Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Annals of Behavioral Medicine 5/2016

09.03.2016 | Original Article

The Tripartite Model of Risk Perception (TRIRISK): Distinguishing Deliberative, Affective, and Experiential Components of Perceived Risk

verfasst von: Rebecca A. Ferrer, PhD, William M. P. Klein, PhD, Alexander Persoskie, PhD, Aya Avishai-Yitshak, BS, Paschal Sheeran, PhD

Erschienen in: Annals of Behavioral Medicine | Ausgabe 5/2016

Einloggen, um Zugang zu erhalten

Abstract

Background

Although risk perception is a key predictor in health behavior theories, current conceptions of risk comprise only one (deliberative) or two (deliberative vs. affective/experiential) dimensions.

Purpose

This research tested a tripartite model that distinguishes among deliberative, affective, and experiential components of risk perception.

Method

In two studies, and in relation to three common diseases (cancer, heart disease, diabetes), we used confirmatory factor analyses to examine the factor structure of the tripartite risk perception (TRIRISK) model and compared the fit of the TRIRISK model to dual-factor and single-factor models. In a third study, we assessed concurrent validity by examining the impact of cancer diagnosis on (a) levels of deliberative, affective, and experiential risk perception, and (b) the strength of relations among risk components, and tested predictive validity by assessing relations with behavioral intentions to prevent cancer.

Results

The tripartite factor structure was supported, producing better model fit across diseases (studies 1 and 2). Inter-correlations among the components were significantly smaller among participants who had been diagnosed with cancer, suggesting that affected populations make finer-grained distinctions among risk perceptions (study 3). Moreover, all three risk perception components predicted unique variance in intentions to engage in preventive behavior (study 3).

Conclusions

The TRIRISK model offers both a novel conceptualization of health-related risk perceptions, and new measures that enhance predictive validity beyond that engendered by unidimensional and bidimensional models. The present findings have implications for the ways in which risk perceptions are targeted in health behavior change interventions, health communications, and decision aids.
Fußnoten
1
The reliabilities and ranges for the TRIRISK scales were similar for the cancer vs. no cancer groups. Among participants with no diagnosis of cancer, the standardized (z-score) deliberative scale (α = 0.917) has a range of −2.26–2.38, and skewness of −0.054 (SE = 0.193);, the standardized (z-score) affective scale (α = 0.972) has a range of −1.88–1.51, and skewness of −0.080 (SE = 0.193); and, the standardized (z-score) experiential scale (α = 0.873) has a range of −2.77–1.62, and skewness of −0.353 (SE = 0.193). Among those with cancer, the standardized (z-score) deliberative scale (α = 0.832) has a range of −2.23–2.29, and skewness of −0.248 (SE = 0.203); the standardized (z-score) affective scale (α = 0.967) has a range of −1.88–1.51, and skewness of −0.469 (SE = 0.203); and the standardized (z-score) experiential scale (α = 0.841) has a range of −2.77–1.62, and skewness of −0.716 (SE = 0.203). To test the scale distributions for the cancer vs. no cancer groups, we divided skewness values by their standard errors. Only two values exceeded the threshold of 1.96, namely, the affective and experiential scales for participants who had been diagnosed with cancer. We therefore transformed these variables which reduced skewness to −0.266 and −0.216, SE = 0.203 and 0.203, respectively [52]. We then recomputed the correlations; r values were virtually unchanged.
 
2
We also tested whether the three risk components statistically mediated the impact of cancer diagnosis (yes, no) on intention using the Preacher and Hayes SPSS Macro for Multiple Mediation with 5000 as the number of bootstrap resamples [53]. Diagnosis reliably predicted deliberative, affective, and experiential risk perceptions (B = 0.44, 0.42, and 0.51, SE = 0.11, 0.11. and 0.11, ps < 0.001) and deliberative, affective, and experiential risk perceptions reliably predicted intention (B = −0.76, 0.48, and 0.34, SE = 0.09, 0.10, and 0.11, ps < 0.001, < 0.001, and 0.003). The direct effect of diagnosis on intention remained significant controlling for risk perceptions (B = 0.36, SE = 0.14, p = 0.013). Importantly, however, the indirect effects of deliberative, affective, and experiential risk perceptions each proved reliable (estimates = −0.34, 0.20, and 0.17; 95 % CI = −0.55 to −0.17, 0.09 to 0.36, and 0.06 to 0.35, respectively). These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the three components of the TRIRISK model mediate the impact of cancer diagnosis on cancer-protective intentions.
 
