Occipital
− 3.52
.0165
 
PPC (r.)
Visual
lateral (r.)
− 3.49
.0185
Language
pSTG (r.)
Default mode
PCC
4.07
.0021
   
MPFC
3.61
.0060
   
LP (l.)
2.78
.0463
  
Frontoparietal
PPC (r.)
− 2.92
.0405
 
pSTG (l.)
Language
IFG (l.)
3.59
.0051
  
Default mode
PCC
3.57
.0051
   
MPFC
2.79
.0357
  
Cerebellar
Anterior
3.49
.0051
  
Salience
SMG (r.)
− 3.46
.0051
 
IFG (r.)
Language
IFG (r.)
− 3.48
.0096
  
Default mode
MPFC
4.09
.0020
  
Frontoparietal
LPFC (l.)
− 3.23
.0124
   
PPC (r.)
− 2.81
.0341
  
Dorsal attention
IPS (r.)
− 3.20
.0124
 
IFG (l.)
Language
pSTG (l.)
3.59
.0064
   
IFG (r.)
− 3.48
.0064
  
Default mode
MPFC
3.54
.0064
  
Frontoparietal
LPFC (l.)
− 3.07
.0192
  
Salience
RPFC (r.)
− 2.79
.0357
   
RPFC (l.)
− 2.65
.0448
Cerebellum
Anterior
Language
pSTG (l.)
3.49
.0188
 
Posterior
Sensorimotor
Superior
− 4.38
.0006*
   
Lateral (r.)
− 3.31
.0006*
   
Lateral (l.)
− 3.09
.0174
Significant resting state fMRI network connectivity age interactions with T and p values on the right. Seed networks and sub regions are displayed in the left two columns, and connected network connectivity regions in the middle. All analyses were corrected for biological sex, and p values were FDR corrected for multiple comparisons. p < 0.001 are highlighted with *
MPFC medial prefrontal cortex, LP lateral parietal cortex, PCC posterior cingulate cortex, ACC anterior cingular cortex, RPFC rostral prefrontal cortex, SMG supramarginal gyrus, FEF frontal eye field, IPS intraparietal sulcus, LPFC lateral prefrontal cortex, PPC posterior parietal cortex, IFG inferior frontal gyrus, pSTG posterior superior temporal gyrus

Functional network connectivity correlates with Mullen development domains

Investigating connectivity changes associated with specific cognitive domains in our full child cohort, we found that improved visual, motor, and language functioning was associated with changes in connectivity across distinct functional networks (Figs. 2, 3, 4, Table 3). Specifically, visual reception was associated with increased connectivity in visual and dorsal attention networks, salience, frontoparietal, and cerebellar networks (Fig. 2, Table 3). Both increasing gross and fine motor ability independently were associated with increased default mode, visual, salience, dorsal attention, frontoparietal, and cerebellar network connectivity, while gross motor was additionally associated with sensorimotor and language network connectivity (Fig. 3, Table 3).
Table 3
Resting state fMRI network connectivity with Mullen scores
Seed network
Seed region
Connected network
Connected region
T
p corr
(a) Gross motor
     
Default mode
MPFC
Language
pST (l)
3.58
.0146
   
pST (r.)
3.31
.0185
Sensorimotor
 
Salience
RPFC (l.)
3.90
.0047
Visual
lateral (l.)
Visual
Lateral (r.)
3.77
.0074
  
Dorsal attention
IPS (l.)
3.44
.0119
   
IPS (r.)
2.93
.0416
 
lateral (r.)
Dorsal attention
IPS (l.)
3.11
.0354
Salience
SMG (r.)
Salience
RPFC (r.)
3.15
.0210
 
SMG (l.)
Salience
RPFC (l.)
3.08
.0384
   
RPFC (r.)
2.77
.0491
 
RPFC (r.)
Visual
Occipital
3.90
.0047
   
Medial
3.08
.0384
 
RPFC (l.)
Sensorimotor
Lateral (l.)
3.22
.0495
 
Anterior insula
Cerebellar
Anterior
3.22
.0495
Dorsal attention
IPS (l.)
Visual
Lateral (l.)
3.77
.0074
   
Lateral (r.)
3.11
.0354
Frontoparietal
LPFC (r.)
Frontoparietal
PPC (r.)
3.28
.0410
Language
pSTG (l.)
Default mode
MPFC
3.58
.0146
 
pSTG (r.)
 
