Skip to main content
Erschienen in: International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction 2/2018

Open Access 18.07.2017 | Brief Report

The Psychometric Properties of the Smartphone Application-Based Addiction Scale (SABAS)

verfasst von: Sándor Csibi, Mark D. Griffiths, Brian Cook, Zsolt Demetrovics, Attila Szabo

Erschienen in: International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction | Ausgabe 2/2018

Abstract

The goal of the study was to validate the English version of the Smartphone Application-Based Addiction Scale (SABAS; Csibi et al. 2016), which is a short and easy-to-use tool for screening the risk of smartphone application-based addiction. Another aim was to identify the most frequently used smartphone applications and their perceived importance by the participants. Data were collected online from 240 English-speaking volunteers, aged 18 to 69 years. The instruments used were the SABAS, the Nomophobia Questionnaire (NMP-Q), the Brief Sensation Seeking Scale (BSSS), the Deprivation Sensation Scale (DSS), and the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9). Participants also ranked the importance of their most frequently used smartphone applications. The six items of the SABAS yielded one component, which accounted for 52.38% of the total variance. The internal reliability of the scale was good (Cronbach’s alpha 0.81). NMP-Q was a significant predictor of SABAS, explaining 17.6% of the total variance. The regression analysis, with SABAS score as the dependent variable and NMP-Q, DSS, PHQ-9, and BSSS scores as predictors, indicated that approximately 47% of the variance in SABAS was accounted for by the predictors (R 2 = 0.47). The English version of the SABAS appears to be a valid and reliable ultra-brief tool for a quick and easy assessment of smartphone application-based addiction symptoms.
Smartphones allow a permanent online presence for users, and studies have asked the important question concerning which smartphone applications are the most prevalent and engrossing, and the underlying psychological or social mechanisms (Demirci et al. 2015; Jeong et al. 2016; Salehan and Negahban 2013) such as anxiety, depression, or daily dysfunction.
Recent reports on smartphone use have suggested that only a small minority of users display problems including addictive-like symptoms (e.g., Billieux et al. 2015; Elhai et al. 2017a, b; Lopez-Fernandez et al. 2017). A systematic review of 117 papers (Elhai et al. 2017a) concluded that depression severity, anxiety, and stress are associated with problematic smartphone use. Although there are innumerable benefits of smartphones, scholars have discussed several detrimental effects including mental health issues (Elhai et al. 2017a, b), poor physical fitness (Lepp et al. 2015; Rebold et al. 2016), and poor academic performance (David et al. 2015; Lepp et al. 2015) resulting as consequences of excessive use.
The literature describes several commonalities and differences between internet and smartphone misuse, such as having similar symptoms to other forms of addiction (Kuss and Griffiths 2012; Kuss et al. 2014; Lopez-Fernandez 2015). Currently, the terminology describing problematic smartphone use is inconsistent, as evidenced by the use of the various terms including “addiction,” “excessive,” “compulsive,” “compensatory,” and “problematic” (e.g., Kardefelt-Winther 2014a, b; Widyanto and Griffiths 2006). These terms have led to a complex and conceptually composite definition of the behavior that causes functional impairment, lack of control, and/or dysfunctional coping (Long et al. 2016).