3
Because (a) some of the deliberative risk perceptions could be seen as conditional on current behavior (e.g., “The way I look after my health means that my odds of getting cancer in the future are . . .”), and (b) conditional risk perceptions can be a strong predictor of future behavior, we examined correlations between each individual deliberative risk perception item and intentions in the full sample to see if items performed similarly. Items 3, 4, and 5 each had a statistically significant negative correlation with intentions (rs = −0.254, −0.183, −0.264, respectively; all ps < 0.001). However, item 2 also had a marginally significant, negative association with intentions (r = −0.108, p = 0.062); the other items had non-significant correlations with intentions that were mostly negative. Thus, while conditional items have some role in explaining the negative correlation between deliberative risk perception and intention, this does not entirely explain the negative correlation between deliberative risk and intention.
 
4
We also tested the interactions between cancer diagnosis and risk components but none of the interaction terms proved reliable. Deliberative, affective, and experiential risk perceptions were equally powerful and reliable predictors of intention for participants with and without a diagnosis of cancer.
 
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Becker MH. The health belief model and sick role behavior. Health EducMonogr. 1974; 2: 409-419. Becker MH. The health belief model and sick role behavior. Health EducMonogr. 1974; 2: 409-419.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Rogers RW. A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change. J Psychol: Interdiscy Appl. 1975; 91: 93-114.CrossRef Rogers RW. A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change. J Psychol: Interdiscy Appl. 1975; 91: 93-114.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Rosenstock IM. Historical origins of the health belief model. Health Educ Behav. 1974; 2: 328-335.CrossRef Rosenstock IM. Historical origins of the health belief model. Health Educ Behav. 1974; 2: 328-335.CrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Shafir E, Simonson I, Tversky A. Reason-based choice. Cogn. 1993; 49: 11-36.CrossRef Shafir E, Simonson I, Tversky A. Reason-based choice. Cogn. 1993; 49: 11-36.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Tversky A, Kahneman D. Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychol Rev. 1983; 90: 293.CrossRef Tversky A, Kahneman D. Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychol Rev. 1983; 90: 293.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Damasio A: Descartes’ error: Emotion, rationality, and the human brain. New York, NY: Putnam, 1994/2003. Damasio A: Descartes’ error: Emotion, rationality, and the human brain. New York, NY: Putnam, 1994/2003.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Denes-Raj V, Epstein S. Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: When people behave against their better judgment. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994; 66: 819-829.CrossRefPubMed Denes-Raj V, Epstein S. Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: When people behave against their better judgment. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994; 66: 819-829.CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat von Neumann J, Morgenstern O. Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1974. von Neumann J, Morgenstern O. Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1974.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Loewenstein G, Weber EU, Hsee CK, Welch N. Risk as feelings. Psychol Bull. 2001; 127: 267-286.CrossRefPubMed Loewenstein G, Weber EU, Hsee CK, Welch N. Risk as feelings. Psychol Bull. 2001; 127: 267-286.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Brewer NT, Chapman GB, Gibbons FX, et al. Meta-analysis of the relationship between risk perception and health behavior: The example of vaccination. Health Psychol. 2007; 26: 136.CrossRefPubMed Brewer NT, Chapman GB, Gibbons FX, et al. Meta-analysis of the relationship between risk perception and health behavior: The example of vaccination. Health Psychol. 2007; 26: 136.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Brewer NT, Weinstein ND, Cuite CL, Herrington JE Jr. Risk perceptions and their relation to risk behavior. Ann Behav Med. 2004; 27: 125-130.CrossRefPubMed Brewer NT, Weinstein ND, Cuite CL, Herrington JE Jr. Risk perceptions and their relation to risk behavior. Ann Behav Med. 2004; 27: 125-130.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Sheeran P, Harris PR, Epton T. Does heightening risk appraisals change people’s intentions and behavior?A meta-analysis of experimental studies. Psychol Bull. 2014; 140: 511.CrossRefPubMed Sheeran P, Harris PR, Epton T. Does heightening risk appraisals change people’s intentions and behavior?A meta-analysis of experimental studies. Psychol Bull. 2014; 140: 511.CrossRefPubMed
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Anal. 2004; 24: 311-322.CrossRefPubMed Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Anal. 2004; 24: 311-322.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Leventhal H, Meyer D, Merenz D. The common sense representation of illness danger. Contrib Med Psychol. 1980; 2: 7-30. Leventhal H, Meyer D, Merenz D. The common sense representation of illness danger. Contrib Med Psychol. 1980; 2: 7-30.