MPFC
3.31
.0370
Cerebellum
Anterior
Salience
Anterior insula
3.22
.0495
(b) Fine motor
     
Default mode
MPFC
Default mode
LP (r.)
3.20
.0491
  
Frontoparietal
LPFC (r.)
3.42
.0233
Visual
lateral (l.)
Visual
Lateral (r.)
3.46
.0207
  
Dorsal attention
IPS (l.)
3.04
.0279
   
IPS (r.)
3.53
.0104
Salience
RPFC (l.)
Salience
SMG (l.)
3.16
.0256
   
ACC
3.26
.0234
  
Sensorimotor
Lateral (l.)
3.07
.0256
 
Anterior insula
Language
IFG (l.)
− 2.99
.0493
  
Cerebellar
Posterior
3.36
.0296
 
ACC
Cerebellar
Anterior
3.22
.0234
Dorsal attention
IPS (r.)
Visual
Lateral (l.)
3.53
.0160
Frontoparietal
LPFC (r.)
Frontoparietal
PPC (r.)
4.09
.0020
  
Default mode
MPFC
3.42
.0117
Cerebellum
Anterior
Salience
ACC
3.22
.0468
 
Posterior
 
Anterior insula (r.)
3.36
.0296
(c) Visual reception
     
Visual
Lateral (l.)
Visual
Lateral (r.)
3.78
.0064
  
Sensorimotor
Superior
− 2.80
.0438
  
Dorsal attention
IPS (l.)
3.38
.0107
   
IPS (r.)
3.33
.0107
Salience
SMG (l.)
Salience
RPFC (l.)
3.57
.0138
 
ACC
Cerebellar
Anterior
3.43
.0230
Dorsal attention
IPS (l.)
Dorsal attention
FEF (l.)
3.18
.0265
  
Visual
Lateral (l.)
3.38
.0265
 
IPS (r.)
Visual
Lateral (l.)
3.33
.0322
Frontoparietal
LPFC (r.)
Frontoparietal
PPC (r.)
3.77
.0067
  
Default mode
MPFC
3.09
.0353
Cerebellar
Anterior
Salience
ACC
3.43
.0230
(d) Receptive language
     
Default mode
LP (l.)
Salience
SMG (l.)
3.04
.0414
  
Language
pSTG (r.)
3.25
.0414
 
MPFC
Frontoparietal
LPFC (l.)
3.54
.0079
   
LPFC (r.)
3.70
.0079
Visual
Lateral (l.)
Visual
Lateral (r.)
3.73
.0026
  
Dorsal attention
IPS (l.)
3.86
.0024
   
IPS (r.)
4.03
.0024
Salience
ACC
Language
IFG (l.)
− 3.46
.0208
Dorsal attention
IPS (l.)
Visual
Lateral (l.)
3.86
.0048
 
IPS (r.)
 
Lateral (l.)
4.03
.0025
Frontoparietal
LPFC (l.)
Default mode
MPFC
3.54
.0157
 
LPFC (r.)
 