Theoretical approaches consider negative/positive reinforcement as a modality of coping, and incentive sensitization as a process of mood enhancement, in time modifying the experiences of “liking” into “wanting” (or needing) a behavior, such as (in the case of smartphone use) checking messages or engaging in social media use (Robinson and Berridge 2008). Scholars have also discussed the role of personality-related variables, such as extraversion, loneliness, anxiety, and impulsivity in the process of ensuring positive emotions and reducing negative ones through smartphone use (Billieux et al. 2015). According to Billieux et al. (2015), the habit of checking notifications may provide such positive emotions through the social reassurance stemming from the behavior of the friends. Authors have described constructs such as the “reassurance seeking” and the “fear of missing out” pathways leading to an excessive use of smartphone including symptoms such as low self-esteem, loneliness, depression, and anxiety (Billieux et al. 2015; Elhai et al. 2016). Other personality-related variables, such as impulsivity and extraversion, drive the individual to sustain constant relationship with others, involving sensation seeking and reward sensitivity in media use and online behavior (Billieux et al. 2015; Hoffner and Lee 2015).
Among media content types, excessive users of social networking, games, and entertainment are more likely to develop addiction symptoms than those who use smartphones for study/work-related purposes. Furthermore, addiction to social media has a significant association with the increase of depressive symptoms and the decrease of psychological well-being (Jeong et al. 2016). Other studies have shown that lower levels of depression are associated with more engagement in social smartphone use (Elhai et al. 2017a, b). However, it is not known whether increased behavioral activation offsets the impact of problematic smartphone use in relation to depression outcomes. According to Billieux et al. (2015), the frequent use of a smartphone cannot necessarily be treated as an addictive behavior.
Researchers suggest that sensation seeking has a genetic basis responsible for dopamine release (Zuckerman and Kuhlman 2000). This personality trait might manifest itself in preference for violent content and multitasking with different types of online media, evidencing the high sensation seekers’ online activity characteristics (Cservenka et al. 2013; Jeong et al. 2016; Kim et al. 2016a, b; Lin et al. 2015; Roser et al. 2016; Velezmoro et al. 2010).
Research has identified a heterogeneous spectrum of problematic use of smart devices for online activities and applications, such as Facebook or other forms of socializing in the virtual world (Billieux et al. 2015; Király et al. 2015; Kuss and Griffiths 2011). Specialists in behavioral addictions highlight the importance of generating valid instruments suitable for the screening and the measurement of technology-related addictive behavior (Kardefelt-Winther 2016; Lopez-Fernandez 2017). The literature features several adaptations and generations of new instruments, such as the adapted Smartphone Addiction Scale-Short Version (SAS-SV) in Spanish and Francophone Belgian countries by Lopez-Fernandez (2017). The factor analyses of both the Spanish and Francophone Belgian versions supported the scale’s unidimensionality that explained 49% and 54% of the variance respectively, good construct validity, and excellent reliability. Other smartphone addiction measures have also demonstrated good reliability, including the Smartphone Addiction Inventory (SPAI) in China (Lin et al. 2014) and the Smartphone Addiction Proneness Scale (SAPS) in South Korea (Kim et al. 2014). Studies testing the factorial structure of smartphone assessment instruments (e.g., Lin et al. 2014; Lopez-Fernandez 2017; Pavia et al. 2016; Kwon et al. 2013) have identified several common variables acting as risk factors in smartphone addiction, including time distortion in using a smartphone, compulsivity with a negative impact on daily life activities and relationships, symptoms of tolerance, craving, and withdrawal symptoms.
In the current study, the primary aim was to validate an English version of the original Hungarian version of the Smartphone Application-Based Addiction Scale (SABAS; Csibi et al. 2016). In contrast to the existing instruments, the SABAS is a short and easy-to-use tool for screening the risk for smartphone addiction. A secondary aim was analyze the preferences for smartphone-based applications in relation to SABAS and Nomophobia1 Questionnaire (NMP-Q) scores. Finally, the study also aimed to examine the ways in which these choices influence scores reported on deprivation sensation, depression, and sensation seeking variables.