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Leventhal H. Findings and theory in the study of fear communications. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 1970; 5: 119-186.CrossRef Leventhal H. Findings and theory in the study of fear communications. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 1970; 5: 119-186.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Dillard AJ, Ferrer RA, Ubel PA, Fagerlin A. Risk perception measures’ associations with behavior intentions, affect, and cognition following colon cancer screening messages. Health Psychol. 2012; 31: 106.CrossRefPubMed Dillard AJ, Ferrer RA, Ubel PA, Fagerlin A. Risk perception measures’ associations with behavior intentions, affect, and cognition following colon cancer screening messages. Health Psychol. 2012; 31: 106.CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Ferrer RA, Hall KL, Portnoy DB, et al. Relationships among health perceptions vary depending on stage of readiness for colorectal cancer screening. Health Psychol. 2011; 30: 525-535.CrossRefPubMed Ferrer RA, Hall KL, Portnoy DB, et al. Relationships among health perceptions vary depending on stage of readiness for colorectal cancer screening. Health Psychol. 2011; 30: 525-535.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Lipkus IM, Klein WMP, Skinner CS, Rimer BK. Breast cancer risk perceptions and breast cancer worry: What predicts what? J Risk Res. 2005; 8: 439-452.CrossRef Lipkus IM, Klein WMP, Skinner CS, Rimer BK. Breast cancer risk perceptions and breast cancer worry: What predicts what? J Risk Res. 2005; 8: 439-452.CrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Hay JL, McCaul KD, Magnan RE. Does worry about breast cancer predict screening behaviors? A meta-analysis of the prospective evidence. Prev Med. 2006; 42: 401-408.CrossRefPubMed Hay JL, McCaul KD, Magnan RE. Does worry about breast cancer predict screening behaviors? A meta-analysis of the prospective evidence. Prev Med. 2006; 42: 401-408.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Ferrer RA, Portnoy DB, Klein WM. Worry and risk perceptions as independent and interacting predictors of health protective behaviors. J Health Commun. 2013; 18: 397-409.CrossRefPubMed Ferrer RA, Portnoy DB, Klein WM. Worry and risk perceptions as independent and interacting predictors of health protective behaviors. J Health Commun. 2013; 18: 397-409.CrossRefPubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Sinclair M, Ashkanasy NM, Chattopadhyay P. Affective antecedents of intuitive decision making. J Mant Organ. 2010; 16: 382-398.CrossRef Sinclair M, Ashkanasy NM, Chattopadhyay P. Affective antecedents of intuitive decision making. J Mant Organ. 2010; 16: 382-398.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Windschitl PD. Judging the accuracy of a likelihood judgment: The case of smoking risk. J Behav Decis Mak. 2002; 15: 19-35.CrossRef Windschitl PD. Judging the accuracy of a likelihood judgment: The case of smoking risk. J Behav Decis Mak. 2002; 15: 19-35.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Weinstein ND, Kwitel A, McCaul KD, et al. Risk perceptions: Assessment and relationship to influenza vaccination. Health Psycholy. 2007; 26: 146-151.CrossRef Weinstein ND, Kwitel A, McCaul KD, et al. Risk perceptions: Assessment and relationship to influenza vaccination. Health Psycholy. 2007; 26: 146-151.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Mills B, Reyna VF, Estrada S. Explaining contradictory relations between risk perception and risk taking. Psychol Science. 2008; 19: 429-433.CrossRef Mills B, Reyna VF, Estrada S. Explaining contradictory relations between risk perception and risk taking. Psychol Science. 2008; 19: 429-433.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Fisher JD, Williams SS, Fisher WA, Malloy TE. Understanding AIDS risk behavior among sexually active urban adolescents: An empirical test of the information–motivation–behavioral skills model. AIDS Behav. 1999; 13: 13-23.CrossRef Fisher JD, Williams SS, Fisher WA, Malloy TE. Understanding AIDS risk behavior among sexually active urban adolescents: An empirical test of the information–motivation–behavioral skills model. AIDS Behav. 1999; 13: 13-23.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Janssen E, van Osch L, Lechner L, Candel M, de Vries H. Thinking versus feeling: differentiating between cognitive and affective components of perceived cancer risk. Psychol Health. 2012; 27: 767-783.CrossRefPubMed Janssen E, van Osch L, Lechner L, Candel M, de Vries H. Thinking versus feeling: differentiating between cognitive and affective components of perceived cancer risk. Psychol Health. 2012; 27: 767-783.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Gerrard M, Gibbons FX, Houlihan AE, Stock ML, Pomery EA. A dual-process approach to health risk decision making: The prototype willingness model. Dev Rev. 2008; 28: 29-61.CrossRef Gerrard M, Gibbons FX, Houlihan AE, Stock ML, Pomery EA. A dual-process approach to health risk decision making: The prototype willingness model. Dev Rev. 2008; 28: 29-61.CrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Portnoy DB, Ferrer RA, Bergman HE, Klein WM: Changing deliberative and affective responses to health risk: A meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. 2013, ePub ahead of print. Portnoy DB, Ferrer RA, Bergman HE, Klein WM: Changing deliberative and affective responses to health risk: A meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. 2013, ePub ahead of print.