MPFC
3.70
.0087
Language
IFG (l.)
Salience
Anterior insula (r.)
− 3.46
.0208
 
pSTG (r.)
Default mode
LP (r.)
3.25
.0422
(e) Executive language
     
Default mode
MPFC
Frontoparietal
LPFC (l.)
3.14
.0453
   
LPFC (r.)
3.01
.0453
Visual
Lateral (l.)
Visual
Lateral (r.)
3.15
.0195
  
Dorsal attention
IPS (l.)
3.74
.0075
   
IPS (r.)
3.34
.0158
Dorsal attention
IPS (l.)
Visual
Lateral (l.)
3.74
.0075
 
IPS (r.)
Visual
Lateral (l.)
3.34
.0316
  
Frontoparietal
LPFC (r.)
3.11
.0338
Frontoparietal
LPFC (r.)
Dorsal attention
IPS (r.)
3.11
.0453
  
Default mode
MPFC
3.01
.0453
Significant resting state fMRI network connectivity and relevant motor (a, b), visual (c), and language (d, e) Mullen score interactions with T and p values on the right. Seed networks and sub regions are displayed in the left two columns, and connected network connectivity regions in the middle. All analyses were corrected for gestational age and biological sex, and p values were FDR corrected for multiple comparisons
MPFC medial prefrontal cortex, LP lateral parietal cortex, PCC posterior cingulate cortex, ACC anterior cingular cortex, RPFC rostral prefrontal cortex, SMG supramarginal gyrus, FEF frontal eye field, IPS intraparietal sulcus, LPFC lateral prefrontal cortex, PPC posterior parietal cortex, IFG inferior frontal gyrus, pSTG posterior superior temporal gyrus
Increasing receptive and expressive language ability were independently associated with increased default mode, visual, dorsal attention, and frontoparietal network connectivity, while receptive language was additionally associated with salience and language network connectivity (Fig. 4, Table 3).
Thus, increasing motor and language ability overlapped in default mode, visual, salience, and dorsal attention network connectivity patterns (Figs. 3, 4, Table 3).
While independent t tests revealed no significant differences in Mullen raw scores between boys and girls (i.e., gross motor: F = 6.984, p = 0.701; fine motor: F = 1.070, p = 0.932; visual reception: F = 0.500, p = 0.954; receptive language: F = 0.011, p = 903; expressive language: F = 0.698, p = 0.988), we did find subtle differences in network development differences with respect to these different skills (Tables S3–S7). Specifically, girls showed more overall connectivity in language domains, with additional language network activation for executive language and visual medial activation in receptive language as well as increased functional connectivity in language and attention networks for visual reception. Boys displayed more overall connectivity in the motor domains with additional activation of visual motor and sensorimotor superior activity for gross motor and cerebellar networks in executive language as well as increased functional connectivity in sensorimotor networks for visual reception. A reference of the distribution of all functional connectivity networks in the infant brain can be found in Figure S1.

Discussion

Prior neuroimaging studies exploring early development of functional networks have provided a fragmented view, with many studies focusing on either infancy (0–2 years) or later childhood (> 4 years), but few linking these important periods. However, significant gains and refinements in motor control and language abilities occur during ages 2 and 5, laying the foundation for the establishment and refinement of higher cognitive skills and ensuring school readiness. To fill this gap, we applied a data-driven approach in a large neurotypically developing cohort spanning from early infancy to young childhood, demonstrating distinct functional network connectivity patterns with age that overlap with network patterns connected to major visual, motor, and language development.

Network connectivity with age

Network maturation occurs in specific brain regions associated with goal-directed behavior as well as higher order networks for daily maintenance. While most of these functions are present during early development, their maturation can continue through adolescence (for a review, Barber et al. 2012).
Similarly, we observed functional connectivity patterns for a mixture of networks enabling complex cognitive functions (for example dorsal attention and language networks) as well as higher order networks enabling the increasingly complex daily maintenance (for example default mode and salience networks; Fig. 1, Table 2). This cross-age shift to networks linked to higher-order cognitive processes parallels previous findings about developmental courses of functional connectivity networks.