Methods

Participants

The participants comprised 240 volunteers, aged between 18 and 69 years (mean age = 25.4 years), 155 males (64.6%) and 85 females (35.4%). They completed an online questionnaire following recruitment via various internet forums (such as university students’ blogs, CNET, LinkedIn, Facebook), especially those related to mobile application discussion groups. By the nature of the study, self-selection was an a priori delimitation (as well as a later limitation). The research was approved by the Research Ethics Board of a large urban university. All the participants read and agreed to informed consent form prior to taking part in the study.

Instruments

In addition to a brief demographic questionnaire (e.g., gender, age), the questionnaire also included the SABAS (6 items; Csibi et al. 2016; See Appendix 1), the NMP-Q (20 items; Yildirim and Correia 2015), the Brief Sensation Seeking Scale (BSSS, 8 items; Hoyle et al. 2002), the Deprivation Sensation Scale (DSS, 9 items; Robbins and Joseph 1985), and the Patient Health Questionnaire Depression Scale (PHQ-9, 9 items; Kroenke et al. 2001). Participants were also asked about the most frequently used smartphone applications and asking them to rank by their importance (“Please list the three most important and most used smartphone applications by yourself in a ranked order [i.e., first, second, and third most important]ˮ).

Procedure

The responses to the particular question regarding the most often used applications were transformed into quantitative variables according to nine types of applications (apps): (i) traditional mobile communication and phone function, (ii) internet-based communication, (iii) social media, (iv) information, (v) entertainment, (vi) games, (vii) directions and timetable, (viii) lifestyle applications, and (ix) health-related applications. As noted above, the categories were ranked (1, 2, or 3) and scored according to the participant’s responses (scoring 3 if it was the most popular, 2 if it was second, and 1 if it was ranked third).
To fulfill the secondary aim of the study, a two-step cluster analysis of the data was performed. This generated two distinct clusters, the first with 139 participants (57.9%) and the second with 101 participants (42.1%), with the ratio of sizes 1.38. The clusters were generated based on the test scores. Therefore, in the first cluster, the scores were higher on DSS, SABAS, and NMP-Q and lower on PHQ-9 and BSSS scales; in the second, the scores were lower on DSS, SABAS, and NMP-Q and higher on PHQ-9 and BSSS scales (Table 1).
Table 1
The size and the score differences of the applied tests in the two clusters
Cluster
Size
BASS
NMP-Q
DSS
PHQ-9
BSSS
1
57.9% (139)
25.26
93.24
12.77
12.90
25.55
2
42.1% (101)
17.90
64.96
23.61
18.04
26.90
The first cluster had low values on DSS (smartphone use without sensation of deprivation) and high values on SABAS and NMP-Q (showing addiction vulnerability), low scores on depression scale (lack of negative feelings), and moderated values on BSSS. The second cluster had the opposite value on every scale, except the BSSS (which had approximately the same value).
Statistical analysis was performed with PASW 18 (SPSS, Chicago, IL). To explore the underlying factor structure of the SABAS, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was performed on the data. Internal consistence reliability using Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was examined. A correlation coefficient was computed to assess the relationship between the scores of the participants on the SABAS, DSS, NMP-Q, PHQ-9, and BSSS. Two-step cluster analysis was used to distribute the sample according to addiction vulnerability. For the analysis of the differences between the two clusters, ANOVA and Mann-Whitney tests were performed.

Results

Theoretically, the SABAS was expected to assess a single construct (i.e., the level of smartphone addiction). Therefore, the six items of the scale were subjected to factor analyses using the principal component analysis method. Each of the six items of the SABAS was significantly correlated with all the other items in the scale (p < 0.01), supporting factorability. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy was 0.84. The Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (χ 2 (15) = 426.87, p < 0.01). Furthermore, the diagonals of anti-image correlation matrix were all over 0.80 supporting the inclusion of each item in the factor analysis. Finally, the commonalities were mostly above 0.40, further confirming that each item shared some common variance with the other items. Given these indicators, a principal axis factoring was performed with the six items of the SABAS. One factor (i.e., a single component, eigenvalue = 3.14) was identified and accounted for 52.38% of the total variance (see Table 2).
Table 2
Total variance of the SABAS items
Total variance explained
Factor
Initial eigenvalues
Total
% of variance
Cumulative %
1
3.13
52.29
52.29
2
0.76
12.80
65.10
3
0.66
11.02
76.12
4
0.60
10.13
86.25
5
0.44
7.33
93.58
6
0.38
6.42
100.00
Extraction method: principal axis factoring
The internal reliability of the scale was examined using Cronbach’s alpha and demonstrated good reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.81; mean score = 22.16; std. deviation = 5.69). The Cronbach’s alpha value did not change significantly when one item was deleted suggesting good internal consistency. The congruent validity of the test also appeared to be good. The correlation with the 20-item NMP-Q (Yildirim and Correia 2015) was statistically significant (r = 0.63, p < 0.01). A significant (negative) correlation (r = −0.60, p < 0.01) was also found with the nine-item DSS (Robbins and Joseph 1985). The other two scales applied in the present study also showed significant negative correlations with the SABAS (see Table 3).
Table 3
Correlation coefficient between the SABAS and NMP-Q, DSS, PHQ, and BSSS
Correlations
 