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Janssen E, van Osch L, de Vries H, Lechner L. Measuring risk perceptions of skin cancer: Reliability and validity of different operationalizations. Brit J Health Psychol. 2011; 16: 92-112.CrossRef Janssen E, van Osch L, de Vries H, Lechner L. Measuring risk perceptions of skin cancer: Reliability and validity of different operationalizations. Brit J Health Psychol. 2011; 16: 92-112.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Janssen E, Waters EA, van Osch L, Lechner L, de Vries H. The importance of affectively-laden beliefs about health risks: the case of tobacco use and sun protection. J Behav Med. 2014; 37: 11-21.CrossRefPubMed Janssen E, Waters EA, van Osch L, Lechner L, de Vries H. The importance of affectively-laden beliefs about health risks: the case of tobacco use and sun protection. J Behav Med. 2014; 37: 11-21.CrossRefPubMed
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Klein WMP, Harris PR, Ferrer RA, Zajac LE. Feelings of vulnerability in response to threatening messages: Effects of self-affirmation. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2011; 46: 1237-1242.CrossRef Klein WMP, Harris PR, Ferrer RA, Zajac LE. Feelings of vulnerability in response to threatening messages: Effects of self-affirmation. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2011; 46: 1237-1242.CrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Crites SL, Fabrigar LR, Petty RE. Measuring the affective and cognitive properties of attitudes: Conceptual and methodological issues. Persy Soc Psychol Bull. 1994; 20: 619-634.CrossRef Crites SL, Fabrigar LR, Petty RE. Measuring the affective and cognitive properties of attitudes: Conceptual and methodological issues. Persy Soc Psychol Bull. 1994; 20: 619-634.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Keer M, van den Putte B, Neijens P. The role of affect and cognition in health decision making. Brit J Soc Psychol. 2010; 49: 143-153.CrossRef Keer M, van den Putte B, Neijens P. The role of affect and cognition in health decision making. Brit J Soc Psychol. 2010; 49: 143-153.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Meyers-Levy J, Malaviya P. Consumers’ processing of persuasive advertisements: An integrative framework of persuasion theories. J Mark. 1999; 63: 45-60.CrossRef Meyers-Levy J, Malaviya P. Consumers’ processing of persuasive advertisements: An integrative framework of persuasion theories. J Mark. 1999; 63: 45-60.CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Tramifow D, Sheeran P. Some tests of the distinction between cognitive and affective beliefs. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1998; 34: 378-397.CrossRef Tramifow D, Sheeran P. Some tests of the distinction between cognitive and affective beliefs. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1998; 34: 378-397.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat van den Berg H, Manstead ASR, van der Pligt J. J. WDH: The role of affect in attitudes toward organ donation and donor-relevant decisions. Psychol Health. 2005; 20: 789-802.CrossRef van den Berg H, Manstead ASR, van der Pligt J. J. WDH: The role of affect in attitudes toward organ donation and donor-relevant decisions. Psychol Health. 2005; 20: 789-802.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Peer E, Vosgerau J, Acquisti A: Reputation as a sufficient condition for data quality on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Behav Res Methods. 2-14, ePub ahead of print. Peer E, Vosgerau J, Acquisti A: Reputation as a sufficient condition for data quality on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Behav Res Methods. 2-14, ePub ahead of print.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011; 6: 3-5.CrossRefPubMed Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011; 6: 3-5.CrossRefPubMed
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Paolacci G, Chandler J. Inside the Turk understanding Mechanical Turk as a participant pool. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014; 23: 184-188.CrossRef Paolacci G, Chandler J. Inside the Turk understanding Mechanical Turk as a participant pool. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014; 23: 184-188.CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat HINTS: Health Information National Trends Survey database, National Cancer Institute. Retrieved Retrieved from hints.cancer.gov, November 17, 2014, HINTS: Health Information National Trends Survey database, National Cancer Institute. Retrieved Retrieved from hints.cancer.gov, November 17, 2014,