Visual function networks

Visual reception describes the ability to interpret information about the surrounding environment that the eyes receive. Visual networks are already present and functioning at birth and are amongst the first to be fully developed reaching an adult-like status (Gao et al. 2017). Similarly, all networks increased in functional connectivity with visual reception in our study.
Increased efficiency and wiring-cost of functional brain networks have been positively associated with increased visual task complexity (Wen et al. 2015). During early development, synaptogenesis precedes later pruning eliminating excess connections (Silbereis et al. 2016), leading to functional network reorganization through more efficient but also more costly network configuration when there is greater demand for cognitive processing. In line with these prior findings, we were able to demonstrate increased functional connectivity with visual networks as well as networks involved in broader higher cognitive functions such as the salience, dorsal attention, and frontoparietal network, supporting optimal visual processing.

Motor function networks

Work in human and nonhuman primates has indicated that specific brain regions contribute directly to motor function, such as the primary motor cortex, and indirectly, such as supplementary motor areas, to accurate motor execution together with areas involved in cognitive functions (Fink et al. 1997; Rizzolatti and Ruppino 2001; Hanakawa et al. 2003). As a result, a range of networks are required for appropriate motor action including sensorimotor and cerebellar networks, as mirrored in our findings (Fig. 3, Table 3).
Motor function is further typically divided into gross and fine motor abilities. Here, gross motor describes abilities required to control large muscles of the body for functions such as walking, sitting, and crawling, while fine motor function describes the coordination of small muscles, usually involving the synchronization of hands and fingers. In a recent infant study (Marrus et al. 2018), a dynamic subset of resting state motor networks displayed strong relationships with walking and Mullen gross motor scores. These infant/toddler motor associations overlapped with documented adult findings, possibly implying a continuous relationship between functional connectivity and motor skills originating in early development. Specifically, they found gross motor associations with enriched default mode, somatomotor, attention, visual, salience, language, and frontoparietal networks combined for both infant and toddler cohorts—all networks we found showed significant associations with gross motor scores as well (Fig. 3a).
In contrast to the early developing gross motor function, fine motor skills involve a wider range of functions to compose a precisely coordinated movement. As a result, a broader span of functional networks would be required to perform the increasingly complex tasks, as reflected in our results where fine motor scores were associated with more network connectivity than gross motor scores (Fig. 3b).
Interestingly, both gross and fine motor network connectivity patterns overlapped in network connectivity patterns for both primary and non-primary motor areas. This could suggest that while gross and fine motor functions differ, both underlie a consistent network connectivity pattern for motor behavior.

Language function networks

Support for very early language-related abilities demonstrate that newborns can discriminate between different speech sounds already after birth (for review, Perani et al. 2011). Receptive language describes this ability of understanding words and sentences as well as the meaning of what others say or what is read. Similar to adults (Perani et al. 1996; Binder et al. 2000), speech processing is supported by inferior frontal and temporal brain regions already established at the age of 3 months (Dehaene-Lambertz et al. 2002, 2006). In line with previous findings, our study detected a positive relationship between receptive language scores and a variety of network connectivity patterns (Fig. 4a).
In contrast to the more passive ability receptive language requires, expressive language describes the ability to actively put thoughts into words and sentences, in a senseful and grammatically accurate way. When learning how to speak, one of the predominant developmental transition is the lateralization of the brain. Studies in adults have shown that some systems, like the primary sensory systems require fine-tuned integration between the hemispheres while other higher-order functions like language typically show asymmetry (Binder et al. 2000; Toga and Thomspson 2003; Friederici and Alter 2004; Stark et al. 2008; Perani et al. 2011).
These language-relevant brain areas are structurally and functionally connected (Dubois 2006), with a set of lateral brain regions in the left frontal, temporal, and parietal cortices being activated in previous studies during linguistic processing (Perani et al. 2011). This is paralleled by our findings, as frontal, temporal, and parietal regions increased with expressive language scores and all network connectivity patterns included bilateral and/or left hemisphere regions only (Fig. 4b).
While language networks are functional from infancy on, their functional network connectivity starts bilateral and then develops towards unilaterally with development. Interestingly, we are able to support this notion. Similarly to motor functions, both receptive and expressive language network connectivity patterns overlapped in network connectivity patterns, namely the default mode, visual, salience, dorsal attention, and frontoparietal network connectivity (Table 3, Fig. 4). In addition, as language function becomes more complex (from receptive to executive), these overlapping networks become more left hemisphere dominant.