PHQ-9
DSS
NMP-Q
SABAS
BSSS
Spearman’s rho
PHQ
Correl. coefficient
1.000
0.388**
−0.347**
−0.349**
0.146*
Sig. (two-tailed)
0.001
0.001
0.001
0.023
N
240
240
240
240
240
DSS
Correl. coefficient
0.388**
1.000
−0.541**
−0.595**
0.018
Sig. (two-tailed)
0.001
0.001
0.001
0.779
N
240
240
240
240
240
NMP-Q
Correl. coefficient
−0.347**
−0.541**
1.000
0.626**
−0.039
Sig. (two-tailed)
0.001
0.001
0.001
0.547
N
240
240
240
240
240
SABAS
Correl. coefficient
−0.349**
−0.595**
0.626**
1.000
−0.130*
Sig. (two-tailed)
0.001
0.001
0.001
0.044
N
240
240
240
240
240
BSSS
Correl. coefficient
0.146*
0.018
−0.039
−0.130*
1.000
Sig. (two-tailed)
0.023
0.779
0.547
0.044
N
240
240
240
240
240
**Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed)
*Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (two-tailed)
The stepwise linear regression analysis, with SABAS score being the dependent variable and NMP-Q, DSS, PHQ, and BSSS being predictors, indicated that NMP-Q score was a significant predictor of SABAS score (b = 0.12, SE = 0.01, p < 0.01). Approximately 47% of the variance in SABAS was accounted for by all the predictors (R 2 = 0.47). Of this, the NMP-Q score explained 17.6% of the variance in SABAS (see Fig. 1).
The mean rank analysis between the two clusters of data (using a Mann-Whitney test) demonstrated significant differences in application use, such as traditional mobile communication, social media, and information gathering (see Table 4).
Table 4
The differences in application preferences between subjects of the two clusters
Application categories
Cluster
Number
Mean rank
Z
Asymp. sig. (two-tailed)
Traditional mobile communication
1
139
128.01
−2.46
0.014
2
101
110.16
Internet-based communication
1
139
114.92
−1.51
0.129
2
101
128.17
Social media
1
139
106.98
−3.68
0.001
2
101
139.11
Information
1
139
128.07
−2.61
0.009
2
101
110.08
Entertainment
1
139
123.45
−1.01
0.313
2
101
116.45
Games
1
139
120.86
−0.19
0.844
2
101
120.00
Directions and timetable
1
139
122.00
−0.90
0.365
2
101
118.44
Lifestyle applications
1
139
121.39
−0.43
0.66
2
101
119.27
Health-related applications
1
139
122.40
−1.43
0.150
2
101
117.88
ANOVA results show statistically significant differences between the test scores and the application categories (Table 5).
Table 5
The differences in application preferences with between subject’s test score
Application categories
SABAS
NMP-Q
DSS
PHQ-9
BSSS
Traditional mobile communication
F
2.82
5.50
2.26
2.06
1.41
Sig.
0.039
0.001
0.081
0.105
0.239
Internet-based communication
F
2.32
1.64
3.40
0.45
1.37
Sig.
0.075
0.180
0.018
0.716
0.250
Social media
F
8.40
5.10
3.35
1.26
0.78
Sig.
0.001
0.002
0.020
0.287
0.505
Information
F
4.19
3.08
1.65
1.02
0.92
Sig.
0.006
0.028
0.177
0.384
0.432
Entertainment
F
1.17
1.40
2.12
1.34
0.03
Sig.
0.319
0.241
0.097
0.262
0.993
Games
F
0.30
0.66
0.67
0.21
0.96
Sig.
0.822
0.576
0.570
0.889
0.409
Directions and timetable
F
0.39
0.31
0.40
0.31
0.87
Sig.
0.756
0.812
0.752
0.815
0.453
Lifestyle applications
F
1.08
0.18
1.66
0.87
1.02
Sig.
0.355
0.905
0.175
0.457
0.383
Health-related applications
F
1.26
0.40
0.22
0.46
0.61
Sig.
0.287
0.748
0.878
0.705
0.605

Discussion

In the present study, the psychometric properties of an English language version of a short form of smartphone addiction scale were assessed. The factor analysis of the six SABAS items identified one factor (i.e., a single component). By analyzing the psychometric characteristics of the SABAS, the analysis demonstrated good internal reliability and consistency of the scale. Congruent validity was also confirmed via its significant correlation with the NMP-Q and DSS. These results are in agreement with other scale validating studies regarding smartphone-related addiction supporting a one-factor construct alongside good reliability and validity.
The regression analysis of the data demonstrated that nomophobia, deprivation, depression, and sensation seeking predicted 47% of the total variance of the SABAS. In contrast to our findings, Jeong et al. (2016) found that sensation seeking was not a significant predictor of smartphone addiction. A possible explanation provided by other authors for the aforementioned association is that addictive symptoms among healthy populations do not reach pathological levels and that the patterns of addictive behaviors are not necessarily directly related to psychological constructs such as depression or sensation seeking (Billieux et al. 2016; Van Rooij and Prause 2014).
The present study found an inverse relationship between depression and excessive smartphone use, which is in accord with other studies suggesting that the active use of social applications may be beneficial in lowering depressive symptoms (Verduyn et al. 2017; Elhai et al. 2017a, b). A possible explanation for these results might be that depressive symptoms are not necessarily associated with social withdrawal and general avoidance of social interaction. Alternatively, smartphone use may contribute to increased social engagement and decrease feelings of loneliness and isolation (Kim et al. 2016a, b). Our results are in agreement with those of a representative Hungarian population of 5961 adolescents, which reported 4.5% at risk for social media use addiction, and association with low self-esteem, and high level of depression symptoms (Banyai et al. 2017).
The present study demonstrated significant differences in application use, such as traditional mobile communication, social media, and information gathering. According to the results, it can be argued that mobile communication, social media, and information gathering were the most relevant smartphone applications in influencing the scores obtained on the SABAS, NMP-Q, DSS, PHQ, and BSSS. The results here are in line with previous research, which found that social networking was one of the most popular online activities, followed by e-mail, chat, and videos and movies (Griffiths and Szabo 2014).
A limitation of the present study is that the research was conducted on a small healthy volunteer sample, rather than on a clinical one. Therefore, it is difficult to document the presence of other psychological deficiencies present in the participants’ lives. Furthermore, the analysis utilized self-reported cross-sectional data from a self-selected sample of participants, which might lead to distortions in results. The present authors cannot argue for a determinant category of symptoms manifesting along the excessive smartphone use. However, the study supports the existence of smartphone use-related addictive symptoms. Finally, the study does not provide any information regarding the harm and persistency of the problems assumed to be assessed through the new instrument. The study of the associations with other personality-related variables is recommended in further research.