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Sheeran P, Orbell S. How confidently can we infer health beliefs from questionnaire responses? Psychol Health. 1996; 11: 273-290.CrossRef Sheeran P, Orbell S. How confidently can we infer health beliefs from questionnaire responses? Psychol Health. 1996; 11: 273-290.CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Appel M, Gnambs T, Maio GR. A short measure of the need for affect. J Pers Assess. 2012; 94(4): 418-426.CrossRefPubMed Appel M, Gnambs T, Maio GR. A short measure of the need for affect. J Pers Assess. 2012; 94(4): 418-426.CrossRefPubMed
49.
Zurück zum Zitat McEachan R, Conner M, Taylor N, Lawton RJ: Prospective prediction of health-related behaviours with the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis supplemental tables. 2011. McEachan R, Conner M, Taylor N, Lawton RJ: Prospective prediction of health-related behaviours with the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis supplemental tables. 2011.
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Sheeran P. Intention—behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. EurJ Soc Psychol. 2002; 12: 1-36.CrossRef Sheeran P. Intention—behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. EurJ Soc Psychol. 2002; 12: 1-36.CrossRef
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Webb TL, Sheeran P. Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol Bull. 2006; 132: 249.CrossRefPubMed Webb TL, Sheeran P. Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol Bull. 2006; 132: 249.CrossRefPubMed
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Bulmer MG. Principles of Statistics. New York: Dover; 1979. Bulmer MG. Principles of Statistics. New York: Dover; 1979.
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Stoeber J, Kobori O, Brown A. Examining mutual suppression effects in the assessment of perfectionism cognitions: Evidence supporting multidimensional assessment. Assessment. 2014; 21(6): 647-660.CrossRefPubMed Stoeber J, Kobori O, Brown A. Examining mutual suppression effects in the assessment of perfectionism cognitions: Evidence supporting multidimensional assessment. Assessment. 2014; 21(6): 647-660.CrossRefPubMed
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods. 2008; 40: 879-891.CrossRefPubMed Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods. 2008; 40: 879-891.CrossRefPubMed
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Rimal RN, Juon HS. Use of the risk perception attitude framework for promoting breast cancer prevention. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2010; 40: 287-310.CrossRef Rimal RN, Juon HS. Use of the risk perception attitude framework for promoting breast cancer prevention. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2010; 40: 287-310.CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Ajzen I, Brown TC, Carvajal F. Explaining the discrepancy between intentions and actions: The case of hypothetical bias in contingent valuation. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2004; 30: 1108-1122.CrossRefPubMed Ajzen I, Brown TC, Carvajal F. Explaining the discrepancy between intentions and actions: The case of hypothetical bias in contingent valuation. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2004; 30: 1108-1122.CrossRefPubMed
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Waters EA, Hay JL, Orom H, Kiviniemi MT, Drake BF. “Don’t know” responses to risk perception measures implications for underserved populations. Med Decis. 2013; 33: 271-281.CrossRef Waters EA, Hay JL, Orom H, Kiviniemi MT, Drake BF. “Don’t know” responses to risk perception measures implications for underserved populations. Med Decis. 2013; 33: 271-281.CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwartz MD, Taylor KL, Willard KS. Prospective association between distress and mammography utilization among women with a family history of breast cancer. J Behav Med. 2003; 26: 105-117.CrossRefPubMed Schwartz MD, Taylor KL, Willard KS. Prospective association between distress and mammography utilization among women with a family history of breast cancer. J Behav Med. 2003; 26: 105-117.CrossRefPubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Janis IL. Effects of fear arousal on attitude change: Recent developments in theory and research. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 1967; 3: 166-224.CrossRef Janis IL. Effects of fear arousal on attitude change: Recent developments in theory and research. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 1967; 3: 166-224.CrossRef
60.
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Ferrer RA, Klein WMP. Risk perceptions and health behavior. Curr Opin Psychol. 2015, ePub ahead of print. Ferrer RA, Klein WMP. Risk perceptions and health behavior. Curr Opin Psychol. 2015, ePub ahead of print.
Metadaten
Titel
The Tripartite Model of Risk Perception (TRIRISK): Distinguishing Deliberative, Affective, and Experiential Components of Perceived Risk
verfasst von
Rebecca A. Ferrer, PhD
William M. P. Klein, PhD
Alexander Persoskie, PhD
Aya Avishai-Yitshak, BS
Paschal Sheeran, PhD
Publikationsdatum
09.03.2016
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Annals of Behavioral Medicine / Ausgabe 5/2016
Print ISSN: 0883-6612
Elektronische ISSN: 1532-4796
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-016-9790-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 5/2016

Annals of Behavioral Medicine 5/2016 Zur Ausgabe