Possible motor and language contributions to other network development

The more complex the ability was, the more functional connectivity networks were recruited (i.e. gross to fine motor and receptive to expressive language). In addition, both motor and language network connectivity patterns overlapped in network connectivity patterns for the default mode, visual, salience, and dorsal attention networks. These overlaps in network connectivity could implicate their overarching contribution to each other’s and higher cognitive development, as for example gross motor skill development in infants and toddlers is predictive of future cognitive outcomes (Marrus et al. 2018). The many positive and negative brain–behavior relationships we observed throughout our study could further imply that increases and decreases in network-level connectivity may underlie the characteristic complexity needed for both motor and language functioning.

Conclusion

Our study investigated the typical development of functional network connectivity during infancy and childhood, as well as their relationship with the emergence and establishment of cognitive abilities. With age, these behaviors become more and more refined and occur with increasing complexity, starting with gross to fine motor skills and finally progressing to more complex cognitive abilities. Our findings capture the developmental timeline of these early visual, motor, and language skills, as well as their gradual development towards adult-like networks. We further found a distinct brain–behavior relationship involving the default mode, visual, salience, and dorsal attention network, supporting both motor and language refinement. This could implicate a contribution to the maturation of cognitive functions during infancy and childhood development.
While our study focused on neuro-typically developing children, our findings will be important for the understanding and detection of abnormal development. Children may fail to reach important early motor and language milestones for a variety of reasons, including neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., autism spectrum disorder), prenatal insults (e.g., drug exposure), or other early environmental adversity (e.g., severe neglect). Our results provide a normative template to which these children may be compared to and enabling early deviations to be identified. Future work is needed to understand how these developmental processes are linked to the emergence of cognitive functions and how they are guided by complex underlying biological mechanisms.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the National Institutes of Health (UH3 ODD023313 and R01 MH087510) and the Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation (OPP11002016) for their continuous support of this work.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflict of interest to state.