Conclusion

By validating an English version of the Hungarian version of the SABAS (Csibi et al. 2016), we provided a short, easy-to-use, and easy-to-score tool for screening the risk of smartphone addiction, with the potential of exceeding cultural boundaries. The study adds some empirical data concerning the possible core determinants of smartphone application-related addictive behavior without establishing a clear causality in the effects. The most highly ranked preferences for smartphone-based applications were communication, social media, and information gathering in the sample with relevant impact on SABAS and NMP-Q scores. Significant associations were found between these choices and scores reported on deprivation sensations, nomophobia, and SABAS, alongside a negative significant correlation with depression and sensation seeking variables.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Financial Support

Present work was supported by the Hungarian Scientific Research Fund (grant numbers 111938 and K109375).

Ethical Clearance

Ethical permission for the study was obtained from the Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Education and Psychology at Eötvös Loránd University.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Jetzt e.Med zum Sonderpreis bestellen!

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Jetzt bestellen und 100 € sparen!

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

e.Med Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Psychiatrie, den Premium-Inhalten der psychiatrischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Anhänge

Appendix 1

Smartphone Application-Based Addiction Scale (SABAS)
Please indicate the extent to which you agree or disagree with the statements below in relation to your smartphone use habits.
 
Strongly Disagree
Disagree
Slightly Disagree
Slightly Agree
Agree
Strongly Agree
My smartphone is the most important thing in my life.
1
2
3
4
5
6
Conflicts have arisen between me and my family (or friends) because of my smartphone use.
1
2
3
4
5
6
Preoccupying myself with my smartphone is a way of changing my mood (I get a buzz, or I can escape or get away, if I need to).
1
2
3
4
5
6
Over time, I fiddle around more and more with my smartphone.
1
2
3
4
5
6
If I cannot use or access my smartphone when I feel like, I feel sad, moody, or irritable.
1
2
3
4
5
6
If I try to cut the time I use my smartphone, I manage to do so for a while, but then I end up using it as much or more than before.
1
2
3
4
5
6
The total score is calculated by adding the individual scores (1-6) of each item (providing a minimum score of 6, and a maximum score of 36)
Fußnoten
1
Nomophobia: a specific phobia caused by the anxiety of the lack of availability of a smartphone or other communication device (after Yildirim and Correia 2015).
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Banyai, F., Zsila, A., Kiraly, O., Maraz, A., Elekes, Z., Griffiths, M. D., et al. (2017). Problematic social media use: results from a large-scale nationally representative adolescent sample. PloS ONE, 12(1). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169839. Banyai, F., Zsila, A., Kiraly, O., Maraz, A., Elekes, Z., Griffiths, M. D., et al. (2017). Problematic social media use: results from a large-scale nationally representative adolescent sample. PloS ONE, 12(1). doi:10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0169839.
Zurück zum Zitat Billieux, J., Maurage, P., Lopez-Fernandez, O., Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2015). Can disordered mobile phone use be considered a behavioral addiction? An update on current evidence and a comprehensive model for future research. Current Addiction Reports, 2(2), 156–162. doi:10.1007/s40429-015-0054-y.CrossRef Billieux, J., Maurage, P., Lopez-Fernandez, O., Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2015). Can disordered mobile phone use be considered a behavioral addiction? An update on current evidence and a comprehensive model for future research. Current Addiction Reports, 2(2), 156–162. doi:10.​1007/​s40429-015-0054-y.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Billieux, J., Blaszczynski, A., Colder Carras, M., Edman, J., Heeren, A., Kardefelt-Winther, D., et al. (2016). Behavioral Addiction: Open definition development. doi:10.17605/OSF.IO/Q2VVA. Billieux, J., Blaszczynski, A., Colder Carras, M., Edman, J., Heeren, A., Kardefelt-Winther, D., et al. (2016). Behavioral Addiction: Open definition development. doi:10.​17605/​OSF.​IO/​Q2VVA.
Zurück zum Zitat Csibi, S., Demetrovics, Z., & Szabo, A. (2016). Hungarian adaptation and psychometric characteristics of Brief Addiction to Smartphone Scale (BASS) [In Hungarian]. Psychiatria Hungarica, 31(1), 71–77. Csibi, S., Demetrovics, Z., & Szabo, A. (2016). Hungarian adaptation and psychometric characteristics of Brief Addiction to Smartphone Scale (BASS) [In Hungarian]. Psychiatria Hungarica, 31(1), 71–77.
Zurück zum Zitat Demirci, K., Akgonul, M., & Akpinar, A. (2015). Relationship of smartphone use severity with sleep quality, depression, and anxiety in university students. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4(2), 85–92.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Demirci, K., Akgonul, M., & Akpinar, A. (2015). Relationship of smartphone use severity with sleep quality, depression, and anxiety in university students. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4(2), 85–92.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Dvorak, R. D., & Hall, B. J. (2016). Fear of missing out, need for touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior, 63, 509–516. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.079.CrossRef Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Dvorak, R. D., & Hall, B. J. (2016). Fear of missing out, need for touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior, 63, 509–516. doi:10.​1016/​j.​chb.​2016.​05.​079.