Ethical approval

This study was approved by the local Internal Review Board and active written informed consent was collected for all participants given by the parent and/or caretaker on behalf of their child.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Neurologie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes, den Premium-Inhalten der neurologischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Neurologie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Weitere Produktempfehlungen anzeigen
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Avants BB, Tustison NJ, Stauffer M, Song G, Wu B, Gee JC (2014) The insight ToolKit image registration framework. Front Neuroinform 20:8 Avants BB, Tustison NJ, Stauffer M, Song G, Wu B, Gee JC (2014) The insight ToolKit image registration framework. Front Neuroinform 20:8
Zurück zum Zitat Barber AD, Caffo BS, Pekar JJ, Mostofskya SH (2012) Developmental changes in within- and between-network connectivity between late childhood and adulthood. Neuropsychologia 51:156–167CrossRef Barber AD, Caffo BS, Pekar JJ, Mostofskya SH (2012) Developmental changes in within- and between-network connectivity between late childhood and adulthood. Neuropsychologia 51:156–167CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Behzadi Y, Restom K, Liau J, Liu TT (2007) A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. NeuroImage 37:90–101CrossRef Behzadi Y, Restom K, Liau J, Liu TT (2007) A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. NeuroImage 37:90–101CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Bilenberg N (1999) The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and related material: standardization and validation in Danish population based and clinically based samples. Acts Psychiatry Stand Suppl 398:2–52CrossRef Bilenberg N (1999) The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and related material: standardization and validation in Danish population based and clinically based samples. Acts Psychiatry Stand Suppl 398:2–52CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Bellgowan PS, Springer JA, Kaufman JN, Possing ET (2000) Human temporal lobe activation by speech and non-speech sounds. Cereb Cortex 10:512–528CrossRef Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Bellgowan PS, Springer JA, Kaufman JN, Possing ET (2000) Human temporal lobe activation by speech and non-speech sounds. Cereb Cortex 10:512–528CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dehaene-Lambertz G, Dehaene S, Hertz-Pannier L (2002) Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science 298:2013–2015CrossRef Dehaene-Lambertz G, Dehaene S, Hertz-Pannier L (2002) Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science 298:2013–2015CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dehaene-Lambertz G, Hertz-Pannier L, Dubois J, Mériaux S, Roche A, Sigman M, Dehaene S (2006) Functional organization of perisylvian activation during presentation of sentences in preverbal infants. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:14240–14245CrossRef Dehaene-Lambertz G, Hertz-Pannier L, Dubois J, Mériaux S, Roche A, Sigman M, Dehaene S (2006) Functional organization of perisylvian activation during presentation of sentences in preverbal infants. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:14240–14245CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Dubois J, Hertz-Pannier L, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Cointepas Y, Le Bihan D (2006) Assessment of the early organization and maturation of infants’ cerebral white matter fiber bundles: a feasibility study using quantitative diffusion tensor imaging and tractography. Neuroimage 30:1121–1132CrossRef Dubois J, Hertz-Pannier L, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Cointepas Y, Le Bihan D (2006) Assessment of the early organization and maturation of infants’ cerebral white matter fiber bundles: a feasibility study using quantitative diffusion tensor imaging and tractography. Neuroimage 30:1121–1132CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fink GR, Frackowiak RS, Pietrzyk U, Passingham RE (1997) Multiple nonprimary motor areas in the human cortex. J Neurophysiol 77:2164–2174CrossRef Fink GR, Frackowiak RS, Pietrzyk U, Passingham RE (1997) Multiple nonprimary motor areas in the human cortex. J Neurophysiol 77:2164–2174CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Fontenot, K., Semega, J., Kollar, M. (2018). Income and poverty in the United States: 2017. Current population reports. U.S. Department of Commerce, Economics and Statistics Administration, U.S. Census Bureau, pp 60–263 Fontenot, K., Semega, J., Kollar, M. (2018). Income and poverty in the United States: 2017. Current population reports. U.S. Department of Commerce, Economics and Statistics Administration, U.S. Census Bureau, pp 60–263
Zurück zum Zitat Friederici AD, Alter K (2004) Lateralization of auditory language functions: a dynamic dual pathway model. Brain Lang 89(2):267–276CrossRef Friederici AD, Alter K (2004) Lateralization of auditory language functions: a dynamic dual pathway model. Brain Lang 89(2):267–276CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Gao W, Lin W, Grewen K, Gilmore JH (2017) Functional connectivity of the infant human brain: plastic and modifiable. Neuroscientist 23(2):169–184CrossRef Gao W, Lin W, Grewen K, Gilmore JH (2017) Functional connectivity of the infant human brain: plastic and modifiable. Neuroscientist 23(2):169–184CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Hanakawa T, Immisch I, Toma K, Dimyan MA, Van Gelderen P, Hallett M (2003) Functional properties of brain areas associated with motor execution and imagery. J Neurophysiol 89:989–1002CrossRef Hanakawa T, Immisch I, Toma K, Dimyan MA, Van Gelderen P, Hallett M (2003) Functional properties of brain areas associated with motor execution and imagery. J Neurophysiol 89:989–1002CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lin W, Zhu Q, Gao W, Chen Y, Toh CH, Styner M, Gerig G, Smith JK, Biswal B, Gilmore JH (2008) Functional connectivity MR imaging reveals cortical functional connectivity in the developing brain. Am J Neuroradiol 29:1883–1889CrossRef Lin W, Zhu Q, Gao W, Chen Y, Toh CH, Styner M, Gerig G, Smith JK, Biswal B, Gilmore JH (2008) Functional connectivity MR imaging reveals cortical functional connectivity in the developing brain. Am J Neuroradiol 29:1883–1889CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Marrus N, Eggebrecht AT, Todorov A, Elison JT, Wolff JJ, Cole L, Gao W, Pandey J, Shen MD, Swanson MR, Emerson W, Emerson RW, Klohr CL, Adams CM, Estes AM, Zwaigenbaum L, Botteron KN, McKinstry RC, Constantino JN, Evans AC, Hazlett HC, Dager SR, Paterson SJ, Schultz RT, Styner MA, Gerig G, Network IBIS, Schlaggar BL, Piven J, Pruett JR Jr (2018) Walking, gross motor development, and brain functional connectivity in infants and toddlers. Cereb Cortex 28(2):750–763CrossRef Marrus N, Eggebrecht AT, Todorov A, Elison JT, Wolff JJ, Cole L, Gao W, Pandey J, Shen MD, Swanson MR, Emerson W, Emerson RW, Klohr CL, Adams CM, Estes AM, Zwaigenbaum L, Botteron KN, McKinstry RC, Constantino JN, Evans AC, Hazlett HC, Dager SR, Paterson SJ, Schultz RT, Styner MA, Gerig G, Network IBIS, Schlaggar BL, Piven J, Pruett JR Jr (2018) Walking, gross motor development, and brain functional connectivity in infants and toddlers. Cereb Cortex 28(2):750–763CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Mullen EM (1995) Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines, AGS Mullen EM (1995) Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines, AGS
Zurück zum Zitat Perani D, Dehaene S, Grassi F, Cohen L, Canna SF, Dupoux E, Fazio F, Mehler J (1996) Brain processing of native and foreign languages. NeuroReport 7:2439–2444CrossRef Perani D, Dehaene S, Grassi F, Cohen L, Canna SF, Dupoux E, Fazio F, Mehler J (1996) Brain processing of native and foreign languages. NeuroReport 7:2439–2444CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Perani D, Saccuman MC, Scifo P, Anwander A, Spada D, Baldly C, Poloniato A, Lohmann G, Friedrich AD (2011) Neural language networks at birth. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108(38):16056–16061CrossRef Perani D, Saccuman MC, Scifo P, Anwander A, Spada D, Baldly C, Poloniato A, Lohmann G, Friedrich AD (2011) Neural language networks at birth. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108(38):16056–16061CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, Vogel AC, Laumann TO, Miezin FM, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2011) Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron 72(4):665–678CrossRef Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, Vogel AC, Laumann TO, Miezin FM, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2011) Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron 72(4):665–678CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Rizzolatti G, Luppino G (2001) The cortical motor system. Neuron 31:889–901CrossRef Rizzolatti G, Luppino G (2001) The cortical motor system. Neuron 31:889–901CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Robins DL, Fein D, Barton M (1999) Modified checklist for autism in toddlers. Self-published Robins DL, Fein D, Barton M (1999) Modified checklist for autism in toddlers. Self-published
Zurück zum Zitat Silbereis JC, Pochareddy S, Zhu Y, Li M, Sestan N (2016) The cellular and molecular landscapes of the developing human central nervous system. Neuron 89(2):248–268CrossRef Silbereis JC, Pochareddy S, Zhu Y, Li M, Sestan N (2016) The cellular and molecular landscapes of the developing human central nervous system. Neuron 89(2):248–268CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobniak I, Flitney DE, Niazy RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage 23(1):S208–219CrossRef Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobniak I, Flitney DE, Niazy RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage 23(1):S208–219CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Stark DE, Margulies DS, Shehzad ZE, Reiss P, Kelly AMC, Uddin LQ, Gee DG, Roy AK, Banich MT, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2008) Regional variation in interhemispheric coordination of intrinsic hemodynamic fluctuations. J Neurosci 28:13754–13764CrossRef Stark DE, Margulies DS, Shehzad ZE, Reiss P, Kelly AMC, Uddin LQ, Gee DG, Roy AK, Banich MT, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2008) Regional variation in interhemispheric coordination of intrinsic hemodynamic fluctuations. J Neurosci 28:13754–13764CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Toga AW, Thompson PM (2003) Mapping brain asymmetry. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:37–48CrossRef Toga AW, Thompson PM (2003) Mapping brain asymmetry. Nat Rev Neurosci 4:37–48CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wen X, Zhang D, Liang B, Zhang R, Wang Z, Wang J, Liu M, Huang R (2015) Reconfiguration of the brain functional network associated with visual task demands. PLoS ONE 10(7):e0132518CrossRef Wen X, Zhang D, Liang B, Zhang R, Wang Z, Wang J, Liu M, Huang R (2015) Reconfiguration of the brain functional network associated with visual task demands. PLoS ONE 10(7):e0132518CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A (2012) Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect 2:125–141CrossRef Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A (2012) Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect 2:125–141CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zielinski BA, Gennatas ED, Zhou J, Seeley WW (2010) Network-level structural covariance in the developing brain. PNAS 107(42):18191–18196CrossRef Zielinski BA, Gennatas ED, Zhou J, Seeley WW (2010) Network-level structural covariance in the developing brain. PNAS 107(42):18191–18196CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Functional connectivity correlates of infant and early childhood cognitive development
verfasst von
Muriel M. K. Bruchhage
Giang-Chau Ngo
Nora Schneider
Viren D’Sa
Sean C. L. Deoni
Publikationsdatum
15.02.2020
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
Brain Structure and Function / Ausgabe 2/2020
Print ISSN: 1863-2653
Elektronische ISSN: 1863-2661
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-020-02027-4