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Elhai, J. D., Dvorak, R. D., Levine, J. C., & Hall, B. J. (2017a). Problematic smartphone use: a conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 207, 251–259.CrossRefPubMed Elhai, J. D., Dvorak, R. D., Levine, J. C., & Hall, B. J. (2017a). Problematic smartphone use: a conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 207, 251–259.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Elhai, J., Levine, J., Dvorak, R., & Hall, B. (2017b). Non-social features of smartphone use are most related to depression, anxiety and problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior, 69, 75–82. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.12.023.CrossRef Elhai, J., Levine, J., Dvorak, R., & Hall, B. (2017b). Non-social features of smartphone use are most related to depression, anxiety and problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior, 69, 75–82. doi:10.​1016/​j.​chb.​2016.​12.​023.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Griffiths, M. D., & Szabo, A. (2014). Is excessive online usage a function of medium or activity? An empirical pilot study. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 3, 74–77.CrossRefPubMed Griffiths, M. D., & Szabo, A. (2014). Is excessive online usage a function of medium or activity? An empirical pilot study. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 3, 74–77.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Hoyle, R. H., Stephenson, M. T., Palmgreen, P., Lorch, E. P., & Donohew, R. L. (2002). Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(3), 401–414. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00032-0 Hoyle, R. H., Stephenson, M. T., Palmgreen, P., Lorch, E. P., & Donohew, R. L. (2002). Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(3), 401–414. doi:10.​1016/​S0191-8869(01)00032-0
Zurück zum Zitat Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2016). Conceptualizing internet use disorders: addiction or coping process?Conceptualizing internet use disorders. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. doi:10.1111/pcn.12413. Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2016). Conceptualizing internet use disorders: addiction or coping process?Conceptualizing internet use disorders. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. doi:10.​1111/​pcn.​12413.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim, Y., Jeong, J., Cho, H., Jung, D., Kwak, M., Rho, M. J., et al. (2016a). Personality factors predicting smartphone addiction predisposition: behavioral inhibition and activation systems, impulsivity, and self-control. PloS ONE, 11(8). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0159788. Kim, Y., Jeong, J., Cho, H., Jung, D., Kwak, M., Rho, M. J., et al. (2016a). Personality factors predicting smartphone addiction predisposition: behavioral inhibition and activation systems, impulsivity, and self-control. PloS ONE, 11(8). doi:10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0159788.
Zurück zum Zitat Kim, Y., Wang, Y., & Oh, J. (2016b). Digital media use and social engagement: how social media and smartphone use influence social activities of college students. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 19, 264–269.CrossRef Kim, Y., Wang, Y., & Oh, J. (2016b). Digital media use and social engagement: how social media and smartphone use influence social activities of college students. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 19, 264–269.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Király, O., Griffiths, M. D., & Demetrovics, Z. (2015). Internet gaming disorder and the DSM-5: conceptualization, debates, and controversies. Current Addiction Reports, 2, 254–262. doi:10.1007/s40429-015-0066-7.CrossRef Király, O., Griffiths, M. D., & Demetrovics, Z. (2015). Internet gaming disorder and the DSM-5: conceptualization, debates, and controversies. Current Addiction Reports, 2, 254–262. doi:10.​1007/​s40429-015-0066-7.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kuss, D. J., Shorter, G. W., van Rooij, A. J., Griffiths, M. D., & Schoenmakers, T. M. (2014). Assessing internet addiction using the parsimonious internet addiction components model—a preliminary study. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 12(3), 351–366. doi:10.1007/s11469-013-9459-9. Kuss, D. J., Shorter, G. W., van Rooij, A. J., Griffiths, M. D., & Schoenmakers, T. M. (2014). Assessing internet addiction using the parsimonious internet addiction components model—a preliminary study. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 12(3), 351–366. doi:10.​1007/​s11469-013-9459-9.
Zurück zum Zitat Kwon, M., Lee, J. Y., Won, W. Y., Park, J. W., Min, J. A., Hahn, C., et al. (2013). Development and validation of a smartphone addiction scale (SAS). PloS One, 8(2), e56936.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kwon, M., Lee, J. Y., Won, W. Y., Park, J. W., Min, J. A., Hahn, C., et al. (2013). Development and validation of a smartphone addiction scale (SAS). PloS One, 8(2), e56936.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Lepp, A., Barkley, J. E., & Karpinski, A. C. (2015). The relationship between cell phone use and academic performance in a sample of U.S. college students. SAGE Open, 5(1), 1–9. doi:10.1177/2158244015573169.CrossRef Lepp, A., Barkley, J. E., & Karpinski, A. C. (2015). The relationship between cell phone use and academic performance in a sample of U.S. college students. SAGE Open, 5(1), 1–9. doi:10.​1177/​2158244015573169​.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lin, Y. H., Chang, L. R., Lee, Y. H., Tseng, H. W., Kuo, T. B. J., & Chen, S. H. (2014). Development and validation of the Smartphone Addiction Inventory (SPAI). PloS ONE, 9, e98312.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Lin, Y. H., Chang, L. R., Lee, Y. H., Tseng, H. W., Kuo, T. B. J., & Chen, S. H. (2014). Development and validation of the Smartphone Addiction Inventory (SPAI). PloS ONE, 9, e98312.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Zurück zum Zitat Lopez-Fernandez, O. (2015). How has internet addiction research evolved since the advent of internet gaming disorder? An overview of cyberaddictions from a psychological perspective. Current Addiction Reports, 2, 263–271. doi:10.1007/s40429-015-0067-6.CrossRef Lopez-Fernandez, O. (2015). How has internet addiction research evolved since the advent of internet gaming disorder? An overview of cyberaddictions from a psychological perspective. Current Addiction Reports, 2, 263–271. doi:10.​1007/​s40429-015-0067-6.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Lopez-Fernandez, O., Kuss, D. J., Romo, L., Morvan, Y., Kern, L., Graziani, P., & Schimmenti, A. (2017). Self-reported dependence on mobile phones in young adults: A European cross-cultural empirical survey. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6, 168–177. Lopez-Fernandez, O., Kuss, D. J., Romo, L., Morvan, Y., Kern, L., Graziani, P., & Schimmenti, A. (2017). Self-reported dependence on mobile phones in young adults: A European cross-cultural empirical survey. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6, 168–177.
Zurück zum Zitat Pavia, L., Cavani, P., Di Blasi, M., & Giordano, C. (2016). Smartphone addiction inventory (SPAI): Psychometric properties and confirmatory factor analysis. Computers in Human Behavior, 63, 170–178. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.039 Pavia, L., Cavani, P., Di Blasi, M., & Giordano, C. (2016). Smartphone addiction inventory (SPAI): Psychometric properties and confirmatory factor analysis. Computers in Human Behavior, 63, 170–178. doi:10.​1016/​j.​chb.​2016.​05.​039
Zurück zum Zitat Rebold, M., Sheehan, T., Dirlam, M., Maldonado, T., & O’Donnell, D. (2016). The impact of cell phone texting on the amount of time spent exercising at different intensities. Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 167–171. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.09.005.CrossRef Rebold, M., Sheehan, T., Dirlam, M., Maldonado, T., & O’Donnell, D. (2016). The impact of cell phone texting on the amount of time spent exercising at different intensities. Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 167–171. doi:10.​1016/​j.​chb.​2015.​09.​005.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Robbins, J.M., & Joseph, P. (1985). Experiencing Exercise Withdrawal: Possible Consequences of Therapeutic and Mastery Running. Journal of Sport Psychology, 7, 23–39. Robbins, J.M., & Joseph, P. (1985). Experiencing Exercise Withdrawal: Possible Consequences of Therapeutic and Mastery Running. Journal of Sport Psychology, 7, 23–39.
Zurück zum Zitat Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2008). The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: some current issues. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1507), 3137–3146. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0093.CrossRef Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2008). The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: some current issues. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1507), 3137–3146. doi:10.​1098/​rstb.​2008.​0093.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Roser, K., Schoeni, A., Foerster, M., & Röösli, M. (2016). Problematic mobile phone use of swiss adolescents: is it linked with mental health or behaviour? International Journal of Public Health, 61(3), 307–315. doi:10.1007/s00038-015-0751-2.CrossRefPubMed Roser, K., Schoeni, A., Foerster, M., & Röösli, M. (2016). Problematic mobile phone use of swiss adolescents: is it linked with mental health or behaviour? International Journal of Public Health, 61(3), 307–315. doi:10.​1007/​s00038-015-0751-2.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Szabo, A., Csibi, S., & Demetrovics, Z. (2017). Development and validation of a smartphone deprivation scale (SDS) for use with schoolchildren. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6, 53–53. Szabo, A., Csibi, S., & Demetrovics, Z. (2017). Development and validation of a smartphone deprivation scale (SDS) for use with schoolchildren. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6, 53–53.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Rooij, A. J., & Prause, N. (2014). A critical review of ‘internet addiction’ criteria with suggestions for the future. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 3, 203–213.CrossRefPubMed Van Rooij, A. J., & Prause, N. (2014). A critical review of ‘internet addiction’ criteria with suggestions for the future. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 3, 203–213.CrossRefPubMed
Zurück zum Zitat Verduyn, P., Ybarra, O., Résibois, M., Jonides, J., & Kross, E. (2017). Do social network sites enhance or undermine subjective wellbeing? A critical review. Social Issues and Policy Review, 11(1), 274–302. doi:10.1111/sipr.12033.CrossRef Verduyn, P., Ybarra, O., Résibois, M., Jonides, J., & Kross, E. (2017). Do social network sites enhance or undermine subjective wellbeing? A critical review. Social Issues and Policy Review, 11(1), 274–302. doi:10.​1111/​sipr.​12033.CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zuckerman, M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). Personality and risk-taking: common biosocial factors. Journal of Personality, 68, 999–1029.CrossRefPubMed Zuckerman, M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). Personality and risk-taking: common biosocial factors. Journal of Personality, 68, 999–1029.CrossRefPubMed
Metadaten
Titel
The Psychometric Properties of the Smartphone Application-Based Addiction Scale (SABAS)
verfasst von
Sándor Csibi
Mark D. Griffiths
Brian Cook
Zsolt Demetrovics
Attila Szabo
Publikationsdatum
18.07.2017
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction / Ausgabe 2/2018
Print ISSN: 1557-1874
Elektronische ISSN: 1557-1882
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-017-9787-2