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2020

Brain Structure and Function 2/2020 Zur Ausgabe

Leitlinien kompakt für die Neurologie

Mit medbee Pocketcards sicher entscheiden.

Seit 2022 gehört die medbee GmbH zum Springer Medizin Verlag

Sozialer Aufstieg verringert Demenzgefahr

24.05.2024 Demenz Nachrichten

Ein hohes soziales Niveau ist mit die beste Versicherung gegen eine Demenz. Noch geringer ist das Demenzrisiko für Menschen, die sozial aufsteigen: Sie gewinnen fast zwei demenzfreie Lebensjahre. Umgekehrt steigt die Demenzgefahr beim sozialen Abstieg.

Hirnblutung unter DOAK und VKA ähnlich bedrohlich

17.05.2024 Direkte orale Antikoagulanzien Nachrichten

Kommt es zu einer nichttraumatischen Hirnblutung, spielt es keine große Rolle, ob die Betroffenen zuvor direkt wirksame orale Antikoagulanzien oder Marcumar bekommen haben: Die Prognose ist ähnlich schlecht.

Was nützt die Kraniektomie bei schwerer tiefer Hirnblutung?

17.05.2024 Hirnblutung Nachrichten

Eine Studie zum Nutzen der druckentlastenden Kraniektomie nach schwerer tiefer supratentorieller Hirnblutung deutet einen Nutzen der Operation an. Für überlebende Patienten ist das dennoch nur eine bedingt gute Nachricht.

Thrombektomie auch bei großen Infarkten von Vorteil

16.05.2024 Ischämischer Schlaganfall Nachrichten

Auch ein sehr ausgedehnter ischämischer Schlaganfall scheint an sich kein Grund zu sein, von einer mechanischen Thrombektomie abzusehen. Dafür spricht die LASTE-Studie, an der Patienten und Patientinnen mit einem ASPECTS von maximal 5 beteiligt waren.

Update Neurologie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.