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 2/2018

International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction 2/2018 Zur Ausgabe

Hörschwäche erhöht Demenzrisiko unabhängig von Beta-Amyloid

29.05.2024 Hörstörungen Nachrichten

Hört jemand im Alter schlecht, nimmt das Hirn- und Hippocampusvolumen besonders schnell ab, was auch mit einem beschleunigten kognitiven Abbau einhergeht. Und diese Prozesse scheinen sich unabhängig von der Amyloidablagerung zu ereignen.

So wirken verschiedene Alkoholika auf den Blutdruck

23.05.2024 Störungen durch Alkohol Nachrichten

Je mehr Alkohol Menschen pro Woche trinken, desto mehr steigt ihr Blutdruck, legen Daten aus Dänemark nahe. Ob es dabei auch auf die Art des Alkohols ankommt, wurde ebenfalls untersucht.

„Übersichtlicher Wegweiser“: Lauterbachs umstrittener Klinik-Atlas ist online

17.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Sie sei „ethisch geboten“, meint Gesundheitsminister Karl Lauterbach: mehr Transparenz über die Qualität von Klinikbehandlungen. Um sie abzubilden, lässt er gegen den Widerstand vieler Länder einen virtuellen Klinik-Atlas freischalten.

ADHS-Medikation erhöht das kardiovaskuläre Risiko

16.05.2024 Herzinsuffizienz Nachrichten

Erwachsene, die Medikamente gegen das Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-Hyperaktivitätssyndrom einnehmen, laufen offenbar erhöhte Gefahr, an Herzschwäche zu erkranken oder einen Schlaganfall zu erleiden. Es scheint eine Dosis-Wirkungs-Beziehung zu bestehen.

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.